Next Article in Journal
Effects of Early versus Standard Central Line Removal on the Growth of Preterm Infants with Very Low Birth Weight: A Non-Inferiority, Randomized Clinical Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Knowledge and Practices of Digestive Surgeons concerning Specialized Nutritional Support in Cancer Patients: A Survey Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Association between Body Weight Misclassification in Adolescence and Body Fat and Waist Circumference in Adulthood: A Longitudinal Study

by
Abdulaziz D. Aloufi
1,
Jake M. Najman
2,3 and
Abdullah A. Mamun
4,*
1
Ministry of Health, Medina 42351, Saudi Arabia
2
School of Public Health, The University of Queensland, Herston, QLD 4006, Australia
3
School of Social Science, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
4
Poche Centre for Indigenous Health, The University of Queensland, Toowong, QLD 4066, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(22), 4765; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14224765
Submission received: 23 September 2022 / Revised: 27 October 2022 / Accepted: 8 November 2022 / Published: 11 November 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Lipids)

Abstract

:
This study examined the longitudinal association between adolescent body weight misclassifications and body fat and waist circumference during adulthood. A sample was derived from a large Australian birth cohort study. The data analyses were restricted to 1002 participants for whom data on both measured and perceived weight at a 14-year follow-up and the actual measure of adult body fat and waist circumference at a 30-year follow-up were available. To determine misclassifications, we compared the perceived weight with the measured weight. The results were presented as means and mean differences (with a 95% confidence interval) of the body fat percentages and waist circumference levels across the weight misclassification groups, adjusting for potential covariates. For both male and female adolescents, weight underestimation was significantly associated with an increase in body fat percentages and waist circumference in adulthood as compared to those who correctly estimated their weight. In the mean difference analyses, adolescent males and females who underestimated their weight were found to have significantly higher body fat, and waist circumference means than those who correctly estimated their weight in the unadjusted and adjusted comparisons. The adolescent males who overestimated their weight had higher body fat, and waist circumference means when they reached adulthood. Increased awareness of weight misclassification and actual weight among adolescents might contribute to better control of weight gain in adulthood.

1. Introduction

The adolescence period is arguably a critical developmental stage where body weight concerns become more prevalent [1]. Body weight perception may constitute an important motivation for engaging in weight modification strategies [2]. Studies have consistently provided evidence that when one’s perceived weight was compared with one’s measured weight, a large proportion of adolescents misclassified their body weight [3,4,5,6]. There is also convincing evidence that body weight misclassification, which refers to an inaccurate perception of one’s measured body weight, is associated with one’s current and future body weight [3,7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. Several studies have reported that body weight misclassification is common among adolescents and is associated with body weight management [3,14,15,16,17,18,19], suggesting that weight misclassification may be an important factor for body weight development. Underestimating or overestimating one’s body weight might have an impact on decreases or increases in one’s weight [20]. Nevertheless, very little is known about the longitudinal impact of adolescent body weight misclassification on body fat in adulthood.
Several studies have investigated the cross-sectional association between body weight misclassification and body weight and found that overweight and obese people are more likely to underestimate their body weight as compared to their underweight or normal weight counterparts, while body weight overestimation is more common for those who are underweight [3,5,8,21]. Moreover, there is some evidence to suggest that adolescents who perceive that they are overweight when this is not the case may subsequently experience an increase in body weight [11,13]. Conversely, some research suggests that body weight misclassification may limit increases in body weight [10]. For example, adolescents’ underestimation of their body weight has been associated with weight-losing behaviors [10,22]. Despite the evidence from a few relevant studies, the association of body weight misclassification with subsequent weight gain has received little attention.
There are only a few longitudinal studies that have reported an association between body weight misclassification and body composition. An 11-year Norwegian study of 1196 adolescents who were followed until they reached young adulthood found that the normal-weight adolescents who overestimated their body weight subsequently gained 0.66 body mass index (BMI) units and 3.46 cm in their waist circumference (WC) as compared to normal weight adolescents who perceived their body weight correctly [12]. Another 6-year follow-up study in Houston found that adolescents who perceived themselves as overweight, regardless of their actual body weight at the baseline, were 2.5 times more likely to gain weight after six years as compared to those who did not perceive themselves as overweight, adjusting for depression, physical activity, and dieting behaviors [13]. Additionally, longitudinal studies of adult samples in the US and UK have found that perceiving oneself as overweight led to weight gains among normal and overweight participants (0.3–0.8 BMI points from baseline to follow-up) [9]. These studies were focused mainly on measuring BMI rather than body fat and waist circumference, which are important indicators of health risks rather than BMI alone. Additionally, none of these studies considered the effect of the longitudinal association of adolescent body weight underestimation and overestimation with body fat in adulthood. Thus, investigating the impact of body weight misclassification on weight gain, especially during the adolescent period, may contribute to efforts to control excess weight gain in adulthood.
Although weight misclassification has been reported to be associated with body weight, it is unclear if misclassification may lead to a gain or decrease in body weight or weight-control behaviors. Moreover, there are several factors that may have significant associations with weight misclassification, such as racial/ethnic disparities [23], depression [24], unhealthy weight-control practices [3], physical activity [25], parental body weight status, and education level [6]. Thus, understanding the association between weight misclassification and its related factors, particularly during the early stages of one’s life course, may contribute to a better understanding of further physical and psychological health outcomes during adulthood.
While it is established that body weight misclassification affects a significant proportion of adolescents, very little is known about the longitudinal impact of adolescent weight misclassification on increases in body weight (i.e., body fat and waist circumference) in adulthood, taking into account associated predictors of weight misclassification and body weight. We hypothesize that adolescents who misclassify their body weight, particularly those who underestimate their body weight, will experience greater body fat as compared to adolescents who correctly classify their body weight. Using data from a large longitudinal Australian study, we aimed to examine the longitudinal association between adolescent body weight misclassification and body fat and waist circumference during young adulthood. The findings of this longitudinal study may guide policymakers and health professionals to develop a strategy to reduce weight misclassification in adolescence and childhood so as to prevent excess weight gain in adulthood.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The data were from the Mater-University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy (MUSP) and its outcomes, which was a prospective study involving mothers and their offspring. Sequential offspring who were delivered as a live singleton child and who were not adopted out before leaving the Mater Misericordiae Hospital in Brisbane, Australia, between 1981 and 1983 were selected for this study. The cohort consisted of 7223 children who were followed up prospectively for 30 years. The mothers provided details of themselves and their children at child delivery; at 3–5 days post-delivery; and at 6-month, 5-year, and 14-year follow-ups. After this, the children responded to their own questionnaires at 21-year and 30-year follow-ups. More details of this cohort study have appeared elsewhere [26,27].
The analytical sample was limited to 1002 offspring for whom their perceived and measured body weight data at the 14-year follow-up and body fat and WC data at the 30-year follow-up were available (Figure 1). Those who were lost to follow-up were more likely to be from Asian or Aboriginal/Islander backgrounds; from a low socioeconomic status background; or those with a mother who was less educated, consumed tobacco or alcohol during pregnancy, or had poor mental health [26,28].

2.2. Measurements

Outcome (at 30-year follow-up)
Body fat and waist circumference were measured by trained research staff. Body fat was measured using the Tanita body composition monitor, which uses validated bioelectric impedance analysis technology [29]. WC was measured horizontally using a non-elastic disposable tape measure roughly in line with the umbilicus level of the participant and above the iliac crest. The tape measure was in a straight position parallel to the floor and held against the subject’s abdominal skin without compressing the skin. Two or more measures were taken at the end of the expiration phase. The averages of these measures were calculated and recorded [30,31].
Exposure (at 14-year follow-up)
The participant’s weight was recorded by taking the average of two measures with a 5-min interval on a scale accurate to 0.2 kg, with the participants in light clothes. Height was measured for each participant without shoes using a portable stadiometer. To categorize overweight and obesity, Cole et al.’s international survey standard definition for overweight and obesity was used to define the BMI cut-off values for the overweight and obese categories [32]. To categorize underweight and normal weight, participants whose BMI fell under the 10th percentile were classified as underweight; the others were classified as having normal weight.
Weight perception was obtained from the adolescents’ questionnaires [26]. The adolescents were asked: “do you think of yourself as” with the following response options: “very underweight,” “underweight” “about the right weight,” “overweight,” or “very overweight.” The very underweight was merged into the underweight category, and the very overweight was merged into the overweight category. A new variable (BMI-perceived weight) was created using these merged categories (of underweight and overweight).
Weight misclassification was defined by comparing the weight perception categories with the BMI-measured weight categories. The participants who correctly estimated their perceived body weight were categorized as the “correct estimation” group. The underestimation group contained participants whose weight perception was less than their BMI-measured weight category, while the overestimation group contained participants whose weight perception was recorded as higher than their BMI-measured weight category. Weight misclassification was categorized into three levels: correct, underestimation, and overestimation.
Confounders (from the first phase to the 14-year follow-up)
The main confounders were selected based on published studies or previous knowledge regarding their association with weight misclassification or body composition [3,5,13,14,15,33,34]. These factors were listed according to the phases of this study.
For the first phase of this study: Maternal information, including maternal height and weight, maternal education and age, and the ethnicity of the parents, was obtained from the first clinical visit and maternal obstetrician records. Maternal BMI cut-off values were defined according to the standard definition of BMI [35]. Maternal education was classified according to the mother’s completion of high school (“incomplete high school,” “complete high school,” and “post-high school”), and parental ethnic origin was classified as “White,” “Asian,” and “Aboriginal Islander.”
At the 5-year follow-up, the children’s weight and height were measured in a manner similar to that of the 14-year follow-up BMI measurement methods. The average of two measures was taken when the participants were lightly clothed, on a scale accurate to the nearest 0.2 kg, to measure their body weight, while height was measured using a portable stadiometer. Additionally, BMI cut-off values were identified based on Cole et al.’s international survey definition of overweight and obesity, while participants who were under 10% of the BMI percentile were classified in the underweight category. Too few subjects remained in the obese and underweight categories; thus, they were merged into the overweight and normal categories, respectively.
At the 14-year follow-up, several measures were obtained from the adolescents’ questionnaires. These factors included “how often do you go on a diet to lose weight?” with the following response options: “always or often” and “rarely or never”; “how many days did you do leisure physical exercise?” with the following response options: “0–1”, “2–3”, “4–5”, and “6–7 days”; and “time spent on watching television a week,” with the following response options: “less than 3 h” and “more than 3 h”. Additionally, pubertal development was divided into five stages according to the Tanner Scale [36,37]. For meaningful analysis, the first two stages were merged into one category, as were the last three stages. Additionally, mental health factors, which include internalizing behaviors (depression and anxiety) and externalizing behaviors (delinquent and aggressive behaviors), were identified using Achenbach’s Youth Self Report (YSR) [38]. The adolescents’ responses to their behavioral and psychological questionnaire items were summed up using the YSR scale, and the subjects who remained in the top 90th percentile of the scores were considered a “case.”

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Regarding the analyses of missing values, the participants were divided into two groups as follows: those who were not lost at the baseline (14-year follow-up) and follow-up (30-year follow-up) periods; and those who were not lost only at the baseline period. We calculated the mean difference in body fat percentages and WC among these groups. We used the Chi-square test (χ2) and a p-value of <0.05 to perform bivariate analyses of these groups with confounders.
The BMI-perceived weight categories were matched with the BMI-measured weight categories to determine the occurrence of weight misclassifications. The mean and standard deviation values of the body fat percentages and WC among weight misclassification categories were calculated. The statistical significance of the results was reported using a two-tailed p-value where a value <0.05 was used to determine the significance level.
A multiple linear regression model was used to calculate the differences in mean and significance between the exposure (weight misclassification as an independent variable) and the outcome (body fat percentages and WC as dependent variables). Confidence intervals were used to guide the assessment of statistical significance, with the “correct estimation” group being used as the reference group. This analysis was then repeated after we included potential confounders. In this adjusted analysis, BMI at the 5-year follow-up and maternal BMI were initially involved in the model, and the rest of the factors were included in the final model. The missing values of the confounders were treated using multiple imputations. Each potential confounder was imputed before including it in the analysis.
SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was used to perform all the calculations.

3. Results

There were a total of 1620 participants for whom WC and body fat percentage at the 30-year follow-up were available. From these, the final sample comprised 1002 participants (43% male) in which the BMI-perceived and BMI-measured weight were also available at the 14-year follow-up. We compared the characteristics of the participants who were not lost to follow-up at the 14-year follow-up against those who remained at both the 14-year and 30-year follow-ups. None of the comparisons were statistically significant (see Supplementary Tables S1 and S2).
Table 1 shows the mean and standard deviation values of body fat percentages and WC of both genders at the 30-year follow-up, stratified by weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up. The mean values of body fat percentages and WC among males and females appeared to be higher in the underestimation group as compared to the correct estimation group. In the underestimation group, the mean of the body fat percentages of the males was 24.1% (SD: 8.5), which was higher than the mean of 20.3% (SD: 7.1) in the correct estimation group, while the mean of the WC of the males was 98.5 cm in the underestimation group (SD: 16.6), which was higher than the mean of 92.9 cm (SD: 12.3) in the correct estimation group. Similarly, among the females who underestimated their body weight, the mean body fat percentage was 37.7% (SD: 8.9), which was higher than the mean of 34.4% (SD: 8.7) in the correct estimation group, while the mean of the WC was 92.1 cm (SD: 17), which was higher than the mean of 85.9 cm (SD: 14.8) in the correct estimation group. The previous differences in the mean values of the body fat percentages and WC among those who underestimated their body weight as compared to those who correctly estimated their body weight was statistically significant (Table 2). In the overestimation analyses, while the mean of the body fat percentages and WC among the males appeared to be relatively higher as compared to the correct estimation group, the mean among the females appeared to be relatively lower as compared to the correct estimation group.
Table 2 shows the association between weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up and body fat percentages at the 30-year follow-up for the males and females. The results are presented in mean differences with a 95% confidence interval (95% CI) across the weight misclassification categories considering the correct estimation group as the reference group. Among the males, the mean difference of the body fat percentages in the underestimation group as compared to the correct estimation group was statistically significant, as they were in the overestimation group as compared to the correct estimation group in both the non-adjusted and adjusted analyses. However, the magnitude of the mean difference between the underestimation and correct estimation groups was relatively higher (3.86%; 95% CI: 2.14, 5.58) than that of the overestimation and correct estimation groups (2.59%; 95% CI: 0.61, 4.68). Among the females, the mean difference in the body fat percentage was higher (3.33%; 95% CI: 1.33, 5.34) among the underestimation group as compared to the correct estimation group. In the overestimation group, the mean difference was not significant. The respondents who underestimated their body weight at 14 years had a substantially higher level of body fat at the 30-year follow-up.
Similar to Table 2, Table 3 shows the association of the differences between the mean values of the WC across the weight misclassification categories for males and females. Similar results to those in the body fat percentage analysis appeared in the WC analyses. In both genders, the mean difference of the WC was significant among the underestimation group as compared to the correct estimation group in the adjustment comparison. However, among the overestimation group, for the females, the mean difference was not statistically significant as compared to the correct estimation group.

4. Discussion

The current study is the first to examine the longitudinal association between body weight misclassification during the adolescent period and body fat and waist circumference during adulthood. We found that both male and female adolescents who underestimated their body weight had increased body fat levels and increased waist circumference when they were adults as compared to those who correctly estimated their body weight. We also found that adolescent males who overestimated their body weight showed an increase in body fat and waist circumference as compared to those who correctly estimated their body weight, but to a lesser degree. The adjustment of potential confounders did not substantially alter these associations. The findings of this study suggest that adolescents who misclassify their body weight may experience higher body fat and a higher WC in adulthood.
Overall, the findings of this study are consistent with those of a longitudinal study that examined the association between adolescent weight misclassification and waist circumference [12].
Previous research has indicated that those who underestimate their body weight are less likely to engage in weight-losing behaviors as compared with those who correctly estimate their body weight [3,14,15,16,18,19]. Several studies have found that body weight underestimation might be an obstacle to attempts to maintain or lose weight or to maintain a healthy diet to lose weight, especially among overweight and obese individuals [14,39]. Many individuals may not accept that they are at risk of being overweight or obese. Body weight underestimation might reflect an underlying attitude or belief that influences obesity-related lifestyles. Body weight underestimation could hinder efforts to take steps toward maintaining a healthy weight or losing harmful weight. On the other hand, body weight overestimation has been reported to be associated with going on a diet to lose weight and weight-losing behaviors; however, people who overestimated their body weight have also been reported to be prone to adopting unhealthy weight-control behaviors, such as extreme diets to lose weight or taking drugs such as diet pills or laxatives [40,41]. Body weight overestimation might lead to increased body weight over time, as weight-control behaviors used by those who overestimate their weight might be ineffective and lead to weight gain in the longer term. The findings of this study suggest that, after controlling for related confounders, the adolescent males who overestimated their body weight at the 14-year follow-up had a higher waist circumference and more body fat in their adulthood as compared to those who correctly estimated their body weight.
This study has the advantage of having data available for assessing the longitudinal association between adolescent body weight misclassification and the development of body fat and WC increases during adulthood, taking into account early-life predictors. Most of the research in this area has been cross-sectional or has not involved early-life predictors. Additionally, this study used a large representative sample that covered the adolescent and early adult periods, which are significant life stages for studying obesity and body weight misclassification. The main limitations of this study are the missing values, which may have affected the magnitude of our findings of body fat percentages and WC. However, in our baseline characteristics analysis, there was no significant difference when we compared the main characteristics of those who were not lost to follow-up at the baseline of this study with those who remained at both baseline and follow-up. Additionally, the findings did not significantly change when we applied multiple imputations to the predictors. Moreover, previous studies that used MUSP data showed that missing data generally did not have a significant impact on their findings, even after applying multiple imputations and inverse probability weighting for the missing data [26,27,28,33].
Further research is needed to better understand the role of body weight underestimation and overestimation in body weight fluctuations over the life course, along with other related factors associated with weight misclassification. While obesity during childhood predicts obesity during adulthood [42], our previous research found that the effect of body weight misclassification may persist from the adolescent period into adulthood. Body weight misclassification has been consistently reported to be associated with healthy weight management and attempts to lose weight [3,14,15,16,18,19]. Weight misclassification, especially body weight underestimation, during childhood, might contribute to weight gain in adulthood. Furthermore, parents’ perception of their children’s weight status might have an impact on children’s body weight, as parents usually facilitate children’s access to physical activity and influence what their children eat. Parents who underestimate their children’s body weight may be less likely to help their children to engage in weight-losing programs [43,44,45,46] as compared to parents who correctly estimate their children’s body weight. Children who live with overweight or obese parents or interact with overweight or obese schoolmates have been reported to experience more body weight misclassifications as compared to those who do not [8]. Clinical and school-based BMI screening might be useful in delivering a positive message in regard to one’s body weight and might encourage steps that can be taken to maintain a healthy weight, as well as help inform parents about their children’s weight status. These factors, along with body image distortion; body weight stigma; unhealthy diets; and other related psychosocial, environmental, and cultural factors, should be considered when studying the longitudinal association between weight misclassification and body weight. There is currently little known about the impact of interventions and the health benefits of maintaining a correct body weight perception. This is a gap in the knowledge that needs to be rectified.

5. Conclusions

In this paper, we report a longitudinal association between body weight misclassification during the adolescent period and the development of body fat and waist circumference during adulthood. We also found that there are early-life factors that might have an impact on such an association. Overall, the adolescent males and females who underestimated their body weight had a significant increase in their body fat and waist circumference as compared to those who correctly estimated their body characteristics. While the adolescent males who overestimated their body weight experienced an increase in their body fat and WC, the adolescent females who overestimated their body weight had a non-significant decrease in both body fat and waist circumference in adulthood. There is a case here for considering the development of educational programs that encourage adolescents and young adults to assess their body weight more accurately. In so doing, we may reduce the prevalence of weight misclassification and increase correct estimations of weight among adolescents, and also reduce the risk of excess weight and body fat in adulthood. In addition, increasing awareness of one’s own body weight status needs to be taken into consideration when addressing unhealthy weight-control behaviors, which may hinder tackling excess body weight and body fat [47,48]. In all of these policy options, there is a need to balance the concerns of adolescents regarding their body image and the need to accurately assess body weight.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14224765/s1, Table S1: Mean and mean difference of baseline characteristic between participants who were available at 14-year follow up and those who remained at 14-year and 30-year follow-ups. Table S2: Comparing baseline characteristics of participants who were available at 14-year follow up and those who remained at 14-year and 30-year follow-ups.

Author Contributions

A.D.A. and A.A.M. conceived the idea. A.D.A. performed the analyses and drafted the manuscript. J.M.N. designed the study. A.A.M. and J.M.N. guided the explanation of the results. All the authors critically edited the manuscript. All the authors have full access to the data in this study and submitted the article under their responsibility. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no specific grants from any commercial funding agency. The study was funded by government-awarded competitive research grants from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council and the Australian Research Council.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The research proposal and all the procedures involving research study participants were approved by the The University of Queensland ethical clearance; Mater Misericordiae Children’s Hospital ethical clearance; The University of Queensland ethical clearance; Mater Misericordiae Children’s Hospital ethical clearance; The University of Queensland ethical clearance; B/88/Anth/94/NHMRC; 8408436; B/660/SS/01/NHMRC; 505A; 1525A; Human Ethics Review Committee at the Hospital and/or The University of Queensland for each follow-up of this study using ethical guidelines that are consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki statement. The mothers provided signed informed consent for their participation and that of their offspring for each phase of this study up to the 14-year follow-up. Afterward, separate consent was obtained from the mothers and from their offspring for each consecutive follow-up.

Informed Consent Statement

Up to the 14-year follow-up, written informed consent was obtained from the mothers for their participation and for their children’s information, while the offspring gave their own informed consent at the 30-year follow-up.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data and analyses conducted in this study are under restrictions due to the confidentiality requirements by ethical approval committee but can be available upon receiving a plausible request and approval from the ethical committee.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to all the MUSP participants and the MUSP team who collected the data at each follow-up. Greg Shuttlewood helped with data management. The MUSP study was funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC). The views and suggestions expressed in this paper are the author’s own and do not necessarily reflect the funders’ views or opinions, and none of the research funding bodies influenced the data analysis or presentation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest to disclose.

References

  1. Bucchianeri, M.M.; Arikian, A.J.; Hannan, P.J.; Eisenberg, M.E.; Neumark-Sztainer, D. Body Dissatisfaction from Adolescence to Young Adulthood: Findings from a 10-Year Longitudinal Study. Body Image 2013, 10, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Wharton, C.M.; Adams, T.; Hampl, J.S. Weight loss practices and body weight perceptions among US College students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2008, 56, 579–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ibrahim, C.; El-Kamary, S.S.; Bailey, J.; St George, D.M. Inaccurate weight perception is associated with extreme weight-management practices in U.S. high school students. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2014, 58, 368–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Datar, A.; Chung, P.J. Accuracy of Weight Perceptions in a Nationally Representative Cohort of US 8th Grade Adolescents. Acad. Pediatr. 2016, 16, 267–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Aloufi, A.D.; Najman, J.M.; Mamun, A.A. Predictors of adolescents’ weight misclassification: A longitudinal study. Obes. Res. Clin. Pract. 2017, 11, 576–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Shin, A.; Nam, C.M. Weight perception and its association with socio-demographic and health-related factors among Korean adolescents. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Linder, J.; McLaren, L.; Siou, G.L.; Csizmadi, I.; Robson, P.J. The epidemiology of weight perception: Perceived versus self-reported actual weight status among Albertan adults. Can. J. Public Health 2010, 101, 56–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Maximova, K.; McGrath, J.J.; Barnett, T.; O’Loughlin, J.; Paradis, G.; Lambert, M. Do you see what I see? Weight status misperception and exposure to obesity among children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, 1008–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Robinson, E.; Hunger, J.M.; Daly, M. Perceived weight status and risk of weight gain across life in US and UK adults. Int. J. Obes. 2015, 39, 1721–1726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Sonneville, K.R.; Thurston, I.B.; Milliren, C.E.; Kamody, R.C.; Gooding, H.C.; Richmond, T.K. Helpful or harmful? Prospective association between weight misperception and weight gain among overweight and obese adolescents and young adults. Int. J. Obes. 2015, 40, 328–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Sutin, A.R.; Terracciano, A. Body weight misperception in adolescence and incident obesity in young adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 507–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Cuypers, K.; Kvaloy, K.; Bratberg, G.; Midthjell, K.; Holmen, J.; Holmen, T.L. Being Normal Weight but Feeling Overweight in Adolescence May Affect Weight Development into Young Adulthood-An 11-Year Followup: The HUNT Study, Norway. J. Obes. 2012, 2012, 601872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Duong, H.T.; Roberts, R.E. Perceived weight in youths and risk of overweight or obesity six years later. J. Psychosom. Res. 2014, 76, 23–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Chung, A.E.; Perrin, E.M.; Skinner, A.C. Accuracy of child and adolescent weight perceptions and their relationships to dieting and exercise behaviors: A NHANES study. Acad. Pediatr. 2013, 13, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Yan, A.F.; Zhang, G.; Wang, M.Q.; Stoesen, C.A.; Harris, B.M. Weight perception and weight control practice in a multiethnic sample of US adolescents. South. Med. J. 2009, 102, 354–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Matthiessen, J.; Biltoft-Jensen, A.; Fagt, S.; Knudsen, V.K.; Tetens, I.; Groth, M.V. Misperception of body weight among overweight Danish adults: Trends from 1995 to 2008. Public Health Nutr. 2014, 17, 1439–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Duncan, D.T.; Wolin, K.Y.; Scharoun-Lee, M.; Ding, E.L.; Warner, E.T.; Bennett, G.G. Does perception equal reality? Weight misperception in relation to weight-related attitudes and behaviors among overweight and obese US adults. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011, 8, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Yaemsiri, S.; Slining, M.M.; Agarwal, S.K. Perceived weight status, overweight diagnosis, and weight control among US adults: The NHANES 2003–2008 Study. Int. J. Obes. 2011, 35, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Edwards, N.M.; Pettingell, S.; Borowsky, I.W. Where perception meets reality: Self-perception of weight in overweight adolescents. Pediatrics 2010, 125, e452–e458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Haynes, A.; Kersbergen, I.; Sutin, A.; Daly, M.; Robinson, E. A systematic review of the relationship between weight status perceptions and weight loss attempts, strategies, behaviours and outcomes. Obes. Rev. 2018, 19, 347–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Isomaa, R.; Isomaa, A.L.; Marttunen, M.; Kaltiala-Heino, R.; Bjorkqvist, K. Longitudinal concomitants of incorrect weight perception in female and male adolescents. Body Image 2011, 8, 58–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Jones, M.M. Psychological and behavioral correlates of excess weight: Misperception of obese status among persons with Class II obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2010, 43, 628–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Richmond, T.K.; Thurston, I.; Sonneville, K.; Milliren, C.E.; Walls, C.E.; Austin, S.B. Racial/ethnic differences in accuracy of body mass index reporting in a diverse cohort of young adults. Int. J. Obes. 2015, 39, 546–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Roberts, R.E.; Duong, H.T. Perceived weight, not obesity, increases risk for major depression among adolescents. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 1110–1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Patte, K.A.; Laxer, R.E.; Qian, W.; Leatherdale, S.T. An analysis of weight perception and physical activity and dietary behaviours among youth in the COMPASS study. SSM-Popul. Health 2016, 2, 841–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Najman, J.; Bor, W.; O’Callaghan, M.; Williams, G.; Aird, R.; Shuttlewood, G. Cohort Profile: The Mater-University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy (MUSP). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 34, 992–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Najman, J.M.; Alati, R.; Bor, W.; Clavarino, A.; Mamun, A.; McGrath, J.J.; McIntyre, D.; O’Callaghan, M.; Scott, J.; Shuttlewood, G.; et al. Cohort Profile Update: The Mater-University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy (MUSP). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2015, 44, 78–78f. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ware, R.S.; Williams, G.M.; Aird, R.L. Participants Who Left a Multiple-Wave Cohort Study Had Similar Baseline Characteristics to Participants Who Returned. Ann. Epidemiol. 2006, 16, 820–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Kushner, R.F. Bioelectrical impedance analysis: A review of principles and applications. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 1992, 11, 199–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. World Health Organization. Waist Circumference and Waist–Hip Ratio; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  31. National Institutes of Health NH, Lung, and Blood Institute; NHLBI Obesity Education Initiative; North American Association for the Study of Obesity. The Practical Guide: Identification, Evaluation and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults; Bethesda: Rockville, MD, USA, 2000.
  32. Cole, T.J.; Bellizzi, M.C.; Flegal, K.M.; Dietz, W.H. Establishing A Standard Definition For Child Overweight And Obesity Worldwide: International Survey. BMJ Br. Med. J. 2000, 320, 1240–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Mamun, A.A.; McDermott, B.M.; O’Callaghan, M.J.; Najman, J.M.; Williams, G.M. Predictors of maternal misclassifications of their offspring’s weight status: A longitudinal study. Int. J. Obes. 2007, 32, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Choi, J.; Bender, M.S.; Arai, S.; Fukuoka, Y. Factors Associated with Underestimation of Weight Status among Caucasian, Latino, Filipino, and Korean Americans—DiLH Survey. Ethn. Dis. 2015, 25, 200–207. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. National Institutes of Health; National Heart Lung and Blood Institute. Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults--The Evidence Report. Obes. Res. 1998, 6 (Suppl. S2), 51s–209s. [Google Scholar]
  36. Morris, N.M.; Udry, J.R. Validation of a self-administered instrument to assess stage of adolescent development. J. Youth Adolesc. 1980, 9, 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tanner, J.M. Growth at Adolescence, 2nd ed.; Thomas: Springfield, IL, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
  38. Achenbach, T.M. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile; Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont: Burlington, VT, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  39. Quick, V.; Nansel, T.R.; Liu, D.; Lipsky, L.M.; Due, P.; Iannotti, R.J. Body size perception and weight control in youth: 9-year international trends from 24 countries. Int. J. Obes. 2014, 38, 988–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Talamayan, K.S.; Springer, A.E.; Kelder, S.H.; Gorospe, E.C.; Joye, K.A. Prevalence of overweight misperception and weight control behaviors among normal weight adolescents in the United States. Sci. World J. 2006, 6, 365–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Ursoniu, S.; Putnoky, S.; Vlaicu, B. Body weight perception among high school students and its influence on weight management behaviors in normal weight students: A cross-sectional study. Wien. Klin. Wochenschr. 2011, 123, 327–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Simmonds, M.; Llewellyn, A.; Owen, C.G.; Woolacott, N. Predicting adult obesity from childhood obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2016, 17, 95–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Rivera-Soto, W.T.; Rodriguez-Figueroa, L. Childhood obesity among Puerto Rican children: Discrepancies between child’s and parent’s perception of weight status. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1427–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Rietmeijer-Mentink, M.; Paulis, W.D.; van Middelkoop, M.; Bindels, P.J.E.; van der Wouden, J.C. Difference between parental perception and actual weight status of children: A systematic review. Matern. Child Nutr. 2013, 9, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Vallejo, M.; Cortes-Rodríguez, B.A.; Colin-Ramirez, E. Maternal Underestimation of Child’s Weight Status and Health Behaviors as Risk Factors for Overweight in Children. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 2015, 30, e29–e33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Warschburger, P.; Kröller, K. Childhood overweight and obesity: Maternal perceptions of the time for engaging in child weight management. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Schneider, B.C.; Dumith, S.C.; Orlandi, S.P.; Assunção, M.C.F. Diet and body fat in adolescence and early adulthood: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Cienc. Saude Coletiva 2017, 22, 1539–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Al-Jawaldeh, A.; Abbass, M.M.S. Unhealthy Dietary Habits and Obesity: The Major Risk Factors Beyond Non-Communicable Diseases in the Eastern Mediterranean Region. Front. Nutr. 2022, 9, 817808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the MUSP cohort, body mass index, weight perception, and the analytical sample.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the MUSP cohort, body mass index, weight perception, and the analytical sample.
Nutrients 14 04765 g001
Table 1. Distribution of males’ and females’ body fat percentages and waist circumference at 30-year follow-up by body weight misclassifications at 14-year follow-up.
Table 1. Distribution of males’ and females’ body fat percentages and waist circumference at 30-year follow-up by body weight misclassifications at 14-year follow-up.
Misclassification at 14-Years F/UBody Fat % at 30-Years F/UWC at 30-Years F/U
MaleFemaleMaleFemale
NMeanSDNMeanSDNMeanSDNMeanSD
Correct estimation26320.267.1031534.388.7426392.8612.2731585.9114.83
Underestimation10424.128.509737.728.9210498.5116.589792.1516.97
Overestimation6022.866.8516333.528.636096.1211.2116385.1013.31
Abbreviations: WC: waist circumference; F/U: follow-up.
Table 2. Mean difference and 95% confidence interval of body weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up with body fat percentages at the 30-year follow-up.
Table 2. Mean difference and 95% confidence interval of body weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up with body fat percentages at the 30-year follow-up.
Misclassification at 14 YearsNo.Body Fat % at 30 Years
Unadjusted Mean Diff (95% CI)Adjusted Mean Diff * (95% CI)Adjusted Mean Diff ** (95% CI)
Male
Correct estimation263000
Underestimation 1043.86 (2.14, 5.58)3.40 (1.66, 5.15)3.16 (1.39, 4.93)
Overestimation 602.59 (0.61, 4.68)2.74 (0.71, 4.77)2.68 (0.56, 4.80)
Female
Correct estimation315000
Underestimation 973.33 (1.33, 5.34)2.85 (0.80, 4.90)2.66 (0.56, 4.76)
Overestimation 163−0.86 (−2.51, 0.79)−0.89 (−2.55, 0.77)−1.13 (−2.77, 0.52)
* Adjusted for maternal body mass index; ** adjusted for maternal body mass index, maternal age, and maternal education; at 14-year follow-up: dieting, physical activity, race, puberty development, internal and external behaviors (YSR), and TV watching.
Table 3. Mean difference and 95% confidence interval of body weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up with waist circumference at the 30-year follow-up.
Table 3. Mean difference and 95% confidence interval of body weight misclassification at the 14-year follow-up with waist circumference at the 30-year follow-up.
Misclassification at 14 YearsNo.Waist Circumference at 30 Years
Unadjusted Mean Diff (95% CI)Adjusted Mean Diff * (95% CI)Adjusted Mean Diff ** (95% CI)
Male
Correct estimation263000
Underestimation 1043.86 (2.15, 5.58)3.40 (1.66, 5.15)3.16 (1.40, 4.93)
Overestimation 603.26 (−0.14, 6.66)3.57 (0.13, 7.02)3.89 (0.27, 7.50)
Female
Correct estimation315000
Underestimation 976.23 (2.73, 9.74)5.10 (1.61, 8.58)4.35 (0.79, 7.91)
Overestimation 163−0.82 (−3.53, 1.90)−0.75 (−3.42, 1.93)−0.92 (−3.55, 1.72)
* Adjusted for maternal body mass index; ** adjusted for maternal body mass index, maternal age, and maternal education; at 14-year follow-up: dieting, physical activity, race, puberty development, internal and external behaviors (YSR), and TV watching.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Aloufi, A.D.; Najman, J.M.; Mamun, A.A. The Association between Body Weight Misclassification in Adolescence and Body Fat and Waist Circumference in Adulthood: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4765. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14224765

AMA Style

Aloufi AD, Najman JM, Mamun AA. The Association between Body Weight Misclassification in Adolescence and Body Fat and Waist Circumference in Adulthood: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients. 2022; 14(22):4765. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14224765

Chicago/Turabian Style

Aloufi, Abdulaziz D., Jake M. Najman, and Abdullah A. Mamun. 2022. "The Association between Body Weight Misclassification in Adolescence and Body Fat and Waist Circumference in Adulthood: A Longitudinal Study" Nutrients 14, no. 22: 4765. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14224765

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop