Next Article in Journal
Fas-Fas Ligand Interplay in the Periphery of Salivary Gland Carcinomas as a New Checkpoint Predictor for Disease Severity and Immunotherapy Response
Next Article in Special Issue
Editorial of Special Issue “Crosstalk between Depression, Anxiety, and Dementia: Comorbidity in Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry”
Previous Article in Journal
Atherosclerotic Plaque Fissuration and Clinical Outcomes in Pre-Diabetics vs. Normoglycemics Patients Affected by Asymptomatic Significant Carotid Artery Stenosis at 2 Years of Follow-Up: Role of microRNAs Modulation: The ATIMIR Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Crosstalk between Existential Phenomenological Psychotherapy and Neurological Sciences in Mood and Anxiety Disorders
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Psychiatric Neural Networks and Precision Therapeutics by Machine Learning

1
Medical Affairs, Kyowa Pharmaceutical Industry Co., Ltd., Osaka 530-0005, Japan
2
Department of Biological Science, Graduate School of Science, Nagoya University, Nagoya City 464-8602, Japan
3
Interactive Group, Accenture Japan Ltd., Tokyo 108-0073, Japan
4
Laboratory of Molecular Neuroscience, Faculty of Pharmacy, Takasaki University of Health and Welfare, Gunma 370-0033, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biomedicines 2021, 9(4), 403; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines9040403
Submission received: 1 February 2021 / Revised: 28 March 2021 / Accepted: 6 April 2021 / Published: 8 April 2021

Abstract

:
Learning and environmental adaptation increase the likelihood of survival and improve the quality of life. However, it is often difficult to judge optimal behaviors in real life due to highly complex social dynamics and environment. Consequentially, many different brain regions and neuronal circuits are involved in decision-making. Many neurobiological studies on decision-making show that behaviors are chosen through coordination among multiple neural network systems, each implementing a distinct set of computational algorithms. Although these processes are commonly abnormal in neurological and psychiatric disorders, the underlying causes remain incompletely elucidated. Machine learning approaches with multidimensional data sets have the potential to not only pathologically redefine mental illnesses but also better improve therapeutic outcomes than DSM/ICD diagnoses. Furthermore, measurable endophenotypes could allow for early disease detection, prognosis, and optimal treatment regime for individuals. In this review, decision-making in real life and psychiatric disorders and the applications of machine learning in brain imaging studies on psychiatric disorders are summarized, and considerations for the future clinical translation are outlined. This review also aims to introduce clinicians, scientists, and engineers to the opportunities and challenges in bringing artificial intelligence into psychiatric practice.

1. Introduction

Living organisms have self-sustaining properties that are absent in purely physical systems. They acquire energy from food for sustenance, growth, and reproduction. Another self-sustaining ability of animals is the adaptive behavior. Behavioral strategies that improve their ability to acquire energy and produce successful offspring to help each species proliferating evolutionarily through the process of natural selection [1]. Three essential principles for biological behaviors have been proposed as materiality (an embodied brain embedded in the world), agency (action-perception closed loops and purpose), and historicity (individuality and historical contingencies) [2]. The three principles are arguably unique to life and imperative for neuroscience. We believe that these considerations will shed light on our typical approaches to address not only the conundrum of animal behaviors but also the nature of mental disorders.
The complexity of environment and social dynamics often make it difficult to identify optimal behaviors in the real world. Decision-making in this process involves many different brain areas and circuits that are exacerbated in numerous neurological and psychiatric disorders [3]. Traditional approaches have dominated the studies on optimal behaviors. Among these, a prescriptive approach addresses the question of what is the best choice for a given challenge. For instance, economists and game theorists describe how self-interested rationales should behave individually or in a group [4,5]. However, the real behaviors of animals and humans seldom follow the predictions of such prescriptive theories. Indeed, prospect theory can predict the decision-making of animals as well as humans more accurately than the prescriptive theories [6,7,8]. Likewise, humans often behave altruistically and thus deviate from the predictions of the game theory [9,10]. Recently, these traditional approaches have merged with neuroscientific theories, in which learning plays a crucial role in choosing optimal behaviors and decisions. Particularly, reinforcement learning theory presents a worthwhile framework to model how an individual’s behaviors are tuned by experience [11,12]. Neuroscientists have begun to uncover numerous core mechanisms in the brain responsible for various computational processes of learning and decision-making. Their findings are now frequently featured in the literatures in many disciplines, such as ethics [13], law [14], politics [15], marketing [16], and economic and financial decisions [17,18].
A distinct set of computational algorithms evoked through coordination among myriad brain systems are abnormal in many types of neurological and psychiatric disorders, leading to aberrant and maladaptive behaviors [19,20,21,22,23,24]. Notwithstanding, psychiatric status is still diagnosed and treated according to the experiential schemes based on symptomatic phenotypes. The definitions of many mental disorders described in the DSM and ICD manuals do not always match well with neuroscientific, psychopathological, and genetic evidences [25,26]. Thus, there is a greater desire to redefine mental illness as a discrete disease. To satisfy this aspect, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative has been launched to reconceptualize mental disorders as a dimensional approach that incorporates many different levels of data from molecular factors to social determinants and linked more precisely to interventions for a given individual [27,28,29]. This approach is more likely to be compatible with the facts that psychiatric patients comprise of clusters of psychopathological symptoms and that many symptoms are shared among different mental disorders. Machine learning approaches are well suited to achieve this goal.
Machine learning approaches for psychiatry feature statistical learning functions from multidimensional data sets to unveil general principles underlying a series of observations without definite guidance. Machine learning algorithms can be generally classified into three categories, namely supervised, unsupervised, and reinforcement learning methods [30,31]. Supervised learning models, such as support vector machines (SVM) and neural-network algorithms, are designed to predict a discrete outcome (e.g., healthy group vs. psychiatric group), or continuous outcome (e.g., psychiatric severity degrees) from the qualitative data on behaviors (e. g., questionnaire), genetics (e. g., single nucleotide polymorphisms, gene expressions), or brain function (e. g., neural activity). Unsupervised learning describes models to discover unknown statistical configurations across subjects without reference to a specific outcome. Reinforcement learning investigates how actions in one’s environment (such as treatment) change behaviors [31]. Among these algorithms, supervised learning, especially SVM, is most widely used in psychiatry to classify individuals into groups within a statistical framework. This approach has already shown promising results in neuroimaging-based psychosis prediction and treatment-response estimation [32,33,34]. With increasing digitized phenotypic data, improved computing power, and less expensive data storage, machine learning, as well as deep learning that is the artificial neural network algorithms to learn complex representations of high-dimensional data patterns such as images and language, would offer findings with important implications for the development of therapeutic interventions, leading to precision psychiatry and stratified clinical trial designs.
A main purpose of this review is to exemplify the new insights provided by recent applications of machine learning in neuroimaging and clinical studies on major psychiatric disorders. To this end, decision-making in real life and mental illness is briefly described. Next, our current knowledge of neuronal circuits or functional connectivity in major psychiatric disorders is summarized. This review also argues that machine learning is predisposed to address many challenges in the era of precision psychiatry.

2. Decision-Making in Real Life and Psychiatric Disorders

The understanding of decision-making processes helps us develop machine learning tools for precision psychiatry. Flawed decision-making has been commonly observed in major psychiatric disorders, often causing poor real-life outcomes. In many cases, flawed decision-making in mental illness results from abnormalities in fundamental neuropsychological processes, including impaired attention, reward processing, associative learning, and working memory. For instance, defects in reward and avoidance learning are reported in patients with depression. Aberrant hedonic capacity and cognitive impairment occur in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Flawed decision-making can contribute to abnormal behaviors such as nonadherence to medications or outpatient appointments, failing to exercise, or poor diet. Downstream consequences of poor decisions can lead to worsening of symptoms, reduced life satisfaction, impaired everyday functioning, relapse and rehospitalization, poor physical health, and even more tragic outcomes such as accidental death, homicide, or suicide [35].
Decision-making is a constant process in real life and takes place from when we wake up until we go to bed. Its processes are largely divided into three steps: (1) identification and depiction of all alternatives, (2) assessment of the consequences of each alternative, and (3) a comparison of the accuracy and efficiency of each of these consequences [35]. Different disciplines attempt to systematize the understanding of decision-making. For instance, consumer decision-making has long been of interest to economists. Consumers are viewed as rational decision-makers who are only concerned with self-interest. One of the most prevalent consumer decision models is the Engel-Blackwell-Miniard Model (Figure 1) [36], in which every conceptual step of decision-making is instrumental for developing machine learning algorithms to implement customized advertising tactics [37]. The model has the following decision processes: need recognition followed by a search of information, the evaluation of alternatives, purchase, and finally, post purchase reflection. These decisions can be influenced by two main factors, namely memories of previous experiences and external variables in the form of either environmental influences or individual differences. In other words, this model shows that various computational steps of decision-making can be affected by the environment, individual factors, and memory. Similarly, the environment and individual factors, including polygenic architecture and epigenetic risk elements, underlie the manifestation of psychiatric disorders [38]. The pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders is complex and not well understood. Thus, it will be interesting to illustrate mental illness with common denominators across diagnostic boundaries. One common element in severe mental illness is pervasive poor decision-making. Since innumerable combinations of computational algorithms in the brain are evoked in a flexible manner for optimal decision-making, it would be challenging to elucidate the nature of decision-making impairments in different psychiatric disorders. Therefore, econometric models are becoming valuable tools for computational psychiatry [26,39,40,41,42].

2.1. Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is a severe psychiatric disorder that affects approximately 1% of the population worldwide. It is characterized by positive symptoms (e.g., delusion, hallucination, and thought disorder), negative symptoms (e.g., apathy, poor social functioning, and emotional blunting), cognitive deficits and other psychopathological symptoms (e.g., psychomotor retardation, lack of insight, poor attention, and impulse control) [43]. These symptoms are likely linked not only to excessive dopaminergic transmissions in the mesolimbic pathway but also to the dopamine release decline in the prefrontal cortex. Although dysfunctions of N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) receptor systems and weaker prefrontal GABAergic actions are also implicated, the precise manner in which plural neurotransmitter systems interact with one another in schizophrenia remains elusive [44].
Various types of cognitive functions are impaired in schizophrenia [45]. Altered prefrontal functions might also be responsible for abnormalities in decision-making and reinforcement learning observed in patients with schizophrenia [46]. For instance, during economic decision-making challenges, schizophrenic patients incline to put less weight on potential losses compared to the healthy subjects [47], and also show impairments in feedback-based learning [48,49]. Consistent with these results, activity involved in reward prediction error in striatum and the frontal cortex is attenuated in patients with schizophrenia [50,51]. A pervasive clinical burden in schizophrenia is the high prevalence of comorbidity with substance abuse disorders. Approximately half of patients with schizophrenia exhibit a lifetime history of substance abuse disorders [52], and almost all of them are smokers [53,54]. These unusual high rates of substance-use comorbidity may be attributed to disrupted reward processing [55]. These global cognitive impairments are translated in the devastating functional toll of this disorder.

2.2. Bipolar Disorder

Bipolar disorder and schizophrenia share high levels of polygenic and pleiotropic molecular architecture [38]. In accord with this, these two disorders share similar types of impairments in cognitive domains including processing speed, attention, working memory, and executive function, although bipolar disorder usually exhibits less severe deficits [56]. These cognitive deficits bring about a substantial clinical burden in up to 60% of patients with bipolar disorder and can be observed not only in depressed, manic, and mixed episodes but also in the euthymic state. These pervasive impairment in bipolar disorder indicates that it may be a trait marker linked with genetic vulnerability [57]. In addition to cognitive dysfunctions, emotion processing is severely altered in patients with bipolar disorder. Upregulated processing of positive emotion regardless of the context is central to the manic bipolar episode [58]. Theory of mind and emotion processing are significantly disrupted in the euthymic bipolar state [59]. Thus, bipolar patients display defects in their ability to understand other emotions and intentions, with a resultant impact on everyday functioning. Moreover, euthymic bipolar patients show moderate to severe impairments in a broad range of executive functions including mental manipulation, verbal learning, abstraction, sustained attention, and response inhibition [60,61,62,63,64]. In addition to these deficits, there seems to be specific decision-making biases in bipolar patients including impulsivity and deficits in risk assessment and reward processing. The weights of these aberrant decision-making are evident in depressed bipolar patients [35].

2.3. Depression and Anxiety Disorders

Depression and anxiety disorder are characterized by disturbances in mood and emotion and feature poor concentration and negative mood states, such as sadness and anger, with high levels of their comorbidities [65,66,67]. However, they have some important differences. Anxiety is needed to improve individual’s readiness for impending danger, whereas depression might prohibit previously unsuccessful actions and enhance more reflective cognitive processes. Both depression and anxiety disorder often cause systematic biases in decision-making [67,68]. Patients with anxiety disorders are hypersensitive to threatening cues without apparent memory bias. In contrast, depressed patients show a bias to memorize negative events and ruminate excessively [69].
The symptoms of these two mood disorders have been extensively investigated, and some responsible brain regions have been identified. For instance, symptoms of depression is associated with abnormalities in frontostriatal monoamines involved in reinforcement learning [70]. Meanwhile, the brain regions responsible for anxiety disorders include the amygdala, insula, and anterior cingulate cortex [71,72]. Interestingly, the default network is overactive in patients with depression. The levels of excessive rumination and negative self-referential memory in depressive states are likely to be correlated with the default network function [73]. Indeed, deep brain stimulation in the subgenual cingulate cortex of patients with major depressive disorder produces therapeutic effects [74].
Although the neurochemical mechanism of mood disorders remains unclear, much attention has been paid to the role of changed serotonin metabolism [75]. For instance, it has been hypothesized that future reward is discounted markedly in patients with depression due to a low level of serotonin [76]. Serotonin has been proposed to be primarily involved in the inhibition of thoughts and behaviors associated with aversive outcomes, including the heuristic process of unpromising decision-making [77,78,79].

2.4. Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, featuring early-onset impairments in social cognition, poor communication abilities, restricted repetitive and stereotyped behaviors, and narrow interests that hurt the individual’s ability to function properly in school, work, and other fields of life [80]. Particularly, patients with ASD show impairments in their ability to make inferences about the intentions and beliefs of others, namely, theory of mind [81,82]. Such impaired abilities might underlie differences in socially interactive decision-making tasks between autistic patients and healthy subjects. Autism is known as a “spectrum” disorder because there is a wide range of variations in the type and severity of symptoms [83]. Under the DSM-5 criteria, patients with ASD must display symptoms from early childhood, even if those symptoms are not recognized later. They also may not be fully recognized until social demands surpass their capacity to receive the diagnosis. The earliest symptoms include the lack of attention to faces [84], imitative behaviors [85], and motor deficits [86].
In ASD, a variety of brain architectures are altered, ranging from the brain stem to the cerebellum and cerebral cortex [87,88,89,90]. In particular, the connectivity deficit in the parieto-frontal circuit involved in the mirror mechanism has been proposed to underlie some cognitive aspects of ASD [91,92]. Several brain regions affected in ASD have also been shown to be involved in decision-making. Neurobiological studies have revealed that ASD has functional abnormalities in the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, and fusiform gyrus whose brain regions are thought to constitute the “social brain” [93,94]. Especially, the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex are implicated in decision-making according to functional neuroimaging and lesion analyses [95,96,97].

3. Psychiatric Neural Networks

Brain imaging can give important insights into the underlying neural mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. For instance, the resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) analyses in humans have identified a large number of potentially important abnormal functional connections that may underlie psychiatric manifestations [98,99,100,101]. Most of MRI-based neuroimaging biomarker studies have applied the machine learning algorithm of SVM to achieve high accuracy with many features and shown that different diagnoses are related to unique patterns of functional connections [102,103,104,105].
Functional connectivity (FC) shows how brain regions are temporally coordinated and is becoming more and more used to investigate neuronal network architecture. Resting state FC has been linked with a diverse range of individual traits [106,107,108,109,110]. For instance, whole-brain FC models have revealed that patterns of functional connections across brain regions can predict cognitive abilities not only in healthy individuals but also in individuals with mental illness [111,112,113,114,115]. Intriguingly, a prediction model of working memory on letter three-back task performance using whole-brain FC shows the order of working memory impairment for major psychiatric disorders (i.e., schizophrenia > major depressive disorder > obsessive-compulsive disorder > ASD) [115]. This suggests that specific cognitive processes may be represented by the corresponding FC patterns among distributed neuronal networks. Whole-brain FC-based models also have been shown to predict psychiatric disease, including schizophrenia, ASD, and major depressive disorder, as well as individual clinical severities [105,116,117], suggesting that FC alteration is quantitatively associated with psychiatric abnormality.
MRI-based delineation of psychiatric disorders has been explored as a complement to the current symptom-based diagnoses. While a number of studies have identified numerous disease-specific functional and structural aberrations, none of them are practically used as a credible biomarker mostly because of the lack of its generalizability. Namely, the reliability of the developed classifiers has not been demonstrated with regard to the variety of population demographics and data attributes [118,119,120,121,122,123,124]. These elements include different ethnicities, sex, ages, medication profiles, scanner specifications, imaging parameters, and instructions to participants, all of which are known to affect the MRI results [108,125,126,127,128,129]. Thus, little attention has been paid to the neuroimaging-based biomarkers in neuropsychiatry until recently [130,131].
To identify a generalizable classifier, we must surmount the following two major difficulties: over-fitting and nuisance variables (NVs). First, certain situations in data and model properties bring about over-fitting problems where the model fitting to the training data can be so precise that the associated errors become artificially smaller compared with the inherent data variance [130]. Second, any machine learning algorithms used for classification is doomed to employ NVs specific to a given sample data and to falsely choose neuroimaging features that are associated with the NVs. NVs include site-specific conditions in image acquisition and properties in the sample population such as demographic attributes, treatment status, and illness severity. To abrogate the over-fitting and the effects of NVs, advanced approaches with a unique combination of machine learning algorithms across multiple imaging sites have recently identified generalizable FC classifiers correlated with specific psychiatric disorders as described below.

3.1. Functional Connectivity as ASD Classifier

The rs-fMRI studies have revealed a reliable neuroimaging-based classifier for ASD that shows the spatial distribution of the 16 FCs identified from the data at multiple sites in Japan by the machine-learning algorithm (Figure 2 and Table 1) [105]. They also have demonstrated that both a sophisticated machine learning algorithm and a large training data set are prerequisite for identifying a reliable and generalizable classifier. Concerning the hemispheric distribution of the 16 FC related-brain regions, there are significantly more regions in the right hemisphere than in the left. Concerning the functional network attributes of the 32 brain regions constituting these 16 FCs, the 13 brain regions participate in the cingulo-opercular network [105,108,132]. This ASD classifier achieves a diagnosis prediction accuracy of 85% for each individual with balanced sensitivity and specificity of 80% and 89%, respectively [105]. Intriguingly, the ASD classifier is intermediately generalizable to schizophrenia, whereas it hardly exhibits any generalizability to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and major depressive disorder (MDD). This suggests that ASD shares more intrinsic neural networks with schizophrenia than with ADHD or MDD [105]. In concordance with this, genome-level studies have found that ASD shares a high degree of polygenic risk with schizophrenia, but not with ADHD or MDD [133,134]. Accumulating evidence by clinical and behavioral studies also has shown a close relationship between ASD and schizophrenia [135,136].

3.2. Functional Connectivity as Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorder Classifier

The rs-fMRI studies also have identified a reliable classifier for schizophrenia spectrum disorder (SSD) using L1-norm regularized sparse canonical correlation analyses and sparse logistic regression (SLR) [137,138]. The machine-learning algorithms automatically selected SSD-specific FCs from about 10,000 FCs of whole-brain rs-fMRI [116]. The SSD classifier shows the distinctive 16 FCs that are distributed as interhemispheric (44%), left intra-hemispheric (25%), and right intra-hemispheric connections (31%) (Figure 2 and Table 1) [116]. The classifier differentiates SSD from healthy controls with an accuracy of 76% [116]. The 16 FCs as SSD classifier are different from the 16 FCs as ASD classifier mentioned above (Figure 2d). The weighted linear summation (WLS) of the selected FCs predicts the categorical diagnostic label for each individual. The values of WLS provide a degree of classification certainty, which can be construed as neural classification certainty for the disorders. Then, each biological dimension can be determined based on the WLS [116]. On the basis of the SSD and ASD biological dimensions, the WLS distributions of individuals with SSD and ASD display overlapping but asymmetrical patterns in the two biological dimensions. This suggests that the neuronal network of SSD is characterized by a larger diversity and that it partially shares spatial distributions with the smaller network of ASD. In accord with this, the recent genetic findings demonstrate that ASD shares a significant degree of polygenic architecture with SSD [133], and that common genetic variants explain nearly 50% of total liability to ASD and approximately 30% of total liability to SSD [139,140].

3.3. Functional Connectivity as MDD Classifier

According to the meta-analysis published results of MRI-based biomarkers in depressive disorders [141], approximately 30% of them harnessed rs-fMRI as modality, of which only one-third employed FCs among region of interests (ROIs). As most of those studies have applied the SVM algorithm to achieve high diagnostic accuracy, it remains unknown that which are the most critical FCs in depression across the whole brain. Approximately half of depressed patients are inadequately treated by available interventions, as there are no reliable guidelines to match patients to optimal treatments. This mainly derives from the heterogeneity of depression [142]. Thus, it would be important to pay attention to a specific subtype of depression in order to identify target FCs.
Melancholic major depressive disorder is a subtype of MDD that is considered to be the most drug-responsive [143,144,145,146]. The sparse machine learning algorithm identified melancholic MDD-specific 10 FCs from rs-fMRI data of 130 individuals including melancholic MDD patients and healthy controls (Figure 2 and Table 1) [117]. Importantly, this biomarker does not generalize to non-melancholic and treatment-resistant MDD, ASD, and schizophrenia. Out of 10 FCs, the top two FCs as the melancholic MDD-specific classifier includes the FCs (SN-ECN connectivity) with left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in executive control network (ECN) and right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC)/frontal eye field (FEF)/supplementary motor area (SMA) in salience network (SN), and the FCs (DMN-ECN connectivity) with left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)/inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in executive control network (ECN) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/Precuneus in default mode network (DMN). These brain regions are tightly linked with cognitive flexibility, such as reversal learning tasks, in which patients with depression often have functional impairments [147,148]. In SN-ECN connectivity, activation in IFG and DMPFC leads to empathic accuracy with compassion meditation training [149], and adolescents with depression show reduced connectivity between DMPFC and IFG during cognitive reappraisal of emotional images [150]. Priming transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies demonstrate that the DMPFC play an essential role in forming social-relevant impression, such as processing verbal emotional stimuli and face-adjective pair [151,152]. In DMN-ECN connectivity, bilateral DLPFC/IFG relate to conflict processing and attention control [153], and PCC/Precuneus are linked with anhedonic anxious arousal and depression [154]. In addition to the accumulated evidence that links the connections of DLPFC to depression [110,155,156,157,158,159,160,161], there is another evidence that supports therapeutic relevance. For instance, DLPFC is a well-known target for repetitive TMS (rTMS) therapy for treatment-resistant depression [162,163,164]. Besides, neurofeedback therapy targeting DMN-ECN connectivity has been reported to be effective [165,166,167], suggesting that DLPFC plays a causal role in manifestation of depression.

4. Machine Learning Approach to Predict Therapeutic Outcomes in Psychiatric Disorders

Psychiatric research and treatment are based on a diagnostic system exclusively dependent on human experiential terms rather than on objective biological markers. Psychiatrists use a prolonged trial-and-error process to identify the optimal medications for each patient [168,169]. Although the standard diagnostic classifications have been constructed from expert opinions and defined in DSM and ICD, they are not sufficient for judging an appropriate treatment for each patient. Modern drug treatment choices are only effective in roughly half of the patients [25,141]. This is because of the heterogeneity of psychiatric disorders and the unknown precise mechanism of action of antipsychotic drugs. The psychiatrist’s choice from the best-possible treatment options does not rely on what has caused or maintained the mental illness of a given patient. While current clinical research goal is mainly to discover novel treatment options that benefit some majority of a certain patient group, an attractive alternative research goal is to improve the choice from existing treatment options by predicting their effectiveness of individual patients. In fact, a specific drug or psychotherapy treatment has been successful in a particular clinical group and unsuccessful in another patient group labeled even with the identical diagnosis [170]. This approach might help reduce the time spent in trial-and error treatment and accompanying personal and economic burden.
Machine learning methods can offer a set of tools that are especially suited to achieve clinical predictions at the individual level. Predictive models are conceptually positioned between genetic vulnerability as an individual’s architecture and clinical symptoms as an individual’s behavioral manifestations. The exploitation of various endophenotypes has the translational potential to refine individual clinical management by early diagnosis, disease stratification, optimal choices of drug treatments and dosages, and prognosis for psychiatric care (Figure 3) [99]. Machine learning models have a long-standing focus on prediction as a metric of statistical quality and are able to predict an outcome from single observation, such as behavioral, neural, or genetic measurements of individuals [141,171]. In contrast, traditional statistical methods, such as Student’s t test, are often used in medical research to explain variance of group effects.
An observed impact evaluated to be statistically significant by a p value does not always produce a high prediction accuracy in new and independent data. A classical null-hypothesis method takes a one-step procedure. Namely, a given dataset is routinely used to yield a p value or an effect size in a single process. This result itself cannot be used to judge other data in later steps. In a two-step procedure of machine learning models, a learning algorithm is fitted on a larger amount of available data (training data) and the resulting “trained” learning model is evaluated by application to new data (test data). In a first step, structured knowledge in openly available or hospital-provided data sets can be extracted. In a second step, the resulting trained algorithms can be applied with little effort in a large number of individuals in diverse mental health contexts.

4.1. Prediction of Therapeutic Outcomes in Schizophrenia

Despite the established pharmacological treatments for schizophrenia, up to 50% of the patients develop poor disease outcomes [172,173]. Stratifying treatment through the early recognition of outcome indicators might alleviate unfavorable disease progression in these patients [174,175]. Group-level studies have discovered many potential outcome predictors, such as disease course variables, treatment adherence and response, comorbidity, and cognitive impairments [176]. It remains unclear which of these characteristics should be combined for prediction, whether these group-level findings can be used to generate significant predictions for individual patients, or how accurate these predictions might be at another sites. Furthermore, the outcome can be not merely defined by symptomatic remission, but has to include various concepts that focus on restored functioning to widely cover treatment effects [177].
Large multisite treatment databases containing prospective phenotypic data of psychiatric disorders, such as the European First Episode Schizophrenia Trial (EUFEST), enable us to develop powerful machine learning methods to reliably predict treatment outcomes. By using data from EUFEST, the pooled non-linear SVMs predicted patients’ 4-week and 52-week outcomes with cross-validated balanced accuracies (BAC) of 75.0% and 73.8%, respectively [34]. This non-linear SVMs surpassed linear SVMs, univariate and multivariate logistic regression, and decision tree ensembles. Intriguingly, the most useful predictors of poor 4-week and 52-week treatment outcome were unemployment, daytime activities, psychological distress, educational difficulties, low educational status of the patient and patient’s mother. In patients with good 52-week prognosis, haloperidol was linked with shorter adherence compared with ziprasidone, amisulpride, and olanzapine due to insufficient response and side effects. Olanzapine and amisulpride were associated with higher global assessment of functioning (GAF) scores than quetiapine [34].
Recent machine learning methods have revealed that functional striatal abnormalities (FSA) are significantly correlated with a spectrum of severity across psychiatric disorders, where dysfunction is most severe in schizophrenia, milder in bipolar disorder, and indistinguishable from individuals in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), depression, and ADHD [178]. FSA scores provide a personalized index of striatal dysfunction and distinguish individuals with schizophrenia from healthy controls with an accuracy exceeding 80% (sensitivity, 79.3%; specificity, 81.5%). FSA scores are also significantly linked with antipsychotic treatment response. Interestingly, clozapine, the only drug approved for treatment-resistant schizophrenia, is not associated with FSA scores. This may suggest that FSA can characterize treatment response across different types of antipsychotics, preferentially the types with lower Meltzer’s ratio (5-HT2A/D2 affinity ratio) [178].

4.2. Prediction of Therapeutic Outcomes in Depression

Antidepressant medication is the first option for the treatment of MDD. However, remission rates are approximately 30% to 55% after first or second medication trials and then drop with subsequent medication trials [168]. Thus, the patients experience trial and error periods with different treatments before finding the optimal one. One solution is to identify the biological predictors of response to an antidepressant. This may expedite the treatment and lead to faster relief of the symptoms.
Electroencephalography (EEG) and fMRI have been used for predicting treatment response in MDD patients [179,180,181]. Since EEG is relatively more available and cost-effective than fMRI, it is a good option for developing such treatment biomarkers. The accumulated findings suggest that EEG-derived features before treatment may predict clinical response to antidepressants [182,183]. Several EEG studies have shown that characteristics of resting-state neural oscillations, especially in alpha and theta bands, may predict the drug response [184,185,186,187,188]. For instance, posterior alpha activity is associated with response to amitriptyline and fluoxetine [189,190]; theta activity with imipramine, venlafaxine, and several selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) [188,191,192]; interhemispheric delta asymmetry with fluoxetine [193]; delta activity with paroxetine and imipramine [188,194]; delta activity in rostral anterior cingulate cortex with nortriptyline, venlafaxine, and fluoxetine [195,196]; nonlinear features of EEG signals with clomipramine, escitalopram, citalopram, bupropion, and mirtazapine [197,198,199].
Although resting-state EEG can be useful for predicting drug response, the aforementioned studies fail to address several questions that are important for translating this finding into a clinical tool. First, they use small and homogeneous sample sets. Second, most of them report low prediction accuracy. Third, their prediction accuracies are biased upward by the lack of an independent testing set. One of the most powerful methods for addressing these questions is applying machine learning techniques. Zhdanov et al. used a SVM classifier to predict EEG-derived escitalopram treatment outcome using data from a large, Canada-wide, multicenter study, the first Canadian Biomarker Integration Network in Depression (CAN-BIND-1) study [200]. This classifier identified responders with an estimated balanced accuracy of 79.2% (sensitivity, 67.3%; specificity, 91.0%) when using EEG data recorded before the treatment, whereas additional EEG data after first 2 weeks of treatment increased the accuracy to 82.4% (sensitivity, 79.2%; specificity, 85.5%). In rTMS, Hasanzadeh et al. used k-nearest neighbors (KNN) classifier to predict its EEG-derived treatment response in MDD [201]. Using beta bands, this classifier discriminated between responders and non-responders to rTMS treatment with the accuracy of 91.3% (sensitivity, 91.3%; specificity, 91.3%) when resting-state EEG data from 46 MDD patients were used.

5. Conclusions

The infusion of economic and machine learning framework into neuroscience has rapidly advanced our understanding of neural mechanisms for various cognitive processes including decision-making. Because flawed decision-making is the most prominent symptoms in numerous psychiatric disorders, it is essential for neuroscientists and psychiatrists to combine their expertise to develop more effective treatment. They also need to redefine mental illness to meet biological and pathological evidence as seen in the RDoC initiative, while DSM/ICD manuals often reflect public values, such as the definition of sexual identity disorders and the advent of internet gaming disorder [202,203], and have been frequently revised so far. Since substantial progress in major mental illness research has been made in understanding the molecular mechanisms through basic and translational approaches, including cell and animal models, those types of big data may also be helpful for reinforcing weakness of RDoC [204]. Following the growing data richness and changing research questions, machine learning or deep learning algorithms could enable clinical translation of empirically trusted prediction for individual patients in a fast, cost-effective, and practical manner. Such artificial intelligence algorithms may be particularly tuned to precision psychiatry because they can directly translate large-scale multidimensional data into clinical relevance. From a long-term and larger perspective, it is particularly challenging to verbalize mechanistic hypotheses for mental disorders at the abstraction level, ranging from molecular mechanisms to urbanization trends in society. Individual consumer decision patterns would be useful for this purpose. Ultimately, we may more effectively impact psychiatric disorders that arise from the interplay between genetic vulnerability and life experience, both of which are unique to each individual.

Author Contributions

H.K. conceived the idea and wrote the manuscript. E.W. and M.F. depicted the figures and table. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data sharing not applicable. No new data were created or analyzed in this study.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Abul K. Azad and Wei-hsuan Yu for reviewing this manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Pontzer, H.; Brown, M.H.; Raichlen, D.A.; Dunsworth, H.; Hare, B.; Walker, K.; Luke, A.; Dugas, L.R.; Durazo-Arvizu, R.; Schoeller, D.; et al. Metabolic acceleration and the evolution of human brain size and life history. Nat. Cell Biol. 2016, 533, 390–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Gomez-Marin, A.; Ghazanfar, A.A. The Life of Behavior. Neuron 2019, 104, 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Ferguson, B.R.; Gao, W.-J. PV Interneurons: Critical Regulators of E/I Balance for Prefrontal Cortex-Dependent Behavior and Psychiatric Disorders. Front. Neural Circuits 2018, 12, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Chowdhury, S.M. The attack and defense mechanisms: Perspectives from behavioral economics and game theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2019, 42, e121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Robalino, N.; Robson, A. The economic approach to ‘theory of mind’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 2224–2233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Brosnan, S.F.; Jones, O.D.; Lambeth, S.P.; Mareno, M.C.; Richardson, A.S.; Schapiro, S.J. Endowment Effects in Chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1704–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Lakshminaryanan, V.; Chen, M.K.; Santos, L.R. Endowment effect in capuchin monkeys. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 363, 3837–3844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Santos, L.R.; Hughes, K.D. Economic cognition in humans and animals: The search for core mechanisms. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009, 19, 63–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Camerer, C.F. Behavioural studies of strategic thinking in games. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 225–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Camerer, C.F. Psychology and economics. Strategizing in the brain. Science 2003, 300, 1673–1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mackintosh, N.J. Varieties of perceptual learning. Learn. Behav. 2009, 37, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Laurent, P.A. The emergence of saliency and novelty responses from Reinforcement Learning principles. Neural Netw. 2008, 21, 1493–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Liao, S.M. Neuroscience and Ethics. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 64, 82–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Jones, O.D.; Marois, R.; Farah, M.J.; Greely, H.T. Law and Neuroscience. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 17624–17630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Arciniegas, D.B.; Anderson, C.A. Toward a Neuroscience of Politics. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 84–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Appleton, K.M.; Bray, J.; Price, S.; Liebchen, G.; Jiang, N.; Mavridis, I.; Saulais, L.; Giboreau, A.; Perez-Cueto, F.J.A.; Coolen, R.; et al. A Mobile Phone App for the Provision of Personalized Food-Based Information in an Eating-Out Situation: Development and Initial Evaluation. JMIR Form. Res. 2019, 3, e12966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Frydman, C.; Camerer, C.F. The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 661–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Padoa-Schioppa, C.; Conen, K.E. Orbitofrontal Cortex: A Neural Circuit for Economic Decisions. Neuron 2017, 96, 736–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Rosenberg, A.; Patterson, J.S.; Angelaki, D.E. A computational perspective on autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 9158–9165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Dayan, P.; Niv, Y.; Seymour, B.; Daw, N.D. The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Netw. 2006, 19, 1153–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rangel, A.; Camerer, C.F.; Montague, P.R. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 545–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Lee, D.; Seo, H.; Jung, M.W. Neural Basis of Reinforcement Learning and Decision Making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 287–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Van Der Meer, M.; Kurth-Nelson, Z.; Redish, A.D. Information Processing in Decision-Making Systems. Neuroscience 2012, 18, 342–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Delgado, M.R.; Dickerson, K.C. Reward-Related Learning via Multiple Memory Systems. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Hyman, S.E. Can neuroscience be integrated into the DSM-V? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 725–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sharp, C.; Monterosso, J.; Montague, P.R. Neuroeconomics: A Bridge for Translational Research. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Kelly, J.R.; Clarke, G.; Cryan, J.F.; Dinan, T.G. Dimensional thinking in psychiatry in the era of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). Ir. J. Psychol. Med. 2017, 35, 89–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Cuthbert, B.N. The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry 2014, 13, 28–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Insel, T.; Cuthbert, B.; Garvey, M.; Heinssen, R.; Pine, D.S.; Quinn, K.; Sanislow, C.; Wang, P. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Toward a New Classification Framework for Research on Mental Disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 2010, 167, 748–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Tai, A.M.; Albuquerque, A.; Carmona, N.E.; Subramanieapillai, M.; Cha, D.S.; Sheko, M.; Lee, Y.; Mansur, R.; McIntyre, R.S. Machine learning and big data: Implications for disease modeling and therapeutic discovery in psychiatry. Artif. Intell. Med. 2019, 99, 101704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Galatzer-Levy, I.R.; Ruggles, K.V.; Chen, Z. Data Science in the Research Domain Criteria Era: Relevance of Machine Learning to the Study of Stress Pathology, Recovery, and Resilience. Chronic Stress 2018, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Koutsouleris, N.; Riecher-Rössler, A.; Meisenzahl, E.M.; Smieskova, R.; Studerus, E.; Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L.; Von Saldern, S.; Cabral, C.; Reiser, M.; Falkai, P.; et al. Detecting the Psychosis Prodrome Across High-Risk Populations Using Neuroanatomical Biomarkers. Schizophr. Bull. 2014, 41, 471–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chekroud, A.M.; Zotti, R.J.; Shehzad, Z.; Gueorguieva, R.; Johnson, M.K.; Trivedi, M.H.; Cannon, T.D.; Krystal, J.H.; Corlett, P.R. Cross-trial prediction of treatment outcome in depression: A machine learning approach. Lancet Psychiatry 2016, 3, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Koutsouleris, N.; Kahn, R.S.; Chekroud, A.M.; Leucht, S.; Falkai, P.; Wobrock, T.; Derks, E.M.; Fleischhacker, W.W.; Hasan, A. Multisite prediction of 4-week and 52-week treatment outcomes in patients with first-episode psychosis: A machine learning approach. Lancet Psychiatry 2016, 3, 935–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cáceda, R.; Nemeroff, C.B.; Harvey, P.D. Toward an Understanding of Decision Making in Severe Mental Illness. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 196–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Blackwell, R.D.; Miniard, P.W.; Engel, J.F. Consumer Behavior, 9th ed.; Harcourt College Publishers: Ft. Worth, TX, USA, 2001; p. 570. [Google Scholar]
  37. Ammerman, W. The Invisible Brand: Marketing in the Age of Automation, Big Data, and Machine Learning; McGraw-Hill Education: New York, NY, USA, 2019; p. xiv. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gandal, M.J.; Haney, J.R.; Parikshak, N.N.; Leppa, V.; Ramaswami, G.; Hartl, C.; Schork, A.J.; Appadurai, V.; Buil, A.; Werge, T.M.; et al. Shared molecular neuropathology across major psychiatric disorders parallels polygenic overlap. Science 2018, 359, 693–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Kishida, K.T.; King-Casas, B.; Montague, P.R. Neuroeconomic Approaches to Mental Disorders. Neuron 2010, 67, 543–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Hasler, G. Can the neuroeconomics revolution revolutionize psychiatry? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Montague, P.R.; Dolan, R.J.; Friston, K.J.; Dayan, P. Computational psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Morningstar, J. The Mind Within the Brain: How We Make Decisions and How Those Decisions Go Wrong. Libr. J. 2013, 138, 94. [Google Scholar]
  43. Andreasen, N.C.; Carpenter, W.T. Diagnosis and Classification of Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1993, 19, 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Lewis, D.A. Cortical circuit dysfunction and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia—implications for preemptive interventions. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 1871–1878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Barch, D.M.; Ceaser, A. Cognition in schizophrenia: Core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Weinberger, D.R.; Berman, K.F.; Zec, R.F. Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1986, 43, 114–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Heerey, E.A.; Bell-Warren, K.R.; Gold, J.M. Decision-Making Impairments in the Context of Intact Reward Sensitivity in Schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 64, 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Waltz, J.A.; Frank, M.J.; Robinson, B.M.; Gold, J.M. Selective Reinforcement Learning Deficits in Schizophrenia Support Predictions from Computational Models of Striatal-Cortical Dysfunction. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 62, 756–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Strauss, G.P.; Frank, M.J.; Waltz, J.A.; Kasanova, Z.; Herbener, E.S.; Gold, J.M. Deficits in Positive Reinforcement Learning and Uncertainty-Driven Exploration Are Associated with Distinct Aspects of Negative Symptoms in Schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 424–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Corlett, P.R.; Murray, G.K.; Honey, G.D.; Aitken, M.R.F.; Shanks, D.R.; Robbins, T.; Bullmore, E.; Dickinson, A.; Fletcher, P.C. Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: Evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain 2007, 130, 2387–2400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gradin, V.B.; Kumar, P.; Waiter, G.; Ahearn, T.; Stickle, C.; Milders, M.; Reid, I.; Hall, J.; Steele, J.D. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 2011, 134, 1751–1764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Volkow, N.D. Substance Use Disorders in Schizophrenia—Clinical Implications of Comorbidity. Schizophr. Bull. 2009, 35, 469–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Goff, D.C.; Henderson, D.C.; Amico, E. Cigarette smoking in schizophrenia: Relationship to psychopathology and med-ication side effects. Am. J. Psychiatry 1992, 149, 1189–1194. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  54. De Leon, J.; Dadvand, M.; Canuso, C.; White, A.O.; Stanilla, J.K.; Simpson, G.M. Schizophrenia and smoking: An epi-demiological survey in a state hospital. Am. J. Psychiatry 1995, 152, 453–455. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  55. Krystal, J.H.; D’Souza, D.C.; Gallinat, J.; Driesen, N.; Abi-Dargham, A.; Petrakis, I.; Heinz, A.; Pearlson, G. The vulner-ability to alcohol and substance abuse in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. Neurotox. Res. 2006, 10, 235–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Harvey, P.D.; Wingo, A.P.; Burdick, K.E.; Baldessarini, R.J. Cognition and disability in bipolar disorder: Lessons from schizophrenia research. Bipolar Disord. 2010, 12, 364–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Martino, D.J.; Strejilevich, S.A.; Scápola, M.; Igoa, A.; Marengo, E.; Ais, E.D.; Perinot, L. Heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2008, 109, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gruber, J. A Review and Synthesis of Positive Emotion and Reward Disturbance in Bipolar Disorder. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2011, 18, 356–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Samame, C.; Martino, D.J.; Strejilevich, S.A. Social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: Systematic review and me-ta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2012, 125, 266–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Robinson, L.J.; Thompson, J.M.; Gallagher, P.; Goswami, U.; Young, A.H.; Ferrier, I.N.; Moore, P.B. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2006, 93, 105–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Thompson, J.M.; Gallagher, P.; Hughes, J.H.; Watson, S.; Gray, J.M.; Ferrier, I.N.; Young, A.H. Neurocognitive im-pairment in euthymic patients with bipolar affective disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 2005, 186, 32–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Torres, I.J.; Boudreau, V.G.; Yatham, L.N. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2007, 116, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Rau, G.; Blair, K.S.; Berghorst, L.; Knopf, L.; Skup, M.; Luckenbaugh, D.A.; Pine, D.S.; Blair, R.J.; Leibenluft, E. Processing of Differentially Valued Rewards and Punishments in Youths with Bipolar Disorder or Severe Mood Dysregulation. J. Child. Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2008, 18, 185–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ernst, M.; Dickstein, D.P.; Munson, S.; Eshel, N.; Pradella, A.; Jazbec, S.; Pine, D.S.; Leibenluft, E. Reward-related pro-cesses in pediatric bipolar disorder: A pilot study. J. Affect. Disord. 2004, 82 (Suppl. 1), S89–S101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kovacs, M.; Devlin, B. Internalizing disorders in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Krueger, R.F. Internalization and Externalization and the Structure of Common Mental Disorders. Psycextra Dataset 2004, 56, 921–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Mineka, S.; Watson, D.; Clark, L.A. Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1998, 49, 377–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Paulus, M.P.; Yu, A.J. Emotion and decision-making: Affect-driven belief systems in anxiety and depression. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 476–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2000, 109, 504–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Eshel, N.; Roiser, J.P. Reward and Punishment Processing in Depression. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 68, 118–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Craske, M.G.; Rauch, S.L.; Ursano, R.; Prenoveau, J.; Pine, D.S.; Zinbarg, R.E. What is an anxiety disorder? Depress. Anxiety 2009, 26, 1066–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Hartley, C.A.; Phelps, E.A. Anxiety and Decision-Making. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 113–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Sheline, Y.I.; Barch, D.M.; Price, J.L.; Rundle, M.M.; Vaishnavi, S.N.; Snyder, A.Z.; Mintun, M.A.; Wang, S.; Coalson, R.S.; Raichle, M.E. The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 1942–1947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Mayberg, H.S.; Lozano, A.M.; Voon, V.; McNeely, H.E.; Seminowicz, D.; Hamani, C.; Schwalb, J.M.; Kennedy, S.H. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron 2005, 45, 651–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Dayan, P.; Huys, Q.J.M. Serotonin in Affective Control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 32, 95–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Doya, K. Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw. 2002, 15, 495–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Daw, N.D.; Kakade, S.; Dayan, P. Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Netw. 2002, 15, 603–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dayan, P.; Huys, Q.J. Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2008, 4, e4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Huys, Q.J.M.; Eshel, N.; O’Nions, E.; Sheridan, L.; Dayan, P.; Roiser, J.P. Bonsai Trees in Your Head: How the Pavlovian System Sculpts Goal-Directed Choices by Pruning Decision Trees. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012, 8, e1002410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Geschwind, D.H.; Levitt, P. Autism spectrum disorders: Developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007, 17, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Baron-Cohen, S.; Leslie, A.M.; Frith, U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 1985, 21, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Frith, U. Mind Blindness and the Brain in Autism. Neuron 2001, 32, 969–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Wiggins, L.D.; Rice, C.E.; Barger, B.; Soke, G.N.; Lee, L.-C.; Moody, E.; Edmondson-Pretzel, R.; Levy, S.E. DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder maximizes diagnostic sensitivity and specificity in preschool children. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2019, 54, 693–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Hadjikhani, N.; Joseph, R.M.; Snyder, J.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Abnormal activation of the social brain during face perception in autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006, 28, 441–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Nebel, M.B.; Joel, S.E.; Muschelli, J.; Barber, A.D.; Caffo, B.S.; Pekar, J.J.; Mostofsky, S.H. Disruption of functional organization within the primary motor cortex in children with autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 35, 567–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Staples, K.L.; Reid, G. Fundamental Movement Skills and Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2010, 40, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Breveglieri, R.; Galletti, C.; Gamberini, M.; Passarelli, L.; Fattori, P. Somatosensory Cells in Area PEc of Macaque Posterior Parietal Cortex. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 3679–3684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Courchesne, E. Brainstem, cerebellar and limbic neuroanatomical abnormalities in autism. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1997, 7, 269–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Frith, U. Autism: Explaining the Enigma, 2nd ed.; Blackwell Pub.: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  90. Müller, R.-A.; Kleinhans, N.; Kemmotsu, N.; Pierce, K.; Courchesne, E. Abnormal Variability and Distribution of Functional Maps in Autism: An fMRI Study of Visuomotor Learning. Am. J. Psychiatry 2003, 160, 1847–1862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Hadjikhani, N.; Joseph, R.M.; Snyder, J.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Anatomical Differences in the Mirror Neuron System and Social Cognition Network in Autism. Cereb. Cortex 2005, 16, 1276–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Williams, J.; Whiten, A.; Suddendorf, T.; Perrett, D. Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2001, 25, 287–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Baron-Cohen, S.; Ring, H.; Bullmore, E.; Wheelwright, S.; Ashwin, C.; Williams, S. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000, 24, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Brothers, L. Brain mechanisms of social cognition. J. Psychopharmacol. 1996, 10, 2–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Ernst, M.; Bolla, K.; Mouratidis, M.; Contoreggi, C.; Matochik, J.A.; Kurian, V.; Cadet, J.-L.; Kimes, A.S.; London, E.D. Decision-making in a Risk-taking Task A PET Study. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2002, 26, 682–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bar-On, R.; Tranel, D.; Denburg, N.L.; Bechara, A. Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelli-gence. Brain 2003, 126, 1790–1800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Bechara, A. The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn. 2004, 55, 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Cao, H.; Plichta, M.M.; Schafer, A.; Haddad, L.; Grimm, O.; Schneider, M.; Esslinger, C.; Kirsch, P.; Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Tost, H. Test-retest reliability of fMRI-based graph theoretical properties during working memory, emotion processing, and resting state. NeuroImage 2014, 84, 888–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Drysdale, A.T.; Grosenick, L.; Downar, J.; Dunlop, K.; Mansouri, F.; Meng, Y.; Fetcho, R.N.; Zebley, B.; Oathes, D.J.; Etkin, A.; et al. Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysio-logical subtypes of depression. Nat. Med. 2017, 23, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Sundermann, B.; Feder, S.; Wersching, H.; Teuber, A.; Schwindt, W.; Kugel, H.; Heindel, W.; Arolt, V.; Berger, K.; Pfleiderer, B. Diagnostic classification of unipolar depression based on resting-state functional connectivity MRI: Effects of generali-zation to a diverse sample. J. Neural. Transm. 2017, 124, 589–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Kupfer, D.J.; Frank, E.; Phillips, M.L. Major depressive disorder: New clinical, neurobiological, and treatment perspectives. Lancet 2012, 379, 1045–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Baker, J.T.; Holmes, A.J.; Masters, G.A.; Yeo, B.T.T.; Krienen, F.; Buckner, R.L.; Öngür, D. Disruption of Cortical Association Networks in Schizophrenia and Psychotic Bipolar Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2014, 71, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Harrison, B.J.; Soriano-Mas, C.; Pujol, J.; Ortiz, H.; Lopez-Sola, M.; Hernandez-Ribas, R.; Deus, J.; Alonso, P.; Yucel, M.; Pantelis, C.; et al. Altered corticostriatal functional connectivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 1189–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Kaiser, R.H.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Wager, T.D.; Pizzagalli, D.A. Large-Scale Network Dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: A Meta-analysis of Resting-State Functional Connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry 2015, 72, 603–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Yahata, N.; Morimoto, J.; Hashimoto, R.; Lisi, G.; Shibata, K.; Kawakubo, Y.; Kuwabara, H.; Kuroda, M.; Yamada, T.; Megumi, F.; et al. A small number of abnormal brain connections predicts adult autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Commun. 2016, 7, 11254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Shaposhnyk, V.; Villa, A.E. Reciprocal projections in hierarchically organized evolvable neural circuits affect EEG-like signals. Brain Res. 2012, 1434, 266–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Baldassarre, A.; Lewis, C.M.; Committeri, G.; Snyder, A.Z.; Romani, G.L.; Corbetta, M. Individual variability in functional connectivity predicts performance of a perceptual task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 3516–3521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Dosenbach, N.U.F.; Nardos, B.; Cohen, A.L.; Fair, D.A.; Power, J.D.; Church, J.A.; Nelson, S.M.; Wig, G.S.; Vogel, A.C.; Lessov-Schlaggar, C.N.; et al. Prediction of Individual Brain Maturity Using fMRI. Science 2010, 329, 1358–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Lewis, C.M.; Baldassarre, A.; Committeri, G.; Romani, G.L.; Corbetta, M. Learning sculpts the spontaneous activity of the resting human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 17558–17563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Seeley, W.W.; Menon, V.; Schatzberg, A.F.; Keller, J.; Glover, G.H.; Kenna, H.; Reiss, A.L.; Greicius, M.D. Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 2349–2356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Finn, E.S.; Shen, X.; Scheinost, D.; Rosenberg, M.D.; Huang, J.; Chun, M.M.; Papademetris, X.; Constable, R.T. Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 2015, 18, 1664–1671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Rosenberg, M.D.; Finn, E.S.; Scheinost, D.; Papademetris, X.; Shen, X.; Constable, R.T.; Chun, M.M. A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 2016, 19, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  113. Smith, S.M.; Nichols, T.E.; Vidaurre, D.; Winkler, A.M.; Behrens, T.E.J.; Glasser, M.F.; Ugurbil, K.; Barch, D.M.; Van Essen, D.C.; Miller, K.L. A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2015, 18, 1565–1567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Yamashita, M.; Kawato, M.; Imamizu, H. Predicting learning plateau of working memory from whole-brain intrinsic network connectivity patterns. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 7622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Yamashita, M.; Yoshihara, Y.; Hashimoto, R.; Yahata, N.; Ichikawa, N.; Sakai, Y.; Yamada, T.; Matsukawa, N.; Okada, G.; Tanaka, S.C.; et al. A prediction model of working memory across health and psychiatric disease using whole-brain functional connectivity. eLife 2018, 7, e38844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Yoshihara, Y.; Lisi, G.; Yahata, N.; Fujino, J.; Matsumoto, Y.; Miyata, J.; Sugihara, G.-I.; Urayama, S.-I.; Kubota, M.; Yamashita, M.; et al. Overlapping but Asymmetrical Relationships Between Schizophrenia and Autism Revealed by Brain Connectivity. Schizophr. Bull. 2020, 46, 1210–1218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Ichikawa, N.; Lisi, G.; Yahata, N.; Okada, G.; Takamura, M.; Hashimoto, R.I.; Yamada, T.; Yamada, M.; Suhara, T.; Moriguchi, S.; et al. Primary functional brain connections associated with melancholic major depressive disorder and modulation by antidepressants. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 3542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Ecker, C.; Marquand, A.; Mourao-Miranda, J.; Johnston, P.; Daly, E.M.; Brammer, M.J.; Maltezos, S.; Murphy, C.M.; Robertson, D.; Williams, S.C.; et al. Describing the brain in autism in five dimensions--magnetic resonance im-aging-assisted diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder using a multiparameter classification approach. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 10612–10623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Ecker, C.; Rocha-Rego, V.; Johnston, P.; Mourao-Miranda, J.; Marquand, A.; Daly, E.M.; Brammer, M.J.; Murphy, C.; Murphy, D.G. Investigating the predictive value of whole-brain structural MR scans in autism: A pattern classification approach. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 44–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Uddin, L.Q.; Menon, V.; Young, C.B.; Ryali, S.; Chen, T.; Khouzam, A.; Minshew, N.J.; Hardan, A.Y. Multivariate Searchlight Classification of Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Children and Adolescents with Autism. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 70, 833–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. Anderson, J.S.; Nielsen, J.A.; Froehlich, A.L.; DuBray, M.B.; Druzgal, T.J.; Cariello, A.N.; Cooperrider, J.R.; Zielinski, B.A.; Ravichandran, C.; Fletcher, P.T.; et al. Functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging classification of autism. Brain 2011, 134, 3742–3754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Ingalhalikar, M.; Parker, D.; Bloy, L.; Roberts, T.P.; Verma, R. Diffusion based abnormality markers of pathology: Toward learned diagnostic prediction of ASD. NeuroImage 2011, 57, 918–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Wang, H.; Chen, C.; Fushing, H. Extracting Multiscale Pattern Information of fMRI Based Functional Brain Connectivity with Application on Classification of Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e45502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Edeshpande, G.; Libero, L.E.; Sreenivasan, K.R.; Deshpande, H.D.; Kana, R.K. Identification of neural connectivity signatures of autism using machine learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  125. Tomasi, D.; Volkow, N.D. Gender differences in brain functional connectivity density. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 33, 849–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Klaassens, B.L.; Van Gorsel, H.C.; Khalili-Mahani, N.; Van Der Grond, J.; Wyman, B.T.; Whitcher, B.; Rombouts, S.A.; Van Gerven, J.M. Single-dose serotonergic stimulation shows widespread effects on functional brain connectivity. NeuroImage 2015, 122, 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Friedman, L.; Glover, G.H.; Fbirn, C. Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: Con-trolling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio (SFNR) differences. NeuroImage 2006, 33, 471–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Thulborn, K.R.; Chang, S.Y.; Shen, G.X.; Voyvodic, J.T. High-resolution echo-planar fMRI of human visual cortex at 3.0 tesla. Nmr Biomed. 1997, 10, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Van Dijk, K.R.A.; Hedden, T.; Venkataraman, A.; Evans, K.C.; Lazar, S.W.; Buckner, R.L. Intrinsic Functional Connectivity as a Tool for Human Connectomics: Theory, Properties, and Optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 2010, 103, 297–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Whelan, R.; Garavan, H. When Optimism Hurts: Inflated Predictions in Psychiatric Neuroimaging. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 75, 746–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. The IMAGEN Consortium; Whelan, R.J.; Watts, R.; Orr, C.A.; Althoff, R.R.; Artiges, E.; Banaschewski, T.; Barker, G.J.; Bokde, A.L.W.; Büchel, C.; et al. Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nat. Cell Biol. 2014, 512, 185–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Tu, P.-C.; Hsieh, J.-C.; Li, C.-T.; Bai, Y.-M.; Su, T.-P. Cortico-striatal disconnection within the cingulo-opercular network in schizophrenia revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity analysis: A resting fMRI study. NeuroImage 2012, 59, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: A genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2013, 381, 1371–1379. [Google Scholar]
  134. Geschwind, D.H.; Flint, J. Genetics and genomics of psychiatric disease. Science 2015, 349, 1489–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  135. King, B.H.; Lord, C. Is schizophrenia on the autism spectrum? Brain Res. 2011, 1380, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Pinkham, A.E.; Hopfinger, J.B.; Pelphrey, K.A.; Piven, J.; Penn, D.L. Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 2008, 99, 164–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  137. Witten, D.M.; Tibshirani, R.; Hastie, T. A penalized matrix decomposition, with applications to sparse principal compo-nents and canonical correlation analysis. Biostatistics 2009, 10, 515–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Yamashita, O.; Sato, M.-A.; Yoshioka, T.; Tong, F.; Kamitani, Y. Sparse estimation automatically selects voxels relevant for the decoding of fMRI activity patterns. NeuroImage 2008, 42, 1414–1429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  139. Gaugler, T.; Klei, L.; Sanders, S.J.; Bodea, C.A.; Goldberg, A.P.; Lee, A.B.; Mahajan, M.C.; Manaa, D.; Pawitan, Y.; Reichert, J.G.; et al. Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat. Genet. 2014, 46, 881–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  140. Sullivan, P.F.; Magnusson, C.; Reichenberg, A.; Boman, M.; Dalman, C.; Davidson, M.; Fruchter, E.; Hultman, C.M.; Lundberg, M.; Långström, N.; et al. Family History of Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder as Risk Factors for Autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2012, 69, 1099–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  141. Arbabshirani, M.R.; Plis, S.; Sui, J.; Calhoun, V.D. Single subject prediction of brain disorders in neuroimaging: Promises and pitfalls. NeuroImage 2017, 145, 137–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  142. Akil, H.; Gordon, J.; Hen, R.; Javitch, J.; Mayberg, H.; McEwen, B.; Meaney, M.J.; Nestler, E.J. Treatment resistant de-pression: A multi-scale, systems biology approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 272–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Kendler, K.S. The Diagnostic Validity of Melancholic Major Depression in a Population-Based Sample of Female Twins. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1997, 54, 299–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Sun, N.; Li, Y.; Cai, Y.; Chen, J.; Shen, Y.; Sun, J.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, J.; Wang, L.; Guo, L.; et al. A comparison of melancholic and nonmelan-cholic recurrent major depression in Han Chinese women. Depress. Anxiety 2012, 29, 4–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  145. Consortium, C. Sparse whole-genome sequencing identifies two loci for major depressive disorder. Nature 2015, 523, 588–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Hyett, M.P.; Breakspear, M.J.; Friston, K.J.; Guo, C.C.; Parker, G.B. Disrupted Effective Connectivity of Cortical Systems Supporting Attention and Interoception in Melancholia. JAMA Psychiatry 2015, 72, 350–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  147. Greening, S.G.; Finger, E.C.; Mitchell, D.G. Parsing decision making processes in prefrontal cortex: Response inhibition, overcoming learned avoidance, and reversal learning. NeuroImage 2011, 54, 1432–1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Dombrovski, A.Y.; Szanto, K.; Clark, L.; Aizenstein, H.J.; Chase, H.W.; Reynolds, C.F., 3rd; Siegle, G.J. Corticostria-tothalamic reward prediction error signals and executive control in late-life depression. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 1413–1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  149. Mascaro, J.S.; Rilling, J.K.; Negi, L.T.; Raison, C.L. Compassion meditation enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. LeWinn, K.Z.; Strigo, I.A.; Connolly, C.G.; Ho, T.C.; Tymofiyeva, O.; Sacchet, M.D.; Weng, H.Y.; Blom, E.H.; Simmons, A.N.; Yang, T.T. An exploratory examination of reappraisal success in depressed adolescents: Preliminary evidence of functional differences in cognitive control brain regions. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 240, 155–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Ferrari, C.; Lega, C.; Vernice, M.; Tamietto, M.; Mende-Siedlecki, P.; Vecchi, T.; Todorov, A.; Cattaneo, Z. The Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex Plays a Causal Role in Integrating Social Impressions from Faces and Verbal Descriptions. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 26, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Mattavelli, G.; Cattaneo, Z.; Papagno, C. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex modulates face expressions processing in a priming task. Neuropsychology 2011, 49, 992–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Jonides, J.; Nee, D. Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience 2006, 139, 181–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Engels, A.S.; Heller, W.; Spielberg, J.M.; Warren, S.L.; Sutton, B.P.; Banich, M.T.; Miller, G.A. Co-occurring anxiety influences patterns of brain activity in depression. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 10, 141–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  155. Ardila, A.; Bernal, B.; Rosselli, M. How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2016, 31, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Pascual-Leone, A.; Rubio, B.; Pallardó, F.; Catalá, M.D. Rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in drug-resistant depression. Lancet 1996, 348, 233–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Brzezicka, A.; Kamiński, J.; Kamińska, O.K.; Wołyńczyk-Gmaj, D.; Sedek, G. Frontal EEG alpha band asymmetry as a predictor of reasoning deficiency in depressed people. Cogn. Emot. 2016, 31, 868–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Mennella, R.; Patron, E.; Palomba, D. Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for the reduction of negative affect and anxiety. Behav. Res. 2017, 92, 32–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Zotev, V.; Yuan, H.; Misaki, M.; Phillips, R.; Young, K.D.; Feldner, M.T.; Bodurka, J. Correlation between amygdala BOLD activity and frontal EEG asymmetry during real-time fMRI neurofeedback training in patients with depression. NeuroImage Clin. 2016, 11, 224–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  160. Okada, G.; Okamoto, Y.; Morinobu, S.; Yamawaki, S.; Yokota, N. Attenuated Left Prefrontal Activation during a Verbal Fluency Task in Patients with Depression. Neuropsychobiology 2003, 47, 21–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Takamura, M.; Okamoto, Y.; Okada, G.; Toki, S.; Yamamoto, T.; Yamamoto, O.; Jitsuiki, H.; Yokota, N.; Tamura, T.; Kurata, A.; et al. Disrupted Brain Activation and Deactivation Pattern during Semantic Verbal Fluency Task in Patients with Major Depression. Neuropsychobiology 2016, 74, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Chen, A.C.; Oathes, D.J.; Chang, C.; Bradley, T.; Zhou, Z.-W.; Williams, L.M.; Glover, G.H.; Deisseroth, K.; Etkin, A. Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 19944–19949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  163. Cho, S.S.; Strafella, A.P. rTMS of the Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Modulates Dopamine Release in the Ipsilateral Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Orbitofrontal Cortex. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e6725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  164. Fox, M.D.; Buckner, R.L.; White, M.P.; Greicius, M.D.; Pascual-Leone, A. Efficacy of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Targets for Depression Is Related to Intrinsic Functional Connectivity with the Subgenual Cingulate. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 595–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  165. Bassett, D.S.; Khambhati, A.N. A network engineering perspective on probing and perturbing cognition with neu-rofeedback. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2017, 1396, 126–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  166. Orndorff-Plunkett, F.; Singh, F.; Aragón, O.R.; Pineda, J.A. Assessing the Effectiveness of Neurofeedback Training in the Context of Clinical and Social Neuroscience. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  167. Yamada, T.; Hashimoto, R.I.; Yahata, N.; Ichikawa, N.; Yoshihara, Y.; Okamoto, Y.; Kato, N.; Takahashi, H.; Kawato, M. Resting-State Functional Connectivity-Based Biomarkers and Functional MRI-Based Neurofeedback for Psychiatric Dis-orders: A Challenge for Developing Theranostic Biomarkers. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017, 20, 769–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Rush, A.J.; Trivedi, M.H.; Wisniewski, S.R.; Nierenberg, A.A.; Stewart, J.W.; Warden, D.; Niederehe, G.; Thase, M.E.; Lavori, P.W.; Lebowitz, B.D.; et al. Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: A STAR*D report. Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 1905–1917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  169. Arroll, B.; Macgillivray, S.; Ogston, S.; Reid, I.; Sullivan, F.; Williams, B.; Crombie, I. Efficacy and tolerability of tricyclic antidepressants and SSRIs compared with placebo for treatment of depression in primary care: A meta-analysis. Ann. Fam. Med. 2005, 3, 449–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  170. Gabrieli, J.D.; Ghosh, S.S.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. Prediction as a Humanitarian and Pragmatic Contribution from Human Cognitive Neuroscience. Neuron 2015, 85, 11–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  171. Stephan, K.; Schlagenhauf, F.; Huys, Q.; Raman, S.; Aponte, E.; Brodersen, K.; Rigoux, L.; Moran, R.; Daunizeau, J.; Dolan, R.; et al. Computational neuroimaging strategies for single patient predictions. NeuroImage 2017, 145, 180–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  172. Freyhan, F.A. Course and outcome of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 1955, 112, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  173. Häfner, H.; Der Heiden, W.A. The course of schizophrenia in the light of modern follow-up studies: The ABC and WHO studies. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 1999, 249, S14–S26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Kane, J.M.; Correll, C.U. Past and Present Progress in the Pharmacologic Treatment of Schizophrenia. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2010, 71, 1115–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  175. Mourao-Miranda, J.; Reinders, A.A.T.S.; Rocha-Rego, V.; Lappin, J.; Rondina, J.; Morgan, C.; Morgan, K.D.; Fearon, P.; Jones, P.B.; Doody, G.A.; et al. Individualized prediction of illness course at the first psychotic episode: A support vector machine MRI study. Psychol. Med. 2011, 42, 1037–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  176. Tsang, H.W.H.; Leung, A.Y.; Chung, R.C.K.; Bell, M.; Cheung, W.-M. Review on vocational predictors: A systematic review of predictors of vocational outcomes among individuals with schizophrenia: An update since 1998. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2010, 44, 495–504. [Google Scholar]
  177. Jääskeläinen, E.; Juola, P.; Hirvonen, N.; McGrath, J.J.; Saha, S.; Isohanni, M.; Veijola, J.; Miettunen, J. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Recovery in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2013, 39, 1296–1306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  178. Li, A.; Zalesky, A.; Yue, W.; Howes, O.; Yan, H.; Liu, Y.; Fan, L.; Whitaker, K.J.; Xu, K.; Rao, G.; et al. A neu-roimaging biomarker for striatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Nat. Med. 2020, 26, 558–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Patel, M.J.; Andreescu, C.; Price, J.C.; Edelman, K.L.; Reynolds, C.F., 3rd; Aizenstein, H.J. Machine learning approaches for integrating clinical and imaging features in late-life depression classification and response prediction. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 1056–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Redlich, R.; Opel, N.; Grotegerd, D.; Dohm, K.; Zaremba, D.; Bürger, C.; Münker, S.; Mühlmann, L.; Wahl, P.; Heindel, W.; et al. Prediction of Individual Response to Electroconvulsive Therapy via Machine Learning on Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data. JAMA Psychiatry 2016, 73, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  181. Wade, B.S.C.; Joshi, S.H.; Njau, S.; Leaver, A.M.; Vasavada, M.; Woods, R.P.; A Gutman, B.; Thompson, P.M.; Espinoza, R.; Narr, K.L. Effect of Electroconvulsive Therapy on Striatal Morphometry in Major Depressive Disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016, 41, 2481–2491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  182. Iosifescu, D.V. Electroencephalography-Derived Biomarkers of Antidepressant Response. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 2011, 19, 144–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Baskaran, A.; Milev, R.; McIntyre, R.S. The neurobiology of the EEG biomarker as a predictor of treatment response in depression. Neuropharmacology 2012, 63, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  184. Leuchter, A.F.; Cook, I.A.; Uijtdehaage, S.H.; Dunkin, J.; Lufkin, R.B.; Anderson-Hanley, C.; Abrams, M.; Rosenberg-Thompson, S.; O’Hara, R.; Simon, S.L.; et al. Brain structure and function and the outcomes of treatment for depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1997, 58, 22–31. [Google Scholar]
  185. Rabinoff, M.; Kitchen, C.; Cook, I.; Leuchter, A. Evaluation of Quantitative EEG by Classification and Regression Trees to Characterize Responders to Antidepressant and Placebo Treatment. Open Med. Inform. J. 2011, 5, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Bareš, M.; Brunovsky, M.; Kopecek, M.; Novak, T.; Stopkova, P.; Kožený, J.; Šóš, P.; Krajca, V.; Höschl, C. Early reduction in prefrontal theta QEEG cordance value predicts response to venlafaxine treatment in patients with resistant depressive disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 350–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Cook, I.A.; Leuchter, A.F.; Morgan, M.L.; Stubbeman, W.; Siegman, B.; Abrams, M. Changes in prefrontal activity characterize clinical response in SSRI nonresponders: A pilot study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2005, 39, 461–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Knott, V.J.; I Telner, J.; Lapierre, Y.D.; Browne, M.; Horn, E.R. Quantitative EEG in the prediction of antidepressant response to imipramine. J. Affect. Disord. 1996, 39, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  189. Bruder, G.E.; Sedoruk, J.P.; Stewart, J.W.; McGrath, P.J.; Quitkin, F.M.; Tenke, C.E. Electroencephalographic Alpha Measures Predict Therapeutic Response to a Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Antidepressant: Pre- and Post-Treatment Findings. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 1171–1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  190. Ulrich, G.; Renfordt, E.; Zeller, G.; Frick, K. Interrelation between Changes in the EEG and Psychopathology under Pharmacotherapy for Endogenous Depression. Pharmacopsychiatry 1984, 17, 178–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Iosifescu, D.V.; Greenwald, S.; Devlin, P.; Mischoulon, D.; Denninger, J.W.; Alpert, J.E.; Fava, M. Frontal EEG predictors of treatment outcome in major depressive disorder. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009, 19, 772–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Iosifescu, D.V.; Nierenberg, A.A.; Mischoulon, D.; Perlis, R.H.; Papakostas, G.I.; Ryan, J.L.; Alpert, J.E.; Fava, M. An open study of triiodothyronine augmentation of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in treatment-resistant major de-pressive disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2005, 66, 1038–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Bruder, G.E.; Stewart, J.W.; Tenke, C.E.; McGrath, P.J.; Leite, P.; Bhattacharya, N.; Quitkin, F.M. Electroencephalo-graphic and perceptual asymmetry differences between responders and nonresponders to an SSRI antidepressant. Biol. Psychiatry 2001, 49, 416–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Knott, V.; Mahoney, C.; Kennedy, S.; Evans, K. Pre-treatment EEG and it’s relationship to depression severity and par-oxetine treatment outcome. Pharmacopsychiatry 2000, 33, 201–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  195. Korb, A.S.; Hunter, A.M.; Cook, I.A.; Leuchter, A.F. Rostral anterior cingulate cortex theta current density and response to antidepressants and placebo in major depression. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 1313–1319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  196. Pizzagalli, D.; Pascual-Marqui, R.D.; Nitschke, J.B.; Oakes, T.R.; Larson, C.L.; Abercrombie, H.C.; Schaefer, S.M.; Koger, J.V.; Benca, R.M.; Davidson, R.J. Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major de-pression: Evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 2001, 158, 405–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Méndez, M.A.; Zuluaga, P.; Hornero, R.; Gómez, C.; Escudero, J.; Rodríguez-Palancas, A.; Ortiz, T.; Fernández, A. Complexity analysis of spontaneous brain activity: Effects of depression and antidepressant treatment. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 26, 636–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  198. Jaworska, N.; Wang, H.; Smith, D.M.; Blier, P.; Knott, V.; Protzner, A.B. Pre-treatment EEG signal variability is associated with treatment success in depression. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 17, 368–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Thomasson, N.; Pezard, L. Dynamical systems and depression: A framework for theoretical perspectives. Acta Biotheor. 1999, 47, 209–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  200. Zhdanov, A.; Atluri, S.; Wong, W.; Vaghei, Y.; Daskalakis, Z.J.; Blumberger, D.M.; Frey, B.N.; Giacobbe, P.; Lam, R.W.; Milev, R.; et al. Use of Machine Learning for Predicting Escitalopram Treatment Outcome From Electroencephalography Recordings in Adult Patients With Depression. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e1918377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  201. Hasanzadeh, F.; Mohebbi, M.; Rostami, R. Prediction of rTMS treatment response in major depressive disorder using machine learning techniques and nonlinear features of EEG signal. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 256, 132–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  202. Garg, G.; Elshimy, G.; Marwaha, R. Gender Dysphoria (Sexual Identity Disorders); StatPearls: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  203. Petry, N.M.; Rehbein, F.; Ko, C.-H.; O’Brien, C.P. Internet Gaming Disorder in the DSM-5. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015, 17, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  204. Ross, C.A.; Margolis, R.L. Research Domain Criteria: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Potential Alternatives for Future Psy-chiatric Research. Mol. Neuropsychiatry 2019, 5, 218–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the Consumer Decision Model. The Consumer Decision Model (also known as the Engel-Blackwell-Miniard Model) that was originally developed by Engel, Kollat, and Blackwell is modified [36]. This model comprises conceptual steps of decision making in the real world and thus could be extended to be applied for developing precision medicine in psychiatry.
Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the Consumer Decision Model. The Consumer Decision Model (also known as the Engel-Blackwell-Miniard Model) that was originally developed by Engel, Kollat, and Blackwell is modified [36]. This model comprises conceptual steps of decision making in the real world and thus could be extended to be applied for developing precision medicine in psychiatry.
Biomedicines 09 00403 g001
Figure 2. The specific FCs identified for ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers. The 70 terminal regions connected by the FCs are numbered as described in Table 1. The state of FC exhibiting the smaller (more negative) and greater (more positive) correlations than the healthy control is termed under- and over-connectivity, respectively. Specific FCs identified for (a) ASD [105], (b) SSD [116], and (c) MDD classifiers [117], are illustrated. These three classifiers are summarized in (d), where they did not overlap.
Figure 2. The specific FCs identified for ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers. The 70 terminal regions connected by the FCs are numbered as described in Table 1. The state of FC exhibiting the smaller (more negative) and greater (more positive) correlations than the healthy control is termed under- and over-connectivity, respectively. Specific FCs identified for (a) ASD [105], (b) SSD [116], and (c) MDD classifiers [117], are illustrated. These three classifiers are summarized in (d), where they did not overlap.
Biomedicines 09 00403 g002
Figure 3. Challenges for precision medicine in psychiatry by artificial intelligence. (a) Traditional psychiatric research investigates a given patient group by comparison against the healthy group, possibly creating artificial dichotomies. (b) Each psychiatric disorder has various types of the symptoms with their varying degrees. (c) The interplay between altered gene expression and neural circuit, and environment such as stress, may elicit psychiatric manifestations. Instead of relying on diagnostic category, a given patient can be classified based on biological and pathological properties (i.e. endophenotypes). (d) Artificial intelligence such as machine learning can be extended to compare observations from numerous groups in the same statistical estimation. (e) Machine learning algorithms, such as SVM, can automatically extract unknown patterns of variations in individuals simultaneously from heterogenous data labeled with traditional diagnosis. Predictive models could improve patient care by early detection, treatment choice, and prognosis.
Figure 3. Challenges for precision medicine in psychiatry by artificial intelligence. (a) Traditional psychiatric research investigates a given patient group by comparison against the healthy group, possibly creating artificial dichotomies. (b) Each psychiatric disorder has various types of the symptoms with their varying degrees. (c) The interplay between altered gene expression and neural circuit, and environment such as stress, may elicit psychiatric manifestations. Instead of relying on diagnostic category, a given patient can be classified based on biological and pathological properties (i.e. endophenotypes). (d) Artificial intelligence such as machine learning can be extended to compare observations from numerous groups in the same statistical estimation. (e) Machine learning algorithms, such as SVM, can automatically extract unknown patterns of variations in individuals simultaneously from heterogenous data labeled with traditional diagnosis. Predictive models could improve patient care by early detection, treatment choice, and prognosis.
Biomedicines 09 00403 g003
Table 1. The identified terminal regions in ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers. The terminal regions identified for ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers are numbered. See Figure 2.
Table 1. The identified terminal regions in ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers. The terminal regions identified for ASD/SSD/MDD classifiers are numbered. See Figure 2.
NoAnatomical NameNoAnatomical Name
1anterior lateral fissure36posterior sub-central ramus of the lateral fissure
2anterior ramus of the lateral fissure37calloso-marginal posterior fissure
3diagonal ramus of the lateral fissure38collateral fissure
4anterior sub-central ramus of the lateral fissure39intraparietal sulcus
5calloso-marginal anterior fissure40secondary intermediate ramus of the intraparietal sulcus
6calcarine fissure41insula
7superior postcentral intraparietal superior sulcus42paracentral lobule central sulcus
8primary intermediate ramus of the intraparietal sulcus43central sylvian sulcus
9parieto-occipital fissure44cuneal sulcus
10lobe occipital45anterior interior frontal sulcus
11central sulcus46intermediate frontal sulcus
12subcallosal sulcus47median frontal sulcus
13inferior frontal sulcus48polar frontal sulcus
14internal frontal sulcus49sulcus of the supra-marginal gyrus
15marginal frontal sulcus50posterior intra-lingual sulcus
16orbital frontal sulcus51internal occipito-temporal lateral sulcus
17superior frontal sulcus52posterior occipito-temporal lateral sulcus
18anterior intralingual sulcus53olfactory sulcus
19anterior occipito-temporal lateral sulcus54internal parietal sulcus
20median occipito-temporal lateral sulcus55transverse precentral sulcus
21occipito-polar sulcus56intermediate precentral sulcus
22orbital sulcus57median precentral sulcus
23superior parietal sulcus58superior postcentral sulcus
24interior precentral sulcus59rhinal sulcus
25marginal precentral sulcus60posterior inferior temporal sulcus
26superior precentral sulcus61superior temporal sulcus
27inferior rostral sulcus62superior terminal ascending branch of the superior temporal sulcus
28anterior inferior temporal sulcus63sub-parietal sulcus
29polar temporal sulcus64Thalamus
30anterior terminal ascending branch of the superior temporal sulcus65Amygdala
31paracentral sulcus66Accumbens
32ventricle67Caudate
33posterior lateral fissure68Pallidum
34ascending ramus of the lateral fissure69Putamen
35retro central transverse ramus of the lateral fissure70Vermis
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Komatsu, H.; Watanabe, E.; Fukuchi, M. Psychiatric Neural Networks and Precision Therapeutics by Machine Learning. Biomedicines 2021, 9, 403. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines9040403

AMA Style

Komatsu H, Watanabe E, Fukuchi M. Psychiatric Neural Networks and Precision Therapeutics by Machine Learning. Biomedicines. 2021; 9(4):403. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines9040403

Chicago/Turabian Style

Komatsu, Hidetoshi, Emi Watanabe, and Mamoru Fukuchi. 2021. "Psychiatric Neural Networks and Precision Therapeutics by Machine Learning" Biomedicines 9, no. 4: 403. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines9040403

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop