Next Article in Journal
Knowledge and Self-Assessment of Dental Injuries and Oral Health among Croatian Professional Water Polo Players: A Cross-Sectional Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Analysis of Technical–Tactical Actions in High-Level Table Tennis Players: Differences between Sexes
Previous Article in Journal
Does the Installation or the Improvement of Existing Outdoor Parks Increase Physical Activity Levels? A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Recovery Phase Nutrition and Insulin Strategies for a Collegiate Distance Runner with Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus: A Case Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Pre-Event Self-Efficacy and Sports Performance: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis

by
Marc Lochbaum
1,2,*,
Cassandra Sisneros
1,
Sydney Cooper
3 and
Peter C. Terry
4,5
1
Department of Kinesiology and Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA
2
Research Institute, Education Academy, Vytautas Magnus University, 44248 Kaunas, Lithuania
3
Honors College, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA
4
School of Psychology and Wellbeing, University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba, QLD 4350, Australia
5
Centre for Health Research, University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba, QLD 4350, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sports 2023, 11(11), 222; https://doi.org/10.3390/sports11110222
Submission received: 2 October 2023 / Revised: 31 October 2023 / Accepted: 8 November 2023 / Published: 12 November 2023

Abstract

:
The relationship between self-efficacy and performance exclusively within the sports environment is yet to be quantified. Hence, we meta-analysed this relationship by following the PRISMA guidelines. Two previous meta-analyses, five relevant databases, and Google Scholar were searched. Forty-four articles published between 1983 and 2021 met the inclusion criteria, with 55 independent samples. Comprehensive meta-analysis software version 4 was used for all meta-analytic calculations using a random-effects model to calculate the mean effect size, and a mixed-effects model was used for moderation analyses. The mean pre-event self-efficacy and performance effect size was r = 0.31 (95% CI 0.22, 0.40). For moderation analyses, notable mean differences (p values ≥ 0.08) resulted for concordance [concordant (r = 0.37), nonconcordant (r = 0.22)], sports skill [closed (r = 0.37), open (r = 0.23)], and athlete level [elite (r = 0.40), sub-elite (r = 0.28)]. The true effect prediction interval ranged from negative (i.e., self-efficacy impairing performance) to positive (self-efficacy improving performance) for all moderator variables except self-referenced vs. other-referenced performance. In conclusion, the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and performance is positive and moderate in magnitude, although with prediction intervals ranging from debilitating to facilitating performance. Researchers and practitioners should note that high athlete-rated self-efficacy might not always improve impending competitive sports performance.

1. Introduction

Ever since Bandura [1] produced his seminal publication, titled Self-efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change, self-efficacy has been a prominent motivation construct in achievement domains. The construct of self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief in their own ability to perform a specific task or achieve a specific goal, compared to the self-confidence construct, which is a broader belief in one’s overall abilities and personal qualities [1]. Across many decades of research, findings suggest that self-efficacy is a key factor in sports performance [2]. Athletes with high self-efficacy are more likely to set challenging goals, persist in the face of setbacks, and perform better under pressure [2]. In contrast, athletes with low self-efficacy are more likely to set easier goals, avoid difficult tasks, and have lower levels of motivation [2].
According to Bandura [1], self-efficacy is formed based on four main sources of information: mastery experiences (successful performance in the past), which is the most authentic and influential source of self-efficacy; vicarious experiences (observing others succeed); social persuasion (verbal encouragement or feedback from others); and control of physiological and emotional states (such as anxiety or stress). Maddux [3] subsequently introduced a fifth source of self-efficacy, referred to as imaginal experiences, or visualization. Researchers have studied the sources and effects of self-efficacy extensively in both sports [2,4,5] and physical activity [6,7] domains since the 1980s.
In the sports and physical activity domains, sufficient published studies were accumulated by the late 1990s to warrant a meta-analysis being conducted by Moritz and colleagues [8]. Their results showed a mean correlation between self-efficacy and sports performance of 0.38, based on 45 published studies in sports plus several other physical activity domains. Other meta-analyses before and after Moritz et al. [8] reported an identical [9] or near-identical [10] relationship between self-efficacy and work performance and self-efficacy and academic achievement, respectively. Moritz and her colleagues reported concordance between self-efficacy and performance measures as the strongest moderator of the efficacy–performance relationship, which accounted for 12.6% of the variance. Moritz et al. [8] also reported that differences in the self-efficacy measure (task-specific, domain-specific, single-item), performance measure (subjective, objective, self-report), nature of the task (novel, familiar), and time of assessment in relation to performance (before, after) were all significant moderators of the effect of self-efficacy on performance.
The meta-analysis by Moritz et al. [8] was published 23 years ago at the time of writing this manuscript, and many studies investigating the efficacy–performance relationship have been published during the intervening period. Moreover, in a recent self-confidence and sports performance review, Jekauc and colleagues [11] included just four studies that assessed self-efficacy. Taken together, a systematic review with meta-analysis specific to pre-event self-efficacy assessment and competitive sports is still absent from the literature, given that Moritz and colleagues included several non-sports samples and also included post-performance efficacy and Jekauc and colleagues included very few self-efficacy and sports performance samples. Such an omission represents a significant gap in the sports psychology and performance meta-analysis literature [12]. Hence, we extended the sports psychology and performance literature by conducting a systematic review using a meta-analysis of the pre-event self-efficacy and competitive sports literature. We delimited our review to studies with explicit statements, indicating that self-efficacy was assessed prior to sports competitions, either actual or contrived. We excluded studies of related constructs, such as state self-confidence [13], self-efficacy assessed post-performance, and self-efficacy assessed in relation to physical activities (e.g., vertical jump) or sports-related tests (e.g., agility run).

1.1. Moderating Variables

In addition to quantifying the overall pre-event efficacy and sports performance relationship, we examined several characteristics of the sports and the participants that have been proposed to moderate the relationship between pre-event psychological states and sports performance outcome [14]. In terms of the type of sport, the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship may be stronger in sports like diving [15], which are performed in a predictable, unchanging environment, referred to as closed-skill sports, compared to sports like soccer [16], which are performed in a dynamic, rapidly changing environment, referred to as open-skilled sports, because the direct influence of opponents on performance is absent. Second, the potential for self-efficacy to vary during the performance is greater in long-duration sports than in short-duration sports. Therefore, the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship may be stronger in short-duration sports such as sprints [17] than in long-duration sports such as the triathlon [18] because performance outcomes in short-duration sports occur in closer temporal proximity to the assessment of self-efficacy. Related to the type of sport, although not hypothesized in the literature, we coded each sports competition as actual or contrived to assess whether the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship differed between the two. From an applied practitioner perspective, it would be important for the evidence to indicate that the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship is at least as strong in actual sporting events as it is in contrived competitions.
Regarding the performance measure, it has been proposed [14] that the relationship between psychological variables, including self-efficacy, and sports performance may be stronger if the performance is self-referenced or relative to previous performances by the same athlete, such as running a personal best time, rather than other-referenced or relative to the performance of other athletes, such as the finish position. Similarly, the relationship may be stronger if the performance measure is subjective (e.g., coach-rated or self-rated) rather than objective (e.g., finish position or finish time). Although objective measures are more precise indicators of performance, subjective ratings are proposed to be more sensitive to individual fluctuations in performance [14]. In previous meta-analyses, subjective performance measures have been shown to be more closely related to some pre-competition psychological states, such as moods, than objective performance measures [19,20], although this finding was not supported in the self-confidence literature [13].
In terms of the characteristics of the athlete, Terry [14] proposed that the relative ability and fitness of those competing are influential. For example, Olympic champion sprinters will beat club sprinters simply due to their superior ability. In such scenarios, psychological factors play no, or almost no, role in determining the outcome. However, among athletes who are similar in terms of ability and fitness such as the eight finalists of an Olympic 100 m race, psychological factors play a more decisive role in determining winners and losers. It is proposed, therefore, that psychological factors, such as self-efficacy, are more closely related to performance at the elite level, where athletes tend to have relatively similar levels of ability and fitness, than at the sub-elite level, where the ability and fitness of participants may vary widely. However, this proposition was not supported in a recent meta-analysis that focused on self-confidence in sports [13], in which athlete level did not moderate the relationship with performance. Finally, the self-efficacy and performance relationship may be moderated by the sex of the athlete as reported for the self-confidence and sports performance relationship [13].

1.2. Aims of the Study

The main aim of the meta-analysis was to quantify the overall strength and robustness of the relationship (i.e., the mean standardized effect and sensitivity analysis) between pre-event self-efficacy and subsequent sports performance. Our secondary aims, insofar as the methodological details provided in the primary studies facilitated analysis, were to explore the moderating influence of the characteristics of the sports event (duration; skill type; actual, contrived), the participants involved (elite, sub-elite; male, female), and the measures used to assess sports performance (objective, subjective; other-referenced, self-referenced), and self-efficacy (concordant, non-concordant).

2. Materials and Methods

This systematic review with meta-analysis followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines [21] (see Supplement Table S1 for specifics). We performed the meta-analysis using Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein’s [22] Comprehensive Meta-Analyses (CMA) Version 4 program along with the provided interpretation suggestions [23].

2.1. Eligibility Criteria and Selection Process

The included studies met the following criteria: (a) participants engaged in a competitive sports event or contest; (b) a reported self-efficacy measure; (c) a reported measure of sports performance; (d) a statement that self-efficacy was assessed prior to the sports competition; (e) sufficient data provided for effect size calculation; and (f) original data published in a peer-reviewed scholarly journal. We excluded studies conducted in a non-sports setting such as a physical education class and excluded studies that used physical performance measures such as vertical jump height rather than a measure of sports performance. We also excluded studies that assessed self-confidence rather than self-efficacy, as indicated by the questionnaire described in the methods section of the studies. We did not inquire about missing data or clarifications. If an article was not in English, we used Google Translate https://translate.google.com/ (last accessed 6 June 2022) to assist in identifying pertinent information.

2.2. Information Sources, Search Strategy, and Search Protocol

Information sources (see Figure 1) included references from the Moritz et al. [8] and Jekauc et al. [11] meta-analyses, databases found within EBSCOhost (first search ended 15 September 2022 and second search covering 2022–2023 ended 1 September 2023), and Google Scholar (first search ended 26 September 2022 and second search covering 2022–2023 ended 1 September 2023). The specific databases within EBSCOhost were APA PsycArticles, ERIC, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsychINFO, and SPORTDiscus. Supplement Table S2 contains the records of all articles from the searches. We performed all searches year by year starting from 1977 corresponding to Bandura’s [1] publication in which the term “self-efficacy” was first used, which provided the impetus for future self-efficacy research. The following represents our searches:
  • Examined all of Moritz et al.’s [8] studies.
  • Began the EBSCOhost search.
  • Selected individual databases (e.g., PsychINFO).
  • Selected EBSCOhost advanced search.
  • Typed in the following search terms using the Boolean operator AND: self-efficacy AND sport performance AND competitive.
  • Limited EBSCOhost to scholarly peer-reviewed journals.
  • Selected page options for 50 records per page.
  • Limited search time for a specific year (e.g., 1996).
  • Repeated until all years of the search were completed.
  • Began another search, substituting athlete for competitive with the Boolean operator AND.
  • Opened Google Scholar at https://scholar.google.com (last accessed 26 September 2022)
  • Typed self-efficacy and sport performance.
  • Limited year to one (e.g., 2010).
  • Searched the first three pages of the results.
  • Repeated all searches covering 2022–2023 as time passed, completing our manuscript.
  • Examined Jekauc et al.’s [11] included studies.
  • Hand searched SPORTDiscus.

2.3. Data Collection and Items Retrieved

Based on past co-authored works [13,20,64], the first and fourth authors developed a data collection worksheet. Then, in pairs (e.g., first and second author, first and fourth author), we extracted the following information: participants’ age, total number, percent male, country, sport, participant level description (i.e., Olympic athlete, collegiate athlete); self-efficacy measure description; sports performance characteristics (e.g., time and placement, team statistics); and data available (i.e., mean scores or correlation).

2.4. Study Quality and Risk of Bias Assessments

We pre-planned to examine whether study quality (i.e., lower quality studies biased the results with a higher effect size) impacted the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship. Table 1 contains the study quality questions and rating explanations. The categories and questions stemmed from Hoy and colleagues’ [65] assessment tool and from Lochbaum and colleagues’ [13] confidence and sports performance meta-analysis. The second author rated all studies with a discussion of all ratings with the first author.
For the risk of bias across studies, the classic fail-safe n, Orwin’s fail-safe n, the funnel plot, and Duval and Tweedie’s [66] trim and fill results were examined. The classic fail-safe n statistic represents the number of null samples required to change a significant value into a non-significant value [67]. We specified the one-tailed test when we conducted the classic fail-safe n analysis. Orwin’s fail-safe n [68] is the number of potential missed studies that, when added to the actual data, would move the new correlation past a chosen threshold. We chose zero as our missed study value and 0.10 as our threshold, as this value is the lower end of a correlation with small meaningfulness. Hence, the greater the value for both fail-safe n calculations, the greater the confidence that the result is safe from publication bias. To determine whether the entered studies dispersed in a similar manner on either side of the overall effect, we examined funnel plots [69]. Full plot symmetry indicates that the retrieved studies captured the essence of all studies. Lastly, we examined Duval and Tweedie’s [66] trim and fill analysis, which is used to adjust for potential missing studies. If required, data points filled to the right increase the effect size value, whereas those filled to the left lower the effect size value.

2.5. Summary Statistics, Planned Analyses, and Certainty Assessment

The random-effects model was the summary statistic, given our studies are best thought of as a random sampling of studies in the literature [22,23] using the correlation coefficient (r) as the primary effect size parameter. Cohen’s [70] guidelines for correlation values of 0.10–0.29 as small, 0.30–0.49 as medium, and 0.50 or greater as large, defined meaningfulness. For the main pre-event self-efficacy and performance analysis, we reported the number of samples, total number of participants, mean effect size, 95% confidence and prediction intervals, two heterogeneity statistics, Tau-squared (τ2) and I-squared (I2), and publication bias statistics. We used a mixed-effects analysis for our moderator analyses. For the moderator analyses, we reported the number of samples, the mean effect size, 95% confidence and prediction intervals, and the Q total between statistics with the associated p-value. The Q total indicates the level of difference between different moderator levels. Moderator analyses with small and unequal samples are unpowered to assess for differences [71]. Though we set statistical significance at the traditional p < 0.05, our hypotheses with our research questions such as Terry’s [14] propositions predict trends rather than statistical differences. We used a random-effects meta-regression model to test the impact of the study quality scores and the percentage of male participants on the pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship.
To assess the robustness of our results, we conducted two sensitivity analyses provided in the CMA program in addition to the classic fail-safe n and Orwin’s fail-safe n, both of which provide statistics indicating robustness. First, we ran the CMA remove-one analysis to gauge each study’s impact. The remove-one analysis runs the data with all studies except the first, and then all studies except the second, and so on with the resulting plot depicting the impact of each study. Next, we ran the CMA cumulative analysis program by study publication year. The cumulative analysis run by year allowed us to determine the consistency of the self-efficacy to sports performance relationship over time using the forest plots. Lastly, we examined our results (e.g., 95% confidence and prediction intervals, risk of bias assessments, and differences between moderator groups) with the aim of assessing certainty related to our research question hypotheses.

3. Results

3.1. Study Selection, Characteristics, and Quality

Table 2 contains details of the 44 included studies, of which some used more than one sample, resulting in 55 independent samples. As reported in Figure 1, the most common reason for excluding studies was being conducted in non-competitive sports situations. The study publication years spanned from 1983 to 2021, with multiple studies from each decade; 1980s (n = 4), 1990s (n = 12), 2000s (n = 11), 2010s (n = 13), and 2020s (n = 5). In total, the studies included 5373 participants ranging from 6 to 416 participants in a sample from 12 countries in the following continents: Australasia (Australia), Europe (France, Greece, Italy, Poland, Spain, UK), Asia (Islamic Republic of Iran, Israel, and Japan), and North America (Canada, USA). Participants included children, adolescents, and adults. Of samples reporting male and female composition, 35 were at least greater than 50% male participants. Studies reported on both individual sports athletes (e.g., golf, running, and tennis) and team sports athletes (e.g., basketball, soccer, and ice hockey).
Regarding our study quality score (see Figure 2 for details), the mean score (possible range of 9 to 27 points) was 17.63 (SD = 2.07) for our 55 samples with a median of 17 and a range from 13 points to 21 points. We examined the sample study quality with meta-regression (see Supplement Figure S1). The meta-regression R2 was 0.00 (computed value −0.06), indicating that no variance in the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship was explained by the study quality ratings. In Figure 2, the green circles represent number 3, the yellow circles represent number 2, and the red circles represent number 1.

3.2. Individual Study Data, Synthesis of Results, and Risk of Bias across Studies

Figure 3 contains individual data for all studies and the corresponding forest plot. The mean pre-event self-efficacy to sports performance effect size relationship was 0.31 with a 95% confidence interval of 0.22 to 0.40. The mean effect size, with criterion alpha of 0.05, was different from zero with a Z-value = 6.16, p < 0.001. The Q-test for heterogeneity, the test that analysed the studies (samples) that shared the same common effect size, was not supported (Q-value = 1491.58 with 54 degrees of freedom, p < 0.001). The true effect prediction interval was −0.39 to 0.79 (τ2 = 0.13, I2 = 96%), suggesting that 95% of comparable studies would fall within this interval.
Concerning the risk of bias analyses, the classic fail-safe n (1-tail, n = 21,164) and Orwin’s n (n = 185) statistics both indicated that the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship evident in our included studies would require a large number of missed or future studies reporting no relationship for our effect size to be reduced to either non-significant or a small (r of 0.10) value. As presented in Figure 4, some bias existed in the data with the trim being 10 samples to the right using the random-effects model. This suggests that the published literature is an underrepresentation of the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance (r = 0.38, 95% CI 0.29, 0.45). Last, while checking bias, we examined if the study quality biased our mean effect size with meta-regression. The meta-regression result indicated that the total study quality score had no meaningful relationship on the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship (R2 = −0.04, coefficient = 0.00, SE = 0.00, 95% CI −0.00, 0.00, Z = 0.79, p > 0.05).

3.3. Additional Sensitivity Analyses

Figure 5 depicts the CMA remove-one analysis that gauges the impact of each included study. The individual point estimates appear very consistent (ranging from 0.30 to 0.32) and hence no one study unduly impacted the results. The forest plot is a visual representation of the consistency of the results. Figure 6 represents the CMA cumulative analysis program by year. Starting in 1983 until 2021, the cumulative analysis suggests some initial variation in the consistency of the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship. Since 2011, the relationship appears to have been very consistent in both point estimates and confidence limits.

3.4. Moderator Analyses

Table 3 shows the moderator codes used in our analyses and the results of the moderator analyses are shown in Table 4. Though nonsignificant at the traditional p < 0.05, examining the 95% confidence intervals and prediction intervals suggests that pre-event self-efficacy is more predictive (with r difference > 0.10) of sports performance in closed skill sports, with subjective performance measures, self-referenced performance measures, questionnaire and performance concordance, and elite athletes.
We examined sample sex make-up using meta-regression (see Supplementary Figure S2). The meta-regression R2 was 0.00 (computed value −0.04), indicating that no variance in the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship was explained by the sex of the study participants.

3.5. Certainty of Evidence

Some caution is needed to best interpret and provide certainty of evidence ratings, given the observed prediction interval values ranging from negative to positive. It is important to remember that the prediction interval is a range or bandwidth of plausible values that can include the true effect. Hence, we are certain that the overall pre-event self-efficacy and performance relationship is, at best, medium in magnitude, while it is plausible that in some cases the relationship may not support our mean effect size. For instance, new studies with objective performance in sports greater than 10 min in duration may result in a negative and large-magnitude relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and performance. This, of course, is speculative. Concerning our moderators, the evidence suggests with some certainty that measurement concordance, athlete level, and sports skill matter when investigating the self-efficacy and sports performance relationship. Last, of all the data points, certainty is the highest that self-rated performance will result in a positive, small relationship.

4. Discussion

The present study was a systematic review with meta-analysis of the published literature on the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship, which we conducted to fill a significant gap in the literature, given that two previous meta-analyses ostensibly on a very similar subject were either published more than 20 years ago and included non-sports studies [8] or included only a small fraction of primary studies that investigated pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance [11]. We distinguished the construct of self-efficacy from self-confidence based on the specific terminology used in our reviewed studies and also by cross-referencing our included studies against published self-confidence and performance meta-analyses to ensure that there was no overlap [13,72,73]. Overall, our results showed a robust, medium-sized effect of pre-event self-efficacy on sports performance, with the most important moderators being measurement concordance, type of sports skill, performance reference, and athlete level. Our findings mirrored those of previous meta-analyses on sports [8], work [9], and academic [10] domains. Our review addressed Borenstein and colleagues’ [22,23] concerns that cast new speculation on the certainty of our findings and those of past self-efficacy and performance reviews.

4.1. Moderator Effects

Considering the moderating variables individually, it is apparent that the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship is stronger in closed-skill sports than in open-skill sports. The relationship in closed-skill sports such as triathlon [38,47] and diving [15] was often large in magnitude, whereas in open-skill sports such as ice hockey [42] and cricket [60], the relationship was typically minimal to moderate. As Terry [14] explained, when the direct influence of opponents on performance is minimized or eliminated, the true influence of psychological variables such as self-efficacy on performance is more readily identified.
Similarly to the measure of performance, the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance tends to be stronger when performance is assessed subjectively by self or coach rating [30,63] rather than assessed by win/loss records or objective scores [41,46]. This can be explained by the fact that objective measures do not always capture the quality of performance of the individual concerned [14]. For example, an athlete may perform exceptionally well but ultimately lose to a more talented opponent, which would be rated objectively as a loss but subjectively as a good performance.
The notion of concordance between self-efficacy and performance measures was also shown to be an important moderator. For example, studies in which the self-efficacy of achieving a personal goal, such as finishing a triathlon [18] or a marathon [57] at a particular time, was correlated against actual finish time were highly concordant. Such studies tended to produce larger effects than studies where the self-efficacy measure and the performance measure were nonconcordant, such as where participants rated their self-efficacy for sports in general but performance was assessed in, for example, specific boccia competitions [49].
The elite versus the non-elite status of the athlete was another variable shown to be an important moderator of the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship. The relationship reported tended to be stronger in studies of elite athletes [15,30] than in studies that used nonelite athletes as participants [40,60]. Elite athletes tend to display relative homogeneity of skill and fitness (i.e., they are all highly skilled and well-conditioned), whereas non-elite athletes tend to display a greater range of skill and fitness levels. In elite competitions, where important physical determinants of performance are evened out, psychological factors such as pre-event self-efficacy come to the fore, whereas at lower levels, the influence of psychological variables is masked by the relative heterogeneity of physical determinants of performance [14].
Contrary to the proposed moderating influence of sports duration [14], the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance was somewhat stronger for longer duration (>10 min.) than shorter duration (<10 min.) sports. Terry proposed that pre-event psychological states, including self-efficacy, would be more closely related to performance in short-duration sports than in long-duration sports because the scope for psychological states to change after the contest had started would be greater if the sport lasted longer. Our findings indicate that this proposition does not hold true for self-efficacy, whereas it was supported in a meta-analysis of the literature on self-confidence and sports performance [13].
The relationship via meta-regression between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance was shown to be unimportant for male and female sample make-ups. This finding does not align with the results of a meta-analysis summarizing the self-confidence and sports performance literature [13], which showed a stronger relationship for men than women. There is no obvious explanation for this inconsistency, which suggests that additional studies exploring sex and gender differences in self-efficacy and sports performance relationships are warranted.
It should be noted that in all instances, the trends described above did not reach statistical significance (the closest being for concordance at p = 0.08); therefore, statistical purists might argue that none of the variables we examined truly moderated the strength of the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance. However, in the words of Andersen and Stoove [74], “the sanctity of p < 0.05 obfuscates good stuff”, and we contend that moderation effects of the type of sports and performance measure, level of athlete, and especially concordance between the self-efficacy and performance measures should be given due consideration by researchers when designing future studies.

4.2. Strengths, Limitations, Future Directions, and Practical Applications

A strength of our meta-analysis was the inclusion of 44 investigations of relationships specifically between pre-event self-efficacy and subsequent sports performance, spanning over five decades from the 1980s to the 2020s. Unlike previous related meta-analyses [8,11], we restricted our inclusion criteria to studies of sports events, excluding studies of physical education or fitness tasks. We focused on the pre-event assessment of self-efficacy and excluded post-event assessment, and we retained conceptual clarity by only including studies measuring self-efficacy rather than also including studies of self-confidence. A second strength was our comprehensive use of meta-analysis statistics, which included sensitivity analyses and the introduction of prediction interval statistics to this area of the literature. A third strength of our meta-analysis involved the assessment of moderators, the results of which will help guide future researchers and practitioners. As an example, moderator analyses showed that using a self-referenced performance criterion rather than an objective measure is more likely to reveal a positive relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance. Also, investigations in closed-skill sports and with elite athletes are more likely to reveal a positive relationship between self-efficacy and sports performance than those conducted in open-skill sports and with sub-elite athletes.
Limitations in the pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance literature result from non-uniform measures and different styles of self-efficacy measurement, vague reporting of pertinent details, and extensive use of convenience samples. As discussed in this review and other similar meta-analyses with pre-event measures [13,19,20], these issues are common. Overcoming such limitations would require a uniform reporting system to be adopted by all the journals. Even if such a lofty ambition is achieved, previously published literature cannot be changed, so these limitations are inevitable for the meta-analyst.
To conduct our systematic review with meta-analysis, we followed the PRISMA statement [21], which provides a step-by-step methodology with many specific elements. Despite following this structured approach, there are limitations in formulating and conducting such reviews. For instance, even with a thorough search of several relevant sources, the number of missed studies is unknown. Also, we attempted to include non-English studies to best represent the totality of the literature. Our search was conducted in English; thus, non-English studies of relevance were harvested typically where either a title, abstract, or keywords were written in English. To extract the relevant details of methodology and results, we then applied Google Translate, which we acknowledge may provide less than 100% accurate translation. However, although the use of Google Translate may be a potential limitation, this was balanced by our inclusion of studies without a language restriction, which can be seen as a methodological strength.
Another potential limitation relates to our categorization of athletes as elite or sub-elite. Although we applied the coding systems developed by Kyllo and Landers [75] and Swann et al. [76] to categorize athlete samples, we encountered great variation across studies in the terminology used to describe athletes of different ages and levels of competition. Finally, the risk of bias and overall quality of the study scores that we attributed to the studies may also represent a potential limitation. Locating all pertinent information in the primary studies was a painstaking and, at times, difficult process due to inconsistent reporting of methods and results. Few studies used random sampling procedures, and although we have confidence in our findings, we acknowledge that they are based on a body of literature that is generally limited in overall quality.
In terms of future directions in this area of research, we see much scope to improve the overall quality of self-efficacy and sports performance studies. With this aim in mind, we recommend more repeated-measures designs and cross-lagged designs that attempt to address issues of causality related to whether high self-efficacy improves performance, whether good performance improves self-efficacy, or whether a virtuous circle is evident whereby high self-efficacy leads to better performance, which further enhances self-efficacy, and so on. We also recommend a more random selection of participants and better reporting of details involving large data collections (e.g., at marathon events), including the number of people approached and the number who accepted. Another productive line of enquiry would be to focus on one or more moderator variables. For example, collecting both self and other referenced performance statistics and/or assessing both elite and non-elite participants in the same event. Large-scale events with both elite and non-elite competitors, such as marathons or triathlons, offer suitable environments for such research.
The key practical application of the findings is that the relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance is of sufficient strength and robustness to warrant athletes in all sports and at all levels, but especially those in closed-skill sports at an elite level, devoting time to activities specifically designed to enhance self-efficacy. There are many sports psychology self-help books [77,78,79,80] that include activities and exercises to promote self-efficacy that athletes, coaches, and sports psychology practitioners may find beneficial.

5. Conclusions

The relationship between pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance is medium in size and robust from 1983 to 2021 across a wide variety of sports and athletes at all levels. The pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance relationship varied little over time, and no one study impacted the relationship unduly. For researchers investigating pre-event self-efficacy as a predictor of sports performance, considering the concordance between the self-efficacy measure and sports performance measure is important, as are other moderator variables common to the self-efficacy and sports performance literature. Practitioners should be aware that higher pre-event self-efficacy does not always result in better sports performance. However, evidence suggests that pre-event self-efficacy has a meaningful impact on sports performance and hence coaches, sports psychology practitioners, and athletes themselves should work at maximizing athlete self-belief to perform and thus achieve in competitive sports contests.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/sports11110222/s1, Supplementary Table S1. PRISMA Checklist, Supplementary Figure S1. Study quality meta-regression, Supplementary Figure S2. Percent male meta-regression, and Supplementary Table S2. Search strategy.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L. and P.C.T.; methodology, M.L., C.S., S.C. and P.C.T. software use, M.L. and C.S.; formal analysis, M.L. and C.S.; data curation, M.L., C.S., S.C. and P.C.T.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L., P.C.T. and C.S.; writing—review and editing, M.L., C.S. and P.C.T.; supervision, M.L. and P.C.T.; project administration, M.L.; funding acquisition, M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The Texas Tech University TrUE SPARK program provided funding for C.S. and S.C.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable for studies not involving humans or animals.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable for studies not involving humans.

Data Availability Statement

All data are contained in the article tables.

Acknowledgments

The research team wishes to acknowledge the Department of Kinesiology and Sport Management for supporting the research by purchasing the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funder for C.S. and S.C. had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Review Registration

Review not registered.

References

  1. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Feltz, D.L.; Short, S.E.; Sullivan, P.J. Self-Efficacy in Sport. Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  3. Maddux, J.E. Self-efficacy theory: An introduction. In Self-Efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory, Research, and Application; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 3–33. [Google Scholar]
  4. Short, S.; Ross-Stewart, L. A review of self-efficacy based interventions. In Advances in Applied Sport Psychology; Mellalieu, S.D., Manton, S., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 231–290. [Google Scholar]
  5. Wurtele, S.K. Self-efficacy and athletic performance: A review. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1986, 4, 290–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. McAuley, E.; Szabo, A.; Gothe, N.; Olson, E.A. Self-efficacy: Implications for physical activity, function, and functional limitations in older adults. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 2011, 5, 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Samson, A.; Solmon, M. Examining the sources of self-efficacy for physical activity within the sport and exercise domains. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011, 4, 70–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Moritz, S.E.; Feltz, D.L.; Fahrbach, K.R.; Mack, D.E. The relation of self-efficacy measures to sport performance: A meta-analytic review. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2000, 71, 280–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Stajkovic, A.D.; Luthans, F. Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 124, 240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Talsma, K.; Schüz, B.; Schwarzer, R.; Norris, K. I believe, therefore I achieve (and vice versa): A meta-analytic cross-lagged panel analysis of self-efficacy and academic performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018, 61, 136–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Jekauc, D.; Fiedler, J.; Wunsch, K.; Mülberger, L.; Burkart, D.; Kilgus, A.; Fritsch, J. The effect of self-confidence on performance in sports: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2023, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lochbaum, M.; Stoner, E.; Hefner, T.; Cooper, S.; Lane, A.M.; Terry, P.C. Sport psychology and performance meta-analyses: A systematic review of the literature. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0263408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lochbaum, M.; Sherburn, M.; Sisneros, C.; Cooper, S.; Lane, A.M.; Terry, P.C. Revisiting the self-confidence and sport performance relationship: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 6381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Terry, P. The efficacy of mood state profiling with elite performers: A review and synthesis. Sport Psychol. 1995, 9, 309–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Slobounov, S.; Yukelson, D.; O’Brien, R. Self-efficacy and movement variability of Olympic-level springboard divers. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 1997, 9, 171–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lowther, J.; Lane, A.; Lane, H. Self-efficacy and psychological skills during the amputee soccer world cup. Athl. Insight 2002, 4, 23–34. [Google Scholar]
  17. Gernigon, C.; Delloye, J.-B. Self-efficacy, causal attribution, and track athletic performance following unexpected success or failure among elite sprinters. Sport Psychol. 2003, 17, 55–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bueno García, J.; Capdevila Ortís, L.; Fernández-Castro, J. Sufrimiento competitivo y rendimiento en deportes de resistencia. Rev. De Psicol. Del Deporte 2002, 11, 209–226. [Google Scholar]
  19. Beedie, C.J.; Terry, P.C.; Lane, A.M. The profile of mood states and athletic performance: Two meta-analyses. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2000, 12, 49–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lochbaum, M.; Zanatta, T.; Kirschling, D.; May, E. The Profile of Moods States and athletic performance: A meta-analysis of published studies. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2021, 11, 50–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int. J. Surg. 2021, 88, 105906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.E.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Rothstein, H.R. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software Version 4; Biostat, Inc.: Englewood, NJ, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  23. Borenstein, M. Common Mistakes in Meta-Analysis and How to Avoid Them; Biostat, Inc.: Englewood, NJ, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  24. Aikawa, S.; Takai, H. Relationship between imagery ability, performance, and variables related to performance. Sport Psychol. 2021, 35, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Arruza, J.A.; Telletxea, S.; Gil de Montes, L.; Arribas, S.; Balagué, G.; Cecchini, J.A.; Brustad, R.J. Understanding the relationship between perceived development of the competition plan and sport performance: Mediating effects of self-efficacy and state depression. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2009, 109, 304–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Avugos, S.; Bar-Eli, M.; Ritov, I.; Sher, E. The elusive reality of efficacy–performance cycles in basketball shooting: An analysis of players’ performance under invariant conditions. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 11, 184–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Baretta, D.; Greco, A.; Steca, P. Understanding performance in risky sport: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and sensation seeking in competitive freediving. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 117, 161–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Beauchamp, M.R.; Bray, S.R.; Albinson, J.G. Pre-competition imagery, self-efficacy and performance in collegiate golfers. J. Sports Sci. 2002, 20, 697–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Beauchamp, M.R.; Whinton, L.C. Self-efficacy and other-efficacy in dyadic performance: Riding as one in equestrian eventing. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2005, 27, 245–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Besharat, M.A.; Pourbohlool, S. Moderating effects of self-confidence and sport self-efficacy on the relationship between competitive anxiety and sport performance. Psychology 2011, 2, 760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bezjak, J.E.; Langga-Sharifi, E. Validation of the Physical Fitness Opinion Questionnaire against marathon performance. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1991, 73, 993–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Blecharz, J.; Horodyska, K.; Zarychta, K.; Adamiec, A.; Luszczynska, A. Intrinsic motivation predicting performance satisfaction in athletes: Further psychometric evaluations of the Sport Motivation Scale-6. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 46, 309–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Blecharz, J.; Luszczynska, A.; Tenenbaum, G.; Scholz, U.; Cieslak, R. Self-efficacy moderates but collective efficacy mediates between motivational climate and athletes’ well-being. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2014, 6, 280–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Blecharz, J.; Luszczynska, A.; Scholz, U.; Schwarzer, R.; Siekanska, M.; Cieslak, R. Predicting performance and performance satisfaction: Mindfulness and beliefs about the ability to deal with social barriers in sport. Anxiety Stress Coping 2014, 27, 270–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Brace, A.W.; George, K.; Lovell, G.P. Mental toughness and self-efficacy of elite ultra-marathon runners. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0241284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bray, S.R.; Balaguer, I.; Duda, J.L. The relationship of task self-efficacy and role efficacy beliefs to role performance in Spanish youth soccer. J. Sports Sci. 2004, 22, 429–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bueno, J.; Weinberg, R.S.; Fernández-Castro, J.; Capdevila, L. Emotional and motivational mechanisms mediating the influence of goal setting on endurance athletes’ performance. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2008, 9, 786–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Burke, S.T.; Jin, P. Predicting performance from a triathlon event. J. Sport Behav. 1996, 19, 272. [Google Scholar]
  39. Coudevylle, G.R.; Ginis, K.A.M.; Famose, J.-P. Determinants of self-handicapping strategies in sport and their effects on athletic performance. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2008, 36, 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Donovan, J.J.; Williams, K.J. Missing the mark: Effects of time and causal attributions on goal revision in response to goal-performance discrepancies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Estevan, I.; Álvarez, O.; Castillo, I. Autoeficacia percibida y rendimiento técnico-táctico en taekwondistas universitarios. Cuad. De Psicol. Del Deporte 2016, 16, 51–64. [Google Scholar]
  42. Feltz, D.L.; Lirgg, C.D. Perceived team and player efficacy in hockey. J. Appl. Psychol. 1998, 83, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. George, T.R. Self-confidence and baseball performance: A causal examination of self-efficacy theory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1994, 16, 381–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Haney, C.J.; Long, B.C. Coping effectiveness: A path analysis of self-efficacy, control, coping, and performance in sport competitions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 25, 1726–1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hatzigeorgiadis, A.; Zourbanos, N.; Goltsios, C.; Theodorakis, Y. Investigating the functions of self-talk: The effects of motivational self-talk on self-efficacy and performance in young tennis players. Sport Psychol. 2008, 22, 458–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Hayslip, B., Jr.; Petrie, T.A.; MacIntire, M.M.; Jones, G.M. The influences of skill level, anxiety, and psychological skills use on amateur golfers’ performances. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2010, 22, 123–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Heazlewood, I.; Burke, S. Self-efficacy and its relationship to selected sport psychological constructs in the prediction of performance in ironman triathlon. J. Human Sport Exerc. 2011, 6, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kane, T.D.; Marks, M.A.; Zaccaro, S.J.; Blair, V. Self-efficacy, personal goals, and wrestlers’ self-regulation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1996, 18, 36–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Koper, M.; Nadolska, A.; Urbański, P.; Wilski, M. Relationship between pre-competition mental state and sport result of disabled boccia athletes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Laguardia, R.; Labbé, E.E. Self-efficacy and anxiety and their relationship to training and race performance. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1993, 77, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Lee, C. The relationship between goal setting, self-efficacy, and female field hockey team performance. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1989, 20, 147–161. [Google Scholar]
  52. Martin, J.J.; Gill, D.L. The relationships among competitive orientation, sport-confidence, self-efficacy, anxiety, and performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1991, 13, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Martin, J.J.; Gill, D.L. The relationships of competitive orientations and self-efficacy to goal importance, thoughts, and performance in high school distance runners. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 1995, 7, 50–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Martin, J.J. Training and performance self-efficacy, affect, and performance in wheelchair road racers. Sport Psychol. 2002, 16, 384–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. McAuley, E.; Gill, D. Reliability and validity of the physical self-efficacy scale in a competitive sport setting. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1983, 5, 410–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Nicholls, A.R.; Polman, R.; Levy, A.R. Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performance among athletes. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2010, 10, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Okwumabua, T.M. Psychological and physical contributions to marathon performance: An exploratory investigation. J. Sport Behav. 1985, 8, 163. [Google Scholar]
  58. Treasure, D.C.; Monson, J.; Lox, C.L. Relationship between self-efficacy, wrestling performance, and affect prior to competition. Sport Psychol. 1996, 10, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Turner, M.J.; Jones, M.V.; Sheffield, D.; Slater, M.J.; Barker, J.B.; Bell, J.J. Who thrives under pressure? Predicting the performance of elite academy cricketers using the cardiovascular indicators of challenge and threat states. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 35, 387–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Turner, M.J.; Massie, R.; Slater, M.J.; Braithwaite, E. Do challenge and threat evaluations predict netball performance and selection at trials in youth netball players? Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2021, 10, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Watkins, B.; Garcia, A.W.; Turek, E. The relation between self-efficacy and sport performance: Evidence from a sample of youth baseball players. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 1994, 6, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Weiss, M.R.; Wiese, D.M.; Klint, K.A. Head over heels with success: The relationship between self-efficacy and performance in competitive youth gymnastics. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1989, 11, 444–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Yang, Z. An empirical analysis of the relationship between self-efficacy, motivation, and team performance of high-level basketball players. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 2020, 29, 221–231. [Google Scholar]
  64. Lochbaum, M.; Cooper, S.; Limp, S. The Athletic Identity Measurement Scale: A systematic review with meta-analysis from 1993 to 2021. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hoy, D.; Brooks, P.; Woolf, A.; Blyth, F.; March, L.; Bain, C.; Baker, P.; Smith, E.; Buchbinder, R. Assessing risk of bias in prevalence studies: Modification of an existing tool and evidence of interrater agreement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2012, 65, 934–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Duval, S.; Tweedie, R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000, 56, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Rosenthal, R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 1979, 86, 638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Orwin, R.G. A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. J. Educ. Stat. 1983, 8, 157–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Egger, M.; Smith, G.D.; Schneider, M.; Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997, 315, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Cohen, J. Things I have learned (so far). Am. Psychol. Assoc. 1990, 45, 1304–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hagger, M. Meta-analysis. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2022, 15, 120–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Craft, L.L.; Magyar, T.M.; Becker, B.J.; Feltz, D.L. The relationship between the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 and sport performance: A meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2003, 25, 44–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Woodman, T.; Hardy, L. The relative impact of cognitive anxiety and self-confidence upon sport performance: A meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci. 2003, 21, 443–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Andersen, M.B.; Stoove, M.A. The sanctity of p < 0.05 obfuscates good stuff: A comment on Kerr and Goss. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 1998, 10, 168–173. [Google Scholar]
  75. Kyllo, B.L.; Landers, D.M. Goal setting in sport and exercise: A research synthesis to resolve the controversy. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1995, 17, 117–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Swann, C.; Moran, A.; Piggott, D. Defining elite athletes: Issues in the study of expert performance in sport psychology. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2015, 16, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Afremow, J. The Champion’s Mind: How Great Athletes Think, Train, and Thrive; Tantor Audio: Old Saybrook, CN, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  78. Collins, D.; Cruikshank, A. Sport Psychology Essentials; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  79. Karageorghis, C.I.; Terry, P.C. Inside Sport Psychology; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  80. Vernon, A. Mind Games; Bloomsbury Sport: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart for the identification of the included studies [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart for the identification of the included studies [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Sports 11 00222 g001
Figure 2. Risk of bias in included study data collection and participant selection methods. Categories explained: 1 = sample, 2 = recruitment, 3 = nonresponse, 4 = collection method, 5 = performance measure, 6 = setting, 7 = collection setting, and 8 = anonymity/confidentiality assured. Colors explained: green = 3, yellow = 2, and red = 1. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Figure 2. Risk of bias in included study data collection and participant selection methods. Categories explained: 1 = sample, 2 = recruitment, 3 = nonresponse, 4 = collection method, 5 = performance measure, 6 = setting, 7 = collection setting, and 8 = anonymity/confidentiality assured. Colors explained: green = 3, yellow = 2, and red = 1. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Sports 11 00222 g002aSports 11 00222 g002b
Figure 3. Sample effect size statistics expressed as correlations (r) and corresponding forest plots. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Figure 3. Sample effect size statistics expressed as correlations (r) and corresponding forest plots. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Sports 11 00222 g003
Figure 4. Random effects plot trimmed and filled. The open circles are the data points, and the filled circles are the result of the trim and fill analysis. The clear rhombus is the mean effect size, and the filled rhombus is the trim and filled mean effect size.
Figure 4. Random effects plot trimmed and filled. The open circles are the data points, and the filled circles are the result of the trim and fill analysis. The clear rhombus is the mean effect size, and the filled rhombus is the trim and filled mean effect size.
Sports 11 00222 g004
Figure 5. Random-effects model statistics and forest plots, with one study removed. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Figure 5. Random-effects model statistics and forest plots, with one study removed. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Sports 11 00222 g005
Figure 6. Random-effects model cumulative analysis statistics and forest plot by year. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Figure 6. Random-effects model cumulative analysis statistics and forest plot by year. References included in the figure [15,16,17,18,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63].
Sports 11 00222 g006
Table 1. Individual study bias questions and rating explanations summed to a study quality score.
Table 1. Individual study bias questions and rating explanations summed to a study quality score.
Rating Explanations
BiasLow Risk (3 Points)Medium Risk (2 Points)High Risk (1 Point)
SampleElite or advancedIntermediate or youthMix
RecruitmentStated random selection occurred from a much larger group (e.g., from all athletes at an event)Random selection occurred within a group of athletes (e.g., a college team at an event)No random selection of any kind stated
NonresponseAppears most participants completed the measuresParticipants recruited; coaches/parents denied participation in at least 25%Many did not do it (e.g., a big race, subjects recruited at the race, most likely most did not do it)
Collection settingAdministered in personMix in person and unsupervisedUnsupervised (e.g., survey left in lockers) or mail, online
Performance measureEvent time, win–loss, placement, event scoresParticipant statisticsSubjective
SettingActual eventNo medium ratingContrived
Collection consistencyAll the sameNo medium ratingA mix of ways (e.g., individual for some, in large groups for others)
AnonymityYes, statedNot stated but informed consent presentedNot stated
Table 2. Study characteristics.
Table 2. Study characteristics.
StudyAge Means, Ranges, or DescriptionsN% MaleCtry.SportPerformance Measure
Aikawa and Takai [24]19.705256.76JPGymnasticsPerformance statistics
Arruza et al. [25]23.601172.73USMix individualSelf-evaluation
Avugos et al. [26]19.7426100ILBasketballShots made percentage
Baretta et al. [27]39.7912966.67ITFreedivingPerformance statistics
Beauchamp et al. [28]20.8060100CAGolfEvent score
Beauchamp and Whinton [29]12.4818720.32UKEquestrianPerformance statistics
Besharat and Pourbohlool [30]23.0324660.57IRMix individual and teamCoach evaluation
Bezjak and Sharifi [31]34.809856.64USMarathonEvent time
Blecharz et al. [32]21.7719757.36USMixPerformance satisfaction
Blecharz et al. [33]18.14101100PLSoccerShots made percentage
Blecharz et al. (2 samples) [34]22.70, 18.2356, 11336.00, 100USMix, soccerPerformance satisfaction
Brace et al. [35]38.865667.85MixUltra-enduranceEvent time
Bray et al. [36]14.80295100ESSoccerSelf-evaluations
Bueno et al. [18]31.0190?ESTriathlonEvent times
Bueno et al. [37]22.00350USTrack and field (endurance)Performance statistics (meters covered in 5 min)
Burke and Jin [38]NR4092.5AUTriathlonEvent times
Coudevylle et al. [39]20.223151.61FRBasketballPerformance statistics scale (shots and passes)
Donovan and Williams [40]17.00 to 21.0010060.5USTrack and fieldMercier scoring of performance
Estevan et al. [41]22.038655.81ESTaekwondoWinners vs. losers
Feltz and Lirgg [42]19.00 to 22.00159 (6 teams)NRUSIce hockeyGame statistics including winning percentage
George [43]17.30, 20.7028, 25100USBaseballHitting statistics
Gernigon and Delloye [17]19.906267.74FRRunning sprintsInternational Amateur Athletic Federation points earned based on 60 m trial
Haney and Long [44]19.161780USMix teamShots scored scale
Hatzigeorgiadis et al. [45]13.264646GRTennisForehand drive score
Hayslip et al. [46]44.46220100USGolfEvent score
Heazlewood and Burke [47]32.40, 33.60416NRAUTriathlonEvent time
Kane et al. [48]Adolescents216, 204100USWrestlingWin/loss records
Koper et al. [49] (4 samples)33.90, 28.10, 29.00, 29.9018, 25, 42, 2478.0, 80.0, 74.0, 83.0MixBocciaEvent results
LaGuardia and Labbe [50]Adults6368.25USRunningPace times
Lee [51]21.0025717.9USMix individual and teamTeam winning percentage
Lowther et al. [16]19.00 to 28.0015100UKSoccerSelf-referenced measure
Martin and Gill [52]16.0073100USTrack and fieldEvent time, placement
Martin and Gill 1995 [53]16.008655.81USTrack and fieldEvent time, placement
Martin [54]35.4051100USWheelchair road racingEvent time, placement
McAuley and Gill [55]19.00 to 22.00520USGymnasticsEvent scores
Nicholls et al. [56]21.3030782.08UKMix individual and teamPerformance rating
Okwumabua [57]35.509091.11USMarathonBest 10K time
Slobounov et al. [15]18.00 to 21.00650USDivingSelf-evaluation
Treasure et al. [58]16.0370100USWrestlingWinning percentage, point statistics
Turner et al. [59]16.4542100UKCricketBatting test statistics
Turner et al. [60]13.26920UKNetballCoach evaluation
Watkins et al. [61]12.50205100USBaseballHitting statistics
Weiss [62]11.5022100USGymnasticsEvent scores
Yang [63]NR270NRESBasketballTeam performance score
Bold country abbreviation = study written in non-English. Abbreviations: Country (Crty.), not reported (NR); Australia (AU), Canada (CA), Spain (ES), France (FR), Greece (GR), Israel (IL), Islamic Republic of Iran (IR), Italy (IT), Japan (JP), Poland (PL), United Kingdom (UK), and United States of America (US). ? = not enough information presented to decide.
Table 3. Moderator coding.
Table 3. Moderator coding.
StudyDurationSkillEventMeasureReferenceConcordanceLevel
Aikawa and Takai [24]>10CACTSUBSelfYesSub
Arruza et al. [25]<10, >10C, OACTSUBSelfYesElite
Avugos et al. [26]<10CCONOBJOTHYesElite
Baretta et al. [27]<10CACTOBJOTHYesElite
Beauchamp et al. [28]>10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Beauchamp and Whinton [29]<10, >10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Besharat and Pourbohlool [30]<10, >10C, OACTSUBOTHYesElite
Bezjak and Sharifi [31]>10CACTOBJOTHNoMix
Blecharz et al. [32]<10, >10C, OACTSUBSelfNoElite
Blecharz et al. [33]<10CCONOBJOTHNoSub
Blecharz et al. [34]>10OACTSUBSelfNoSub
Brace et al. [35]>10CACTOBJOTHNoElite
Bray et al. [36]>10OACTSUBSelfYesSub
Bueno et al. [18]>10CACTOBJSelfYesSub
Bueno et al. [37]<10CCONOBJOTHYesSub
Burke and Jin [38]>10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Coudevylle et al. [39]<10CCONOBJOTHYesSub
Donovan and Williams [40]<10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Estevan et al. [41]<10OACTOBJOTHNoSub
Feltz and Lirgg [42]>10OACTOBJOTHNoSub
George [43]>10OACTOBJOTHYesSub
Gernigon and Delloye [17]<10CCONOBJOTHYesSub
Haney and Long [44]<10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Hatzigeorgiadis et al. [45]<10CCONOBJOTHYesSub
Hayslip et al. [46]>10CACTOBJOTHNoSub
Heazlewood and Burke [47]>10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Kane et al. [48]<10OACTSUBSelfNoSub
Koper et al. [49] (4 samples)<10OACTOBJOTHNoSub
LaGuardia and Labbe [50]<10, >10CACTOBJOTHBothSub
Lee [51]<10, >10C, OACTOBJOTHYesSub
Lowther et al. [16]>10OACTSUBSelfYesElite
Martin and Gill [52]<10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Martin and Gill [53]>10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Martin [54]>10CACTOBJOTHYesElite
McAuley and Gill [55]<10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Nicholls et al. [56]?C, OACTSUBSelfNoSub
Okwumabua [57]>10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Slobounov et al. [15]<10CCONSUBSelfYesElite
Treasure et al. [58]<10OACTOBJOTHBothSub
Turner et al. [59]<10CCONOBJOTHYesElite
Turner et al. [60]>10OACTOBJ/SUBOTHYesSub
Watkins et al. [61]<10CCONOBJOTHYesSub
Weiss [62]<10CACTOBJOTHYesSub
Yang [63]>10OACTSUBOTHNoSub
Abbreviations: L = low, M = medium, and H = high; ? = not enough information presented to decide, O = open skill sport, C = closed skill sport, OBJ = objective, SUB = subjective-referenced performance, OTH = other-referenced performance, Self = performance self-referenced, and Sub = sub-elite athlete level.
Table 4. Moderator results.
Table 4. Moderator results.
ModeratorGroupkr95% CI95% PIQp-Valueτ2I2
Sport duration<10240.270.19, 0.35−0.13, 0.60 0.0483.07
>10250.340.17, 0.49−0.51, 0.850.520.470.1997.63
Sport skillClosed290.370.22, 0.50−0.48, 0.86 0.1997.62
Open210.230.12, 0.33−0.23, 0.612.210.130.0585.27
EventActual450.320.21, 0.42−0.42, 0.81 0.1496.86
Contrived100.260.12, 0.40 −0.22, 0.640.440.500.0493.70
Performance measureObjective420.290.16, 0.41−0.52, 0.82 0.1896.82
Subjective110.380.23, 0.52−0.22, 0.780.950.330.0793.70
Performance referenceOther440.310.19, 0.43−0.50, 0.83 0.1896.89
Self100.250.18, 0.330.02, 0.460.650.420.0167.72
ConcordanceYes320.370.23, 0.49−0.44, 0.85 0.1797.44
No210.220.11, 0.32−0.23, 0.592.980.080.0585.08
Athlete levelElite100.400.17, 0.59−0.44, 0.87 0.1392.19
Sub-elite420.280.16, 0.39−0.46, 0.790.930.330.1596.92
Abbreviation: k = number of samples, r = mean random-effect modeled effect size, CI = confidence interval, PI = prediction interval, τ2 = Tau-squared, and I2 = I-squared.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lochbaum, M.; Sisneros, C.; Cooper, S.; Terry, P.C. Pre-Event Self-Efficacy and Sports Performance: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. Sports 2023, 11, 222. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports11110222

AMA Style

Lochbaum M, Sisneros C, Cooper S, Terry PC. Pre-Event Self-Efficacy and Sports Performance: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. Sports. 2023; 11(11):222. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports11110222

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lochbaum, Marc, Cassandra Sisneros, Sydney Cooper, and Peter C. Terry. 2023. "Pre-Event Self-Efficacy and Sports Performance: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis" Sports 11, no. 11: 222. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports11110222

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop