Next Article in Journal
Evaluation of Multi-Compartment Particle Dampers for the Attenuation of Dynamic Vibrations
Next Article in Special Issue
The Application of Micro-Vibratory Phenomena of a Shape-Memory Alloy Wire to a Novel Vibrator
Previous Article in Journal
Influence of the Suspension Model in the Simulation of the Vertical Vibration Behavior of the Railway Vehicle Car Body
Previous Article in Special Issue
State-of-the-Art Review on the Seismic Performance Assessment of On-Ground Steel Cylindrical Tanks
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Annoyance Caused by Simultaneous Noise and Vibration in Commercial Vehicles: Multimodal Interaction and the Effects of Sinusoidal Components in Recorded Seat Vibrations

by
Maria Mareen Maravich
1,*,
Robert Rosenkranz
1,2 and
M. Ercan Altinsoy
1,2,*
1
Chair of Acoustics and Haptics, Faculty of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Technische Universität Dresden, Helmholtzstraße 18, 01069 Dresden, Germany
2
Centre for Tactile Internet with Human-in-the-Loop (CeTI), 01069 Dresden, Germany
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Vibration 2023, 6(3), 536-555; https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration6030033
Submission received: 31 May 2023 / Revised: 6 July 2023 / Accepted: 7 July 2023 / Published: 13 July 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Feature Papers in Vibration)

Abstract

:
Noise and whole-body vibrations (WBV) inside commercial vehicles can lead to annoyance and reduced comfort. As a result, negative effects on the driver can occur even below the legal exposure limits. In order to understand the annoyance perception and the interaction between noise and WBV, two perception experiments were conducted. For both experiments, recorded signals inside different commercial vehicles were used. Sound pressure and acceleration levels varied. In addition, the frequency content of the recorded vertical seat vibrations was reproduced in different modified variants. The varied parameters (sound pressure level, acceleration level and vibration frequency) were investigated within a three-factorial experimental design. It was found that noise and vibration levels, as well as the vibration spectrum, had a significant effect on total annoyance. Furthermore, an interaction between noise and vibration levels in both experiments could be observed. The results show that for the highest noise level, changing vibration exposure influences annoyance ratings less than the lowest noise level. The results also show that despite the same Wk-weighted RMS level of the WBV according to ISO 2631-1, vibration spectra with sinusoidal components or narrowband vibrations below <10 Hz were significantly perceived as more annoying during a ride in a vehicle.

1. Introduction

Working environments are characterized by different environmental factors, including elements of biological, chemical and/or physical nature. Physical factors in the form of noise and vibrations can occur, for example, in stationary machines or commercial vehicles. When exceeding limit values, sound exposure can lead to hearing loss, and exposure to whole-body vibration (WBV) can cause diseases, e.g., of the spine. However, even below the legally defined lower trigger and daily exposure limit values, sound and vibration can be perceived as annoying in such working situations as well as reduced comfort. This could affect concentration at work, leading to a reduction in performance. Occupational noise and vibration are evaluated based on weighted RMS sound pressure level and, in the case of vibration exposure, on weighted RMS acceleration. However, although both stimuli can occur simultaneously at the workplace, both stimuli are assessed separately, even though humans perceive their environment multimodally [1], and thus, interactions are likely to occur.
Hence, in recent decades, several authors have focused on the exploration of the perception of simultaneous sound and vibration and possible interaction effects between them, aiming to describe this relationship mathematically in the form of models. Many of these studies have been carried out using different exposure values and units as well as different environments where combined noise and vibration can occur, as in buildings [2,3,4,5], vehicles [6,7,8,9], airplanes [10] or helicopters [11,12]. Trolle et al. [13] summarize some studies on WBV in buildings caused by ground transportation. They conclude that in all considered studies, which investigated combined noise and vibration exposure, both stimuli contribute to total annoyance, and, therefore, both should be taken into account.
However, it has not yet been conclusively determined how much the vibration influences total annoyance and how much the noise influences total annoyance. Furthermore, it is unclear whether these influences apply to the entire level range and, if not, how a perceptual interaction might be described. Howarth and Griffin [3] and Paulsen and Kastka [5] suggest prediction equations consisting of additive terms from vibration and noise exposure only but without an extra interaction term. However, in a previous study, Howarth and Griffin [2] considered including a multiplicative term of noise and vibration in the additive equation. However, this did not result in an improvement of the model performance compared to the equation without the interaction term. Paulsen and Kastka [5] concluded that noise has more impact on total annoyance than vibration. Nicht and Altinsoy [7], as well as Maravich and Altinsoy [9], came to similar conclusions in their studies. Maravich and Altinsoy [9] also propose a model in which the interaction is also taken into account as a multiplicative interaction term. Another interaction is presented by Decor et al. [12] for combined noise and vibration exposure in helicopters. The authors found that the discomfort caused by both stimuli can be predicted using an additive equation that includes the difference of both contributions as an additional term. They note that the prediction with the difference of both contributions provides better results than the equation with a multiplicative interaction term. Another way of expressing the interaction or mutual influence of noise and vibration is proposed by Aladdin et al. [14] by defining a perturbation effect of noise on discomfort caused by WBV. They determine ranges in which noise exposure has a synergistic or antagonistic effect on vibration, and predict discomfort by vibration exposure with an additional factor expressing the perturbation effect of noise.
However, not only the exclusion of interaction effects between sound and vibration exposures can lead to deviations in the prediction of total annoyance, but also frequency weightings for noise and for vibration could influence the prediction. In the case of vibration, the International Standard ISO 2631-1 [15] provides frequency weightings for the evaluation of WBV in relation to health considerations as well as comfort aspects. The most common frequency weighting for the vertical direction of vehicle vibration, i.e., WBV during a seated position, is the Wk-weighting. The standard suggests that signals with equal frequency weighted RMS accelerations produce the same perceived discomfort. However, in the past, this standard was also viewed critically [16]. In recent years, there have been studies that have shown that there can be deviations in the evaluation, e.g., that vibrations with identical RMS frequency-weighted signals cause different degrees of comfort [11,16,17,18,19]. In those cases, the discomfort of the vibration could be over- or underestimated. Kaneko et al. [18] investigated whether random vibrations with the same Wk-frequency-weighted RMS acceleration but different vibration spectra cause different comfort reactions. For this purpose, the participants evaluated vibrations with three different spectra (equal intensity at different frequencies; most intensity at the high frequencies and most intensity at the low frequencies) in five different acceleration steps ([0.2, 0.4, 0.8, 1.2, 1.8] m/s2) using a category scale rated with five items, like in ISO 2631-1 [15] in Annex C.2.3. The results show that the participants perceived the low-frequency vibrations as the most annoying vibration followed by the vibration with equal intensity at different frequencies. The vibrations with the high-frequency component were the least annoying vibration. The effect was only present from vibrations with an acceleration of 0.8 m/s2. Low vibration levels at 0.2 m/s2 were not perceived as annoying by the participants, and, therefore, no difference was observed. In contrast, Huang and Li [17] showed that for micro commercial vehicles, despite the same RMS frequency weighted acceleration, vibrations with dominant high frequency components and magnitudes from 1.5 m/s2 were perceived as more annoying than those without.
Thus, in some cases, frequency weighting of the measured acceleration alone might not be sufficient to predict the degree of comfort, and it is possible that other attributes become more important. The use of frequency weightings is also insufficient for the evaluation of noise annoyance in some scenarios. Psychoacoustic parameters such as loudness, sharpness or roughness [20] can be more suitable for quality or annoyance [21]. The question arises whether this equivalence or these methods also apply to vibrations. Declor et al. [11] addressed the issue of improving the evaluation index of the ISO 2631-1 [15] Annex C for the case of amplitude-modulated WBV. They showed that helicopter vibrations frequently contain amplitude modulation, leading to greater discomfort than non-modulated vibrations and thus to an underestimation according to ISO 2631-1 [15]. They suggest a penalty for amplitude-modulated signals, which leads to a better prediction of the discomfort of such signals. Another approach to describe WBV is based on the sensory–perceptual attributes by Rosenkranz and Altinsoy [22]. They describe vibration-based layperson understandable perceptual attributes such as “up and down”, “tingling” and “weak”, which refer to waveforms such as sinusoidal, amplitude-modulated sinusoidal, white Gaussian noise and signal characteristics such as vibration level, frequency, bandwidth and modulation frequency. The relationship between physical parameters and elicited verbal attribute rating were determined through perception experiments with participants on a motion platform. The attribute “tingling” for sinusoidal signals for example, is associated with the frequency range around 50 Hz at levels further above the perception threshold, whereby the sensation described as “up and down” is felt by the participants below 10 Hz. Furthermore, they utilized the observed mappings to synthesize vibration for virtual reality car scenarios [23]. Currently, it is still unclear, whether these attributes are associated with discomfort or annoyance yet. Another approach is provided by the study of Krause et al. [24], in which the unpleasantness of sinusoidal WBV with different frequencies and levels was connected with the relevant body parts in the context of aircraft. Using a body map, they asked their participants in which parts of the body the discomfort was perceived. The results show that different body parts are involved in the perception of unpleasantness, but for all examined frequencies, the buttock is associated with higher unpleasantness.
Many studies mainly concentrate on evaluating the perception of WBV, while others investigate the interaction between sound and vibration and mostly focus on the intensity of both factors. The perception of annoyance could be influenced by different characteristics of sound and vibration, which might vary depending on the type of vehicle. In a field study comparing electric and combustion engine vehicle, Wang et al. [25] searched for sound and vibration characteristics of different vehicle types in terms of overall ride comfort. The characteristics that were important for the annoyance judgment differed depending on the vehicle type. However, only a limited number of studies explicitly investigate the interaction of spectral as well as temporal characteristics in both sound and vibration signals. In a series of studies, Oetjen et al. [26] investigated the perception of modulation-based psychoacoustic measures in the context of WBV. In one study, they investigated the perception of attributes such as roughness and unpleasantness of recorded vehicle sounds in the presence of vibration. They found that the perception of the attributes was affected by the change in vibration level and concluded that the evaluation of sound quality in vehicles by purely acoustical perception experiments might not reflect reality. Töpken et al. [27], for example, examined the interaction between the spectrum of sound and vibrations with respect to congruent tonal components in both stimuli using the example of the pleasantness of sound and vibrations on airplane seats. However, interactions between frequency levels and with or without a congruent tone were not significant. Maravich and Altinsoy [9,28] also studied the influence of different vibration spectra of simultaneous noise and vibration. The interaction effects between noise and vibration levels were significant, but the vibration spectrum had no significant influence on the total annoyance, although there are tendencies that the lower frequency components of the WBV are perceived as more annoying despite equal Wk-weighted levels in one of their studies [9]. However, the results were ambiguous, possibly because of potential crosstalk to other axis and due to the small number of participants in one of their studies [9]. A different type of signal was investigated by Festa et al. [29]. They examined the mutual effects of tactile and auditory stimuli on the intensity perception of transient road excitations. Their results showed that neither vibration nor noise had any influence on the perception of the intensity of the other stimulus.
To investigate the interactions of noise and WBV and the influence of the vibration spectrum of frequency-weighted WBV in commercial vehicles, two experiments with simultaneous sound and WBV were conducted under the following assumptions: (1) There is an interaction between noise and WBV. (2) Sinusoidal components in WBV change the perception of total annoyance in commercial vehicles despite the same Wk-weighted RMS vibration. Both experiments are based on recorded signals, which have been modified.
In experiment 1, the described perceptual attributes of WBV by Rosenkranz and Altinsoy [22], which were presented in this section, are used. Two different frequencies, which are connected to different perceptual attributes, were selected and inserted or removed as sinusoidal components in the recorded vibration signal. In experiment 2, a recorded vibration signal was strongly modified from the original recording, and the categories broadband, narrowband, sinusoidal as well as mixed WBV were created and could be compared.
Both experiments are part of an experimental series of a DFG-funded project. The aim of this project is to create a prediction model for the annoyance of simultaneous noise and vibrations in vehicles. For this purpose, the signals recorded in vehicles are modified to create further realistic scenes. These scenes are connected to the annoyance ratings of the participants in order to create a prediction model from the entirety of the data.

2. Experimental Setup

Both perception experiments were carried out in the multimodal measurement laboratory of the TU Dresden [30], see Figure 1, which can reproduce seat vibrations and noise simultaneously.
For the reproduction of the seat vibrations, there is a hydraulic motion platform (Hagenbuch Hydraulic Systems AG, Ebikon, Switzerland) on which a vehicle seat is mounted. This platform is able to reproduce motions in three translational and three rotational directions. In order to complement the system, there is also an electrodynamic shaker (ESE 201 Typ 11075, VEB Schwingungstechnik und Akustik WIB, Dresden, Germany) under the vehicle seat, which can reproduce vertical vibrations for higher frequencies. For each participant and before each experiment, the transfer function of the electrodynamic shaker was compensated by using an FIR filter. The reproduced vibrations of the whole system, including crosstalk to lateral axes, were measured using a seat pad (Triaxial Seat Accelerometer Type 4515B, Brüel & Kjær Sound & Vibration Measurement A/S, Nærum, Denmark) and described as well as discussed in the respective sections.
The sound was presented via headphones (DT 990 Pro 250 OHM, Sennheiser, Germany) with a preamp (One-AMP G93, Lake People electronic GmbH, Konstanz, Germany). The level of the stimuli was calibrated using a head and torso simulator (45BB KEMAR Head and Torso, GRAS Sound & Vibration, Holte, Denmark) with integrated microphones (Type 40AD, GRAS Sound & Vibration, Holte, Denmark).
In order to make sure that the background noise of the motion platform was masked by the stimuli, the noise was measured by using a binaural headset (BHS II, Head Acoustics, Herzogenrath, Germany). The A-weighted RMS sound pressure level of the lowest reproduced level was more than 15 dB higher than the background noise of the platform.
The Graphical User Interface (GUI) for evaluation was presented by using a projector, and the entries could be made using a mouse and numerical pad.

3. Experiment 1

3.1. Hypothesis

In addition to the interaction effects between sound and vibrations in vehicles, this study also investigates the perception of the vibration spectrum in a multimodal context.
When perceiving acoustic signals, certain perceptual characteristics of noises, such as the presence of sinusoidal components, can lead to greater annoyance, despite the same A-weighted RMS levels, which are taken into account in current standards as a tonality penalty [31,32]. Furthermore, there are also indications that higher-frequency tonal components are perceived as more annoying than those below 1 kHz in comparison to equal loudness contours [33].
Such established psychoacoustic relationships, as in acoustics, in which, in this case, the presence of sinusoidal components leads to greater annoyance, do not exist in the perception of WBV and have only rarely been investigated. Due to the assumption that WBV with the same Wk-weighted RMS values should cause the same discomfort, there are fewer studies on whether some perceptual properties can possibly increase the discomfort or whether such properties only become relevant with simultaneous noise.
Rosenkranz and Altinsoy [22] developed a design language for WBV in the context of virtual environments independent of existing WBV evaluation, such as ISO 2631-1 [15]. They investigate different perceptual attributes for the perception of different frequency ranges of different vibration signal types.
They found out that sinusoidal vibration, 36 dB above the perception threshold, in the range of 5–10 Hz, is perceived as very “up and down”, and sinusoidal vibration over 35 Hz, with a maximum of 50 Hz, is perceived as “tingling”. In an older study by Jones and Saunders [34], female subjects also reported a “tingling” sensation during exposure to WBV in the 60–80 Hz range.
This experiment investigates whether the dominance of different perceptual attributes (“up and down” and “tingling”) in recorded seat vibrations with simultaneous noise, lead to different levels of annoyance. For this purpose, additional sinusoidal components with two different frequencies, 50 Hz for the perceptual sensation “tingling” and 6 Hz for the perceptual sensation “up and down”, were inserted separately and together into the recorded vertical vehicle seat vibrations.

3.2. Stimuli

For experiment 1, acoustic and vibration signals from a big street sweeper on asphalt were used, which were recorded from the driver’s perspective. The street sweeper was provided by Stadtreinigung Dresden. The sound recordings were made with a binaural headset (BHS II, Head Acoustics, Herzogenrath, Germany), and the seat vibrations were recorded using a triaxial seat pad (Triaxial Seat Accelerometer Type 4515B, Brüel & Kjær Sound & Vibration Measurement A/S, Nærum, Denmark).
Sound pressure and vibration levels were not reproduced at the original level for every stimulus because the experiments are part of a series of experiments investigating total annoyance below or close to the daily exposure limits of 106–115 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2) and adjusted to the same levels for this purpose. However, they reflect realistic exposures. The vibration signal of the street sweeper was amplified to 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2), and the frequency content was modified, resulting in four variations, see Figure 2. All four vibration signals were high-pass filtered from 2 Hz, and the following variations were created (VX.X.: VibrationSpectraNumber. Experiment-Number):
V1.1: Frequency content (FC) of a big street sweeper
V2.1: FC + 6 Hz, +50 Hz sinusoidal component
V3.1: FC + 50 Hz sinusoidal component
V4.1: FC + 6 Hz sinusoidal component
Figure 2. FFT (Spectrum size: 262144, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of different vibration spectra of vertical seat vibrations of experiment 1 (t = 4 s, LV = 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)).
Figure 2. FFT (Spectrum size: 262144, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of different vibration spectra of vertical seat vibrations of experiment 1 (t = 4 s, LV = 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)).
Vibration 06 00033 g002
Since the street sweeper contains a 50 Hz sinusoidal component in its original vibration frequency content, this example is based on reality and can certainly have practical relevance. All vibration signals V1.1–V4.1 were adjusted to identical Wk-weighted RMS levels (0.4 m/s2 or 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)), which means that inserting and amplifying the sinusoidal component, might lead to a changing weighting of the rest of the signal, see Figure 2. All peaks of sinusoidal components have the same RMS value of 108 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2) at the stimulus step of 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2).
The four vibration spectra V1.1-V4.1 were adjusted to +3 dB, −3 dB and −6 dB resulting in four different vibration levels LV = (106, 109, 112, 115) dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2), corresponding to (0.20, 0.28, 0.40, 0.56) m/s2 Wk, for each vibration spectrum. The measured crosstalk of the vibration stimuli on the horizontal axes was comparable for all four vibration stimuli types and increased with increasing vertical vibration level. On the x-axis, “fore and aft”, (90–97 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)), the crosstalk was higher than on the y-axis, “left and right”, (79–86 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)). Since signals are only classified as perceptible from approx. 85 dBW, according to VDI 2057 [35], lateral vibrations on the x-axis can be neglected. However, according to VDI 2057 [35], the signals on the y-axis were “easily perceptible”. Since the level differences between the vertical and horizontal axes were at least 10 dB, the vector sum was calculated in accordance with ISO 2631-1 [15]. The vertical axis is, therefore, the dominant axis, and the crosstalk on the x-axis can be neglected.
The sound signal was high-pass filtered for plausible reproduction, while the remaining frequency content was retained, see Figure 3. The sound signal was presented simultaneously with the vibration. Three different noise levels LN = (64, 70, 76) dBA with no change in frequency were used.
There was also a pure acoustical reference signal used in both of the experiments of this study; see Figure 3 (right) and Section 3.3. The reference signal was recorded in a mini excavator with the already described equipment, high-pass filtered for plausible reproduction as well as adjusted to LN = 70 dBA. Both acoustical stimuli (Reference and sweeper) were additionally presented without vibrations in four levels LN = (64, 70, 76, 79) dBA.
In the first experiment, 56 stimuli were presented: 48 vibro-acoustic and eight acoustic stimuli. All stimuli have a duration of four seconds and were reproduced without visual feedback.

3.3. Design and Methodology

Both experiments were fundamentally structured as a 3-factorial design with the three factors “vibration frequency” (4 levels), “sound pressure level” (3 levels), and “vibration level” (4 levels), which corresponds to a total of 48 (4 × 3 × 4) stimuli. In addition, the acoustic signals (2 × 4) were also presented, as described in Section 3.2. The structure of the 56 stimuli to be evaluated is shown in the block diagram of Figure 4.
Magnitude estimation with a reference (numerical value 100), see stimuli in Section 3.2, was used to evaluate the stimuli. With the help of the GUI from Figure 5, the participants were asked to rate the total annoyance of the stimuli in comparison to the reference (in Figure 5 left: field below “A”), with a numerical value (in Figure 5 left: empty field below “B”). The reference and stimulus to be evaluated were played sequentially (8 s total, 4 s per stimulus) after the long button labeled “A B” was pressed. The reference, therefore, had to be listened to at every trial. After the “A B” button was pressed, the reference appeared with the visual feedback from Figure 5, top right, and then the stimulus was evaluated with the visual feedback from Figure 5, bottom right.
The participants had to evaluate 56 stimuli, which could be repeated at any time by pressing the “A B” button again. After adding a number in the field below “B”, the participants had to press the “Rate” button to load the next stimulus. The stimuli were presented in randomized order for each participant. Before the experiment, four training stimuli were presented to familiarize participants with the situation and to explain the procedure. The training stimuli were the same for each subject and in the same order. An experimental run per participant with an explanation lasted a maximum of one hour, with the subjects sitting on the motion platform for a maximum of 30 min.

3.4. Participants

In this experiment, 23 healthy participants voluntarily attended. The participant mean age was 32.4 years, with a standard deviation of 11.8 years. In total, four female participants and 19 male participants attended the experiment. Most of them were students or employers at the university.

3.5. Results

The results of the vibro-acoustic stimuli and the acoustic stimuli were analyzed separately. Therefore, two Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (rmANOVA) were run with the log-transformed data using IBM SPSS Statistics. First, a three-way rmANOVA with the results of the 48 vibro-acoustic stimuli was carried out with the following factors: “vibration frequency” with the levels V1.1, V2.1, V3.1, V4.1, “sound pressure level” with the levels (64, 70, 76) dBA and “acceleration level” with the levels (106, 109, 112, 115) dBW (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2). Second, a two-way rmANOVA with the results of the eight acoustic stimuli was carried out with the factors “vehicle type” (Street sweeper and mini excavator) and “sound pressure level” with the levels (64, 70, 76, 79) dBA. The Mauchly test was used for checking sphericity. In case of violation of sphericity, the degree of freedom was corrected by using the Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment.
Before running the rmANOVA the Shapiro–Wilk test was performed on the 56 stimuli to check the groups for normal distribution. Five of these groups were, according to the Shapiro–Wilk test, not normally distributed. These groups were visually checked by using the histograms. No strong deviations from the normal distribution were observed. The reference (Mini excavator with LN = 70 dBA) was also presented in the experiment. A total of 22 of 23 participants rated this stimulus with 100, and only one person with 110. It can be assumed that all participants evaluated the experiment attentively.

3.5.1. Vibro-Acoustic Stimuli: Main Effects

Figure 6 shows all results of magnitude estimation as boxplots and the geometric mean values. The slope of the ratings at high sound pressure levels (LN = 76 dBA) over the acceleration levels seems flatter than the slope of the ratings at lower sound pressure levels (LN = 64 dBA). The color bars symbolize the different vibration spectra.
The analysis of the vibro-acoustic stimuli showed a highly significant influence of sound pressure and acceleration level (F(1.101, 24.216) = 95.278, p < .001, partial η2 = .812 and F(1.405, 30.901) = 106.445, p < .001, partial η2 = .829) and a very strong effect size according to Cohen [36]. Bonferroni-corrected pair comparisons also showed that all steps of sound pressure and acceleration level differ significantly from each other (p < .001).
The main factor, “vibration frequency”, also showed a significant effect (F(3, 66) = 13.038, p < .001, partial η2 = .372) and a very strong effect size, according to Cohen [36]. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni-correction showed that the vibration spectrum of V2.1 (+6 Hz, +50 Hz) and V4.1 (+6 Hz) differs significantly from V1.1 (frequency content of a big street sweeper) and V3.1 (+50 Hz), see Table 1. The mean values showed that the stimuli with the 6 Hz sinusoidal component, V2.1 and V4.1, were generally perceived as more annoying than stimuli V1.1 and V3.1 without the extra 6 Hz component. Despite the 50 Hz sinusoidal component, there was no difference in the significance between V2.1 and V4.1 (p = 1.000), or between V1.1 and V3.1 (p = 1.000), see Table 1.

3.5.2. Vibro-Acoustic Stimuli: Interaction Effects

The interaction between sound pressure and acceleration level is highly significant (F(6, 132) = 6.206, p < .001, partial η2 = .220), and according to Cohen [36], to be interpreted as a very strong effect.
The higher the sound pressure level, the less the influence of increasing acceleration on the annoyance rating. If the sound pressure level is lower, the acceleration level also has more weight in the evaluation, and the annoyance ratings increase more steeply with increasing acceleration level, see Figure 7.
The other two first-order interactions, sound pressure level and vibration frequency and acceleration level and vibration frequency, showed no statistically significant effect, (F(3.865, 85.029) = 1.694, p = .161) and (F(5.156, 113.435) = 1.294, p = .271), as well as the second-order interaction of all three main factors (F(18, 396) = 0.978, p = .48).

3.5.3. Acoustic Stimuli: Main and Interaction Effects

The unimodal acoustic stimuli were analyzed separately using an rmANOVA with the main factors “vehicle type” (2 levels) and “sound pressure level” (4 levels), see Section 3.5. The results showed a highly significant effect of sound pressure level (F(1.357, 29.855) = 137.197, p < .001, partial η2 = .862) with a very high effect size, according to Cohen [36]. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons showed that all sound pressure level steps are significantly different from each other (p < .001). There were no significant effect between the vehicle types (F(1, 22) = 1.049, p = .317). The interaction of sound pressure level and vehicle type showed no statistically significant effect (F(2, 44) = 1.265, p = .317).

4. Experiment 2

4.1. Hypothesis

Like the previous experiment, see Section 3.1, this experiment examines the interaction effects of noise and vibration levels, the vibration spectrum, and their influence on total annoyance. Since frequency-weightings for WBV can also lead to different ratings, see Section 1, this experiment aims to investigate the possible difference in annoyance in a multimodal context of sinusoidal vibration and random vibration below approx. 20 Hz, as well as a combination of both. Using real recorded signals, the question comes up whether a single sinusoidal vibration is perceived as more annoying than a complex vibration containing a sinusoidal component or as a complex signal without any sinusoidal component.

4.2. Stimuli

Experiment 2 is based on acoustic and vibration signals from a refuse collection vehicle on the highway. Like in experiment 1, the signals were recorded from the driver’s view. Stadtreinigung Dresden provided the refuse collection vehicle, and the scenes were recorded during a normal shift. The sound recordings were made with two microphones (Type 4188 with Preamplifier Type 2671, Brüel & Kjær Sound & Vibration Measurement A/S, Nærum, Denmark) mounted at the driver’s seat at ear position, and the seat vibrations were recorded using a triaxial seat pad (Triaxial Seat Accelerometer Type 4515B, Brüel & Kjær Sound & Vibration Measurement A/S, Nærum, Denmark).
In order to compare both experiments, the chosen vibration signal was also adjusted to 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2), and the frequency content varied, see Figure 8.
All vibration signals were high-pass filtered from 2 Hz, and four variations were generated and labeled as follows (VX.X.: VibrationSpectraNumber. Experiment-Number):
V1.2: Frequency content (FC) of a refuse collection vehicle: “Sine in Broadband”
V2.2: 4.75 Hz Sinus: “Sine”
V3.2: FC without 4.75 Hz Sine: “Broadband”
V4.2: V3.2 filtered from 4 to 6 Hz: “Narrowband”
The vibration spectrum (V1.2) of the refuse collection vehicle on the highway contains a sinusoidal component, 4.75 Hz (“Sine in Broadband”). Based on this signal structure, the frequency variation V2.2 consists of a single sine of 4.75 Hz (“Sine”). V3.2 consists of the vibration spectrum V1.2 without the 4.75 Hz sinusoidal component (“Broadband”), and V4.2 is a narrowband version of V3.2 from 4 to 6 Hz (“Narrowband”). The following four variants of the vibration spectrum result: a sinusoidal component in a broadband signal (V1.2), a pure sine signal (V2.2), a broadband signal with no sinusoidal component (V3.2), and narrowband noise (V4.2).
All vibration signals V1.2–V4.2 were adjusted to identical Wk-weighted RMS levels (0.4 m/s2 or 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)). The vibration spectra V1.2–V4.2 were adjusted to +3 dB, −3 dB and −6 dB resulting in four different levels (106, 109, 112, 115) dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2), corresponding to (0.20, 0.28, 0.40, 0.56) m/s2 Wk, for each vibration spectrum identical to experiment 1.
The measured crosstalk of the vibration stimuli on the horizontal axes was comparable for all four vibration stimuli types and increased with increasing vertical vibration level. On the x-axis, “fore and aft”, (90–100 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)), the crosstalk was higher than on the y-axis, “left and right”, (78–88 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)). “Left and right” vibrations can be neglected, according to VDI 2057 [35]. The signals on the y-axis were “easily perceptible”. Except for signal V3.1 (“Broadband”), all signals had comparable crosstalk on the horizontal axes. While the crosstalk on the x-axis for the vibration spectra V1.2, V2.2 and V4.2 ranged from 92 to 100 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2) depending on the level, the crosstalk for V3.2 was around 5 dB lower at 88–95 dBWd (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2). This should be taken into account when interpreting the results.
Despite the crosstalk, the level difference from the vertical to the horizontal axes was at least over 10 dB, from which it can be assumed that the vertical axis is the dominant axis according to ISO 2631-1 [15].
Like in experiment 1, the sound signal was high-pass filtered for plausible reproduction with no further changes in frequency structure, see Figure 9. The sound signals were presented simultaneously with the vibration signals with three different sound pressure levels (64, 70, 76) dBA. The reference was the same as in experiment 1; see Section 3.2. Both acoustical stimuli (Reference and refuse collection vehicle) were additionally also presented without vibrations in four sound pressure level steps (64, 70, 76, 79) dBA.
Experiment 2 includes 56 stimuli: 48 vibro-acoustic and eight acoustic stimuli. Each of them has a duration of four seconds and no visual feedback.

4.3. Design and Methodology

The design and procedure of the second experiment were identical to the first experiment and were described in more detail in Section 3.2.

4.4. Participants

In this Experiment, 23 healthy participants voluntarily attended. The participant mean age was 31.1 years (Standard deviation 10.8 years). In total, five female and 18 male participants attended the experiment. Most of them were students or employees at the university. Of these 23 participants of this experiment, 16 participants have already taken part in experiment 1 and thus had previous experience with regard to the evaluation method. The experiments took place on different days at least four weeks apart.

4.5. Results

Because of the same design, the results of the annoyance ratings were analyzed, like in experiment 1. Therefore, the log-transformed data of the vibro-acoustic and acoustic stimuli were analyzed separately with two different Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (rmANOVA) using IBM SPSS Statistics. For the results of the 48 vibro-acoustic stimuli, a three-way rmANOVA was carried out with the factors: “vibration frequency” with the levels V1.2, V2.2, V3.2, V4.2, “sound pressure level” with the levels (64, 70, 76) dBA and “acceleration level” with the levels (106, 109, 112, 115) dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2). For the results of the eight acoustic stimuli, a two-way rmANOVA with was carried out with the factors “vehicle type” (refuse collection vehicle and mini excavator) and “sound pressure level” with the levels (64, 70, 76, 79) dBA. Sphericity was checked with the Mauchly test, and Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment was used to correct the degree of freedom in case of any violation.
Before running the rmANOVA, the normal distribution was checked for the results of the 56 stimuli by using the Shapiro–Wilk test. The results show, according to the Shapiro–Wilk test, that approx. a quarter of the groups are not normally distributed. All these groups were visually checked using a histogram. No strong deviations from the normal distribution were observed. The reference (mini excavator LN = 70 dBA) was repeated in the experiment. A total of 22 of 23 participants rated this stimulus with 100 and only one participant with 110, so that can be assumed that all participants evaluated the experiment attentively.

4.5.1. Vibro-Acoustic Stimuli: Main Effects

Figure 10 shows the results of relative total annoyance ratings using the magnitude estimation of each stimulus group as boxplot and the geometric mean values. The different color bars symbolize the vibration spectra (V1.2–V4.2).
Comparable to the results of experiment 1, the slope of the ratings at high sound pressure levels (LN = 76 dBA) over the acceleration levels is flatter than the slope of the ratings at lower sound pressure levels (LN = 64 dBA).
The analysis of the vibro-acoustic stimuli showed a highly significant influence of sound pressure and acceleration level (F(1.068, 23.493) = 57.899, p < .001, partial η2 = .725 and F(1.822, 40.084) = 182.552, p < .001, partial η2 = .892) and a very strong effect size according to Cohen [36]. Bonferroni-corrected pair comparisons also showed that all levels of sound pressure and acceleration level differ significantly from each other (p < .001).
The main factor, “vibration frequency”, also showed a significant effect (F(3, 66) = 33.080, p < .001, partial η2 = .601) and a very strong effect size, according to Cohen [36]. Stimuli combinations with the label “Sine in Broadband” were perceived as more annoying than “Broadband” but less than ”Narrowband” and “Sine”. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction showed that all vibration spectra differ significantly from each other except V2.2 (“Sine”) and V4.2 (“Narrowband”), see Table 2. The stimuli with the vibration spectra labeled “Narrowband” and “Sine” were perceived as the most annoying. Stimuli with the vibration spectrum “Broadband” were perceived as least annoying.

4.5.2. Vibro-Acoustic Stimuli: Interaction Effects

The interaction between sound pressure and acceleration level is highly significant (F(6132) = 11.737, p < .001, partial η2 = .348) and, according to Cohen [36], to be interpreted as a very strong effect. In this experiment, there is a comparable kind of interaction observed, like in the first experiment, which was described in Section 3.5.2.
The other two first-order interactions, sound pressure level and vibration frequency, as well as acceleration level and vibration frequency, showed no statistically significant effect, (F(3.835, 84.375) = 2.396, p = .059) and (F(4.518, 99.404) = 1.198, p = .298), as well as the interaction of all three main factors (F(7.684, 169.044) = 0.974, p = .489).

4.5.3. Acoustic Stimuli: Main and Interaction Effects

The acoustic stimuli were analyzed separately using an rmANOVA with the main factors “vehicle type” (2 levels) and “sound pressure level” (4 levels); see Section 4.5.
The results show a highly significant influence of the sound pressure level (F(1.659, 36.498) = 170.159, p < .001, partial η2 = .886) with a very high effect size, according to Cohen [36]. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons showed that all levels differ significantly from each other (p < .001).
The sound frequency (“vehicle type”) has a significant effect (F(1, 22) = 10.333, p = .004, partial η2 = .320) with a high effect size, according to Cohen [36]. The anchor stimuli of the mini excavator were perceived as more annoying than the noise of the refuse collection vehicle.
The interaction of sound pressure level and sound frequency showed no statistically significant effect (F(1.837, 40.412) = 0.152, p = .928).

5. Discussion

5.1. Influence of Sound Pressure Level, Acceleration Level and Interaction Effects

Both experiments showed high effect sizes (partial η2 from 0.7 to 0.9) for sound pressure and acceleration levels. Both stimuli seem to have an important role in the evaluation of total annoyance, which is also consistent with the literature [8,13].
The interaction of both stimuli showed a strong effect size in both experiments (partial η2 = .220 and partial η2 = .348) as well as the same type of interaction, see Figure 7. When the sound pressure level is higher, acceleration has less influence on total annoyance because the sound pressure level seems to be the dominant stimulus and masks the annoyance caused by vibration; but when the sound pressure level is lower, vibration has more weight, and both of them have an important role in the rating of total annoyance. It seems that the noise masked the discomfort of the vibration because during lower noise exposures, the same vibration levels occur to a higher increase in annoyance than it was the case at high noise levels.
Despite a different design and analysis of the study, Huang and Griffin [37] also reported a masking effect of noise to the discomfort of vibration, which is similar to the interaction effect in the current study. To apply this type of interaction, they used a roots-sums-of-squares (r.s.s.) model; when one stimulus has a particularly high magnitude and the other has a low magnitude, the stimulus with high magnitude masked the other with its annoyance. Mathematically, this procedure is comparable to the calculation of the vector sum of ISO 2631-1 [15], which is intended to estimate the discomfort of triaxial acceleration when there is no dominant axis of exposure. However, this is a mutual effect, but in Huang and Griffin’s and the current study, it was observed that only noise masked the vibration. However, Huang and Griffin note that at low noise levels and higher vibration magnitudes, outside of their investigated exposure values, such a masking effect for vibrations at low noise levels can occur.
Some authors also found, like in the current study, a significant interaction between noise and vibration magnitude with the statistical method of ANOVA [8,9,38]. According to the ANOVA, the interaction can be described as a non-additive connection of two main factors. Using the interaction as an extra term in a prediction equation for total annoyance, in which vibration and noise magnitude are multiplied, was, for example, proposed by Maravich and Altinsoy [9]. Piranda et al. [8] compared several model approaches from the literature and concluded that the purely additive model, i.e., a prediction from the addition of the magnitudes of both stimuli without an interaction term, predicted their data the least. As a result, they decided on the “energy differences” model.
Contrary to the results of the current study, some authors predicted the total annoyance without interaction as a sum of noise and vibration magnitude [2,3,37]. Howarth and Griffin [2] also added an interaction term as a multiplication of both stimuli to their additive equation but did not observe an increase in correlation in predicting annoyance with the interaction term.
However, the results of this study suggest that interaction in some form should be integrated into the prediction of total annoyance, for example, as an additional term in which the magnitude of sound and vibration is multiplied.

5.2. Influence of Sound Frequency

In order to investigate the influence of the spectrum of the sound signal without simultaneous vibration, the anchor stimuli of the mini excavator, including the reference stimulus (Ref), see Figure 3 and Figure 9, as well as the acoustic signals N.1 and N.2, were presented unimodal in four levels LN = (64, 70, 76, 79) dBA.
In experiment 1, there was no significant difference in sound frequency, i.e., between the mini excavator and street sweeper. However, in experiment 2, there was a significant difference between the mini excavator and refuse collection vehicle despite equal A-weighted RMS levels, with the mini excavator signal being perceived as more annoying. Even the loudness values according to the Standard DIN 45631 [39] seem not to be able to fully explain these results. Although the mini excavator signal with an RMS level of 70 dBA has a loudness of 31.1 Sone, the street sweeper and refuse collection vehicle have a more comparable loudness of 28.2 and 27.2 Sone.
When looking at the FFT in Figure 3 and Figure 9, it is noticeable that the mini excavator signal and the sweeper have a more pronounced low, rather a tonal component around 50 Hz, but the refuse collection vehicle does not. Low-frequency noise can lead to considerable annoyance in various contexts [40]. Especially low-frequency tonal components seem to be involved in perceived annoyance, as can occur, for example, with wind turbines and domestic use heat sources [41]. However, these are not taken into account for the noises according to the Standard DIN 45681 [31], which only considers tonal components equal to or greater than 90 Hz. The Standard DIN 45680 [42] gives procedures for the consideration of spectral or temporal peculiarities of low-frequency noise below 100 Hz in buildings, but these are only informative for environments outside buildings. Specifically for low-frequency noise in commercial vehicles, there is no binding, generally valid procedure for assessment so far.
These experiments give an indication that the A-weighting and possibly also the loudness cannot alone explain the annoyance in commercial vehicles and that more experiments should be conducted as well as other parameters should be proposed for the evaluation.

5.3. Influence of Vibration Frequency

In experiment 1, the influence of different sinusoidal components (6 Hz, 50 Hz) in a vibration signal was investigated in the context of the total annoyance of simultaneous noise and vibration in commercial vehicles. Since the perceptual attributes for low (6 Hz) and higher (50 Hz) sinusoidal components are different (“up and down” and “tingling”) [22], it was investigated whether these could also lead to different perceptions of annoyance, even if the vibration has the same Wk-weighted RMS levels. It turned out that the additional 50 Hz sinusoidal component had no influence on the evaluation of the participants. However, adding a 6 Hz sinusoidal component led to an increase in the annoyance of the corresponding signals despite the same Wk-weighted levels. This suggests that Wk- weighting in a multimodal context does not always lead to comparable annoyance and that the “tingling” sensation in vibration signals and, thus, higher-frequency components in the vibration spectrum probably has no influence on the overall annoyance when Wk-weighting is applied for acceleration until 0.56 m/s2 Wk.
In experiment 2, all vibration stimuli were limited below 20 Hz in order to investigate whether there can also be differences in low-frequency spectra, as these are particularly common in the automotive sector. In particular, the stimuli with the vibration spectra V2.2 (“Sine”) and V4.2 (“Narrowband”) were perceived as more annoying than the other two vibrations. It seems that focused energy in the low-frequency range of vibrations increases annoyance. For the perception of annoyance in multimodal vehicle scenes, it made no difference in the experiment whether it was a low-frequent tonal or narrowband vibration signal. The presence of low-frequency weighting in the 5 Hz range was crucial. What band limits this effect has, was not investigated in this experiment. In addition, these statements apply to vehicle surroundings with additional noise exposure.
Compared to V2.2 (“Sine”), V1.1 (“Sine in Broadband”) had lower annoyance but higher annoyance than V3.1 (“Broadband”), i.e., a signal with no centered energy and sine. However, this result should also be discussed in the context of crosstalk on other axes because of the experimental setup. The crosstalk to both horizontal directions of all stimuli was comparable, except for stimulus V3.2 (“Broadband”). The crosstalk on the “fore-aft”-axis of stimulus V3.2 (“Broadband”) was about 5 dB lower than the crosstalk on the lateral axes of the other three signal variations. This means that these stimuli with this vibration could be perceived as less annoying since multiaxial excitations can lead to a higher overall level when calculating the vector sum and could be perceived as more unpleasant, see ISO 2631-1 [15]. However, since the vertical axis was also the dominant axis after calculating the vector sum, this probably has no influence on the results. If there is any influence, there might not be a significant difference between V1.2 (“Sinus in Broadband”) and V3.2 (“Broadband”), but most likely, it would not affect the other assumptions, so it can be assumed that, nonetheless, narrowband low-frequency vibrations are perceived as more annoying and may be underestimated by the Wk-weighting.
The study by Kaneko et al. [18] also showed that low-frequency weighting is generally perceived as more unpleasant despite the use of Wk-weighting. In their study, the broadband signals with a low-frequency weighting were perceived as less pleasant than broadband signals with a higher frequency or without any special weighting. Huang and Li [17] came to contrary results, but only from an acceleration of 1.5 m/s2 Wk, which was no longer examined in the present study.
In the current study, different vibrations with Wk-weighting were investigated in more detail, but also temporal variations in the vibration signal should be considered in research. Overall, more research is needed, especially in the context of vibro-acoustic stimuli and their spectral or temporal variations.

6. Conclusions

Based on two studies with a three-factorial design, it was shown that both, the sound pressure and the acceleration level, make an important contribution to the assessment of the total annoyance of simultaneous noise and WBV in vehicles using the example of commercial vehicles. Furthermore, a significant interaction could also be shown, which can be described as follows: If the sound pressure level is high, the vibration has less influence on the evaluation of the total annoyance, but if the sound pressure level is lower, the vibration level has more influence. In addition, an effect of the vibration frequency could be detected in both experiments, which suggests that despite equally Wk-weighted RMS levels, low-frequency vibration <10 Hz consisting of narrowband vibration or including sinusoidal components are perceived as more annoying than vibrations without such weightings in the spectrum.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.M.M. and M.E.A.; methodology, M.M.M. and M.E.A.; Software, M.M.M. and R.R.; investigation, M.M.M.; writing—original draft, M.M.M.; project administration, M.M.M.; formal analysis, M.M.M.; visualization, M.M.M.; funding acquisition, M.E.A.; supervision, M.E.A.; writing—review and editing, R.R. and M.E.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by German Research Foundation (DFG, Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) AL1473/7-1 and as a part of Germany’s Excellence Strategy—EXC 2050/1—Project ID 390696704—Cluster of Excellence “Centre for Tactile Internet with Human-in-the-Loop” (CeTI) of TU Dresden. The Article Processing Charges (APC) were funded by the joint publication funds of the TU Dresden, including Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine, and the SLUB Dresden as well as the Open Access Publication Funding of the DFG.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Technische Universität Dresden (protocol code SR-EK-111032020 and date of approval 12 June 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The evaluation results of the participants can be received from the corresponding authors in anonymized form.

Acknowledgments

The Authors would like to thank Stadtreinigung Dresden for supporting organizationally and personally the recordings in their commercial vehicles, the refuse collection vehicle and the street sweeper. The Authors also would like to thank Margitta Lachmann for her support to conduct a part of the perception experiments.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Altinsoy, M.E. Auditory-Tactile Interaction in Virtual Environments; Shaker Verlag: Aachen, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  2. Howart, H.V.C.; Griffin, M. Subjective response to combined noise and vibration: Summation and interaction effects. J. Sound Vib. 1990, 143, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Howarth, H.V.C.; Griffin, M.J. The annoyance caused by simultaneous noise and vibration from railways. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1991, 89, 2317–2323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lee, P.J.; Griffin, M.J. Combined effect of noise and vibration produced by high-speed trains on annoyance in buildings. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 133, 2126–2135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Paulsen, R.; Kastka, J. Effects of combined noise and vibration on annoyance. J. Sound Vib. 1995, 181, 295–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Huang, Y.; Griffin, M.J. The effects of sound level and vibration magnitude on the relative discomfort of noise and vibration. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 4558–4569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Nicht, A.; Altinsoy, M.E. Multimodale Lästigkeit von Lärm und Vibrationen in Nutzfahrzeugen [Multimodal annoyance of noise and vibration in commercial vehicles]. Z. Lärmbekämpfung 2015, 5, 212–216. [Google Scholar]
  8. Piranda, D.; Laroche, L.; Parizet, E.; Bornet, F. Multi-excitation discomfort in a driving car: Contribution from sound and vibrations. In Proceedings of the Forum Acusticum, Lyon, France, 7–11 December 2020. [Google Scholar]
  9. Maravich, M.M.; Altinsoy, E. Influence of Seat Vibration Frequency on Total Annoyance and Interaction Effects Caused by Simultaneous Noise and Seat Vibrations in Commercial Vehicles. Vibration 2022, 5, 183–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Quehl, J. Comfort Studies on Aircraft Interior Sound and Vibration; Shaker Verlag: Aachen, Germany, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  11. Delcor, L.; Parizet, E.; Ganivet-Ouzeneau, J.; Caillet, J. Assessment of helicopter passengers’ vibration discomfort: Proposal for improvement of the ISO 2631-1 standard. Ergonomics 2022, 65, 296–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Delcor, L.; Parizet, E.; Ganivet-Ouzeneau, J.; Caillet, J. Model of sound and vibration discomfort in helicopter cabins. Appl. Acoust. 2022, 195, 108847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Trollé, A.; Marquis-Favre, C.; Parizet, É. Perception and Annoyance Due to Vibrations in Dwellings Generated from Ground Transportation: A Review. J. Low Freq. Noise Vib. Act. Control 2015, 34, 413–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Aladdin, M.F.; Jalil, N.A.A.; Guan, N.Y.; Rezali, K.A.M. Perturbation effect of noise on overall feeling of discomfort from vertical whole-body vibration in vibro-acoustic environment. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 2021, 83, 103136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. ISO 2631-1:1997-05; Mechanical Vibration and Shock—Evaluation of Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration—Part 1: General Requirements. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 1997.
  16. Maeda, S. Necessary Research for Standardization of Subjective Scaling of Whole-Body Vibration. Ind. Health 2005, 43, 390–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Huang, Y.; Li, D. Subjective discomfort model of the micro commercial vehicle vibration over different road conditions. Appl. Acoust. 2018, 145, 385–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kaneko, C.; Hagiwara, T.; Maeda, S. Evaluation of Whole-Body Vibration by the Category Judgment Method. Ind. Health 2005, 43, 221–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Maeda, S.; Mansfield, N.J.; Shibata, N. Evaluation of subjective responses to whole-body vibration exposure: Effect of frequency content. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 2008, 38, 509–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fastl, H.; Zwicker, E. Psychoacoustics Facts and Models; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  21. Altinsoy, M.E. The Evaluation of Conventional, Electric and Hybrid Electric Passenger Car Pass-By Noise Annoyance Using Psychoacoustical Properties. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 5146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rosenkranz, R.; Altinsoy, M.E. Mapping the Sensory-Perceptual Space of Vibration for User-Centered Intuitive Tactile Design. IEEE Trans. Haptics 2020, 14, 95–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rosenkranz, R.; Altinsoy, M.E. Tactile Design: Translating User Expectations into Vibration for Plausible Virtual Environments. In Proceedings of the 2019 IEEE World Haptics Conference (WHC), Tokyo, Japan, 9–12 July 2019; pp. 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Krause, L.; van de Par, S.; Töpken, S. Pleasantness ratings for vertical whole-body vibration on an aircraft seat and relevant body parts involved. Appl. Acoust. 2023, 207, 109330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Wang, X.; Osvalder, A.-L.; Höstmad, P. Influence of Sound and Vibration on Perceived Overall Ride Comfort—A Comparison between an Electric Vehicle and a Combustion Engine Vehicle. Int. J. Veh. Dyn. Stab. NVH 2023, 7, 153–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Oetjen, A.; van De Par, S.; Krause, L. Influence of Whole-Body Vibrations on Modulation Based Psychoacoustic Measures; Forum Acusticum: Lyon, France, 2021; pp. 2093–2097. [Google Scholar]
  27. Töpken, S.; Krause, L.; van de Par, S. Influence of congruent audio tones on pleasantness ratings for vertical whole-body vibration on an aircraft seat bench. In Proceedings of the DAGA 2023—49st German Annual Conference on Acoustics, Hamburg, Germany, 6–9 March 2023. [Google Scholar]
  28. Maravich, M.M.; Altinsoy, M.E. Lästigkeit bei gleichzeitiger Exposition von Schall und Schwingungen [Annoyance of simultaneous exposure of sound and vibration]. In Proceedings of the DAGA 2020—46th German Annual Conference on Acoustics, Hannover, Germany, 16–19 March 2020. [Google Scholar]
  29. Festa, M.; Stalter, F.; Tavornmas, A.; Gauterin, F. Human Response to Vehicle Vibrations and Acoustics during Transient Road Excitations. Vibration 2021, 4, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Altinsoy, M.E.; Jekosch, U.; Landgraf, J.; Merchel, S. Progress in Auditory Perception Research Laboratories-Multimodal Measurement Laboratory of Dresden University of Technology; Audio Engineering Society Convention: New York, NY, USA, 2010; p. 129. [Google Scholar]
  31. DIN 45681:2005-03; Akustik–Bestimmung der Tonhaltigkeit von Geräuschen und Ermittlung eines Tonzuschlages für die Beurteilung von Geräuschimmissionen [Acoustics-Determination of Tonal Components of Noise and Determination of a Tone Adjustment for the Assessment of Noise Immissions]. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 2005.
  32. TA Lärm:1998-08-26; Sechste Allgemeine Verwaltungsvorschrift zum Bundes-Immissionsschutzgesetz (Technische Anleitung zum Schutz gegen Lärm - TA Lärm) [Technical Instructions on Noise Abatement]. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 1998.
  33. Sottek, R.; Becker, J. Tonal Annoyance vs. Tonal Loudness and Tonality. In Proceedings of the Inter Noise, Madrid, Spain, 16–19 June 2019. [Google Scholar]
  34. Jones, A.J.; Saunders, D.J. Equal comfort contours for whole body vertical, pulsed sinusoidal vibration. J. Sound Vib. 1972, 23, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. VDI 2057 Blatt 1:2017-08; Einwirkung mechanischer Schwingungen auf den Menschen Ganzkörper-Schwingungen [Human Exposure to Mechanical Vibrations-Whole-Body Vibration]. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 2017.
  36. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  37. Huang, Y.; Griffin, M.J. The discomfort produced by noise and whole-body vertical vibration presented separately and in combination. Ergonomics 2014, 57, 1724–1738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Maigrot, P.; Marquis-Favre, C.; Parizet, É. Two laboratory methods of assessing annoyance due to railway noise and vibration. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2017, 142, 3284–3287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. DIN 45631:1991-03; Berechnung des Lautstärkepegels und der Lautheit aus dem Geräuschspektrum; Verfahren nach E. Zwicker [Procedure for Calculating Loudness Level and Loudness]. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 1991.
  40. Berglund, B.; Hassmén, P.; Job, R.F.S. Sources and effects of low-frequency noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1996, 99, 2985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Yonemura, M.; Lee, H.; Sakamoto, S. Subjective Evaluation on the Annoyance of Environmental Noise Containing Low-Frequency Tonal Components. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. DIN 45680:2020-06-Entwurf; Messung und Beurteilung tieffrequenter Geräuschimmissionen [Measurement and Assessment of Low-frequency Noise Immissions]. Beuth Verlag: Berlin, Germany, 2020.
Figure 1. Multi Modal Measurement Laboratory of Technische Universität Dresden.
Figure 1. Multi Modal Measurement Laboratory of Technische Universität Dresden.
Vibration 06 00033 g001
Figure 3. FFT vs. Time (Spectrum size: 4096, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) and FFT (Spectrum size: 32768, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of sound signals (t = 4 s, LN = 70 dBA) of drivers position from experiment 1; Left: Big street sweeper on asphalt; Right: Mini excavator as reference stimulus of experiment 1 and 2.
Figure 3. FFT vs. Time (Spectrum size: 4096, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) and FFT (Spectrum size: 32768, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of sound signals (t = 4 s, LN = 70 dBA) of drivers position from experiment 1; Left: Big street sweeper on asphalt; Right: Mini excavator as reference stimulus of experiment 1 and 2.
Vibration 06 00033 g003
Figure 4. Block diagram of the structure of the stimuli of the experimental design; “commercial vehicle” in experiment 1 is “big street sweeper” and in experiment 2 “refuse collection vehicle”.
Figure 4. Block diagram of the structure of the stimuli of the experimental design; “commercial vehicle” in experiment 1 is “big street sweeper” and in experiment 2 “refuse collection vehicle”.
Vibration 06 00033 g004
Figure 5. Figure right: GUI for participants to play back and rate the stimuli (The GUI have been translated to English for this figure); Figures left: Visual feedback for participants after pushing button “A B”.
Figure 5. Figure right: GUI for participants to play back and rate the stimuli (The GUI have been translated to English for this figure); Figures left: Visual feedback for participants after pushing button “A B”.
Vibration 06 00033 g005
Figure 6. Boxplots and geometric mean (×) of the relative total annoyance ratings of the vibro-acoustic and acoustic stimuli; ▬Ref: Reference mini excavator (acoustic); ▬N.1: Big street sweeper (acoustic); Modified vibration frequency content (vibro-acoustic): ▬V1.1: “Frequency content (FC) of a big street sweeper”; ▬V2.1: “FC +6 Hz +50 Hz”; ▬V3.1: “FC +50 Hz”; ▬V4.1: “FC +6 Hz”; Noise spectrum for vibro-acoustic stimuli was the same.
Figure 6. Boxplots and geometric mean (×) of the relative total annoyance ratings of the vibro-acoustic and acoustic stimuli; ▬Ref: Reference mini excavator (acoustic); ▬N.1: Big street sweeper (acoustic); Modified vibration frequency content (vibro-acoustic): ▬V1.1: “Frequency content (FC) of a big street sweeper”; ▬V2.1: “FC +6 Hz +50 Hz”; ▬V3.1: “FC +50 Hz”; ▬V4.1: “FC +6 Hz”; Noise spectrum for vibro-acoustic stimuli was the same.
Vibration 06 00033 g006
Figure 7. Interaction of sound pressure and acceleration level; left: Geometric mean of the relative total annoyance ratings over sound pressure level, right: Geometric mean values over acceleration level.
Figure 7. Interaction of sound pressure and acceleration level; left: Geometric mean of the relative total annoyance ratings over sound pressure level, right: Geometric mean values over acceleration level.
Vibration 06 00033 g007
Figure 8. FFT (Spectrum size: 262144, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of different vibration spectra of vertical seat vibrations of experiment 2 (t = 4 s, LV = 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)).
Figure 8. FFT (Spectrum size: 262144, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of different vibration spectra of vertical seat vibrations of experiment 2 (t = 4 s, LV = 112 dBWk (ref 1 × 10−6 m/s2)).
Vibration 06 00033 g008
Figure 9. FFT vs. Time (Spectrum Size: 4096, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) and FFT (Spectrum Size: 32768, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of sound signals (t = 4 s, LN = 70 dBA) of drivers position from experiment 2; Left: Refuse collection vehicle on the highway; Right: Mini excavator as reference stimulus of experiment 1 and 2.
Figure 9. FFT vs. Time (Spectrum Size: 4096, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) and FFT (Spectrum Size: 32768, Overlap: 50%, Window function: HAN) of sound signals (t = 4 s, LN = 70 dBA) of drivers position from experiment 2; Left: Refuse collection vehicle on the highway; Right: Mini excavator as reference stimulus of experiment 1 and 2.
Vibration 06 00033 g009
Figure 10. Boxplots and geometric mean (×) and geometric standard deviation of the relative total annoyance ratings of the vibro-acoustic and acoustic stimuli; ▬Ref: Reference mini excavator (acoustic); ▬N.2: Refuse collection vehicle (acoustic); Modified vibration frequency content (vibro-acoustic): ▬V1.2: Frequency content (FC) of a refuse collection vehicle, “Sine in Broadband“; ▬V2.2: 4.75 Hz Sine, “Sine“; ▬V3.2: FC without Sine “Broadband“; ▬V4.2: V3.2 filtered 4–6 Hz “Narrowband“; Noise spectrum of vibro-acoustic stimuli was the same.
Figure 10. Boxplots and geometric mean (×) and geometric standard deviation of the relative total annoyance ratings of the vibro-acoustic and acoustic stimuli; ▬Ref: Reference mini excavator (acoustic); ▬N.2: Refuse collection vehicle (acoustic); Modified vibration frequency content (vibro-acoustic): ▬V1.2: Frequency content (FC) of a refuse collection vehicle, “Sine in Broadband“; ▬V2.2: 4.75 Hz Sine, “Sine“; ▬V3.2: FC without Sine “Broadband“; ▬V4.2: V3.2 filtered 4–6 Hz “Narrowband“; Noise spectrum of vibro-acoustic stimuli was the same.
Vibration 06 00033 g010
Table 1. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons of the main factor “vibration frequency”.
Table 1. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons of the main factor “vibration frequency”.
Vibration SpectraSig.
V1.1: FC 1V2.1: FC +6 Hz, +50 Hz Sinep = .021
V4.1: FC +6 Hz Sinep < .001
V3.1: FC +50 Hz SineV2.1: FC +6 Hz, +50 Hz Sinep < .001
V4.1: FC +6 Hz Sinep < .001
V1.1: FCV3.1: FC +50 Hz Sinep = 1.000
V2.1: FC +6 Hz, +50 Hz SineV4.1: FC +6 Hz Sine p = 1.000
1 Frequency content of a big street sweeper.
Table 2. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons of the main factor “vibration frequency” with the levels V1.2–V4.2.
Table 2. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons of the main factor “vibration frequency” with the levels V1.2–V4.2.
Vibration SpectraSig.
V1.2: “Sine in Broadband” (FC 1)V2.2: “Sine”p < .001
V4.2: “Narrowband”p < .001
V3.2: “Broadband”V2.2: “Sine”p < .001
V4.2: “Narrowband”p < .001
V1.2: “Sine in Broadband”V3.2: “Broadband”p = .004
V2.2: “Sine”V4.2: “Narrowband”p = 1.000
1 Frequency content of a refuse collection vehicle.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Maravich, M.M.; Rosenkranz, R.; Altinsoy, M.E. Annoyance Caused by Simultaneous Noise and Vibration in Commercial Vehicles: Multimodal Interaction and the Effects of Sinusoidal Components in Recorded Seat Vibrations. Vibration 2023, 6, 536-555. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration6030033

AMA Style

Maravich MM, Rosenkranz R, Altinsoy ME. Annoyance Caused by Simultaneous Noise and Vibration in Commercial Vehicles: Multimodal Interaction and the Effects of Sinusoidal Components in Recorded Seat Vibrations. Vibration. 2023; 6(3):536-555. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration6030033

Chicago/Turabian Style

Maravich, Maria Mareen, Robert Rosenkranz, and M. Ercan Altinsoy. 2023. "Annoyance Caused by Simultaneous Noise and Vibration in Commercial Vehicles: Multimodal Interaction and the Effects of Sinusoidal Components in Recorded Seat Vibrations" Vibration 6, no. 3: 536-555. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration6030033

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop