Next Article in Journal
Metronidazole-Induced Encephalopathy in a 16-Year-Old Girl with Crohn’s Disease: Case Report and Review of the Pediatric Literature
Previous Article in Journal
The Effect of Obesity on Fractional Exhaled Nitric Oxide in School-Aged Children
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Pragmatic Language Development: Analysis of Mapping Knowledge Domains on How Infants and Children Become Pragmatically Competent

1
Department of Human Sciences (Psychology), University of Verona, 37126 Verona, Italy
2
Institute of English Studies, University of Warsaw, 01-445 Warsaw, Poland
3
Department of Curriculum and Instruction, King Saud University, Riyadh 11451, Saudi Arabia
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(9), 1407; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091407
Submission received: 31 July 2022 / Revised: 20 August 2022 / Accepted: 7 September 2022 / Published: 16 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Child and Adolescent Psychiatry)

Abstract

:
New-borns are capable of recognising and producing sounds as they become phonologically competent. Following this, infants develop a system for connecting these sounds, which helps them become increasingly lexically competent over time. Their knowledge of these words grows as they develop, using words to form phrases, turning them into sentences, and ultimately becoming syntactically competent. By making sense of these linguistic elements, these three competencies are enhanced, and this is how infants become semantically competent. As infants continue to develop linguistic and non-linguistic communication behaviours, this miraculous language development becomes even more complex, enabling them to perfect their linguistic abilities while being pragmatically competent. In this study, a scientometric approach was used to examine past, present, and future trends in pragmatic language development (PLD). A total of 6455 documents were analysed from the Scopus, WOS, and Lens databases between 1950 and 2022. The analysis involved the visualisation and tabulation of eight bibliometric and eight scientometric indicators using CiteSpace 5.8.R3 and VOSviewer 1.6.18 software for data analysis. In this study, we highlight the major patterns and topics directing the research on PLD between 1950 and 2022. The themes and topics included (1) analysing PLD as a social behaviour through the lens of executive functions; (2) studying PLD as a social behaviour based on social understanding; (3) examining PLD as a social behaviour associated with autism spectrum disorder; (4) developing an understanding of PLD in academic settings through the examination of executive functions; (5) identifying pragmatic competence versus communicative competence as a social behaviour; (6) analysing pragmatic language skills in aphasic patients via epistemic stances (i.e., attitudes towards knowledge in interaction); (7) investigating PLD as a behavioural problem in the context of a foreign language; (8) assessing PLD as a behavioural problem in individuals with autism spectrum disorder; (9) assessing PLD in persons with traumatic brain injury and closed head injury as a behavioural problem; (10) identifying the role of the right hemisphere in executive functions as a cognitive substrate; (11) assessing the impact of pragmatic failure in speech acts on pragmatic competence; and (12) investigating the patterns of PLD among learning-disabled children.

1. Introduction

1.1. The Rise of Pragmatic Language Development

The study of pragmatic language development (PLD) seems to have first emerged in the mid-1970s. Halliday [1] was one of the pioneers, and began to analyse early child communication within the framework of the Speech Act Theory [2,3,4]. In around 1980, several co-authored books were published that addressed all or part of this developmental stage [5,6,7]. Since then, important progress has been made in several directions. The book by Ninio and Snow [8] is noteworthy for its substantial contribution to the entire field of early PLD. PLD is a component of the whole language system, which is viewed as a tool that children and adults use to explore the social world; create, develop, and maintain social relationships; and engage in culturally significant activities with others [9]. As social animals, it is impossible for speakers to survive without language, which allows them to communicate their own experiences, thoughts, beliefs, and ideas. Humans are distinguished and superior to all other creatures in the universe due to their ability to communicate through language [10].
PLD has drawn the attention of many scholars for several reasons. First, children seem to master lots of pragmatic functions at a time when their vocabulary and syntax are still limited [1,2,11,12]. Ingram [11], for example, presented evidence for the rapid expansion of the range of pragmatic functions during the one-word and very early two-word phases. However, the evidence for this suggestion was based on several different studies rather than on a comprehensive assessment of the same children. Halliday [1] and Keller-Cohen et al. [1] claimed, on the basis of intensive studies with one and two children, respectively, that a relatively universal sequence of the emergence of functions can be observed. Therefore, development during this period may manifest itself in functions rather than in lexical or syntactic forms [13]. Second, there is empirical evidence that pragmatic development is a statistically independent dimension of development. Snyder [14] compared language-delayed children with normal children matched for mean utterance length in terms of their ability to produce declarative and imperative functions under structured elicitation conditions and found that the language-delayed children were more delayed in pragmatic than in syntactic terms. An even clearer example of this independence was provided by Blank et al.’s [15] case study of a boy who, despite relatively well-developed syntactic and semantic systems, showed an almost complete deficit in the ability to use language socially and appropriately. Third, it is often assumed that pragmatic development is the aspect of language most closely related to cognitive development [16]. In normal children, Piaget’s period of one-word utterances (very roughly estimated to span 12 to 20 months of age) coincides with major cognitive changes. Pragmatic measures, if feasible, could be much more fruitful than measures of vocabulary and syntax for studying the relationship between language and cognitive development. All three of these issues could be studied more effectively if appropriate measures of PLD were available and could be used, for example, for correlational studies [16].

1.2. Pragmatic Language Development: Infancy

Children use language to express their needs and wants, negotiate disagreements, participate in games, and engage in other communicative interactions with peers and adults. Basic pragmatic skills develop at a fairly early age but are refined and developed during preadolescence and adolescence, so that over time the child is able to participate in more social activities and become a full member of culture and society [17]. Children’s pragmatic competence—their ability to use language effectively—is developed and refined through participation in family, peer, and school interactions that serve as a means and motivation for efficient and strategic language use. By examining both universal and culturally specific features of children’s interactional skills, studies of pragmatic development aim to describe and detail how children learn to use language as an effective tool for social interaction [18].
One of the basic rights of children is to have a family that takes care of their basic needs and provides them with affection and social support [19]. Pragmatics is a process of social development in which the knowledge necessary for successful interaction with others is acquired through language. This ability is not acquired suddenly; the child must develop skills such as the ability to process information from different sources, appropriate linguistic development, and the ability to respond to social demands [19,20].
When children learn a language, they need to learn more than phonology, semantics, and syntax [21]. A proficient language user knows how to use language appropriately and strategically in social situations. Children need to learn pragmatic skills, also known as communicative competence. They need to learn how to use language in interactions with peers, families, teachers, and others. Children need to learn how to ask questions, make requests, give orders, agree or disagree, apologise, refuse, joke, praise, and tell stories. They need to learn routines such as “trick or treat” and “happy birthday”; polite expressions such as “please” and “thank you”, “hello” and “goodbye”, and “excuse me”; and how to address others. They need to learn how to start, maintain, and end conversations; when to speak or remain silent; how to take turns; how to give and respond effectively to feedback; and how to stay on topic. They need to know and use the right volume and tone of voice. They need to learn how the meaning of terms such as “I” and “you” or “here” and “there” changes depending on who is speaking and who is listening. They need to learn what style of speech to use, when to use jargon, and when and whether to talk about certain topics [21].
Between 9 and 18 months of age, the first milestones of PLD are reached, such as understanding the communicative intent of the speaker and the onset of joint attentional behaviour [22,23,24,25,26,27]. Toward the end of the second year of life, infants become more adept at identifying what others want, intend, and perceive. Meltzoff [28], using the behavioural imitation paradigm, found that 18-month-old infants will imitate an adult’s action even if it fails, but will not repeat that action when performed by a mechanical device. This study and subsequent studies with some variations in experimental conditions [29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37] showed that 18-month-old infants are able to recognise that people, but not animate things, have intentions and are able to infer the intentions of others. In addition, most 18-month-old children have mastered the ability to appropriately match their responses to specific stable characteristics of their conversational partner [38] and to the type of activity they are engaged in with their partner [39,40].
At age 2, children appear to be able to answer simple questions whose responses require only a single element, whereas, at age 3, children are able to make comments and give responses that include both the predicate and the direct object [41]. In addition, an increasing ability to use complex contextual information is observed, which is reflected in 3-year-old children’s ability to answer and formulate questions and comments [42,43]. “Where” and “what” questions are easier for children to answer than “why” questions; however, it is not uncommon for children to use a particular question form in one context but not in another. It is possible that this discrepancy is not due to difficulties with the linguistic form, but rather situational and pragmatic cues that may play a larger role in children’s performance [44].
As toddlers and preschoolers progress, pragmatic skills continue to develop, and children gain more advanced conversational skills and become socially competent speakers (O’Neill, 2007) [45]. Four-year-olds are able to adapt their own language to take into account the age, gender, and status of the listener. Thus, preschoolers start to formulate less polite and more direct requests when interacting with higher-status speakers (e.g., parents and teachers) and more polite and indirect requests when interacting with lower-status speakers (e.g., friends). They develop very detailed expectations about what terms can be used to talk about a particular situation. Specifically, in terms of developing reference choice, preschoolers prefer optimally informative referential terms to both under-informative and over-informative equivalents [46,47,48].

1.3. Pragmatic Language Development: Adolescence

As children enter adolescence, their growing social world both enables and pressures them to develop more sophisticated pragmatic skills. Experience with a greater variety of teachers and peers, exposure to more forms of language through reading and school, and participation in extracurricular activities motivate adolescents to take the perspective of others and use language strategically. In addition, pragmatic behaviour reflects normal progress in identity development and increasing autonomy from parents. The social contexts in which adolescents engage in pragmatic behaviour also include technologies such as mobiles and the internet [21].
During adolescence, language and pragmatics play a particularly important role in identity formation and marking [21]. Family conversations and narratives connect adolescents to their past and their culture. The appropriate use of current slang expressions and gestures typical of the peer group is crucial. These behaviours show solidarity with members of the groups to which the adolescents belong and clarify the adolescents’ distinction from other groups and from younger children and adults. Knowing the current terms for the groups themselves and knowing how to tease and argue become more important during the teen years. Shifting between different registers and varieties of language allows adolescents to associate themselves with particular age, gender, social class, racial, and ethnic groups. Even gossip, insults, and verbal aggression in relationships serve to forge alliances and cross social boundaries, discover norms among peers, and explore identity [21].

1.4. The Scope of Pragmatic Language Development

A search of the pertinent literature yielded a number of varied and general definitions of pragmatic language. Gallagher defines pragmatic language as “linguistic elements and contextual elements as forming a contextual whole” [7] (p. 2). A simpler definition is by Bates [16], who defines pragmatic language as “rules governing the use of language in context” (p. 420). Other researchers do not offer definitions that provide insight into the parameters of pragmatic language, although they acknowledge the complexity of pragmatic language skills [49].
The term pragmatic language refers to the use of language in the context of communication [50,51]. This broad definition encompasses many different pragmatic language skills and abilities, including the ability to sustain a conversation, provide relevant responses, follow politeness norms, write coherent reports, and understand non-literal language such as jokes, sarcasm, and irony [51]. Pragmatic language is defined as a complex and uniquely human skill that is embedded in children’s daily experiences. It enables us-children and adults alike to form relationships with one another, share experiences, and communicate our perspectives and attitudes about those experiences [52,53,54]. Pragmatic language is a multifaceted construct that involves many functional capacities, including cognitive, linguistic, and theory of mind [55,56,57,58].
Pragmatic language refers to the appropriate and effective use of language in interpersonal contexts and is central to children’s ability to perform well at home, at school, and with peers [59,60,61]. It can be distinguished from the structural aspects of language, which have traditionally been viewed as relatively independent of context: phonology, syntax, and semantics. Difficulties in the pragmatic language domain are manifested in a variety of behaviours, such as talking too much; taking turns poorly in conversation; the inability to adapt a message to a listener’s needs; the inability to respond to verbal cues from others; the overuse of stereotypical phrases; and difficulty understanding sarcasm, jokes, and metaphors [58,59,60].
Pragmatic language has been studied in the disciplines of anthropology, sociology, psychology, and linguistics [5]. In the last two decades, cognitive–behavioural psychology, linguistics, and social cognitive psychology have had a significant impact on the development of pragmatic language theories. Each of these traditions offers a different perspective on the boundaries of pragmatic language and the appropriate unit of analysis.
PLD refers to children’s linguistic and non-linguistic communication skills, which include various influences such as socialisation by caregivers, parents, siblings, teachers, and peers; cognition; knowledge; and effort (Bryant, 2018, as cited in [62]). Pragmatic language is defined as the use of appropriate communication in social contexts; in other words, knowing what to say, how to say it, and when to say it [63]. Pragmatic skills enable children to produce and understand words and sentences in ways appropriate to the conversational context [64]. According to the American Speech–Language Association (ASHA), social communication consists of three major communication skills, namely using language, changing language, and following rules [65], as is shown in Table 1.

1.5. Scientific Contributions for Pragmatics

Table 2 shows the 10 most important source journals for PLD research along with a brief description of the scope of each source journal. The title of the source journal, the country in which the journal was published, the name of the publisher, the starting date, the number of volumes published to date, and web addresses are also outlined.

1.6. Purpose of the Present Study

In recent years, pragmatics and PLD have experienced a significant increase in research [66]. Whether infants and children can undergo typical or atypical PLD has been researched internationally in both school and clinical settings [67]. Recent research has examined how children develop pragmatic language skills in school and clinical settings in light of the different theories and assessment methods for PLD. This review is significant in that it provides an overview of how pragmatic language skills are acquired from infancy to adulthood [68]. In light of this, the present study examines PLD from a bibliometric and scientometric perspective. We attribute the significance of this study to the presentation of historical evidence regarding PLD, the analysis of existing evidence concerning PLD, and the prediction of future trends related to PLD research. We raised the following questions: (1) What is the size of knowledge production in PLD measured by year, region, higher education institution, journal, publisher, and author? (2) What are the most cited documents in PLD? (3) Who are the most influential authors in PLD? (4) Which topics and themes are most frequently explored in PLD? (5) Which research trends are emerging in PLD?

2. Methods

2.1. Research Methods

Scientometrics is defined as “the study of artifacts; one examines not science and scholarship but the products of those activities” [69] (p. 491). It is usually the objective of researchers in this field to analyse “the quantitative aspects of the production, dissemination and use of scientific information with the aim of achieving a better understanding of the mechanisms of scientific research as a social activity” [70] (p. 6). Researchers debate whether scientometric studies can be used to assess the quality of published research. In a previous study, it was identified that “the task of determining quality papers is especially difficult in BIS [bibliometrics, informetrics and scientometrics] due to the very heterogeneous origin of the researchers” [71] (p. 390). There is, however, a purpose in these studies, which remains to provide “reveal characteristics of scientometric phenomena and processes in scientific research for more efficient management of science” [72] (p. 1).
Several scientometric indicators have been developed to guide researchers in the conduct of such studies. These indicators can be categorised as either referring to elements (e.g., publication, citation and reference, and potential) or type indicators (e.g., quantitative, impact) [72]. “Mapping knowledge domains” also merits our attention, and it refers to the process by which we can produce “an image that shows the development process and the structural relationship of scientific knowledge”—using maps that are “useful tools for tracking the frontiers of science and technology, facilitating knowledge management, and assisting scientific and technological decision-making” [73] (p. 6201). In recent research, it has been argued that this method should be applied to all fields of research, not just medical, health, and pure sciences [74]. In the context of this study, the field of PLD is explored as a sub-field of pragmatics, one that integrates other fields, including linguistics, psychology, and education.

2.2. Measures

As mentioned above, both bibliometric and scientometric studies are considered tools to guide the assessment of the knowledge produced in a particular field/concept (e.g., PLD) by evaluating the production of documents. Bibliometric indicators are usually provided in knowledge databases (e.g., Scopus, WOS, and Lens) [75,76,77,78]. The scientometric indicators are generally provided by scientometric software, which is part of the scientometric analysis. During the course of this study, for example, we used CiteSpace 5.8.R3 [79] and VOSviewer 1.6.18 [80]. A summary of the bibliometric and scientometric indicators we used in this study can be found in Table 3.

2.3. Data Collection and Sample

Three databases—Scopus, WOS, and Lens—were used to retrieve the data. These databases were included for a number of reasons. To begin, both Scopus and WOS contain sources that have been evaluated for inclusion based on their quality in addition to being knowledge databases [75,76,77]. Second, the Lens database is regarded as more exhaustive than the first two databases, since it provides data that are unavailable from the other two [78].
A search of the data was conducted on Friday, 25 March 2022. No language limitations were included, provided that the title, abstract, and keywords were in English. Considering the fact that there were few results in other languages, we verified this manually. Articles, review articles, book chapters, and books, including early-access publications, were the only types of documents we considered. The search strings are shown in Table 4, along with other specifications used in the three databases.
To measure the development and size of the research produced in this area, we examined the use of the concept “pragmatic language development” and any synonyms. The keywords we used when searching for works did not include specific works that were restricted to a particular age group, a specific type of learner, a certain language, or any other limitation. According to our preliminary search on Google, as well as our previous experience in the field, we determined that it was best to use the search strings above for identifying knowledge related to PLD. Pragmatic development is defined as “a heterogeneous field on a range of topics associated with the study of how young children develop the skills to use language effectively and appropriately in social interaction” [84] (p. 300). Pragmatic competence is defined as “is a system of knowledge that is neither a system of ‘knowing how’ nor a system of ‘knowing that’… the system of knowledge that governs use of language” [85] (p. 67).

2.4. Data Analysis

Several steps were taken before and during the process of analysing the data. To begin with, the data for the bibliometric analysis were exported from Scopus in three different formats: Excel sheets for the bibliometric analysis, CiteSpace RIS files, and VOSviewer CSV files. RIS files from CiteSpace were converted into WOS files in order to fulfil the requirements imposed by CiteSpace. Furthermore, WOS data were retrieved in two formats: text documents converted into Excel sheets for bibliometric analysis, and plain text documents for CiteSpace and VOSviewer. The Lens data were retrieved in two formats: CSV for bibliometric analysis and full record CSV for VOSviewer.
As a preliminary step, duplicate documents were removed from CiteSpace and Mendeley before starting the analysis in CiteSpace. A bibliometric analysis was carried out using Microsoft Excel for the purpose of this study. The tables were generated in Excel and then converted into figures in order to create the citation reports.
All the scientometric settings were set to default in both software packages in order to perform the analysis. We created separate visualisations for each of the three databases, such as network visualisations, overlay visualisations, and density visualisations. For Scoups and WOS, the analysis was carried out three times each: co-occurrence analysis by the keyword of the author, co-citation analysis by the source, and co-citation analysis by the cited author. Four analyses were conducted for Lens: the co-occurrence analysis by author keywords, citation analysis by author, citation analysis by source, and citation analysis by document. For CiteSpace, the analysis was performed three times for Scopus and WOS: co-citation by document (references), co-citation by cited author, and occurrence (keywords). We prepared narrative summaries, cluster summaries, visual maps, and burst tables based on the data we gathered.

3. Results

3.1. Result Overview

The results of the study can be divided into two sections. In the first section, bibliometric indicators for PLD are presented. These indicators were derived from a number of databases including Scopus, WOS, and Lens and were based on retrieved data. Bibliometric indicators included, for example, publications by year and top 10 countries, universities, journals, publishers, subjects/research areas, and authors. In the second section, we discuss the scientometric indicators for PLD. A combination of CiteSpace and VOSviewer software was used to analyse these indicators. These indicators included citations, co-citations, and co-occurrences.
The first section presents the bibliometric indicators for the study of PLD. First, we introduced the total number of included studies, their type, time span, and knowledge production size by year. Second, we presented the knowledge production size of PLD by country and university and/or research centre. Third, we presented the top journals and publishers disseminating research in PLD. Fourth, we presented PLD knowledge production classified by research area, keywords, and co-occurrence. The bibliometric indicators section is concluded by presenting the top authors contributing to PLD research. The second section presents the scientometric indicators, starting with the strongest citation bursts for keywords in PLD. This is followed by visualisations for co-occurrence, (co)-citation by author, document, and journal. This section is concluded by a tabulation of the most cited documents, top clusters, citation counts, detected bursts, central authors, and sigma metrics for research in PLD.

3.2. Bibliometric Indicators for the Study of Pragmatic Language Development

Overview of PLD Studies from Scopus, Web of Science, and Lens

A total of 1470 PLD papers were retrieved from Scopus, 1063 from WOS, and 3922 from Lens for the purpose of analysis. In each of the three databases, the data period was 1973–2022, 1985–2022, and 1950–2022, respectively. Scopus contained 1201 articles, 100 review articles, 149 book chapters, and 20 books. The WOS documents included 1022 articles, 38 review articles, 65 book chapters, and 24 early-access articles. In Lens, there were 2191 articles, 104 unknown articles, 255 book chapters, 135 books, 178 dissertations, and 28 preprints. In addition to English, other languages were included, such as Spanish, French, German, Korean, Russian, and Italian. Due to the fact that the analysis was based on the title, keywords, abstract, and references, all the papers included this information in English. These papers were included in order to avoid bias towards data published only in English language publications.
Figure 1A–C show the length of production by year for the three databases. It can be seen that there has been a considerable rise in the production of knowledge in PLD, with the peak of knowledge production occurring in 2021 in Scopus with 159 publications, 2021 in WOS with 123 publications, and 2020 in Lens with 286 publications. The range of publications per year was 1–159 in Scopus, 1–123 in WOS, and 1–286 in Lens. In all databases, the lowest number of publications occurred in the previous year. Further, of 6455 documents in PLD, there were 5928 documents published between 2000 and 2022. It is thus true that over the last two decades, there has been an increase in the production of knowledge related to PLD.

3.3. Production of PLD Research by Country and University

Figure 2A–C show the top 10 countries for producing knowledge related to PLD. There is no doubt that the US achieved the highest ranking among the three databases, with a number of publications that appeared to be significantly higher than those of the other countries. The second and third positions in all three databases were exchanged between the UK and China. Aside from North America, Europe, and Australia, the list also included countries in Asia (e.g., China, Japan, Indonesia, and Iran) as well as other countries around the world. It should be mentioned that the selection of these leading countries was based on the ranking created by each database, whereby each author is presumably considered regardless of their order or role, and the 10 countries with the greatest number of authors are listed as the top 10. This was also applicable for universities.
Figure 3A–C present the top 10 universities and/or research centres for producing knowledge related to PLD. The list of the top institutions varied according to the database. While the first university in Scopus is located in the USA, the first in WOS is located in the UK, and the first in Lens is located in Iran. There are two notable institutions from Iran that are located outside the Northern Hemisphere: Islam Azad University and Allameh Tabataba’i University. The rest of the universities are located in North America, Europe, and Australia.

3.4. Production of PLD Research by Journal and Publisher

Figure 4A–D demonstrate the top 10 journals publishing research in PLD. While we found journals that include the word “pragmatics” (for example, Journal of Pragmatics, Intercultural Pragmatics, and Historical Pragmatics), there are also journals in the field of linguistics that have this word as part of their title. An extended list of journals based on their publishers is shown in Figure 4D.
Figure 5A,B show the lists of the top 10 publishers for knowledge in PLD. Due to the fact that Scopus does not include publisher information, these lists are limited to WOS and Lens databases. Although “Elsevier” and “Wiley” achieved the highest ranking in both of the databases, the rankings for the rest of the publishers varied between the databases. As an example, “Cambridge University Press” was ranked 7th in WOS, while it was ranked 9th in Lens.

3.5. Production of PLD by Research Area, Keywords, and Co-Occurrence

While PLD is extensively studied as a branch of pragmatics, it is also integrated with a vast array of other fields, as seen in Figure 6A–C. As depicted in Figure 6A, the four most widely published subject areas involving PLD are the social sciences, arts and humanities, psychology, and medicine. Figure 6B reveals that the four most prominent research fields associated with PLD are linguistics, educational research, psychology, and rehabilitation. The findings are further supported by Figure 6C, which identifies psychology, linguistics, pragmatic competency, and pragmatics as the top four study areas. There are other topics that could be categorised as PLD-related, and Lens displayed some of the more specific ones, including pragmatic competence, pragmatics, competence, and language use.

3.6. Production of PLD Knowledge by Authors

PLD is unquestionably a broad field that does not have a clearly defined number of contributors, as even a single article is considered to be a contribution to the field of PLD. Despite this, we tried to show the authors whose work produced the most knowledge related to PLD, as shown in Figure 7A–C. It can be seen that Taguchi [86] was the first-ranked author in both Scopus and Lens, while Bosco [87] was the first-ranked author in WOS.

3.7. Scientometric Indicators for the Study of PLD

Overview of PLD Studies from Scopus, Web of Science, and Lens

This section provides a scientometric analysis of the data retrieved from the Scopus, Web of Science, and Lens databases. Specifically, it highlights the impact of a number of concepts, references, and emerging trends on the field of neurolinguistics as well as the impact of certain authors.
For the purpose of this study, we first identified the top keywords with the strongest citation bursts using CiteSpace for data obtained from Scopus (Figure 8A,B). The green line indicates the period during which all the research was conducted. An indication of the beginning and the end of the burst period can be seen by the red line. The word with the strongest citation burst in Scopus was (psychology = 12.5) between 2014 and 2020, and in WOS it was (interlanguage = 7.05) between 2009 and 2015. The citation burst may differ depending on the database. For instance, the keywords in Scopus were more related to the study of pragmatics in clinical settings (e.g., physiology, language disorders, persons with hearing impairments). On the other hand, the keywords in WOS were more related to learning settings (e.g., children, adult, instruction).
A network visualisation of these clusters and authors is also used to illustrate these features (Figure 9A–D). It can be seen in Figure 9A that topics such as pragmatic competence, cognitive pragmatics, hearing impairment in children, and pragmatic performance, among others, are some of the most frequently explored topics in the area of pragmatic learning and development. Figure 9B shows more specific concepts, such as pragmatic development, executive functions, and young children. In Figure 9C,D, the most cited authors and topics are displayed. Among these topics are executive functions, social understanding, and autism spectrum disorder (see Figure 9C). Other topics such as foreign language, speech acts, and EFL learners are included in the WOS database (see Figure 9D). It is easier to read the data in these figures if you think of each cluster in terms of the text that′s next to it. The topic in the mentioned cluster is searched for more frequently the more intense the text is.
The co-occurrence of keywords is another important factor. Through the use of VOSviewer, we were able to generate three visual network maps depicting the occurrence of the keywords most commonly used in PLD across the three databases (Figure 10A–C). The colours represent different directions that can be studied in relation to PLD. Yellow represents pragmatic competence, blue represents theory of mind, green represents clinical and neuropragmatics, and red represents speech acts (See Figure 10A). Depending on the database, these colours may change. As shown in Figure 10B, green represents pragmatic competence, purple represents pragmatics, and red represents pragmatic development. Figure 10C shows keywords related to autism and PLD in pink.
We generated three visual network maps for co-citations and citations by authors using VOSviewer (Figure 11A–C). Each colour represents a co-citation or citation network. The circle sizes increase with the number of co-citations or citations the author has. According to Figure 11A, Kasper [88], Bosco [87], Bishop [89], and Cummings [85] are the most co-cited authors. Some of the same authors appeared in the Scopus database with others, such as Siegal [90] (see Figure 11B). Using the Lens database, Figure 11C shows that Taguchi [91], Becker [21], etc., were the most cited authors.
We generated three visual network maps for co-citations and citations by source using VOSviewer (Figure 12A–C). Each colour represents a co-citation or citation network. The larger the circle, the more cited or co-cited the source is. In Figure 12A, Journal of Pragmatics, Journal of Autism, and Applied Linguistics appear to be the most co-cited sources. The sources in Figure 12B are similar to those in Figure 12A, but with more significant journals (e.g., Journal of Child Language). The citation network for journals is shown in Figure 12C, including Frontiers in Psychology and East Asian Pragmatics.
The top 10 cited works were derived from the bibliometric data provided in Scopus, WOS, and Lens. This group of documents was merged, and duplicates were removed as shown in Table 5. While Scopus and WOS identified articles as the most cited works, Lens identified books as the most cited works. These top cited papers could represent the leading research in PLD.

3.8. Impact of Research on PLD by Clusters, Citation Counts, Citation Bursts, Centrality, and Sigma

3.8.1. Clusters

The network was divided into 15 co-citation clusters in the Scopus data (see Table 6 for the full list of clusters.). The largest six clusters are summarised as follows. The largest cluster (#0) has 236 members and a silhouette value of 0.737. It is labelled as executive function by LLR, pragmatic development by LSI, and social behaviour (3.89) by MI. The most relevant citer to the cluster is Ren [111] “L2 pragmatic development in study abroad contexts”.
The network was divided into 16 co-citation clusters in the WOS data (See Table 6 for the full list of clusters.). The largest six clusters are summarised as follows. The largest cluster (#0) has 214 members and a silhouette value of 0.737. It is labelled as foreign language by LLR, pragmatic development by LSI, and behavioural problem (3.33) by MI. The most relevant citer to the cluster is Bella [112] “Length of residence and intensity of interaction: modification in Greek L2 requests”.

3.8.2. Citation Counts

In Scopus, the top-ranked item by citation counts was Kasper [88] in cluster #0, with citation counts of 380. The second-ranked item was Bardovi-Harlig [113] in cluster #0, with citation counts of 300. In WOS, the top-ranked item by citation counts was Kasper [114] in cluster #0, with citation counts of 258. The second-ranked item was Bardovi-Harlig [115] in cluster #0, with citation counts of 187. The remaining top citation counts for PLD can be found in Table 7.

3.8.3. Bursts

In Scopus, the top-ranked item by bursts was Taguchi [91] in cluster #0, with bursts of 12.35. The second-ranked item was Faerch [128] in cluster #0, with bursts of 10.16. In WOS, the top-ranked item by bursts was Prutting [129] in cluster #1, with bursts of 12.76. The second-ranked item was Mcdonald [130] in cluster #2, with bursts of 8.59. See Table 8 and Figure 13A–D for the remaining top bursts detected in PLD.

3.8.4. Centrality

In Scopus, the top-ranked item by centrality was Bates [137] in cluster #1, with a centrality of 148. The second-ranked item was Brown [116] in cluster #0, with a centrality of 120. In WOS, the top-ranked item by centrality was Kasper [114] in cluster #0, with a centrality of 97. The second-ranked item was Bardovi-Harlig [115] in cluster #0, with a centrality of 96. The remaining central authors in PLD are listed in Table 9.

3.8.5. Sigma

In Scopus, the top-ranked item by sigma was Bates E (1977) in cluster #1, with a sigma of 0.00. The second-ranked item was Brown P (1987) in cluster #0, with a sigma of 0.00. The top-ranked item by sigma was KASPER G (1998) in cluster #0, with a sigma of 0.00. The second-ranked item was Bardovi-Harlig K (2001) in cluster #0, with a sigma of 0.00. See Table 10 for the sigma metrics of the remaining authors in PLD.

4. Discussion

The focus of the present study was to investigate PLD as a subfield of pragmatics that is integrated with linguistics, psychology, education, etc. This objective was achieved by analysing the growth of PLD and presenting bibliometric and scientometric data. In the first section, bibliometric indicators such as publications by year and the top 10 regions, universities, journals, publishers, subject/research areas, and authors were provided. In the second section, scientometric indicators, such as citation, co-citation, and co-occurrence, were introduced.
Seven essential points regarding bibliometric indicators were identified: (1) Knowledge output in PLD increased over the past two decades, reaching a peak in 2021 for Scopus and WOS and in 2020 for Lens. (2) The United States ranked first in all three databases, while the United Kingdom and China alternated in second and third place. (3) US universities dominate only Scopus, whereas UK universities and Iranian universities have supplanted them in WOS and Lens, respectively. (4) The journals containing the word ‘pragmatics’ (such as Journal of Pragmatics, Intercultural Pragmatics, and Historical Pragmatics) appear to be the most pertinent. (5) Elsevier and Wiley are the leading publishers for WOS and Lens. (6) PLD-related topic areas include social sciences, arts and humanities, psychology, and medicine. (7) Taguchi [91] and Bosco [166] are the top contributors in the field.
When combined with scientometric indicators, these bibliometric findings have at least five implications. First, identifying the most frequently used keywords assists researchers in following the most commonly researched topics and themes in PLD. For instance, in this study, the most cited keywords included psychology [90], speech act [67], language disability [167], physiology [168], and language disorder [45]. Another list included interlanguage [169], closed head injury [170], pragmatic language skills [171], study abroad [172], and instruction [173]. As evidenced by the keywords listed above, PLD is analysed in terms of three patterns. First, researchers present evidence for PLD in clinical settings and clinical populations by considering various types of disorders. Second, other researchers investigate PLD in individuals with head injuries. Thirdly, researchers who study second language acquisition investigate PLD in an effort to determine how PLD develops in first language vs. second language acquisition/learning.
The second implication relates to data grouping. We mentioned earlier that 6455 documents related to PLD were included. These documents contained the keywords that we used in the search strings. Therefore, in this study, we were able to classify all of these datasets into distinct patterns of interrelation in order to generate clusters that guide PLD research. These clusters included executive function [51], social understanding [167], autism spectrum disorder [174], foreign language [169], traumatic brain injury [170], and pragmatic competence [64]. Overall, these clusters contain the following 12 patterns:
(1)
Analysing PLD as a social behaviour through the lens of executive functions;
(2)
Studying PLD as a social behaviour based on social understanding;
(3)
Examining PLD as a social behaviour associated with autism spectrum disorder;
(4)
Developing an understanding of PLD in academic settings through the examination of executive functions;
(5)
Identifying pragmatic competence versus communicative competence as a social behaviour;
(6)
Analysing pragmatic language skills in aphasic patients via epistemic stances (i.e., attitudes towards knowledge in interaction);
(7)
Investigating PLD as a behavioural problem in the context of a foreign language;
(8)
Assessing PLD as a behavioural problem in individuals with autism spectrum disorder;
(9)
Assessing PLD in persons with traumatic brain injury and closed head injury as a behavioural problem;
(10)
Identifying the role of the right hemisphere in executive functions as a cognitive substrate;
(11)
Assessing the impact of pragmatic failure in speech acts on pragmatic competence;
(12)
Investigating the patterns of PLD among learning-disabled children.
The third implication relates to the most-cited authors in PLD. The detection of such authors has the potential to enhance the appreciation of the current developments in PLD. Although a single publication is a contribution to PLD, the contribution of the most cited authors is more impactful because they have a deeper understanding of the area or have explored themes that are central to the field of PLD. Among these authors were Mentis and Prutting [129], who provided a reliable multidimensional topic analysis, whereas Solberg, Mosser, and McDonald [175] discussed measurement systems. Blum-Kulka and Snow [122] focused on developing children’s autonomy in telling stories. Taguchi [91] comprehensively dealt with teaching pragmatics. Matthews, Biney, and Abbot-Smith [51] analysed the individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability, while Olshtain and Kupferberg [145] were concerned with the professional knowledge of foreign language teachers and how it was reflected in discourse.
The fourth implication pertains to the most-cited PLD publications. Again, recognising these publications is essential because they contain topics and themes in PLD that have drawn the interest of the vast majority of researchers in this subject. Consequently, they are regarded as crucial contributions to leading PLD research. They include topics such as the effect of rejoinders in production questionnaires [176], grounds for instruction in pragmatics [177], pragmatic competence [86], minimisation and conversational inference [117], grammatical errors in language impairment [89], cross-cultural pragmatics [158], interlanguage pragmatics [88], pragmatics and language teaching [113], politeness [116], children’s contributions to dinner talk [118], teaching pragmatics [91], and discourse in the marketplace [178].
The final implication concerns the identification of authors who have the potential to be often cited by other authors. Again, this may be attributable to the fact that their research contains ideas and themes that are currently being disputed and will continue to be investigated in the future. Among the most cited items include the effect of rejoinders in production questionnaires [176], grounds for instruction in pragmatics [177], children’s autonomy in telling stories [122], research in memory [179], the principles for constructing polite speeches [116], and children’s contributions to dinner talk [118].

5. Practical Implications

In order for scientometric studies to be meaningful, researchers must be careful to interpret the findings closely [180], regardless of whether the use of this research method has become more popular in recent years [181,182]. It is recommended that data be retrieved from multiple sources rather than a single database, unless this is well justified (e.g., in this study we used the Scopus, WOS, and Lens databases). We believe that the next step should be to use different tools for the analysis in order to allow for the inclusion of various scientometric indicators (for example, we used both CiteSpace and VOSviewer in this study).

6. Theoretical Implications

At least two theoretical implications can be drawn from this study. First, we presented bibliometric and scientometric indicators highlighting that a great deal of research in PLD takes place in clinical settings. Research should be directed towards school settings and home settings in order to observe PLD in typical language development contexts and compare this evidence with that obtained in clinical settings. Second, most existing evidence focuses on children, adolescents, and adults in either first language or foreign language contexts. Studies on how new-borns and infants acquire pragmatic competence and begin to use communicative competence are limited. Although conducting research on PLD in new-borns and infants may be challenging or unmeasurable, no concrete evidence supports the immeasurability of PLD during the first years of life.

7. Limitations and Future Directions

There are at least two potential limitations to the results of this study. The first limitation concerns the analysis of the evidence for the most researched topics in PLD. While it was possible to group the collected data into several clusters, a detailed examination of these clusters was beyond the scope of this study. As a next step, it would be beneficial to examine these clusters in greater detail in order to identify convergences and divergences between them and thus better document the study of PLD by clinicians and researchers in clinical and educational settings. The second potential limitation is that a scientometric analysis of PLD was not able to identify the most commonly used research methodologies. Following this, a scoping review should be conducted to evaluate existing evidence on PLD based on the methods and measures used to collect and analyse data.

8. Conclusions

The purpose of this study was to examine the size and trends of knowledge production in PLD research. We examined 6455 PLD documents collected from Scopus, WOS, and Lens between 1950 and 2022 using CiteSpace and VOSviewer for scientometric analysis. According to our analysis, there are three major patterns in the study of PLD. Firstly, the leading research institutions and researchers in PLD seem to be centred in the United States, the United Kingdom, China, and Iran. It was beyond the scope of this study to determine whether the intensity of production and contribution to PLD from these regions can be attributed to funding, as well as individual interest in this field. Second, the most common topics examined in PLD were grouped into 12 patterns, as listed in the Discussion section. Third, taking into account the research clusters identified, it is crucial to conduct research on early PLD that considers both linguistic and non-linguistic aspects of language.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, A.A. and I.A.-Q.; data curation, A.A.; formal analysis, A.A.; funding acquisition, H.A.; investigation, A.A. and H.A.; methodology, A.A.; project administration, A.A. and H.A.; resources, I.A.-Q.; software, A.A.; supervision, A.A. and H.A.; validation, A.A. and H.A.; visualisation, A.A.; writing—original draft, A.A. and I.A.-Q.; writing—review and editing, A.A. and H.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by King Saud University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia, under the research project RSP-2021/251.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This research did not require IRB approval.

Informed Consent Statement

Neither human nor non-human subjects were directly involved in this research. Therefore, informed consent was not required.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the first author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the financial support from King Saud University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia for funding the publication of this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Keller-Cohen, D.; Greenfield, P.M.; Smith, J.H. The Structure of Communication in Early Language Development. Language 1979, 55, 444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Dore, J. Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. J. Child Lang. 1975, 2, 21–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bruner, J.S. The ontogenesis of speech acts. J. Child Lang. 1975, 2, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bates, E.; Camaioni, L.; Volterra, V. The Acquisition of Performatives Prior to Speech. Merrill. Palmer. Q. Behav. Dev. 1975, 21, 205–226. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ochs, E. Introduction: What Child Language Can Contribute to Pragmatics. In Developmental Pragmatics; Ochs, E., Scheffelin, B., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1979; pp. 1–17. [Google Scholar]
  6. Feagans, L.; Gar-Vey, C.; Golinkoff, R. The Origins and Growth of Communications. Top. Lang. Disord. 1985, 5, 82–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gallagher, T. Pre-Assessment: A Procedure for Accommodating Language Use Variability. In Pragmatic Assessment and Intervention Issues in Language; Gallagher, T., Prutting, C., Eds.; College Hill Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  8. Ninio, A.; Snow, C.E. Pragmatic Development: Essays in Developmental Science; Westview: Boulder, CO, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  9. Cekaite, A. Child Pragmatic Development. In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics; Blackwell Publishing Ltd.: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  10. Zhunussova, G.; Cortazzi, M.; Jin, L. Roles and models of English teachers in Kazakhstan. World Engl. 2022, 41, 104–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ingram, D. Stages in the Development of One-Word Utterances. In Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Child Language Research Forum, Stanford, CA, USA, 8–10 April 1974. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dore, J. A pragmatic description of early language development. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 1974, 3, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dale, P.S. Is early pragmatic development measurable? J. Child Lang. 1980, 7, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Snyder, L.S. Communicative and Cognitive Abilities and Disabilities in the Sensorimotor Period. Merrill. Palmer. Q. Behav. Dev. 1978, 24, 161–180. [Google Scholar]
  15. Blank, M.; Gessner, M.; Esposito, A. Language without communication: A case study. J. Child Lang. 1979, 6, 329–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bates, E. Pragmatics and Sociolinguisties in Child Language. In Language Deficiency in Children: Selected Readings; University Park: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1976; pp. 411–463. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bishop, D.V.M. Uncommon Understanding (Classic Edition) Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  18. Marcos, H. Introduction: Early pragmatic development. First Lang. 2001, 21, 209–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Moreno-Manso, J.M.; García-Baamonde, M.E.; Alonso, M.B.; Barona, E.G. Pragmatic language development and educational style in neglected children. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2010, 32, 1028–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Papafragou, A. Pragmatic Development. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2018, 14, 167–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bryant, J.B. Pragmatic Development. In The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language; Bavin, E.L., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012; pp. 339–354. [Google Scholar]
  22. Clark, E.V. Conversation and Language Acquisition: A Pragmatic Approach. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2017, 14, 170–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Longobardi, E.; Lonigro, A.; Laghi, F.; O’Neill, D.K. Pragmatic language development in 18- to 47-month-old Italian children: A study with the Language Use Inventory. First Lang. 2017, 37, 252–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Mumme, D.L.; Fernald, A. The Infant as Onlooker: Learning from Emotional Reactions Observed in a Television Scenario. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 221–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Tomasello, M. Origins of Human Communication; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008; ISBN 9780262285070. [Google Scholar]
  26. Woodward, A.L. Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Dev. Sci. 2003, 6, 297–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Woodward, A.L.; Guajardo, J.J. Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cogn. Dev. 2002, 17, 1061–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Meltzoff, A.N. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 31, 838–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bellagamba, F.; Laghi, F.; Lonigro, A.; Pace, C.S. Re-enactment of intended acts from a video presentation by 18- and 24-month-old children. Cogn. Process. 2012, 13, 381–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fitneva, S.A.; Matsui, T. Evidentiality: A Window into Language and Cognitive Development: New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development; Jossey-Bass: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ifantidou, E.; Matsui, T. Pragmatic development in L1, L2, L3: Its biological and cultural foundations. J. Pragmat. 2013, 59, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ifantidou, E. Evidential particles and mind-reading. Pragmat. Cogn. 2005, 13, 253–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Matsui, T.; Yamamoto, T.; McCagg, P. On the role of language in children’s early understanding of others as epistemic beings. Cogn. Dev. 2006, 21, 158–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Perner, J. Understanding the Representational Mind; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1993; ISBN 9780262316033. [Google Scholar]
  35. Pillow, B.H. Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1989, 47, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wimmer, H.; Hogrefe, G.-J.; Perner, J. Children’s Understanding of Informational Access as Source of Knowledge. Child Dev. 1988, 59, 386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Winner, E.; Leekam, S. Distinguishing irony from deception: Understanding the speaker’s second-order intention. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 9, 257–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Tomasello, M.; Conti-Ramsden, G.; Ewert, B. Young children’s conversations with their mothers and fathers: Differences in breakdown and repair. J. Child Lang. 1990, 17, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Marcos, H. Mother-child and father-child communication in the second year: A functional approach. Early Dev. Parent. 1995, 4, 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ryckebusch, C.; Marcos, H. Speech acts, social context and parent-toddler play between the ages of 1;5 and 2;3. J. Pragmat. 2004, 36, 883–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Salomo, D.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. Children’s ability to answer different types of questions. J. Child Lang. 2012, 40, 469–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Loukusa, S.; Ryder, N.; Leinonen, E. Answering Questions and Explaining Answers: A Study of Finnish-Speaking Children. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2007, 37, 219–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Loukusa, S.; Leinonen, E.; Ryder, N. Development of pragmatic language comprehension in Finnish-speaking children. First Lang. 2007, 27, 279–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ryder, N.; Leinonen, E. Use of context in question answering by 3-, 4- and 5-year-old children. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2003, 32, 397–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. O’Neill, D.K. The Language Use Inventory for Young Children: A Parent-Report Measure of Pragmatic Language Development for 18- to 47-Month-Old Children. J. Speech, Lang. Heart Res. 2007, 50, 214–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Davies, C.N.; Katsos, N. Over-informative children: Production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua 2010, 120, 1956–1972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Morisseau, T.; Davies, C.; Matthews, D. How do 3- and 5-year-olds respond to under- and over-informative utterances? J. Pragmat. 2013, 59, 26–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Matthews, D.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. How Toddlers and Preschoolers Learn to Uniquely Identify Referents for Others: A Training Study. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 1744–1759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Philofsky, A.; Fidler, D.J.; Hepburn, S. Pragmatic Language Profiles of School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Williams Syndrome. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2007, 16, 368–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ninio, A.; Snow, C.E. The Development of Pragmatics: Learning to Use Language Appropriately. In Handbook of Child Language Acquisition; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1999; pp. 347–383. [Google Scholar]
  51. Matthews, D.; Biney, H.; Abbot-Smith, K. Individual Differences in Children’s Pragmatic Ability: A Review of Associations with Formal Language, Social Cognition, and Executive Functions. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2018, 14, 186–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Scott-Phillips, T.C. Pragmatics and the aims of language evolution. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016, 24, 186–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hobson, R.P. Communicative depth: Soundings from developmental psychopathology. Infant Behav. Dev. 2007, 30, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Hobson, R.P. Interpersonally Situated Cognition. Int. J. Philos. Stud. 2008, 16, 377–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Achim, A.M.; Fossard, M.; Couture, S.; Achim, A.; Achim, A.M.; Achim, A. Adjustment of speaker’s referential expressions to an addressee’s likely knowledge and link with theory of mind abilities. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Baixauli-Fortea, I.; Casas, A.M.; Berenguer-Forner, C.; Colomer, C.; Roselló-Miranda, B. Pragmatic competence of children with autism spectrum disorder. Impact of theory of mind, verbal working memory, ADHD symptoms, and structural language. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2017, 8, 101–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Farrant, B.M.; Maybery, M.T.; Fletcher, J. Socio-emotional engagement, joint attention, imitation, and conversation skill: Analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. First Lang. 2010, 31, 23–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Perkins, M.R. Pragmatic Impairment. In The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders; Damico, J.S., Müller, N., Ball, M.J., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing Ltd.: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 227–246. ISBN 9780511486555. [Google Scholar]
  59. Green, B.C.; Johnson, K.; Bretherton, L. Pragmatic language difficulties in children with hyperactivity and attention problems: An integrated review. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2013, 49, 15–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Russell, R.L. Social Communication Impairments: Pragmatics. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2007, 54, 483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Russell, R.L.; Grizzle, K.L. Assessing Child and Adolescent Pragmatic Language Competencies: Toward Evidence-Based Assessments. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 11, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Qasem, F.; Alduais, A.; Alfadda, H.; Alfadda, N.; Al Amri, L. A Study on the Relationship between Pragmatic Language Development and Socioeconomic Status in Arab Preschoolers with and without Pragmatic Language Impairment. Sustainability 2022, 14, 6369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Alduais, A.M.S. “Use of an Arabic-language Version of TOPL-2 to Identify Typical and Atypical Manifestations of Pragmatic Language Impairment in Individuals with Developmental Dysphasia”. IOSR J. Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2012, 3, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Andrés-Roqueta, C.; Katsos, N. The Contribution of Grammar, Vocabulary and Theory of Mind in Pragmatic Language Competence in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Social Communication. Available online: https://www.asha.org/public/speech/development/social-communication/ (accessed on 15 July 2022).
  66. Zufferey, S. Acquiring Pragmatics Social and Cognitive Perspectives, 1st ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2015; ISBN 978-1-315-74795-8. [Google Scholar]
  67. Norbury, C.F. Atypical Pragmatic Development. In Trends in Language Acquisition Research. Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition; Matthews, D., Ed.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 10, ISBN 9789027234803. [Google Scholar]
  68. Turkstra, L.S.; Clark, A.; Burgess, S.; Hengst, J.A.; Wertheimer, J.C.; Paul, D. Pragmatic communication abilities in children and adults: Implications for rehabilitation professionals. Disabil. Rehabil. 2016, 39, 1872–1885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Glänzel, W.; Schoepflin, U. Little scientometrics, big scientometrics...and beyond? Scientometrics 1994, 30, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Chellappandi, P.; Vijayakumar, C.S. Bibliometrics, Scientometrics, Webometrics/Cybermetrics, Informetrics and Altmetrics-An Emerging Field in Library and Information Science Research. Int. J. Educ. 2018, 7, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Egghe, L. Little science, big science... and beyond. Scientometrics 1994, 30, 389–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Parkinson, M. The Evaluation of Research by Scientometric Indicators. Libr. Manag. 2011, 32, 226–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Huang, Y.; Glänzel, W.; Zhang, L. Tracing the development of mapping knowledge domains. Scientometrics 2021, 126, 6201–6224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Sooryamoorthy, R. Scientometrics for the Humanities and Social Sciences; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2020; ISBN 9780367626860. [Google Scholar]
  75. Birkle, C.; Pendlebury, D.A.; Schnell, J.; Adams, J. Web of Science as a data source for research on scientific and scholarly activity. Quant. Sci. Stud. 2020, 1, 363–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Burnham, J.F. Scopus database: A review. Biomed. Digit. Libr. 2006, 3, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Pranckutė, R. Web of Science (WoS) and Scopus: The Titans of Bibliographic Information in Today’s Academic World. Publications 2021, 9, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Penfold, R. Using the Lens database for staff publications. J. Med Libr. Assoc. 2020, 108, 341–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Chen, C. The CiteSpace Manual: Version 1.01; Drexel University: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2014; pp. 1–84. [Google Scholar]
  80. Van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L. VOSviewer Manual: Manual for VOSviewer Version 1.6.18; Univeristeit Leiden: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  81. Freeman, L.C. Centrality in social networks conceptual clarification. Soc. Netw. 1978–1979, 1, 215–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Kleinberg, J. Bursty and Hierarchical Structure in Streams. In Proceedings of the Proceedings of the Eighth ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining-KDD ’02, Edmonton, AB, Canada, 23–26 July 2002; ACM Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 91–101. [Google Scholar]
  83. Chen, C. CiteSpace: A Practical Guide for Mapping Scientific Literature; Nova Publishers: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 9781536102956. [Google Scholar]
  84. Rollins, P.R. Developmental Pragmatics. Huang, Y., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  85. Cummings, L. The Routledge Pragmatics Encyclopedia. Cummings, L., Ed.; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  86. Taguchi, N. Pragmatic Competence. Mouton Series in Pragmatics; Taguchi, N., Ed.; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2009; ISBN 9783110218558. [Google Scholar]
  87. Bosco, F.M.; Angeleri, R.; Colle, L.; Sacco, K.; Bara, B.G. Communicative abilities in children: An assessment through different phenomena and expressive means. J. Child Lang. 2013, 40, 741–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Kasper, G.; Schmidt, R. Developmental Issues in Interlanguage Pragmatics. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 1996, 18, 149–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Bishop, D.V.M. Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Appl. Psycholinguist. 1994, 15, 507–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Dammeyer, J. A Longitudinal Study of Pragmatic Language Development in Three Children with Cochlear Implants. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2012, 14, 217–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Taguchi, N. Teaching Pragmatics: Trends and Issues. Annu. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 2011, 31, 289–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. McDonald, S.; Flanagan, S.; Rollins, J.; Kinch, J. TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social reception after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2003, 18, 219–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Barner, D.; Brooks, N.; Bale, A. Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition 2011, 118, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Barron, A. Acquisition in Interlanguage Pragmatics; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2003; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  95. Kanno, K.; Tsujimura, N. An Introduction to Japanese Linguistics. J. Assoc. Teach. Jpn. 1996, 30, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bardovi-Harlig, K.; Dörnyei, Z. Do Language Learners Recognize Pragmatic Violations? Pragmatic versus Grammatical Awareness in Instructed L2 Learning. TESOL Q. 1998, 32, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Bardovi-Harlig, K. Exploring the Interlanguage of Interlanguage Pragmatics: A Research Agenda for Acquisitional Pragmatics. Lang. Learn. 1999, 49, 677–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. McDonald, S. Impairments in Social Cognition Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2013, 19, 231–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Owens, R.E. Language Development: An Introduction; Pearson: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  100. Tweed, R.G.; Lehman, D.R. Learning considered within a cultural context: Confucian and Socratic approaches. Am. Psychol. 2002, 57, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Botting, N. Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2002, 18, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Norbury, C.F.; Bishop, D.V.M. Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2003, 38, 287–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Ware, P.D.; O’Dowd, R. Peer Feedback on Language Form in Telecollaboration. Lang. Learn. Technol. 2008, 12, 43–63. [Google Scholar]
  104. Burt, S.M. Pragmatic Development in a Second Language. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2005, 27, 116–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Farinde, R.O.; Oyedokun-Alli, W.A. Pragmatics and Language Teaching. J. Lang. Teach. Res. 2020, 11, 841–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Brinton, L.J. The Comment Clause in English; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2008; ISBN 9780521886734. [Google Scholar]
  107. Tomasello, M. The social bases of language acquisition. Soc. Dev. 1992, 1, 67–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Verhoeven, L.T. Transfer in Bilingual Development: The Linguistic Interdependence Hypothesis Revisited. Lang. Learn. 1994, 44, 381–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Hardaker, C. Trolling in asynchronous computer-mediated communication: From user discussions to academic definitions. J. Politeness-Res. 2010, 6, 215–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Gorski, P.C. What we’re teaching teachers: An analysis of multicultural teacher education coursework syllabi. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2009, 25, 309–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Ren, W. L2 Pragmatic Development in Study Abroad Contexts; Peter Lang AG: New York, NY, USA, 2015; ISBN 9783035198737. [Google Scholar]
  112. Bella, S. Length of residence and intensity of interaction. Pragmatics. Q. Publ. Int. Pragmat. Assoc. (IPrA) 2022, 22, 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Bardovi-Harlig, K. Pragmatics and Language Teaching: Bringing Pragmatics and Pedagogy Together. Pragmat. Lang. Learn. Monogr. Ser. 1996, 7, 21–39. [Google Scholar]
  114. Kasper, G. Analysing Verbal Protocols. TESOL Q. 1998, 32, 358–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Bardovi-Harlig, K. Another Piece of the Puzzle: The Emergence of the Present Perfect. Lang. Learn. 2001, 51, 215–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Brown, P.; Levinson, S.C.; Gumperz, J.J. Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1987; ISBN 9780521308625. [Google Scholar]
  117. Levinson, S.C. Minimization and conversational inference. In The Pragmatic Perspective: Selected Papers from the 1985 International Pragmatics Conference; Verschueren, J., Bertuccelli Papi, M., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1987; p. 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Blum-Kulka, S. The Dynamics of Family Dinner Talk: Cultural Contexts for Children’s Passages to Adult Discourse. Res. Lang. Soc. Interact. 1994, 27, 1–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Blum-Kulka, S.; House, J.; Freedle, R.; Kasper, G. Cross-Cultural Pragmatics: Requests and Apologies; Ablex Publishing Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  120. Leech, G.; Thomas, J. Language, Meaning and Context: Pragmatics. In An Encyclopedia of Language; Collinge, N.E., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2002; pp. 105–124. [Google Scholar]
  121. Thomas, J. Meaning in Interaction: An Introduction to Pragmatics, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  122. Blum-Kulka, S.; Snow, C.E. Developing Autonomy for Tellers, Tales, and Telling in Family Narrative Events. J. Narrat. Life Hist. 1992, 2, 187–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Ellis, R. Does It “Work”? Evaluating Tasks in Language Teaching. Lang. Teach. 1996, 20, 21–42. [Google Scholar]
  124. Barron, A.; Schneider, K.P. Variational pragmatics: Studying the impact of social factors on language use in interaction. Intercult. Pragmat. 2009, 6, 425–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Grice, P. Studies in the Ways of Words; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  126. Han, Y.; Ellis, R. Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge and general language proficiency. Lang. Teach. Res. 1998, 2, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. House, J. Communicating in English as a lingua franca. EUROSLA Yearb. 2002, 2, 243–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Færch, C.; Kasper, G. Ja und?—og hva’ så ?—A contrastive discourse analysis of gambits in German and Danish. In Contrastive Linguistics; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 1984; pp. 69–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Mentis, M.; Prutting, C.A. Analysis of Topic as Illustrated in a Head-Injured and a Normal Adult. J. Speech, Lang. Heart Res. 1991, 34, 583–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. McDonald, S.; Pearce, S. Requests That Overcome Listener Reluctance: Impairment Associated with Executive Dysfunction in Brain Injury. Brain Lang. 1998, 61, 88–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Kelly, C.; Morgan, G.; Freeth, M.; Siegal, M.; Matthews, D. The Understanding of Communicative Intentions in Children with Severe-to-Profound Hearing Loss. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2019, 24, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Hartley, L.L.; Jensen, P.J. Narrative and procedural discourse after closed head injury. Brain Inj. 1991, 5, 267–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Ben-Rafael, E.; Olshtain, E.; Geijst, I. Identity and Language: The Social Insertion of Soviet Jews in Israel. In Immigration to Israel; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 331–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Ninio, A.; Snow, C.E.; Pan, B.A.; Rollins, P.R. Classifying communicative acts in children’s interactions. J. Commun. Disord. 1994, 27, 157–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. He, Z.; Ren, W. Current address behaviour in China. East Asian Pragmat. 2016, 1, 163–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Billmyer, K. Investigating instrument-based pragmatic variability: Effects of enhancing discourse completion tests. Appl. Linguist. 2000, 21, 517–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Cremona, C.; Bates, E. The development of attitudes toward dialect in Italian children. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 1977, 6, 223–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Bates, E.; Thal, D.; Whitesell, K.; Fenson, L.; Oakes, L. Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 1989, 25, 1004–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Gibbs, R.W. The Fight over Metaphor in Thought and Language. In Figurative Language and Thought; Katz, A.N., Turner, M., Cacciari, C., Gibbs, R.W., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 88–118. [Google Scholar]
  140. Tajeddin, Z.; Alemi, M. Effective Language Teachers as Persons: Exploring Pre-Service and in-Service Teachers’ Beliefs. TESL-EJ 2019, 22, 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  141. Ninio, A. Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. J. Child Lang. 1999, 26, 619–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Kecskes, I.; Zhang, F. Activating, Seeking, and Creating Common Ground: A Socio-Cognitive Approach. Pragmat. Cogn. 2009, 17, 331–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Peterson, C.C.; Siegal, M. Representing Inner Worlds: Theory of Mind in Autistic, Deaf, and Normal Hearing Children. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 10, 126–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Tateyama, Y. Requesting in Japanese: The Effect of Instruction on JFL Learners’ Pragmatic Competence. In Pragmatic Competence; Taguchi, N., Ed.; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  145. Olshtain, E.; Kupferberg, I. Reflective-narrative discourse of FL teachers exhibits professional knowledge. Lang. Teach. Res. 1998, 2, 185–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. House, J. Misunderstanding in Intercultural Communication. Fonetiek Vreemde-Talenonderwijs 1997, 57, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Taguchi, N. Instructed pragmatics at a glance: Where instructional studies were, are, and should be going. Lang. Teach. 2015, 48, 1–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Taguchi, N.; Roever, C. Second Language Pragmatics; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  149. Kasper, G.; Rose, K.R. Introduction to Second Language Pragmatic Development. Lang. Learn. 2002, 52, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Bambini, V.; Arcara, G.; Bechi, M.; Buonocore, M.; Cavallaro, R.; Bosia, M. The communicative impairment as a core feature of schizophrenia: Frequency of pragmatic deficit, cognitive substrates, and relation with quality of life. Compr. Psychiatry 2016, 71, 106–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Bardovi-Harlig, K. Developing L2 Pragmatics. Lang. Learn. 2013, 63, 68–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Adams, C.; Lockton, E.; Freed, J.; Gaile, J.; Earl, G.; McBean, K.; Nash, M.; Green, J.; Vail, A.; Law, J. The Social Communication Intervention Project: A randomized controlled trial of the effectiveness of speech and language therapy for school-age children who have pragmatic and social communication problems with or without autism spectrum disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Shively, R.L. L2 pragmatic development in study abroad: A longitudinal study of Spanish service encounters. J. Pragmat. 2011, 43, 1818–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Alcón-Soler, E. Pragmatic learning and study abroad: Effects of instruction and length of stay. System 2015, 48, 62–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Félix-Brasdefer, J.C. Speech Acts in Interaction: Negotiating Joint Action in a Second Language. In The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition and Pragmatics; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2019; pp. 17–30. [Google Scholar]
  156. Ishihara, N.; Cohen, A.D. Teaching and Learning Pragmatics: Where Language and Culture Meet, 2nd ed.; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  157. Li, S. The Effects of Input-Based Practice on Pragmatic Development of Requests in L2 Chinese. Lang. Learn. 2011, 62, 403–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Kuhn, E.D.; Blum-Kulka, S.; House, J.; Kasper, G. Cross-Cultural Pragmatics: Requests and Apologies. Language 1991, 67, 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Takahashi, S. Assessing Learnability in Second Language Pragmatics. In Pragmatics Across Languages and Cultures; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2010; pp. 391–422. [Google Scholar]
  160. Félix-Brasdefer, J.C. Data collection methods in speech act performance. In Speech Act Performance: Theoretical, Empirical and Methodological Issues; Martinez-Flor, A., Uso-Juan, E., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2010; pp. 41–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Ellis, N.C.; Schmidt, R. Morphology and Longer Distance Dependencies: Laboratory Research Illuminating the A in SLA. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 1997, 19, 145–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Taguchi, N. Pragmatic Competence in Japanese as a Second Language: An Introduction. In Pragmatic Competence; Taguchi, N., Ed.; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  163. Achiba, M. Learning to Request in a Second Language: A Study of Child Interlanguage Pragmatics; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2003; ISBN 9781853596131. [Google Scholar]
  164. Kasper, G.; Blum-Kulka, S. Interlanguage Pragmatics: An Introduction; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  165. Takahashi, S. Pragmalinguistic Awareness: Is it Related to Motivation and Proficiency? Appl. Linguist. 2005, 26, 90–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Bosco, F.M.; Parola, A.; Angeleri, R.; Galetto, V.; Zettin, M.; Gabbatore, I. Improvement of Communication Skills after Traumatic Brain Injury: The Efficacy of the Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment Program using the Communicative Activities of Daily Living. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2018, 33, 875–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Hatton, C. Pragmatic language skills in people with intellectual disabilities: A review. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 1998, 23, 79–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Bishop, D.V.M.; Snow, P.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T.; CATALISE consortium. CATALISE: A Multinational and Multidisciplinary Delphi Consensus Study. Identifying Language Impairments in Children. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0158753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  169. Farnsworth, M. Differentiating Second Language Acquisition from Specific Learning Disability: An Observational Tool Assessing Dual Language Learners’ Pragmatic Competence. Young-Except. Child. 2016, 21, 92–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Deighton, S.; Ju, N.; Graham, S.; Yeates, K. Pragmatic Language Comprehension After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury: A Scoping Review. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2020, 35, E113–E126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  171. Alduais, A.; Qasem, F.; Alfadda, H.; Alfadda, N.; AlAmri, L. Arabic Validation of the Pragmatic Language Skills Inventory to Assess Pragmatic Language Development in Preschoolers with and without Pragmatic Language Impairment. Children 2022, 9, 809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Kinginger, C.; Farrell, K. Assessing Development of Meta-Pragmatic Awareness in Study Abroad. Front. Interdiscip. J. Study Abroad 2004, 10, 19–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  173. Goberis, D.; Beams, D.; Dalpes, M.; Abrisch, A.; Baca, R.; Yoshinaga-Itano, C. The Missing Link in Language Development of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children: Pragmatic Language Development. Semin. Speech Lang. 2012, 33, 297–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Whyte, E.M.; Nelson, K.E. Trajectories of pragmatic and nonliteral language development in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Commun. Disord. 2015, 54, 2–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Solberg, L.I.; Mosser, G.; McDonald, S. The Three Faces of Performance Measurement: Improvement, Accountability, and Research. Jt. Comm. J. Qual. Improv. 1997, 23, 135–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  176. Johnston, B.; Kasper, G.; Ross, S. Effect of Rejoinders in Production Questionnaires. Appl. Linguist. 1998, 19, 157–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  177. Bardovi-Harlig, K. Evaluating the Empirical Evidence: Grounds for Instruction in Pragmatics. In Pragmatics in Language Teaching; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2001; pp. 13–32. [Google Scholar]
  178. Thomas, J. Discourse in the Marketplace: The Making of Meaning in Annual Reports. J. Bus. Commun. 1997, 34, 47–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Kintsch, W.; Bates, E. Recognition memory for statements from a classroom lecture. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1977, 3, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L. Visualizing Bibliometric Networks. In Measuring Scholarly Impact; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2014; pp. 285–320. ISBN 9783319103761. [Google Scholar]
  181. Van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L.; Dekker, R.; van den Berg, J. A comparison of two techniques for bibliometric mapping: Multidimensional scaling and VOS. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2010, 61, 2405–2416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  182. Moral-Muñoz, J.A.; Herrera-Viedma, E.; Santisteban-Espejo, A.; Cobo, M.J. Software tools for conducting bibliometric analysis in science: An up-to-date review. Prof. Inform. 2020, 29, e290103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by year: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 1. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by year: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g001aChildren 09 01407 g001b
Figure 2. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by country: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 2. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by country: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g002aChildren 09 01407 g002b
Figure 3. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by university: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 3. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by university: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g003aChildren 09 01407 g003b
Figure 4. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by journal: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens; (D) Lens.
Figure 4. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by journal: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens; (D) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g004aChildren 09 01407 g004bChildren 09 01407 g004c
Figure 5. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by publisher: (A) WOS; (B) Lens.
Figure 5. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by publisher: (A) WOS; (B) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g005
Figure 6. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by research area: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 6. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by research area: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g006aChildren 09 01407 g006b
Figure 7. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by author: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 7. Pragmatic language development knowledge production size by author: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g007aChildren 09 01407 g007b
Figure 8. Top 10 keywords with the strongest citation bursts: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS.
Figure 8. Top 10 keywords with the strongest citation bursts: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS.
Children 09 01407 g008
Figure 9. Timeline of top keywords, cited authors, and clusters: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Scopus—cited authors; (D) WOS.
Figure 9. Timeline of top keywords, cited authors, and clusters: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Scopus—cited authors; (D) WOS.
Children 09 01407 g009aChildren 09 01407 g009b
Figure 10. Network visualisation of keyword co-occurrence by author: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Figure 10. Network visualisation of keyword co-occurrence by author: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens.
Children 09 01407 g010aChildren 09 01407 g010b
Figure 11. Network visualisation of author (co)-citation: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens—citation.
Figure 11. Network visualisation of author (co)-citation: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens—citation.
Children 09 01407 g011aChildren 09 01407 g011b
Figure 12. Density visualisation of (co)-citation by source: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens—citation.
Figure 12. Density visualisation of (co)-citation by source: (A) Scopus; (B) WOS; (C) Lens—citation.
Children 09 01407 g012aChildren 09 01407 g012b
Figure 13. Top 10 cited authors and references with the strongest citation bursts: (A) Scopus [16,67,86,111,128,140,142,144,145,146]; (B) Scopus [51,91,147,148,149,150,151,152,153]; (C) WOS [8,50,92,118,129,131,132,136,138,139]; (D) WOS [147,148,150,151,153,154,155,156,157].
Figure 13. Top 10 cited authors and references with the strongest citation bursts: (A) Scopus [16,67,86,111,128,140,142,144,145,146]; (B) Scopus [51,91,147,148,149,150,151,152,153]; (C) WOS [8,50,92,118,129,131,132,136,138,139]; (D) WOS [147,148,150,151,153,154,155,156,157].
Children 09 01407 g013aChildren 09 01407 g013b
Table 1. Major communication skills of pragmatic competence (adapted from ASHA, as cited in [63] (p. 12)).
Table 1. Major communication skills of pragmatic competence (adapted from ASHA, as cited in [63] (p. 12)).
Using LanguageChanging LanguageFollowing Rules
Greetings (e.g., Hello. Goodbye. How are you?)Used according to the needs of a
listener or situation
Used for conversations and
storytelling
Informing (e.g., I am leaving.)Talking differently to a baby than
to an adult
Taking turns in conversation
Demanding (e.g., Pick up the toy.)Giving background information
to an unfamiliar listener
Introducing topics of
conversation
Promising (e.g., I am going to the playground.)Speaking differently in a
classroom than on a playground
Staying on topic
Requesting (e.g., Do you want to go along?) Rephrasing when
misunderstood
Using verbal and non-verbal
signals
How close to stand to someone
when speaking
Using facial expressions
Using eye contact
Table 2. Journals publishing research in pragmatic language development.
Table 2. Journals publishing research in pragmatic language development.
Source JournalHost CountryPublisherSpanVolumesWeb AddressScope of the Journal
ChildrenSwitzerlandMDPI AG2020–20219https://www.mdpi.com/journal/children, accessed on 10 July 2022Sharing clinical, epidemiological, and translational science relevant to children’s health.
Children and Youth Services ReviewUnited KingdomElsevier Ltd.1979–202118https://www.sciencedirect.com/journal/children-and-youth-services-review, accessed on 10 July 2022Focusing on disadvantaged or otherwise at-risk children, youth, families, and the systems supporting them. Research relevant to policies, interventions, programs, and services that enhance well-being is conducted in this forum.
Child DevelopmentUnited StatesWiley-Blackwell1945–1948, 1950–202193https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/, accessed on 10 July 2022Providing the latest research to researchers and theorists as well as child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, psychiatric social workers, early childhood education specialists, school psychologists, special education teachers, and other researchers.
Journal of
Child Language
United KingdomCambridge University Press1974–202149https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/journal-of-child-language, accessed on 10 July 2022Taking a comprehensive look at children’s language behavior, the principles underlying it, and the theories that explain it.
Language Learning and DevelopmentUnited KingdomTaylor and Francis Ltd.2010–202118https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hlld20, accessed on 10 July 2022Providing an opportunity for interaction among a wide community of scholars and practitioners interested in language acquisition.
International Journal of Language and Communication DisordersUnited StatesWiley-Blackwell1966–202157International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders-Wiley Online LibrarySpeech, language, and communication disorders, as well as speech and language therapy, are all welcome.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing ResearchUnited StatesAmerican Speech–Language–Hearing Association (ASHA)1996–202165https://pubs.asha.org/journal/jslhr, accessed on 10 July 2022Publishing peer-reviewed research and other scientific articles on speech, language, hearing, cognition, oral motor skills, and swallowing.
Infants and Young ChildrenUnited StatesLippincott Williams and Wilkins Ltd.1988–202135https://journals.lww.com/iycjournal/pages/default.aspx, accessed on 10 July 2022Providing support to children with disabilities and their families, ages birth to five.
Autism and Developmental Language ImpairmentsUnited KingdomSAGE Publications Ltd.2018–20217https://journals.sagepub.com/home/dli, accessed on 10 July 2022Contributing to the shaping of research in developmental communication disorders.
Studies in PragmaticsNetherlandsBrill Academic Publishers2009–2010, 2012, 2014–201721https://brill.com/view/serial/SIP, accessed on 10 July 2022Providing high-quality theoretical, analytical, and applied pragmatic studies in a widely read, respected international forum.
Table 3. Bibliometric and scientometric indicators to measure pragmatic language development.
Table 3. Bibliometric and scientometric indicators to measure pragmatic language development.
ElementDefinition/Specification/Retrieved DataDatabase/Software
IndicatorScopusWOSLens
Bibliometric
YearProduction size by year
CountryTop countries publishing in the field
UniversityTop universities, research centres, etc.
SourceTop journals, book series, etc.
PublisherTop publishersX
Subject areaTop fields associated with the field
AuthorTop authors publishing in the field
CitationTop cited documents
Scientometric CiteSpaceVOSviewer
Betweenness centralityAchieved when located on a path between two nodes [81].X
Burst detectionDetermines the frequency of a certain event in a certain period (e.g., the frequent citation of a certain reference during a period of time) [82].X
Co-citationWhen two references are cited by a third reference [83]. CiteSpace provides a document co-citation network for references and an author co-citation network for authors.
In VOSviewer, co-citation is defined as “the relatedness of items is determined based on the number of times they are cited together” [80] (p. 5). Units of analysis include cited authors, references, or sources.
SilhouetteUsed in cluster analysis to measure the consistency of each cluster with its related nodes [79].X
SigmaTo measure the strength of a node in terms of betweenness centrality and citation burst [79].X
Clusters“We can probably eyeball the visualized network and identify some prominent groupings” [79] (p. 23).
Citation“The relatedness of items is determined based on the number of times they cite each other” [80] (p. 5). Units of analysis include documents, sources, authors, organisations, or countries.
KeywordsCiteSpace provides co-occurring author keywords and keywords plus.
In VOSviewer, for co-occurrence analysis: “the relatedness of items is determined based on the number of documents in which they occur together” [80] (p. 5). Units of analysis include author keywords, all keywords, or keywords plus.
Table 4. Search strings for retrieving data on pragmatic language development.
Table 4. Search strings for retrieving data on pragmatic language development.
Scopus
(TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language development”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic development”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language skills”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic skills”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language competence”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic competence”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language acquisition”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic acquisition”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language learning”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic learning”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic language performance”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“pragmatic performance”)) AND (EXCLUDE (EXACTSRCTITLE, “Journal Of Hazardous Materials”)) AND (LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “ar”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “ch”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “re”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “bk”))
Friday, 25 March 2022, 1470 document results, 1973–2022.
WOS
“pragmatic language development” (Topic) or “pragmatic language skills” (Topic) or “pragmatic language competence” (Topic) or “pragmatic language acquisition” (Topic) or “pragmatic language learning” (Topic) or “pragmatic language performance” (Topic) or “pragmatic development” (Topic) or “pragmatic competence” (Topic) or “pragmatic acquisition” (Topic) or “pragmatic learning” (Topic) or “pragmatic performance” (Topic) or “pragmatic skills” (Topic) and JOURNAL OF HAZARDOUS MATERIALS (Exclude—Publication Titles) and Articles or Book Chapters or Review Articles or Early Access or Books (Document Types)
Friday, 25 March 2022, 1063 results, 1985–2022.
Lens
(Title: (AND (“pragmatic language development” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language development” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language development” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language development” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic development” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic development” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic development” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic development” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic language skills” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language skills” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language skills” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language skills” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic skills” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic skills” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic skills” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic skills” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic language competence” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language competence” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language competence” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language competence” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic competence” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic competence” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic competence” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic competence” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic language acquisition” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language acquisition” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language acquisition” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language acquisition” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic acquisition” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic acquisition” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic acquisition” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic acquisition” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic language learning” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language learning” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language learning” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language learning” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic learning” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic learning” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic learning” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic learning” AND))))) OR ((Title: (AND (“pragmatic language performance” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic language performance” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic language performance” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic language performance” AND))))) OR (Title: (AND (“pragmatic performance” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“pragmatic performance” AND)) OR (Keyword: (AND (“pragmatic performance” AND)) OR Field of Study: (AND (“pragmatic performance” AND)))))))))))))))
Filters: Stemming = Disabled Publication Type = (journal article, unknown, book chapter, dissertation, book, preprint) Author Display Name = (excl John Forester)
Friday, 25 March 2022, Scholarly Works (3922), 1950–2022.
Table 5. Top cited documents in pragmatic language development from Scopus, WOS, and Lens.
Table 5. Top cited documents in pragmatic language development from Scopus, WOS, and Lens.
No. Source TitleCitation Citations by Database
ScopusWOSLens
1A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury[92]X394X
2Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference[93]X139X
3Acquisition in interlanguage pragmatics[94]XX314
4An introduction to Japanese linguistics[95]XX322
5Developmental issues in interlanguage pragmatics[88]212X391
6Do language learners recognize pragmatic violations? Pragmatic versus grammatical awareness in instructed L2 learning[96]254200483
7Exploring the interlanguage of interlanguage pragmatics: A research agenda for acquisitional pragmatics[97]199154X
8Impairments in social cognition following severe traumatic brain injury[98]X126X
9Language development: an introduction[99]XX517
10Learning considered within a cultural context—Confucian and Socratic approaches[100]353296421
11Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments[101]215XX
12Narrative skills of children with communication impairments[102]311273412
13Peer feedback on language form in telecollaboration[103]X126X
14Pragmatic development in a second language[104]XX611
15Pragmatics in language teaching[105]XX590
16The comment clause in English: Syntactic origins and pragmatic development[106]206XX
17The social bases of language acquisition[107]208X410
18Transfer in bilingual development—the linguistic interdependence hypothesis revisited[108]X150X
19Trolling in asynchronous computer-mediated communication: From user discussions to academic definitions[109]310XX
20What we’re teaching teachers: An analysis of multicultural teacher education coursework syllabi[110]186151X
Table 6. Summary of the largest clusters of pragmatic language development.
Table 6. Summary of the largest clusters of pragmatic language development.
Cluster IDSizeSilhouetteLabel (LSI)Label (LLR)Label (MI)Average Year
Scopus
02360.737pragmatic developmentexecutive function (781.1, 1.0 × 10−4)social behaviour (3.89)2009
11640.742pragmatic developmentsocial understanding (520.14, 1.0 × 10−4)social behaviour (0.75)1994
21350.833autism spectrum disorderautism spectrum disorder (1378.46, 1.0 × 10−4)social behaviour (1.18)2007
31200.862executive functionexecutive function (667.96, 1.0 × 10−4)academic setting (1.35)2010
4590.904pragmatic competencecommunicative competence (527.75, 1.0 × 10−4)social behaviour (0.46)2002
5550.879pragmatic language skillepistemic stance (252.15, 1.0 × 10−4)aphasic patient (0.19)1996
WOS
02140.737pragmatic developmentforeign language (729.41, 1.0 × 10−4)behavioural problem (3.33)2009
11470.853autism spectrum disorderautism spectrum disorder (938.03, 1.0 × 10−4)behavioural problem (2.13)2007
21100.942traumatic brain injuryclosed head injury (358.23, 1.0 × 10−4)behavioural problem (0.37)1998
3770.896executive functioncognitive substrate (339.59, 1.0 × 10−4)right hemisphere (0.7)2010
4670.905pragmatic competencespeech act (547.33, 1.0 × 10−4)pragmatic failure (0.76)2000
5620.955learning-disabled childrenlearning-disabled children (93.91, 1.0 × 10−4)pragmatic development (0.03)1993
Table 7. Top citations counts for pragmatic language development.
Table 7. Top citations counts for pragmatic language development.
WoSScopus
CitationReferenceCluster IDCitationReferenceCluster ID
258Kasper [114]0380Kasper [88]0
187Bardovi-Harlig [115]0300Bardovi-Harlig [113]0
162Taguchi [86]0254Brown [116]0
153Levinson [117]0239Blum-Kulka [118]0
131Bishop [89]1220Taguchi [91]0
113House [119]0181[Anonymous], 19816
113Thomas [120]0178Thomas [121]0
111Blum-Kulka [122]0145Ellis [123]0
109Barron [124]0145Grice [125]3
102Ellis [126]0145House [127]0
Table 8. Top bursts detected in pragmatic language development.
Table 8. Top bursts detected in pragmatic language development.
WoSScopus
BurstReferenceCluster IDBurstReferenceCluster ID
12.76Prutting [129]112.35Taguchi [91]0
8.59Mcdonald [130]210.16Faerch [128]0
8.46Blum-Kulka [122]010.16Matthews [131]3
7.77Hartley [132]210.04Olshtain [133]0
7.17Snow [134]79.33Ren [135]0
6.61Billmyer [136]08.96Bates [137]1
6.49Bates [138]18.91House [127]0
5.41Gibbs [139]38.56Tajeddin [140]0
5.37Ninio [141]18.44Kecskes [142]0
4.93Siegal [143]68.21Tateyama [144]0
Table 9. Betweenness centrality for top 10 authors in pragmatic language development.
Table 9. Betweenness centrality for top 10 authors in pragmatic language development.
WoSScopus
CentralityReferenceCluster IDCentralityReferenceCluster ID
97Kasper [114]0148Bates [137]1
96Bardovi-Harlig [115]0120Brown [116]0
85Blum-Kulka [122]0103Blum-Kulka [118]0
78Blum-Kulka [158]0102Bardovi-Harlig [113]0
76Takahashi [159]0100Kasper [88]0
73Felix-Brasdefer [160]091Ellis [123]0
72Bishop [89]190Schmidt [161]0
70Taguchi [162]081Achiba [163]0
70Blum-Kulka [164]081Takahashi [165]0
69House [146]081Rose [149]0
Table 10. Sigma metrics for top 10 authors in pragmatic language development.
Table 10. Sigma metrics for top 10 authors in pragmatic language development.
WoSScopus
SigmaReferenceCluster IDSigmaReferenceCluster ID
0Kasper [114]00Bates [137]1
0Bardovi-Harlig [115]00Brown [116]0
0Blum-Kulka [122]00Blum-Kulka [118]0
0Blum-Kulka [158]00Bardovi-Harlig [113]0
0Takahashi [159]00Kasper [88]0
0Felix-Brasdefer [160]00Ellis [123]0
0Bishop [89]10Schmidt [161]0
0Taguchi [162]00Achiba [163]0
0Blum-Kulka [164]00Takahashi [165]0
0House [146]00Rose [149]0
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Alduais, A.; Al-Qaderi, I.; Alfadda, H. Pragmatic Language Development: Analysis of Mapping Knowledge Domains on How Infants and Children Become Pragmatically Competent. Children 2022, 9, 1407. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091407

AMA Style

Alduais A, Al-Qaderi I, Alfadda H. Pragmatic Language Development: Analysis of Mapping Knowledge Domains on How Infants and Children Become Pragmatically Competent. Children. 2022; 9(9):1407. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091407

Chicago/Turabian Style

Alduais, Ahmed, Issa Al-Qaderi, and Hind Alfadda. 2022. "Pragmatic Language Development: Analysis of Mapping Knowledge Domains on How Infants and Children Become Pragmatically Competent" Children 9, no. 9: 1407. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091407

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop