Next Article in Journal
MHC Genomics and Disease: Looking Back to Go Forward
Next Article in Special Issue
Sex Differences in Liver, Adipose Tissue, and Muscle Transcriptional Response to Fasting and Refeeding in Mice
Previous Article in Journal
Closer to Nature Through Dynamic Culture Systems
Previous Article in Special Issue
Sex-Gender Variable: Methodological Recommendations for Increasing Scientific Value of Clinical Studies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Mismatched Prenatal and Postnatal Maternal Depressive Symptoms and Child Behaviours: A Sex-Dependent Role for NR3C1 DNA Methylation in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study

1
School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading RG6 6BZ, UK
2
Department of Biostatistics and Health Informatics, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK
3
Department of Molecular and Clinical Pharmacology, Institute of Translational Medicine, University of Liverpool, Brownlow Hill, Liverpool L69 3GB, UK
4
School of Healthcare Science, Manchester Metropolitan University, John Dalton Building, Chester Street, Manchester M1 5GD, UK
5
Department of Psychological Science, Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, Whelan Building, University of Liverpool, Brownlow Hill, Liverpool L69 3GB, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Cells 2019, 8(9), 943; https://doi.org/10.3390/cells8090943
Submission received: 20 June 2019 / Revised: 30 July 2019 / Accepted: 14 August 2019 / Published: 21 August 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sex Differences in Signaling Pathways)

Abstract

:
Evolutionary hypotheses predict that male fetuses are more vulnerable to poor maternal conditions (Sex-biased Maternal Investment), but female fetuses are at greater risk of glucocorticoid-mediated disorders where there is a mismatch between fetal and postnatal environments (Predictive Adaptive Response). Self-reported prenatal and postnatal depression and maternal report of child anxious-depressed symptoms at 2.5, 3.5 and 5.0 years were obtained from an ‘extensive’ sample of first-time mothers (N = 794). Salivary NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation was assayed at 14 months in an ‘intensive’ subsample (n = 176) and stratified by psychosocial risk. Generalised structural equation models were fitted and estimated by maximum likelihood to allow the inclusion of participants from both intensive and extensive samples. Postnatal depression was associated with NR3C1 methylation and anxious-depressed symptoms in daughters of mothers with low prenatal depression (prenatal-postnatal depression interaction for methylation, p < 0.001; for child symptoms, p = 0.011). In girls, NR3C1 methylation mediated the association between maternal depression and child anxious-depressed symptoms. The effects were greater in girls than boys: the test of sex differences in the effect of the prenatal-postnatal depression interaction on both outcomes gave X2 (2) = 5.95 (p = 0.051). This was the first human study to show that epigenetic and early behavioural outcomes may arise through different mechanisms in males and females.

1. Introduction

The ‘fetal origins’ hypothesis was first proposed to account for associations between low birth weight and obesity, cardiovascular disease, and type II diabetes in middle and old age [1]. According to this hypothesis, low birth weight reflects evolved adaptive mechanisms that confer advantages later in life where food is scarce but create risk in the presence of high calorie diets, common in industrialised societies. Far from being a mechanism specific to nutrition in humans, adaptations prior to birth that anticipate later environments are found across species, possibly reflecting a conserved ‘Predictive Adaptive Response’ (PAR) mechanism [2,3]. According to the PAR hypothesis, matched prenatal and postnatal conditions will be associated with good outcomes, while mismatching creates risks for poor outcomes. Regarding effects on psychiatric disorders, many studies have reported that anxiety, depression and behavioural symptoms in children are predicted by prenatal stressors, maternal depression and anxiety, and by low birth weight [4,5,6,7,8,9]. We previously reported that the association between prenatal anxiety and child emotional and behavioural outcomes is only seen in the presence of low maternal stroking, consistent with animal studies of the protective effects of postnatal tactile stimulation [8]. In addition, we showed that mismatched prenatal–postnatal maternal anxiety was associated with elevated child irritability at age 7 years, only in the presence of low maternal stroking [10], consistent with a mismatch effect creating vulnerability that is modified by early tactile stimulation.
Fetal adaptations may additionally vary by the sex of the fetus. According to the Trivers–Willard (T–W) hypothesis, if maternal health during pregnancy predicts later reproductive fitness in the offspring, then a male predominance of births will be favoured when maternal conditions are good because healthy males compete successfully for females. By contrast, when maternal conditions are poor, the sex ratio will be reversed, both to avoid bearing males who compete less successfully for females, but also because, compared to females, health outcomes for mothers following male births are poorer [11]. Although this hypothesis has been subject to challenges and modifications [12], the central idea that reproductive strategies associated with poor maternal conditions involve the sacrifice of males and the protection of females has received substantial support. It is also consistent with well-documented observations that male fetuses are more vulnerable to threats such as preterm births and are more likely to suffer neurodevelopmental consequences of fetal insults [13]. This hypothesis appears to predict better outcomes for females following poor maternal conditions. However, if this protective effect in females arises from advantages conferred by fetal anticipation of matched environments (PAR hypothesis), mismatches between maternal conditions during pregnancy and the postnatal environment create vulnerability. Combining the T–W and PAR hypotheses leads to the prediction that the effects of prenatal risks operate differently in males and females. In females, vulnerability is generated by particular combinations of prenatal and postnatal risks, while in males, poor outcomes arise incrementally from the degree of prenatal risk. In the only human study we are aware of to have examined the combined effects predicted by the T–W and PAR hypotheses, matched environments indexed by prenatal and postnatal depression (low–low and high–high) were associated with better cognitive and motor outcomes over the first year of life than mismatched prenatal and postnatal depression, and this effect was seen in females only [14]. However, many studies have reported sex differences in developmental outcomes in relation to prenatal risks without examining for the interplay with postnatal environments. Sex differences in fetal responses to stress [15], and in later emotional and behavioural problems following maternal anxiety or depression during pregnancy and low birth weight [4,7,8,16,17], have been identified.
In animal models, prenatal and postnatal stress cause long-term elevations in hypothalamic pituitary axis (HPA) reactivity and anxiety-like behaviours. This is mediated via reduced glucocorticoid receptor (GR) gene NR3C1 expression, particularly in the hippocampus, which impairs HPA axis feedback mechanisms [18]. The epigenetic process of DNA methylation involves the addition of methyl groups to CpG dinucleotides in gene regulatory regions that are associated with repressed gene expression. Animal findings of the epigenetic effects of early life stress have been translated to humans in a study reporting elevated NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation and reduced NR3C1 expression in post-mortem hippocampal tissue of people who have committed suicide and who were abused during childhood when compared to non-abused [19]. Other studies using peripheral DNA from the blood or saliva of infants and adolescents have shown increased levels of NR3C1 methylation associated with prenatal and childhood adversities [20,21,22]. Several clinical studies examining leukocytes have reported elevated methylation of the homologous human NR3C1 1-F promoter (homologous to the rat 1-7 promoter) at a specific CpG (CpG unit 22,23, Figure 1) associated with prenatal maternal depression [20,23,24,25] and childhood stress [26]. Studies in humans also find associations between prenatal anxiety and postnatal depression in mothers, and adolescent depressive symptoms mediated via HPA axis dysregulation [27,28], consistent with the role of HPA axis dysregulation in adolescent depression [29]. Higher NR3C1 methylation levels, hypothesised to contribute to reduced NR3C1 expression [19], have been associated with increased salivary cortisol stress responses in infants at 3 months [20] and a flattened cortisol recovery slope following stress in adolescents [30], suggesting that methylation of NR3C1 may impair negative feedback of the HPA axis.
In the first study to examine the interplay between prenatal and postnatal depression in relation to NR3C1 gene methylation, we showed that the association between postnatal maternal depression and NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation in children was stronger where mothers had reported lower depression during pregnancy, in line with the PAR hypothesis [32]. However, we did not examine for sex differences. Sex differences in glucocorticoid mechanisms associated with prenatal stress have been shown in animal models. In rats, many effects of prenatal stress on later development are seen only in females, and these are abolished by adrenalectomy [33]. The effects predicted by a combination of the T–W and PAR hypotheses have been demonstrated in starlings where mismatched prehatch-posthatch conditions had a greater effect on corticosterone levels in female than male chicks, but prenatal risk increased mortality in male chicks [34,35]. In humans, a sex difference in associations between prenatal depression and NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation has been reported [36], although the interplay with postnatal depression was not examined.
In this study we examined predictions based on the T–W sex-biased parental investment and PAR hypotheses. In females, where individual and species vulnerability are reduced by matching environments but increased by mismatching, the presence of good prenatal conditions followed by adverse rearing experiences, and vice versa, create vulnerability to child anxiety and depression. Based on the animal models, we predicted that this effect in females would involve altered HPA axis reactivity arising from epigenetic modifications of the GR gene. In males, where individuals are sacrificed for species advantage, the presence of prenatal stress will create vulnerability, unmodified by later environment quality. The animal models suggest that glucocorticoid mechanisms are implicated in excess male deaths under unfavourable maternal conditions, but they may not contribute to effects of prenatal stress on functioning after birth.
These predictions were tested in a longitudinal study, using measures of prenatal and postnatal depression, of NR3C1 1-F promoter region methylation at 14 months of age, and anxious depressed symptoms in children across the preschool period. We predicted that in girls, but not boys, low prenatal depression followed by high postnatal maternal depression, and high prenatal depression followed by low postnatal depression would be associated with elevated anxious depressed symptoms and elevated NR3C1 methylation, which would mediate the association between mismatched maternal depression and child anxious-depressed symptoms. In boys, we hypothesised that prenatal and postnatal depression would be independent risks for elevated anxious-depressed symptoms, without the interaction between them predicted for females.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

The participants were members of the Wirral Child Health and Development Study, a prospective epidemiological longitudinal cohort of first-time mothers recruited in pregnancy to study prenatal and infancy origins of emotional and behavioural disorders. The full cohort of 1233 mothers with live singleton births had participated in several waves of assessment with a stratified random sub-sample of 316 identified for an additional, more intensive assessment (intensive sample). Strata were defined on the basis of low, medium and high psychosocial risk (scores of <=2, 3 or >3 on an inter-partner psychological abuse scale provided on entry to the study at 20 weeks of pregnancy), with higher selection probabilities for those at higher risk. Appropriately analysed, the design allowed estimates of means and coefficients for the whole general population cohort to be derived, even for measures available only in the intensive sample [37].
Approval for the procedures was obtained from the Cheshire North and West Research Ethics Committee (UK) (reference no. 05/Q1506/107). The extensive sample was identified from consecutive first-time mothers who booked for antenatal care at 12 weeks of gestation between 12 February 2007 and 29 October 2008. The booking clinic was administered by the Wirral University Teaching Hospital, which was the sole provider of universal prenatal care on the Wirral Peninsula. Socioeconomic conditions on the Wirral range between the deprived inner city and affluent suburbs, but with few from ethnic minorities. The study was introduced to the women by clinic midwives who asked for their agreement to be approached by study research midwives when they attended for ultrasound scanning at 20 weeks of gestation. After complete description of the study to the women, written informed consent was obtained by the study midwives, who then administered questionnaires and an interview in the clinic.

2.2. Participants

Of those approached by study midwives, 68.4% gave consent and completed the measures, yielding an extensive sample of 1233 mothers with surviving singleton babies. The sampling flow chart has been published previously [36]. The mean age at recruitment of the extensive sample participants was 26.8 years (s.d. 5.8, range 18–51). Using the UK Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) [38] based on data collected from the UK Census in 2001, 36.6% of the extensive sample reported socioeconomic profiles found in the most deprived UK quintile, consistent with the high levels of deprivation in some parts of the Wirral. Forty-eight women (3.9%) described themselves as other than White British.
In addition to assessments of the mothers at 20 weeks of gestation, mothers and infants provided data at birth and postnatally at 5, 9, and 29 weeks, and at 14.19, s.d. 1.71 months (‘14 months’), 30.86, s.d. 2.31 months (‘2.5 years’), 41.90 s.d. 2.48 months (‘3.5 years’) and 58.64 s.d. 3.74 months (‘5 years’). Two hundred and sixty-eight mothers and infants came into the lab at 14 months for detailed observational, interview and physiological measures. This was the first occasion at which saliva for DNA was collected. Seven parents declined consent for DNA collection, 3 samples were spoilt, and 25 assessments were curtailed before saliva collection because of time constraints. Sufficient DNA for methylation analyses was obtained from 181 infants. Maternal reports of child anxious-depressed symptoms were available on 253 of the intensive sub-cohort at 2.5 years, on 825 of the whole cohort at 3.5 years and on 768 of the whole cohort at 4.5 years.

2.3. Measures

Maternal depression. Maternal symptoms of depression were assessed at 20 weeks gestation and at every follow up point using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS), which has been used extensively to assess prenatal and postnatal depression [39].
DNA methylation. Methylation status in the NR3C1 1-F promoter was examined at the same CpGs (CpG unit 22 and 23, shown in Figure 1) identified in previous studies (25). DNA collected from Oragene® saliva samples was extracted, bisulphite treated, amplified (Forward, GACCTGGTCTCTCTGGGG; Reverse, TGCAACCCCGTAGCCCCTTTC) and run on a Sequenom EpiTYPER system (Sequenom Inc., San Diego, US), providing an average for methylation across the two CpG units. Data were transformed to the percentage of methylation at CpG units 22 and 23 to allow for comparison with previous analyses of differential methylation at this locus.
Child anxious-depressed symptoms. Child symptoms were assessed by maternal report at 2.5, 3.5 and 5.0 years using the preschool Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [40]. It has 99 items, each scored 0 (not true), 1 (somewhat or sometimes true), and 2 (very true or often true), which are summed to create seven syndrome scales. Only the anxious/depressed scale was analysed for this report, and as recommended in the CBCL manual, raw scores were used [41].
Stratification variable and confounders. Partner psychological abuse was assessed using a 20-item questionnaire covering humiliating, demeaning or threatening utterances in the partner relationship during pregnancy over the previous year [42]. Maternal age (at this first pregnancy), marital status at 20 weeks of gestation, and socioeconomic status were included as covariates because of their established associations with adult depression. Socioeconomic status was determined using the revised English Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) [38] based on neighborhood deprivation. All the mothers were given IMD ranks according to the postcode of the area where they lived and assigned to a quintile based on the UK distribution of deprivation. The mothers’ years of education at enrolment in the study were recorded. Information about smoking was obtained at 20 and 32 weeks of gestation and was included because of published associations with altered DNA methylation [43]. Birth records provided the sex of the infant, one-minute Apgar score, and birth weight and gestational age, from which a measure of fetal growth was obtained. Low fetal growth is associated with elevated fetal glucocorticoid exposure and therefore, might be associated with elevated NR3C1 gene methylation. Obstetric risk was rated using a weighted severity scale developed by a collaboration of American and Danish obstetricians and paediatric neurologists [44].

2.4. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were undertaken in Stata 14 (StataCorp, 2015). Generalised structural equation models (SEM) were fitted using the sem procedure and estimated by maximum likelihood to allow the inclusion of participants from both intensive and extensive strata. The anxiety-depression scores at 2.5, 3.5 and 5.0 years and NR3C1 percent methylation at 14 months were highly skewed, so scores were log-transformed and Winsorized at 2.5 standard deviations to reduce their skew. For further robustness, we report standard errors and p-values based on the heteroscedastic consistent estimator of the parameter covariance matrix. The main analyses included the stratification variable and confounds, except for perinatal confounds as they may lie on a mediational pathway from prenatal depression, however, the effect of adding those variables was examined. Model estimates and tests allowed for differential missingness associated with any of the covariates and observed responses included in the model, accounting for the stratified study design.
The pre-post environment mismatch predictions on both methylation and child symptoms were examined first by testing for two-way interactions between prenatal and postnatal depression in models estimated separately in females and males. We then tested for the sex difference by examining the three-way, sex by prenatal depression by postnatal depression interactions in a model that included both genders. The effects of combinations of prenatal and postnatal depression giving rise to these interaction effects are shown in the figures. The prediction of additive effects of prenatal and postnatal depression in boys was examined in models without interaction terms.
In the fitted models, methylation was specified as a factor, measured without error by the observed methylation, a device that implicitly imputes rates of methylation where these have not been observed in a manner which recognised our uncertainty in these unobserved values. This enabled participants with partial data that were informative for some parts of the model to be included.

3. Results

Table 1 provides summary statistics for males and females separately for the measures included in the analysis and shows the sample size at each data collection point. As described in the statistical analysis section, differences in the available sample for different measures were accounted for by the use of weighted, maximum likelihood or covariate adjusted estimators. Figure 2 shows the structure of the SEM model in which maternal history of depression predicts NR3C1 methylation (solid red arrows) and maternal report of child anxious-depressed symptoms (solid black arrows). These analyses included the 412 girls and 382 boys on whom there were measures of maternal depression and maternal report of child anxious-depressed symptoms at a minimum of one follow-up point, as well as all confounders.
Table 2 shows, for girls and boys separately, the estimated path coefficients from the standardised prenatal depression, postnatal depression and their interaction (product) of primary interest accounting for the stratification, attrition and confounders. We first tested the prediction that there would be an interaction between prenatal and postnatal depression in girls but not in boys. In girls, there was a significant effect of the interaction between prenatal and postnatal depression on both child anxiety-depression (p = 0.011) and NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation (p < 0.001). For boys, by contrast, anxious-depressed symptoms were not predicted by the prenatal and postnatal depression interaction term (p = 0.920), and the effect on NR3C1 methylation was smaller than for girls, though still significant (p = 0.003). Adding the three additional potential confounders that were assessed after the prenatal exposure (obstetric risk index, 1-min Apgar score and birthweight/gestational age) made no material difference to these associations. Fitting this model to boys and girls together but allowing the effects of prenatal and postnatal depression exposure on the two correlated outcomes to differ by sex (in addition to a gender main effect), a Wald test of the sex differences in the effect of the prenatal-postnatal depression interaction on both outcomes (a difference of 0.20 for anxiety-depression and 0.18 for methylation) gave X2(2) of 5.95 (p = 0.051), with the two individual interactions contributing equally (anxiety-depression p = 0.088, methylation p = 0.069).
The Table 2 shows standardized factor loadings of child CBCL anxious-depressed symptoms at ages 2.5, 3.5 and 5 years, and the main effects and effects of the interaction of prenatal and postnatal depression in the prediction of the anxious-depressed factor and the NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation (effects of stratification factors and confounders not shown). Anxious-depressed symptoms and methylation were analysed together as correlated outcomes in an SEM. Coefficients for the effects of confounders and stratification factors are not shown (stratum, maternal age, maternal smoking, maternal education, no partner, neighbourhood deprivation). The models reported used robust standard errors to guard against inferential errors due to non-normality. In order to provide conventional model fit statistics, the models were run without robust standard errors and the statistics from this are reported in the final row of the Table 2.
We then tested the prediction that there would be independent and additive effects of prenatal and postnatal depression in boys by estimating the model (not shown in the Table) for boys without the interaction term. This showed a significant effect on child anxiety-depression of postnatal depression (standardised coefficient 0.17, CI 0.04 to 0.30, p = 0.011) and an effect of similar magnitude, of prenatal depression which was non-significant, (0.15, CI −0.02 to 0.33, p = 0.080). Independent effects on methylation were not seen (prenatal 0.05, CI −0.17 to 0.27, p = 0.640; postnatal 0.13, CI −0.09 to 0.36, p = 0.241).
Figure 3 displays how the interactions between prenatal and postnatal depression in the prediction of anxious-depressed symptoms differed between girls and boys. It can be seen that in girls, at a low level of prenatal depression (1 standard deviation below the mean), increasing postnatal depression was strongly associated with increasing child anxious-depressed symptoms, while at a high level, there was no association. With prenatal depression at the mean, the association was intermediate between the low and high prenatal levels. In boys, by contrast, as evidenced in parallel regression lines, there was no interplay between prenatal and postnatal maternal depression.
As shown in Figure 4, the effects of prenatal-postnatal mismatch on methylation were again strongly evident in girls, with the greatest association between postnatal depression and methylation in the presence of low prenatal depression, and progressively weaker associations at higher levels of prenatal depression. The progressive effect of prenatal depression was also evident in boys but was less strong.
In girls, replacing the correlation between the methylation and anxiety-depression factors by a causal effect, higher NR3C1 methylation at 14 months, was associated with higher anxiety-depressed symptoms (standardised coefficient 0.36 CI 0.05 to 0.67, p = 0.025), illustrated in the left-hand panel of Figure 5. The residual direct effect of the prenatal-postnatal interaction on child anxiety-depression symptoms was substantially reduced, from −0.19 (shown in Table 2) to −0.06 (CI −0.26 to 0.15), becoming wholly nonsignificant (p = 0.600). For boys there was no evidence of an effect of methylation on symptoms (standardised coefficient −0.03, CI −0.31 to 0.24, p = 0.820).

4. Discussion

Many, although not all, of our predictions based on the evolutionary T–W and PAR hypotheses for sex-biased parental investment and fetal programming were supported in this longitudinal study, from 20 weeks of pregnancy and over the first 5 years of children’s lives. Mismatching between prenatal and postnatal maternal depression was associated with greater anxious-depressed symptoms and NR3C1 methylation in girls. Both effects were most evident in girls exposed to high levels of postnatal depression. Their symptoms and NR3C1 methylation levels were higher when their mothers had reported low levels of depression during pregnancy, in line with the idea that they had not been prepared by the fetal environment for postnatal exposure to maternal depression. In girls only, elevated NR3C1 methylation was associated with higher anxious-depressed symptoms and mediated the association between maternal depression and child symptoms. In boys, there was no evidence of effects of prenatal-postnatal depression mismatch on anxious-depressed symptoms. However, and contrary to our prediction, the prenatal-postnatal mismatch effect on NR3C1 methylation was seen in boys as well as in girls, although the size of the effect was smaller.
The strengths of the investigation include a prospective study with a general population sample, accounting for a number of plausible confounds and factors associated with attrition. Moreover, by using SEM to create a latent variable from measurement at 3 time points over 2.5 years, we reduced the risks arising from multiple testing for each time point and we were able to examine the predictions in relation to persistently elevated symptoms likely to confer risk for an elevated trajectory for anxious-depressed symptoms over childhood [45]. The method adopted for missing methylation data exploited the properties of maximum likelihood for accounting for data assumed missing at random. Most missingness was by design because of the systematic stratification of the intensive sample, thus meeting this assumption, and the inclusion of multiple covariates allowed us account for unplanned attrition. It is nevertheless possible that not all the necessary confounds to deal with non-random missingness were identified.
There were four principal limitations in relation to the measurement of NR3C1 methylation. First, peripheral cell samples from both blood and saliva are heterogeneous, which may account for some of the variability in methylation. This can introduce a confound whereby other variables are associated with cellular heterogeneity [46]. Second, while studies combining peripheral cell and CNS post-mortem estimations have suggested that they are often substantially correlated [47], it cannot be assumed that DNA methylation in peripheral tissues reflects methylation in relevant CNS regions. This is particularly a concern because of substantial variations in epigenetic effects across brain regions and cell types. Specifically, it cannot be assumed that variations in the NR3C1 1-F promoter in saliva reflect variations in glucocorticoid receptor synthesis in the hippocampal regions involved in HPA axis regulation. Third, DNA methylation is one of a number of epigenetic processes that regulate gene expression, and does not provide a direct measure of that expression. ‘Mediation’ in this report, as in the field more widely [48], refers to statistical findings consistent with, but not direct evidence of, epigenetic mediation. Fourth, there are many combinations of CpG sites, even on a relatively circumscribed region such as the NR3C1 1-F promoter that could be examined, leading to the risk of multiple analyses and ‘significant’ findings occurring by chance. Fifth, although we accounted for several plausible confounds, environmental variables other than those included in analyses may better account for the findings.
No one study can establish the validity of estimates of peripheral cell methylation as indices of CNS methylation, however, a finding of the same pattern of associations for peripheral cell methylation and for behaviours that undoubtedly reflect CNS function, and for mediation of the association between maternal depression and symptoms by NR3C1 methylation is relevant to the issue. As is evident from the SEM models, and as seen in Figure 3 and Figure 4, there were striking similarities between the patterns of associations involving interactions between prenatal and postnatal depression and sex differences, not only for child anxious-depressed symptom but also for NR3C1 methylation. Furthermore, in this study, we reduced risks arising from multiple analyses of many potential methylation sites by examining only one site that had been identified from a meta-analysis of previous studies [24].

5. Conclusions

Our findings are important in five major ways. First, they provide pointers to study designs that could be introduced into animal models where mechanisms could be examined using experimentally controlled risks. These would, for example, examine the interplay between prenatal and postnatal risks in relation to the role of the placenta in regulating the passage of maternal glucocorticoids to the foetus, which, in turn, can be controlled by further epigenetic modifications of specific placental genes [47]. Second, they illustrate how evolutionary hypotheses regarding parental investment in offspring can be used to generate novel, and in some ways surprising predictions regarding parenting and early development in humans [48]. Third, testing in this way can generate further productive questions. In this study, while there was good evidence for mismatch effects in females on NR3C1 methylation and child symptoms and for a sex difference in relation to child symptoms, the prenatal-postnatal depression mismatch was also associated with NR3C1 methylation in males, which was contrary to the predictions. Further study is needed into the conditions under which fetal programming effects are seen in males as well as females, and under what conditions there are sex differences in the behavioural implications of NR3C1 methylation. Fourth, these results show that even though human development is subject to many complex social and psychological influences, biological mechanisms conserved across many non-human species can be highly influential. Fifth, they suggest that some prenatal effects on epigenetic and behavioural outcomes in early childhood differ radically in males and females, and so further study of sex-specific mechanisms is needed. This will have implications for our understanding of the biology of psychiatric disorders arising in childhood.

Author Contributions

J.H., H.S., A.P. designed the study, C.M., J.P.Q. conducted the methylation estimations, J.H., H.S., N.W. supervised data collection, J.H., A.P., N.W. analysed the data, J.H., A.P., H.S., N.W. wrote the paper, and all authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by grants from the UK Medical Research Council (G0400577 and G0900654). AP was partially supported by NIHR Senior Investigator award NF-SI-0617-10120.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to all participating families and to the research staff who contributed to this work. We also thank Wirral University Teaching Hospital NHS Foundation Trust who supported initial recruitment and Cheshire and Wirral Partnership NHS Foundation Trust and Wirral Community NHS Trust for their current support and the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. We would also like to extend our thanks to all the staff involved in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study.

Conflicts of Interest

None of the authors has a conflict of interest.

References

  1. Barker, D.J. The origins of the developmental origins theory. J. Intern. Med. 2007, 261, 412–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bateson, P.; Gluckman, P.; Hanson, M. The biology of developmental plasticity and the Predictive Adaptive Response hypothesis. J. Physiol. 2014, 592, 2357–2368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wells, J.C. Environmental quality, developmental plasticity and the thrifty phenotype: A review of evolutionary models. Evol. Bioinform. 2007, 3, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Costello, E.J.; Worthman, C.; Erkanli, A.; Angold, A. Prediction from low birth weight to female adolescent depression: A test of competing hypotheses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007, 64, 338–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Barker, E.D.; Jaffee, S.R.; Uher, R.; Maughan, B. The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depress. Anxiety 2011, 28, 696–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. O’Connor, T.G.; Heron, J.; Golding, J.; Glover, V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: A test of a programming hypothesis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2003, 44, 1025–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Quarini, C.; Pearson, R.M.; Stein, A.; Ramchandani, P.G.; Lewis, G.; Evans, J. Are female children more vulnerable to the long-term effects of maternal depression during pregnancy? J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 189, 329–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sharp, H.; Hill, J.; Hellier, J.; Pickles, A. Maternal antenatal anxiety, postnatal stroking and emotional problems in children: Outcomes predicted from pre- and postnatal programming hypotheses. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 269–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. MacKinnon, N.; Kingsbury, M.; Mahedy, L.; Evans, J.; Colman, I. The Association Between Prenatal Stress And Externalizing Symptoms In Childhood: Evidence From The Avon Longitudinal Study Of Parents And Children. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 83, 100–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Hill, J.; Pickles, A.; Wright, N.; Braithwaite, E.; Sharp, H. Predictions of children’s emotionality from evolutionary and epigenetic hypotheses. Sci. Reps. 2019, 9, 2519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Trivers, R.L.; Willard, D.E. Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 1973, 179, 90–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Schindler, S.; Gaillard, J.M.; Grüning, A.; Neuhaus, P.; Traill, L.W.; Tuljapurkar, S.; Coulson, T. Sex-specific demography and generalization of the Trivers-Willard theory. Nature 2015, 526, 249–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Aiken, C.E.; Ozanne, S.E. Sex differences in developmental programming models. Reproduction 2013, 145, R1–R13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Sandman, C.A.; Glynn, L.M.; Davis, E.P. Is there a viability-vulnerability tradeoff? Sex differences in fetal programming. J. Psychosom. Res. 2013, 75, 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Doyle, C.; Werner, E.; Feng, T.; Lee, S.; Alternus, M.; Isler, J.R.; Monk, C. Pregnancy distress gets under fetal skin: Maternal ambulatory assessment & sex differences in prenatal development. Dev. Psychobiol. 2015, 57, 607–625. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  16. Glover, V.; Hill, J. Sex differences in the programming effects of prenatal stress on psychopathology and stress responses: An evolutionary perspective. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 106, 736–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Van den Bergh, B.R.; Van, C.B.; Smits, T.; Van, H.S.; Lagae, L. Antenatal maternal anxiety is related to HPA-axis dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in adolescence: A prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008, 33, 536–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Meaney, M.J.; Szyf, M.; Seckl, J.R. Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function and health. Trends Mol. Med. 2007, 13, 269–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. McGowan, P.O.; Sasaki, A.; D’Alessio, A.C.; Dymov, S.; Labonte, B.; Szyf, M.; Turecki, G.; Meaney, M.J. Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nat. Neurosci. 2009, 12, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Oberlander, T.F.; Weinberg, J.; Papsdorf, M.; Grunau, R.; Misri, S.; Devlin, A.M. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 2008, 3, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Tyrka, A.R.; Price, L.H.; Marsit, C.; Walters, O.C.; Carpenter, L.L. Childhood adversity and epigenetic modulation of the leukocyte glucocorticoid receptor: Preliminary findings in healthy adults. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e30148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Conradt, E.; Hawes, K.; Guerin, D.; Armstrong, D.A.; Marsit, C.J.; Tronick, E.; Lester, B.M. The contributions of maternal sensitivity and maternal depressive symptoms to epigenetic processes and neuroendocrine functioning. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 73–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Conradt, E.; Lester, B.M.; Appleton, A.A.; Armstrong, D.A.; Marsit, C.J. The roles of DNA methylation of NR3C1 and 11beta-HSD2 and exposure to maternal mood disorder in utero on newborn neurobehavior. Epigenetics 2013, 8, 1321–1329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hompes, T.; Izzi, B.; Gellens, E.; Morreels, M.; Fieuws, S.; Pexsters, A.; Schops, G.; Dom, M.; Van Bree, R.; Freson, K.; et al. Investigating the influence of maternal cortisol and emotional state during pregnancy on the DNA methylation status of the glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) promoter region in cord blood. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 880–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Palma-Gudiel, H.; Cordova-Palomera, A.; Eixarch, E.; Deuschle, M.; Fananas, L. Maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy alters the epigenetic signature of the glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter in their offspring: A meta-analysis. Epigenetics 2015, 10, 893–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Melas, P.A.; Wei, Y.; Wong, C.C.; Sjöholm, L.K.; Äberg, E.; Mill, J.; Schalling, M.; Forsell, Y.; Lavebratt, C. Genetic and epigenetic associations of MAOA and NR3C1 with depression and childhood adversities. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013, 16, 1513–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Halligan, S.L.; Herbert, J.; Goodyer, I.; Murray, L. Disturbances in morning cortisol secretion in association with maternal postnatal depression predict subsequent depressive symptomatology in adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 62, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Van den Bergh, B.R.; Mulder, E.J.; Mennes, M.; Glover, V. Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: Links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 237–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Goodyer, I.M.; Herbert, J.; Tamplin, A. Psychoendocrine antecedents of persistent first-episode major depression in adolescents: A community-based longitudinal enquiry. Psychol. Med. 2003, 33, 601–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Van der Knaap, L.J.; Oldehinkel, A.J.; Verhulst, F.C.; Van Oort, F.V.; Riese, H. Glucocorticoid receptor gene methylation and HPA-axis regulation in adolescents. The TRAILS study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 58, 46–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Weaver, I.C.; Cervoni, N.; Champagne, F.A.; D’Alessio, A.C.; Sharma, S.; Seckl, J.R.; Dymov, S.; Szyf, M.; Meaney, M.J. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 847–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Murgatroyd, C.; Quinn, J.P.; Sharp, H.M.; Pickles, A.; Hill, J. Effects of prenatal and postnatal depression, and maternal stroking, at the glucocorticoid receptor gene. Transl. Psychiatry 2015, 5, e560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Weinstock, M. Sex-dependent changes induced by prenatal stress in cortical and hippocampal morphology and behaviour in rats: An update. Stress 2011, 14, 604–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Love, O.P.; Williams, T.D. Plasticity in the adrenocortical response of a free-living vertebrate: The role of pre- and post-natal developmental stress. Horm. Behav. 2008, 54, 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Love, O.P.; Williams, T.D. The adaptive value of stress-induced phenotypes: Effects of maternally derived corticosterone on sex-biased investment, cost of reproduction, and maternal fitness. Am. Nat. 2008, 172, E135–E149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Braithwaite, E.C.; Kundakovic, M.; Ramchandani, P.G.; Murphy, S.E.; Champagne, F.A. Maternal prenatal depressive symptoms predict infant NR3C1 1F and BDNF IV DNA methylation. Epigenetics 2015, 10, 408–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sharp, H.; Pickles, A.; Meaney, M.; Marshall, K.; Tibu, F.; Hill, J. Frequency of infant stroking reported by mothers moderates the effect of prenatal depression on infant behavioural and physiological outcomes. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e45446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Noble, M.; Wright, G.; Dibben, C.; Smith, G.A.N.; McLennan, D.; Anttila, C.; Barnes, H.; Mokhtar, C.; Noble, S.; Avenell, D.; et al. The English Indices of Deprivation 2004 (Revised); Report to the Office of the Deputy Prime Minister; Neighbourhood Renewal Unit: London, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  39. Cox, J.L.; Chapman, G.; Murray, D.; Jones, P. Validation of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) in non-postnatal women. J. Affect. Disord. 1996, 39, 185–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rescorla, L.A.; Achenbach, T.M.; Ivanova, M.Y.; Harder, V.S.; Otten, L.; Bilenberg, N.; Bjarnadottir, G.; Capron, C.; De Pauw, S.S.; Dias, P.; et al. International comparisons of behavioral and emotional problems in preschool children: parents’ reports from 24 societies. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2011, 40, 456–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L.A. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles; University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  42. Moffitt, T.E.; Caspi, A.; Krueger, R.F.; Magdol, L.; Margolin, G.; Silva, P.A.; Sydney, R. Do partners agree about abuse in their relationship? A psychometric evaluation of interpartner agreement. Psychol. Assess. 1997, 9, 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Knopik, V.S.; Maccani, M.A.; Francazio, S.; McGeary, J.E. The epigenetics of maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and effects on child development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 1377–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Beck, J.E.; Shaw, D.S. The influence of perinatal complications and environmental adversity on boys’ antisocial behavior. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Sterba, S.K.; Prinstein, M.J.; Cox, M.J. Trajectories of internalizing problems across childhood: Heterogeneity, external validity, and gender differences. Dev. Psychopathol. 2007, 19, 345–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Jaffe, A.E.; Irizarry, R.A. Accounting for cellular heterogeneity is critical in epigenome-wide association studies. Genome Biol. 2014, 15, R31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Tylee, D.S.; Kawaguchi, D.M.; Glatt, S.J. On the outside, looking in: A review and evaluation of the comparability of blood and brain “-omes”. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2013, 162, 595–603. [Google Scholar]
  48. Barker, E.D.; Walton, E.; Cecil, C.A. Annual Research Review: DNA methylation as a mediator in the association between risk exposure and child and adolescent psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 303–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Scheme of the human NR3C1 gene analyzed by bisulfite pyrosequencing. The 5′- end of the human NR3C1 gene contains multiple first exons with multiple transcriptional start sites and mRNA splice variants. The region analyzed by bisulfite pyrosequencing contains 29 CpGs and encompasses exon 1-F, which is the human homolog of the rat exon 1–7, previously shown to be differentially methylated [31].
Figure 1. Scheme of the human NR3C1 gene analyzed by bisulfite pyrosequencing. The 5′- end of the human NR3C1 gene contains multiple first exons with multiple transcriptional start sites and mRNA splice variants. The region analyzed by bisulfite pyrosequencing contains 29 CpGs and encompasses exon 1-F, which is the human homolog of the rat exon 1–7, previously shown to be differentially methylated [31].
Cells 08 00943 g001
Figure 2. Structural equation model fitted to NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation at 14 months and Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) anxious-depressed scores at ages 2.5, 3.5, and 5 years.
Figure 2. Structural equation model fitted to NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation at 14 months and Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) anxious-depressed scores at ages 2.5, 3.5, and 5 years.
Cells 08 00943 g002
Figure 3. Regression lines for the interaction between pre- and post-natal depression and child anxious-depressed symptoms, showing the effect of postnatal depression at the mean and one standard deviation on either side of the mean.
Figure 3. Regression lines for the interaction between pre- and post-natal depression and child anxious-depressed symptoms, showing the effect of postnatal depression at the mean and one standard deviation on either side of the mean.
Cells 08 00943 g003
Figure 4. Regression lines for the interaction between pre- and post-natal depression and child NRC31 1-F promoter methylation, showing the effect of postnatal depression at the mean and one standard deviation either side of the mean. The figure also shows a scatterplot of the association.
Figure 4. Regression lines for the interaction between pre- and post-natal depression and child NRC31 1-F promoter methylation, showing the effect of postnatal depression at the mean and one standard deviation either side of the mean. The figure also shows a scatterplot of the association.
Cells 08 00943 g004
Figure 5. Regression line (and 95% CI) for the association between NRC31 1-F promoter methylation and child anxiety-depression symptoms in girls and boys. The figure also shows a scatterplot of the association.
Figure 5. Regression line (and 95% CI) for the association between NRC31 1-F promoter methylation and child anxiety-depression symptoms in girls and boys. The figure also shows a scatterplot of the association.
Cells 08 00943 g005
Table 1. Summary statistics for outcomes, predictors and variables included as potential confounders for the modelled sample (I = measure based on intensively assessed sub-sample only).
Table 1. Summary statistics for outcomes, predictors and variables included as potential confounders for the modelled sample (I = measure based on intensively assessed sub-sample only).
GirlsBoys
NMeanSDNMeanSD
Child anxious-depressed symptoms 2.5 years (I)1251.541.771201.271.61
Child anxious-depressed symptoms 3.5 years3871.601.643661.591.70
Child anxious-depressed symptoms 5 years3721.761.963471.782.01
Child NR3C1 methylation (I)893.421.85873.551.96
Prenatal EPDS maternal depression scores 4126.944.743827.424.54
Mean postnatal EPDS maternal depression scores4125.243.923825.794.35
Stratum low41277% 38275%
Stratum mid8% 7%
Stratum high16% 18%
Maternal age < 21 years 41210% 38212%
Maternal age 21–30 years56% 56%
Maternal age > 30 years34% 32%
No maternal education beyond age 1841262% 38267%
Smoking–none41262% 38264%
Smoking before pregnancy21% 19%
Smoking during pregnancy17% 18%
No partner41217% 38219%
Most Deprived Quintile41237% 38236%
Obstetric risk index4122.201.183822.201.19
Birthweight/gestation (gm/wk)41283.611.938286.511.4
1 Minute Apgar score4128.951.603828.861.76
Table 2. Summary of SEM analyses predicting NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation and child anxious-depressed symptoms.
Table 2. Summary of SEM analyses predicting NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation and child anxious-depressed symptoms.
|Female (n = 412)
Standardized Coefficient 95% Confidence Interval [p-value]
Male (n = 382)
Standardized Coefficient 95% Confidence Interval [p-value]
Effects on child anxious-depressed symptoms
Prenatal maternal depression−0.06−0.23, 0.110.16−0.00, 0.33
Postnatal maternal depression0.210.05, 0.380.170.03, 0.31
Prenatal-postnatal interaction−0.19
[p = 0.011]
0.34, −0.050.01
[p = 0.920]
−0.11, 0.12
Effects on child NR3C1 1-F promoter methylation
Prenatal maternal depression−0.02−0.28, 0.24−0.11−0.34, 0.12
Postnatal maternal depression0.450.16, 0.750.380.11, 0.65
Prenatal-postnatal interaction−0.39
[p = 0.00001]
−0.56, −0.21−0.21
[p = 0.003]
−0.32, −0.08
Child anxious-depressed symptoms factor loadings
2.5 years0.81 0.72
3.5 years0.80 0.67
5 years0.57 0.81
Model fit
RMSEA = 0.05 (95% C.I. 0.03–0.07) CFI = 0.90RMSEA = 0.01 (95% C.I. 0.01–0.04). CFI = 1.00

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hill, J.; Pickles, A.; Wright, N.; Quinn, J.P.; Murgatroyd, C.; Sharp, H. Mismatched Prenatal and Postnatal Maternal Depressive Symptoms and Child Behaviours: A Sex-Dependent Role for NR3C1 DNA Methylation in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study. Cells 2019, 8, 943. https://doi.org/10.3390/cells8090943

AMA Style

Hill J, Pickles A, Wright N, Quinn JP, Murgatroyd C, Sharp H. Mismatched Prenatal and Postnatal Maternal Depressive Symptoms and Child Behaviours: A Sex-Dependent Role for NR3C1 DNA Methylation in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study. Cells. 2019; 8(9):943. https://doi.org/10.3390/cells8090943

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hill, Jonathan, Andrew Pickles, Nicola Wright, John P. Quinn, Chris Murgatroyd, and Helen Sharp. 2019. "Mismatched Prenatal and Postnatal Maternal Depressive Symptoms and Child Behaviours: A Sex-Dependent Role for NR3C1 DNA Methylation in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study" Cells 8, no. 9: 943. https://doi.org/10.3390/cells8090943

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop