Next Article in Journal
Dietary Sources of Salt in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: A Systematic Literature Review
Previous Article in Journal
The Influence of Bullying and Cyberbullying in the Psychological Adjustment of Victims and Aggressors in Adolescence
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Scoping Review Mapping Research on Green Space and Associated Mental Health Benefits

by
Charlotte Wendelboe-Nelson
1,*,
Sarah Kelly
2,
Marion Kennedy
2 and
John W. Cherrie
1,3
1
Institute of Biological Chemistry, Biophysics and Bioengineering, Heriot Watt University, Edinburgh EH14 4AS, UK
2
Information Services, Heriot Watt University, Edinburgh EH14 4AS, UK
3
Centre for Human Exposure Science, Institute of Occupational Medicine, Research Avenue North, Edinburgh EH14 4AP, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16(12), 2081; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122081
Submission received: 5 April 2019 / Revised: 1 June 2019 / Accepted: 7 June 2019 / Published: 12 June 2019
(This article belongs to the Section Environmental Health)

Abstract

:
Background: There is a growing interest in research investigating the association between green space (GS) and mental health and wellbeing (HWB), in order to understand the underlying mechanisms. Accordingly, there is a need to map the literature and create an overview of the research. Methods: A scoping review approach was used to map literature on GS, including context and co-exposures (the GS exposome), and their associations with mental HWB. The review considers mental HWB definitions and measurements and how GS is characterized. Furthermore, the review aims to identify knowledge gaps and make recommendations for future research. Results: We identified a great diversity in study designs, definitions, outcome measures, consideration of the totality of the GS exposome, and reporting of results. Around 70% of the 263 reviewed studies reported a positive association between some aspect of GS and HWB. However, there is a limited amount of research using randomized controlled crossover trails (RCTs) and mixed methods and an abundance of qualitative subjective research. Conclusions: The discords between study designs, definitions, and the reporting of results makes it difficult to aggregate the evidence and identify any potential causal mechanisms. We propose key points to consider when defining and quantifying GS and make recommendations for reporting on research investigating GS and mental HWB. This review highlights a need for large well-designed RCTs that reliably measure the GS exposome in relation to mental HWB.

1. Introduction

Several reviews have highlighted the positive association between green space (GS) and mental health and wellbeing (HWB). These reviews have generally focused on GS in a narrow sense, such as forest therapy [1,2], community GS [3,4], or urban GS [5,6,7], and a number of reviews have looked at GS in relation to urbanicity and urban planning [8,9]. Other reviews have focused on specific GS activities, such as community gardening [10], horticultural therapy [11,12], therapeutic gardening for the elderly, [13], spending time in a forest [2,14], and GS in the living environment [15]. Reviews have also explored the connections between biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and wellbeing [16,17,18]. The reviews generally identify positive associations between the narrowly defined GS investigated and measures of mental HWB.
Design of and access to GS is particularly relevant in cities where GS, among other social and environmental factors, is under pressure due to urbanization [19]. It is estimated that by 2050, more than two-thirds of the world’s population will live in urban areas. This has led to a large number of research studies with a focus on mental HWB and access to urban GS. Urbanization is associated with increased levels of mental illness, including anxiety and depression [20,21,22]. Access to urban GS has been positively associated with mental HWB [23,24], but the underlying reasons for this are still not well-understood.
GS has also been shown to be associated with mental HWB in rural areas [25,26,27,28]. When Gilbert, Colley and Roberts [29] investigated subjective wellbeing in rural areas of Scotland, they found that residents living in remote rural areas reported higher levels of life satisfaction compared with non-rural areas. Other studies investigating associations between mental HWB and GS in rural areas have found a significant relationship with rurality [30,31].
Botanical gardens have been proposed as alternative ways to stay in touch with nature [32,33,34]. A number of studies have shown a positive relation between garden/horticultural therapy and a number of psychological issues, e.g., stress management [35,36,37,38], treatment of depression [39,40,41], rehabilitation of prison inmates [42], and wellbeing among elders [43,44,45].
There is increasing interest in understanding the factors that may make GS beneficial for HWB [46]. However, most reviews do not consider contextual factors, such as culture and accessibility, or co-exposures, such as sound and light. The developing concept of the exposome [47] encompasses the totality of exposures we face as humans, from conception onwards, and the combined effect of these exposures on HWB. An exposome approach to investigating GS could help us understand exactly what is beneficial for mental HWB.
We have carried out a scoping study to map the available literature on different types of GS, including the context and co-exposures, and their associations with mental HWB, considering how mental HWB is defined and measured and how GS is characterized. Furthermore, the review aims to identify any current knowledge gaps and make recommendations for future research on the subject.

2. Materials and Methods

A five-step scoping review methodology was used to collect, evaluate, and present the analysed literature [48]:
  • Identifying the research question(s);
  • Identifying relevant studies;
  • Study selection;
  • Charting the data;
  • Collating, summarizing, and reporting the results.
The following research questions (RQ) were used to underpin the search strategy:
  • How do different types of GS (recreational, residential, urban, rural) affect HWB and how much green space is needed for health improvement?
  • How can we best define, measure, and quantify GS and mental HWB?
  • Do different co-exposures or contextual factors affect the mental HWB outcome?
  • Do different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS?
Theoretical, empirical, and experimental studies were included, with a focus on links between GS of any description and mental HWB of any definition. To our knowledge, no review has attempted to map the totality of literature on GS and the associated effects on mental HWB. In this scoping review, we adopt a wide definition of GS and GS activities, including small urban pockets of GS, remote rural areas, horticultural therapy, allotment gardening, and virtual green space. This was done to try to shed light on the effect of contextual factors and co-exposures potentially influencing the effects of GS on mental HWB.
Studies with a main emphasis on biological diversity or physical activity, not including a detailed investigation of associated mental HWB outcomes, were excluded. Studies focusing on children under the age of 18 were excluded, as the mechanisms and contextual factors related to mental HWB may be different in children than in adults. In situations where the age range of participants included people under the age of 18, a decision to include or exclude the paper was based on each individual study, considering the contribution the study findings and conclusions would make to this review. Studies with an emphasis on GS in war or disaster zones were excluded, as these are extreme circumstances and not applicable to the general population. Studies with a focus on urban design, not investigating any associated mental HWB outcomes, were also excluded. Only peer-reviewed literature was included, and grey literature and all conference proceedings, abstracts, or opinion pieces were excluded. Keywords for two main concepts were generated and used for the literature search (Table 1).
Relevant studies and literature reviews from peer-reviewed journals were identified using Web of Science, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PubMed, Scopus, GreenFILE, and SPORTDiscus. Additionally, research evidence was sought from topic-related networks and relevant organizations, and reference lists of earlier key studies were used to detect relevant publications not identified in the original main search [48].
All papers were pooled and duplicates removed, resulting in a total of 7042 papers. The literature was initially screened by two members of the research team (CWN, JC), using a comparative and consensus orientated method (Figure 1). After exclusion based on the title and abstract, there were 417 papers for review. When applying the inclusion/exclusion criteria, another 173 papers were excluded. An additional 19 papers were included from the reference lists from key-papers, taking the total papers for review to 263.
The included literature was charted following the technique described by Arksey and O’Malley [47], to synthesize and interpret the studies by sorting them according to key issues and themes. Each study was analysed according to the type of GS investigated, health outcomes and measures, experimental design, and methods used. The quality of the included studies was not systematically assessed, so this review does not determine the robustness of findings from the included literature. The reviewed literature was then collated, summarized, and reported in four thematic groups (Table 2).
The literature was further divided into ‘type of study’ (cross-sectional or longitudinal, controlled trial, randomized or non-randomized, with or without crossover); ‘methods’ (what methods have been used to measure mental HWB and GS. Quantitative or qualitative data collection methods); ‘health outcome’ (the type of mental HWB assessed); and whether the study has reached a positive or negative conclusion (were initial hypothesis proven right or wrong). Comprehensive lists were generated, comprising all the different mental HWB outcomes investigated and all the different tools used to assess the health outcomes. This was done to get an overview of the totality and complexity of studies, and to identify the most commonly used methods for assessing mental HWB.

3. Results

3.1. Numerical Analysis

This analysis is used to highlight the dominant areas of research with respect to the study design, type of participants, methods used, main conclusions, and country where the study has been conducted. The papers were divided into groups based on the study design (Table 3). The majority of studies were cross-sectional (86.3%), with only 13 studies being longitudinal (4.9%). There were nine studies with a Randomized Controlled Trial (RCT) study design with a crossover element and 21 studies using an RCT study design without a crossover element.
The majority of studies used only qualitative methods 212 (80.6%), with only 29 studies using a combination of qualitative and quantitative data collection methods (11%). Twenty-two of the publications were reviews (8.4%).
Different countries will face different co-exposures and contextual factors, which may potentially affect the HWB outcomes in different ways (RQ 3). To understand the representation from around the world, the literature was charted according to the continent where the study took place (Table 4). The majority of studies were conducted in Europe (46.8%), followed by North America (24.3%), Asia (11%), and Australia (6.8%). Most of the studies conducted in Europe were from western and northern parts; the UK (38%), followed by Sweden (15.4%) and the Netherlands (6.5%). Based on Table 4, it is evident that a majority of studies have been carried out in the developed part of the world. The identified benefits of GS in developed countries may not be applicable to less developed countries. The same is the case between temperate and tropical areas, with most studies being carried out in the former.
Different population subgroups might benefit differently from exposure to GS (RQ 4). To investigate what population subgroups have typically been used to assess the effects of GS on mental HWB, the literature was sorted according to participant type (Table 5). For ease of overview, the different participant types have been grouped together where reasonable overlap and similarity was identified. The most common type of study participant was the general public (30% of all included studies), followed by university and college students (14.1%) and individuals with mental health issues and disorders (12.2%). There is a long list of studies that have used more specific participant types, i.e., park users, allotment gardeners, adults with burnout syndrome, depression, mental health issues, female prisoners, woodland workers, people building their own houses etc. Therefore, despite the majority of papers focusing on the general public, there is a great variety of specific population subgroups being investigated in relation to the health benefits of various types of GS exposure (Table 5).
Studies were charted as ‘positive’ if the hypotheses were confirmed, ‘negative’ if the main hypotheses were not confirmed, and ‘mixed’ if the hypotheses were only partly confirmed. Note that a study charted as ‘negative’ does not necessarily mean the study found a negative effect of GS exposure on mental HWB. Only 4.6% of studies were charted as negative (see e.g., [49,50]), 25.7% of studies were charted as ‘mixed’, and 70.1% of studies were charted as ‘positive’. It should be noted that a proportion of the studies report a positive finding in the abstract, but when investigating the results in more detail, we found that any mixed or negative findings were played down in the summary. The percentages presented here are based on the abstracts.

3.2. Thematic Analysis

The literature has been organized according to thematic groups to address research question 1. There were 22 literature reviews identified, which are not included in this thematic analysis. An in-depth evaluation of these is beyond the scope of this review.
Group 1 encompasses studies focusing on horticulture, gardens, and allotments (43 studies). Included in this group is garden or horticultural therapy, and access and use of private and public gardens. The literature in this group was further divided into seven categories: private gardens (4.6% of Group 1 studies), complex interventions (14%), allotments (18.6%), horticultural therapy (30.2%), occupation (4.7%), public gardens (18.6%), and community gardening (9.3%). Out of these 43 studies, only seven had a quantitative element [45,51,52,53,54,55,56]. A range of quantitative measurements were used, such as salivary alpha amylase (sAA) levels, an electrocardiogram (ECG), a surface electromyogram (sEMG), a respiration rate, body composition, physical functional ability, hand function ability, BMI, cortisol, sick leave status, and healthcare consumption.
Group 2 encompasses studies focusing on urban GS or mixed GS (140 studies). Included in this group was any GS located in an urban setting, and studies that used a mixture of GS types where it was not possible to assign the study to one of the other groups and where there was a main focus on urbanicity. This group is large and very diverse and for many of the studies, it was difficult to categorize and determine exactly what type of GS was being investigated, due to the lack of details used to describe the space. It was therefore not practical to further divide this group into subgroups in a meaningful way. Out of the 140 studies focusing on urban green space, 130 were cross-sectional and 10 were longitudinal, 125 studies used qualitative methods, and 15 used quantitative or mixed methods.
Group 3 encompasses wild, natural, and rural GS (34). This group includes GS types such as care farms; adventure therapy; rural neighborhoods; and wild nature like mountains, national parks, beaches, and large forests. Due to the diversity of the investigated GS, this group was further divided into eight subgroups: care farms (5.9%), forest GS (29.4%), natural green exercise (2.9%), nature connectedness and restorativeness (8.8%), nature interventions (17.6%), occupational (5.9%), rural communities (11.8%), and wild camping and adventures (17.6%). Out of these 34 studies, 32 were cross-sectional, one was longitudinal, and one study was a secondary narrative analysis. Qualitative data collection methods were used in 27 of the studies, with only seven of the studies using quantitative or mixed methods [1,57,58,59,60,61,62]. A range of objective quantitative data collection methods were used, such as cortisol measurements, cytokine serum levels, blood pressure, and heart rate variability.
The last group, Group 4 (24), includes virtual and indoor GS, e.g., photos, images, videos, and any type of GS enclosed under a roof. This group can be further subdivided into four groups: assessment by questionnaire only and no exposure to GS (50%), indoor GS exposure (8.3%), video of GS (4.2%), and images or photos of GS (37.5%). None of the studies in this group included a quantitative element; 22 studies relied on questionnaire data and two studies have used interviews. Out of the 24 studies, 23 were cross-sectional, with only one study being described as longitudinal [63]. Erikson, Westerberg and Jonsson [63] investigated a therapeutic gardening program taking place in a greenhouse; however, the longitudinal aspect of the study only stretched over three months.
The type of health outcome investigated varied greatly between the included studies. The total number of primary mental HWB outcomes observed and the number of times each outcome has been investigated were summarized (Table 6). Mental health (37), wellbeing (35), and stress (34) were the most used mental HWB outcomes. These were followed by restorativeness (22), depression (19), quality of life (13), psychological wellbeing (12), general health (11), and mental wellbeing (8). It is likely that some of these outcomes are intended to cover the same aspect of mental HWB. However, as a clear definition of the health outcome is rarely presented, it is not possible to confidently and accurately combine these outcomes and group them into fewer groups.
The number of tools used to measure mental HWB and the number of times each tool has been used are summarized in Table 7. Despite the availability of a vast range of validated tools developed to investigate mental HWB, the most common approach was to develop new questionnaires (DOQ; 15.8% of the studies). The most used validated questionnaire was PRS (7.9%), closely followed by PANAS (7.1%), PSS (6.6%), GHQ (6.2%), PS (5.8%), WEMWBS (5.4%), and HS SF-36 (4.1%), and the abbreviations are listed in Table 7.
Table 8 gives an overview of the different tools and what health endpoints the tools have been used to investigate. Some studies have not used tools such as questionnaires, surveys, scales, or inventories, but have instead used interviews, focus groups, observations, or similar methodologies. These studies are not included in Table 8. To measure the ten most used health endpoints (Table 6), the following tools have been most frequently used (Table 9): The GHQ was used in 15.4% of studies investigating mental health; 11.9% of studies developed their own questionnaires (DOQ) when investigating wellbeing; the PSS was used in 13.6% of studies investigating stress; the PRS was used in 32.4% of studies investigating restorativeness; 13.9% of studies used BDI when investigating depression; 16.7% of the studies developed their own questionnaires (DOQ) when investigating quality of life; 8.7% of studies DOQ when investigation psychological wellbeing; the GHQ was used in 17.6% of studies investigating general health; the WEMWBS was used in 21.4% of studies investigating mental wellbeing; and 75% of studies DOQ when investigating life satisfaction. Some studies have used several tools to measure one health endpoint.

3.3. Analysis of Study Design

When testing a research hypothesis, an RCT is the most scientifically rigorous method available [359]. In an RCT, the participants are randomly assigned to one of at least two groups; a design that specifically reduces selection bias and is often considered the gold standard for research designs, when considering the efficacy of different treatments compared to a control.
There were 30 RCTs identified; 11.4% of the total number of papers selected for review. Nine (30%) of these studies included a crossover element: eight had a 2-arm design and one study had a 4-arm design. Out of the 21 RCT without a crossover element, ten had a 2-arm design, seven had a 3-arm design, and four had a 4-arm design. Eight studies used a non-randomized Controlled Trial (CT), with a 2-arm design. Two of these studies used a crossover element, and six studies had no crossover element.
It is not always convenient or possible to introduce randomization. In their study, Sung and colleagues [61] evaluated the health effects of a forest therapy program using what they call a ‘convenient assignment’ and not true randomization, which considers the subjects’ preference and suitability to the intervention or the control group. Bang et al. [1] investigated the effects of a forest-walking program on physical and psychological health using a quasi-experimental design. The participants were assigned to the experimental or control group based on the participants’ preference, to boost motivation. Dewi et al. [52] also used a quasi-experimental design, investigating already existing community garden activities. Beute and de Kort [201] investigated if lower mental health makes an individual more or less responsive to the positive health effects of GS. Accordingly, the participants were not randomized, but split into groups based on their obtained score from the BDI-II, which was an appropriate design to answer their particular research question. Non-randomized study designs like these [1,52,61] may say something about the effect of an intervention or activity on people with a predisposition for the environment chosen, which might not represent a result that is transferable to the general population.
There may be other practicalities preventing the use of randomization. Park et al. [54] used a quasi-experimental design with a non-equivalent control group; the groups being two senior community centres, with one participating in a gardening intervention, while the other one did not. Wood and colleagues [321] investigated the health and wellbeing benefits of allotment gardening, using a case-control study to compare allotment gardeners with non-gardeners. In many real-life situations, such methods [54,321] will be the only possible way to evaluate an intervention and randomization is not an option. However, if the process, context, and delivery of the intervention are considered, this type of evaluation may produce meaningful results.
Another aspect that can increase the rigidity of a study design is the incorporation of a crossover element [360]. In a study with a crossover design, all participants receive both the intervention treatment and the control treatment. The different treatments are given at different times and with a sufficient washout period in between to insure there is no carryover effect from one treatment into the next. The order of the treatments is randomized. When using a crossover design, the between-subject variability is significantly reduced as each participant serves as their own control. This results in a reduction of the variation in factors not related to the treatment, which in turn allows for the detection of smaller effect sizes using a reduced sample size [360]. However, crossover designs need careful design to minimize potential bias.
Barnicle and Midden [289] investigated the effects of a horticultural activity program on psychological wellbeing among older people in two care homes. As randomization would not be practical at the individual level, the randomization took place at the site level. It would, however, have strengthened the study design if a crossover had been introduced and participants from both care homes had been exposed to the intervention and the control treatment. The authors give no explanation as to why they chose not to include a crossover. However, this often comes down to time, funding, and the likelihood of being able to secure participation and retention for an extended period of data collection. A number of studies fall into this category; an RCT study design that would have benefitted significantly from a crossover element introduced to the design (see, for example, [202,306,319,320,324,335]).
Seven studies identified for this review used a 3-arm design; typically, two intervention treatments and one control treatment [304], or three different types of intervention treatment [282,336]. None of these 3-arm RCTs have incorporated a crossover element. This is not unexpected, as adding more arms to a study design will increase the complexity of the study and put strain on resources, such as time, money, and by no means least the participants.
Five studies used a 4-arm RCT. Sonntag-Ostrom and colleagues [310] investigated the restorative effect of visits to one urban area and three different forest environments, with each participant visiting all four outdoor environments. The authors highlight the difficulties in carrying out a study with such a complex design, e.g., a long data collection period and difficulties in recruiting participants. These difficulties resulted in a 3-year project and only 20 participants [310].
Based on the studies included in this review, the strongest design appears to be RCTs with a crossover element, a finding which is also supported in other literature [359,360]. In addition, the results from this review highlight that unless answers to very specific research questions are sought, increasing the complexity of the study design does not necessarily improve the quality of the data collected as constraints and limitations increase with increasing complexity.
There were eight studies using a 2-arm RCT with a crossover element (Table 10). Three of the studies focused on urban GS, four on natural GS, and one study on virtual/indoor GS. Six of the studies were qualitative, and only two studies used qualitative as well as quantitative methods. Seven of the studies predominantly used questionnaires as the main tool to assess the changes in the investigated health outcome.
Berman et al. [209] used a 2-arm RCT with a crossover to show that participants exhibited a significantly increased memory span after a walk in the park compared to an urban walk. The PANAS (positive affect) revealed a significant effect of location (nature vs. urban) but not time (pre-walk vs. post-walk); for a negative effect, there was no significant effect of location and the negative effect did not decrease more for the park walk than for the urban walk. The authors were therefore not able to show conclusively that GS positively affects the mood of individuals with depression. Gatersleben and Andrews [50] found that exposure to GS with high levels of prospect (clear field of vision) and low levels of refuge (places to hide) generated a restorative effect. However, the authors also found that exposure to GS with low levels of prospect and high levels of refuge did not create a restorative effect. Such a scenario was proposed to increase stress levels and reduce attention. Im et al. [57] found that the levels of somatic and depressive symptoms, and of stress responses, were significantly reduced after exposure to a forest environment, when compared to exposure to an urban environment. The authors also found a significant reduction of immunological inflammation and an increase in the antioxidant effect after the forest exposure. However, due to the design of the study (no before-and-after measurements allowing for comparison), it is not clear if the positive changes are related to a reduction in air pollution (or other harmful urban exposures), rather than the presence of the forest environment. Lee et al. [59] found that the salivary cortisol concentration, diastolic blood pressure, and pulse rate were all significantly lower in participants after exposure to a forest environment. Self-reported subjective measures revealed that participants felt more comfortable, soothed, and refreshed when viewing a forest landscape, when compared to an urban environment. Morita and colleagues [326] investigated the psychological effects of exposure to a forest environment, when compared to exposure to a control environment. Co-exposures and contextual factors were considered, such as conditions during the forest visit and on the control day (weather, duration of visit, previous visits, accompanying people, activities undertaken, walking course and distance walked, degree of exercise, subjective feelings, objective activities undertaken). The authors found that exposure to a forest environment significantly decreased feelings of hostility and depression, and increased the feeling of liveliness, when compared to exposure to a control environment. It was also seen that the positive effect of exposure to a forest environment was greater the higher the stress level of the subject. Despite a high number of participants and a generally stringent study design, the study only used qualitative data and would have benefited from the inclusion of quantitative data. South et al. [361] found that when subjects were in view of a green vacant lot, their heart rate decreased significantly, when compared to being in view of a non-greened vacant lot or not in view of any vacant lot. The authors conclude that remediating neighborhood blight can reduce stress and improve health. Takayama et al. [233] investigated the emotional, restorative, and vitalizing effects related to forest and urban exposures and concluded that exposure to a forest environment improved mood and positive affect, and induced a feeling of subjective restoration and subjective vitality. Tenngart Ivarsson and Hagerhall [318] investigated the perceived restorativeness of gardens. Two gardens with differing levels of build and natural elements were photographed, and a set of 12 photos were selected to represent each garden. The PRS was used to examine the perceived restorativeness of the two gardens. The study also aimed to evaluate the ability of the PRS to distinguish between two different gardens with a mix of build and natural elements, rather than to distinguish a contrast between built and natural scene types. The authors found that both gardens were perceived as restorative, and the PRS can be used to discriminate between two gardens from the same scene type. Hence, one garden can be perceived as more restorative than another although they both have the same type of scene. This highlights the importance of considering the contribution of contextual factors and co-exposures to the overall health effect caused by a GS environment.
Out of the eight studies included here, with a 2-arm RCT crossover design, seven had a positive outcome. Only two of the studies included quantitative measures [57,59], with both studies having a low participant number. All studies heavily rely on qualitative subjective data (Table 10), on which it is difficult to draw comparative conclusive interpretations. However, it is evident that in the included RCTs, there is clear agreement of a positive association between GS exposure and mental HWB. Despite the lack of high-quality studies and methodological rigor between studies, the accumulated strength of these findings highlights the importance of the positive associations between GS and mental HWB.

4. Discussion

The effects of GS on mental HWB is relevant to city planning and public health policy, which is becoming increasingly important as the world’s urban population grows. The published research generally shows positive associations between GS and mental HWB. However, this review has identified great diversity in study designs, GS definitions, outcome measures, inclusion of co-exposures and contextual factors, and reporting of results. This makes it difficult to aggregate the evidence to identify the underlying mechanisms for this positive association or to provide advice to help construct GS that is beneficial for mental HWB.
Based on the diversity of research available on the subject, it is not possible to unequivocally answer all of the four research questions we initially posited. However, based on the weight of evidence of the research reviewed, it is possible to conclude the following with reasonable certainty:
  • Different types of GS in many contexts and different environments have a positive effect on mental HWB (RQ 1 & 3);
  • For a variety of different groups of people (RQ 4), GS does have a positive effect on mental HWB;
  • Different types of GS affect the HWB of individuals in different ways (RQ 1 & 4).
However, based on the analysed literature it is clear that there is no universally agreed definition for GS or mental HWB and in many studies, a definition and/or detailed description of the two has been omitted. Only a few studies have attempted to quantify the GS investigated and/or the amount of GS needed for health improvement (RQ 1 & 2).
RQ 1:
How do different types of GS (recreational, residential, urban, rural) affect HWB and how much green space is needed for health improvement?
There are suggestions that different types of GS may affect mental HWB in different ways and that different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS. There is also limited evidence that some threshold amount of GS is needed to generate positive health outcomes. However, there is insufficient coherence in the evidence to generalize the results.
RQ 2:
How can we best define, measure, and quantify GS?
Often, the description of the GS is limited to simple text descriptors, e.g., allotment garden, urban park, or private garden. There are some good examples of studies that have attempted to quantify the GS investigated and assess the GS quality. For example, Tilley et al. [362] included graphic Ordnance Survey maps clearly depicting the urban environments investigated, giving a clear overview of the settings and contexts. A written overview and typology was included, of quartiles of urban green and urban busy areas, derived from a Geographic Information System (GIS). The authors also used photographs giving visual evidence of the different environments, which would make it easy to replicate the study in other cities and countries. Our findings highlight the necessity to investigate further how best to define, measure, and quantify GS. With a systematic review, it would be possible to explore in more detail what types of measurements are used most efficiently to quantify GS, the accuracy of the different methods, and the reproducibility.
RQ 2: How can we best define, measure, and quantify mental HWB? The World Health Organisation (1948) has defined health as “A state of complete physical, mental and social wellbeing and not merely the absence of disease and infirmity”. However, wellbeing is difficult to define. Fleuret and Atkinson [363] reviewed the various ways in which wellbeing has been used in research and policy contexts. They note that the term ‘wellbeing’ mainly originates from Anglophone countries and in many languages, it is difficult to find and appropriate comparable terms. Often, a number of different terms are used interchangeably to describe wellbeing, such as quality of life, happiness, welfare, pleasure, wealth, and subjective and objective wellbeing [363]. These terms are rarely specified, and it is therefore impossible to know if they are synonymous. Additionally, different stakeholders in different countries adhere to the wellbeing concept in various ways and it is a matter of practice amongst stakeholders that determines how a term is defined. As far as possible, it would be an advantage to harmonize definitions of HWB and to at least explicitly describe the definition used in a research study. The definition proposed by The UK Faculty of Public Health is perhaps a good starting point:
  • ‘Realise our abilities, live a life with purpose and meaning, and make a positive contribution to our communities;
  • Form positive relationships with others, and feel connected and supported;
  • Experience peace of mind, contentment, happiness and joy;
  • Cope with life’s ups and downs and be confident and resilient;
  • Take responsibility for oneself and for others as appropriate.’
This holistic definition of wellbeing incorporates a more social aspect, highlighting a change in focus from looking more at physical health to looking at the realization of the individuals’ potential [364]. It is more inclusive and relevant to more diverse population subgroups, such as people with learning disabilities, who in many cases experience chronic conditions on a daily basis [365]. Furthermore, we propose that the quality of the environment, i.e., built or natural, is also taken into consideration when assessing wellbeing in such a holistic way, in line with the GS exposome.
RQ 3:
Do different co-exposures or contextual factors affect the mental HWB outcome?
Very few studies included in this review have taken contextual factors and co-exposures into account; they were generally poorly described and so it is difficult to replicate studies. The importance of this is highlighted in a study by McMahan and Estes [46], who aimed to synthesize research on the effect of exposure to natural environments on positive and negative affect, using a meta-analysis technique. The authors only included studies with an RCT design including a comparison group and a self-report assessment of the current emotional state; 32 papers were identified. Study and design-related characteristics, such as the year of publication, location of study, mean age of sample, percent female, and instrument used to measure affective wellbeing, were examined to reveal if they had a moderating effect on the investigated outcome. The type of exposure was also addressed (i.e., real or laboratory simulations of nature), as was the type of natural environment (i.e., manicured or wild nature). The review concluded that exposure to natural environments was associated with a moderate increase in positive affect and a small decrease in negative affect. The authors found that study location, type of assessment used to measure emotion, and type of exposure moderated the effect of nature on positive affect. This indicates that co-exposures and contextual factors may play a role in mediating positive as well as negative health effects associated with GS exposure. The attempt in this review, to look at context and co-exposures, has highlighted a gap in the available literature; our knowledge on contextual factors and co-exposures in relation to the GS experience (GS exposome) is insufficient and research is needed to investigate the totality and combination of exposures related to GS that affects mental HWB.
RQ 4:
Do different age groups and population subgroups benefit differently from exposure to GS?
Participant type varied greatly between studies and in many cases, the subjects were very specifically specified, e.g., park users, allotment gardeners, or active walkers. These groups may have an affinity for the GS being investigated. This makes it difficult to compare study results and hinders the interpretation of whether a finding can be generalized to other groups within the population. However, based on the weight of evidence, it can be concluded with reasonable certainty that different population subgroups will benefit differently to a variety of GS exposures.
Based on the analysis in this review, we suggest a number of key points that should be assessed and reported when investigating GS exposures:
  • Quantity of greenery or natural elements;
  • Type of vegetation (creating shade or not/natural daylight);
  • Whether the environment is natural or managed;
  • Quantity of built elements;
  • Traffic noise and air pollution levels;
  • General soundscape;
  • Number of people present in the environment;
  • Setting and context.
The majority of studies rely on qualitative data collection methods and there is limited methodological consistency between studies. There is a need for more robust quantitative data collection methods, e.g., using vegetation cover maps from airborne hyperspectral and light detection and ranging (LiDAR) data to derive measures of GS [253], or measurement of stress hormones (cortisol) for the quantification of changes in stress levels after exposure to different urban and natural environments [58,59,61,62,334,335], or in relation to neighborhood GS and long-term exposure [208,329,332,366]. Ng, et al. [367] recently published the findings from an RCT (waitlist-control randomized controlled trial) investigating the effects of horticultural therapy on Asian older adults. Qualitative measures (MOCA, Zung Self-Rating Depression and Anxiety Scales) were used to investigate cognitive functioning, depression, anxiety, psychological wellbeing, and positive relations with others. Quantitative measures were used to measure nine plasma biomarkers ranging from interleukins and chemokines to hormones. Ng, et al. [367] found no significant changes in conventional psychological subjective measures of health and wellbeing after 6 months of horticultural therapy. However, there was a significant reduction in pro-inflammatory cytokines after the intervention; high levels of these cytokines are associated with depression [368]. This highlights the importance of including objective quantitative methods to underpin and clarify any subjective findings.
Recommendations
Overall, we suggest a number of key points that should be included when planning and reporting on findings from research investigating GS and mental HWB:
  • Description of aim and research question(s);
  • Description of the study design;
  • Description of participant type (incl. sex, mean age, min/max age, population subgroup characteristics and other relevant socioeconomic characteristics);
  • Description of recruitment process;
  • Careful description and quantification of the GS investigated (study sites);
  • Clear definition of the mental HWB endpoint(s);
  • Justification of the choice of tools to assess the health endpoint;
  • Measurement of contextual factors and co-exposures.
We advocate that, in future research, the entire GS exposome should be considered when investigating the impact on mental HWB. There is a need for large well-designed randomized controlled crossover trails that reliably measure a range of environmental and personal exposures associated with GS. Future studies should include standardized quantitative data collection methods to describe and define the GS investigated and to quantify the changes in mental HWB. By also including standardized qualitative data collection methods, a meaningful comparison and pooling of data across studies would be possible. This will allow a better understanding of the underlying factors responsible for positive associations between GS and mental HWB.

Author Contributions

C.W.-N. and J.W.C. conceived the study and carried out the planning and initial screening. C.W.-N. undertook the detailed review of the literature, created the initial synthesis of the evidence, and prepared the first draft of the manuscript. S.K. and M.K. advised on the literature search strategy and data management and assisted in preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the text.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to colleagues in the Institute of Occupational Medicine and Heriot Watt University for their helpful discussions about the work.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bang, K.S.; Lee, I.; Kim, S.; Lim, C.S.; Joh, H.K.; Park, B.J.; Song, M.K. The effects of a campus forest-walking program on undergraduate and graduate students’ physical and psychological health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Oh, B.; Lee, K.J.; Zaslawski, C.; Yeung, A.; Rosenthal, D.; Larkey, L.; Back, M. Health and well-being benefits of spending time in forests: Systematic review. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2017, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Elmendorf, W. The importance of trees and nature in community: A review of the relative literature. Arboric. Urban For. 2008, 34, 152–156. [Google Scholar]
  4. Gascon, M.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Martinez, D.; Dadvand, P.; Forns, J.; Plasencia, A.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 4354–4379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Jorgensen, A.; Gobster, P.H. Shades of green: Measuring the ecology of urban green space in the context of human health and well-being. Nat. Cult. 2010, 5, 338–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. van den Bosch, M.; Ode Sang, Å. Urban natural environments as nature-based solutions for improved public health—A systematic review of reviews. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kondo, M.C.; Fluehr, J.M.; McKeon, T.; Branas, C.C. Urban green space and its impact on human health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Douglas, O.; Lennon, M.; Scott, M. Green space benefits for health and well-being: A life-course approach for urban planning, design and management. Cities 2017, 66, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; de Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Okvat, H.A.; Zautra, A.J. Community gardening: A parsimonious path to individual, community, and environmental resilience. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2011, 47, 374–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Whear, R.; Coon, J.T.; Bethel, A.; Abbott, R.; Stein, K.; Garside, R. What is the impact of using outdoor spaces such as gardens on the physical and mental well-being of those with dementia? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2014, 15, 697–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Wright, S.D.; Wadsworth, A.M. Gray and green revisited: A multidisciplinary perspective of gardens, gardening, and the aging process. J. Aging Res. 2014, 2014, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Detweiler, M.B.; Sharma, T.; Detweiler, J.G.; Murphy, P.F.; Lane, S.; Carman, J.; Chudhary, A.S.; Halling, M.H.; Kim, K.Y. What is the evidence to support the use of therapeutic gardens for the elderly? Psychiatry Investig. 2012, 9, 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lee, I.; Choi, H.; Bang, K.S.; Kim, S.; Song, M.; Lee, B. Effects of forest therapy on depressive symptoms among adults: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. van den Berg, M.; Wendel-Vos, W.; van Poppel, M.; Kemper, H.; van Mechelen, W.; Maas, J. Health benefits of green spaces in the living environment: A systematic review of epidemiological studies. Urban For. Urban Green. 2015, 14, 806–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Tzoulas, K.; Korpela, K.; Venn, S.; Yli-Pelkonen, V.; Kazmierczak, A.; Niemela, J.; James, P. Promoting ecosystem and human health in urban areas using green infrastructure: A literature review. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2007, 81, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sandifer, P.A.; Sutton-Grier, A.E.; Ward, B.P. Exploring connections among nature, biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and well-being: Opportunities to enhance health and biodiversity conservation. Ecosyst. Serv. 2015, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Jennings, V.; Larson, L.; Yun, J. Advancing sustainability through urban green space: Cultural ecosystem services, equity, and social determinants of health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Taylor, L.; Hochuli, D.F. Defining greenspace: Multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 158, 25–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Lederbogen, F.; Kirsch, P.; Haddad, L.; Streit, F.; Tost, H.; Schuch, P.; Wust, S.; Pruessner, J.C.; Rietschel, M.; Deuschle, M.; et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 2011, 474, 498–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wang, J.L. Rural-urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and associated impairment. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2004, 39, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Peen, J.; Schoevers, R.A.; Beekman, A.T.; Dekker, J. The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010, 121, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Mitchell, R.J.; Richardson, E.A.; Shortt, N.K.; Pearce, J.R. Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in mental well-being. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015, 49, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Triguero-Mas, M.; Dadvand, P.; Cirach, M.; Martínez, D.; Medina, A.; Mompart, A.; Basagaña, X.; Gražulevičienė, R.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Natural outdoor environments and mental and physical health: Relationships and mechanisms. Environ. Int. 2015, 77, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Alcock, I.; White, M.P.; Lovell, R.; Higgins, S.L.; Osborne, N.J.; Husk, K.; Wheeler, B.W. What accounts for ‘england’s green and pleasant land’? A panel data analysis of mental health and land cover types in rural England. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 142, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Butler, S.S.; Cohen, A.L. The importance of nature in the well-being of rural elders. Nat. Cult. 2010, 5, 150–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hansen-Ketchum, P.A.; Marck, P.; Reutter, L.; Ha’penny, E. Strengthening access to restorative places: Findings from a participatory study on engaging with nature in the promotion of health. Health Place 2011, 17, 558–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Pretty, J.; Peacock, J.; Hine, R.; Sellens, M.; South, N.; Griffin, M. Green exercise in the UK countryside: Effects on health and psychological well-being, and implications for policy and planning. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2007, 50, 211–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gilbert, A.; Colley, K.; Roberts, D. Are rural residents happier? A quantitative analysis of subjective wellbeing in Scotland. J. Rural Stud. 2016, 44, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Weich, S.; Twigg, L.; Lewis, G. Rural/non-rural differences in rates of common mental disorders in Britain —Prospective multilevel cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry 2006, 188, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Verheij, R.A.; Maas, J.; Groenewegen, P.P. Urban-rural health differences and the availability of green space. Eur. Urban Reg. Stud. 2008, 15, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kohlleppel, T.; Bradley, J.C.; Jacob, S. A walk through the garden: Can a visit to a botanic garden reduce stress? HortTechnology 2002, 12, 489–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Carrus, G.; Scopelliti, M.; Panno, A.; Lafortezza, R.; Colangelo, G.; Pirchio, S.; Ferrini, F.; Salbitano, F.; Agrimi, M.; Portoghesi, L.; et al. A different way to stay in touch with ‘urban nature’: The perceived restorative qualities of botanical gardens. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kreski, B. Healing and empowering veterans in a botanic garden. J. Mus. Educ. 2016, 41, 110–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Adevi, A.A.; Lieberg, M. Stress rehabilitation through garden therapy a caregiver perspective on factors considered most essential to the recovery process. Urban For. Urban Green. 2012, 11, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sahlin, E.; Ahlborg, G.; Tenenbaum, A.; Grahn, P. Using nature-based rehabilitation to restart a stalled process of rehabilitation in individuals with stress-related mental illness. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 1928–1951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Sahlin, E.; Ahlborg, G.; Matuszczyk, J.V.; Grahn, P. Nature-based stress management course for individuals at risk of adverse health effects from work-related stress-effects on stress related symptoms, workability and sick leave. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 6586–6611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Adevi, A.A.; Mårtensson, F. Stress rehabilitation through garden therapy: The garden as a place in the recovery from stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2013, 12, 230–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gonzalez, M.T.; Hartig, T.; Patil, G.G.; Martinsen, E.W.; Kirkevold, M. Therapeutic horticulture in clinical depression: A prospective study of active components. J. Adv. Nurs. 2010, 66, 2002–2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Grabbe, L.; Ball, J.; Goldstein, A. Gardening for the mental well-being of homeless women. J. Holist. Nurs. 2013, 31, 258–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Parkinson, S.; Lowe, C.; Vecsey, T. The therapeutic benefits of horticulture in a mental health service. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2011, 74, 525–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Baybutt, M.; Chemlal, K. Health-promoting prisons: Theory to practice. Glob. Health Promot. 2016, 23, 66–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Edwards, C.A.; McDonnell, C.; Merl, H. An evaluation of a therapeutic garden’s influence on the quality of life of aged care residents with dementia. Dementia 2013, 12, 494–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hawkins, J.L.; Mercer, J.; Thirlaway, K.J.; Clayton, D.A. ‘Doing’ gardening and ‘being’ at the allotment site: Exploring the benefits of allotment gardening for stress reduction and healthy aging. Ecopsychology 2013, 5, 110–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Burton, E.; Mitchell, L.; Stride, C. Bed of roses? The role of garden space in older people’s well-being. Proc. Inst. Civ. Eng.—Urban Des. Plan. 2015, 168, 164–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. McMahan, E.A.; Estes, D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: A meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015, 10, 507–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wild, C.P. Complementing the genome with an “exposome”: The outstanding challenge of environmental exposure measurement in molecular epidemiology. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. 2005, 14, 1847–1850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Arksey, H.; O’Malley, L. Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 2005, 8, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bingley, A. Woodland as working space: Where is the restorative green idyll? Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 91, 135–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Gatersleben, B.; Andrews, M. When walking in nature is not restorative—The role of prospect and refuge. Health Place 2013, 20, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Wahrborg, P.; Petersson, I.F.; Grahn, P. Nature-assisted rehabilitation for reactions to severe stress and/or depression in a rehabilitation garden: Long-term follow-up including comparisons with a matched population-based reference cohort. J. Rehabil. Med. 2014, 46, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Dewi, N.S.; Komatsuzaki, M.; Yamakawa, Y.; Takahashi, H.; Shibanuma, S.; Yasue, T.; Okayama, T.; Toyoda, A.; Shimonishi, H.; Sasaki, S. Community gardens as health promoters: Effects on mental and physical stress levels in adults with and without mental disabilities. Sustainability 2017, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hawkins, J.L.; Thirlaway, K.J.; Backx, K.; Clayton, D.A. Allotment gardening and other leisure activities for stress reduction and healthy aging. HortTechnology 2011, 21, 577–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Park, S.A.; Lee, A.Y.; Son, K.C.; Lee, W.L.; Kim, D.S. Gardening intervention for physical and psychological health benefits in elderly women at community centers. HortTechnology 2016, 26, 474–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Soga, M.; Cox, D.T.C.; Yamaura, Y.; Gaston, K.J.; Kurisu, K.; Hanaki, K. Health benefits of urban allotment gardening: Improved physical and psychological well-being and social integration. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. van den Berg, A.E.; Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P. Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 70, 1203–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Im, S.G.; Choi, H.; Jeon, Y.H.; Song, M.K.; Kim, W.; Woo, J.M. Comparison of effect of two-hour exposure to forest and urban environments on cytokine, anti-oxidant, and stress levels in young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Jung, W.H.; Woo, J.M.; Ryu, J.S. Effect of a forest therapy program and the forest environment on female workers’ stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2015, 14, 274–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scand. J. For. Res. 2009, 24, 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Chun, M.H.; Chang, M.C.; Lee, S.-J. The effects of forest therapy on depression and anxiety in patients with chronic stroke. Int. J. Neurosci. 2017, 127, 199–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sung, J.; Woo, J.-M.; Kim, W.; Lim, S.-K.; Chung, E.-J. The effect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest therapy” program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive patients. Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 2012, 34, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Yu, Y.M.; Lee, Y.J.; Kim, J.Y.; Yoon, S.B.; Shin, C.S. Effects of forest therapy camp on quality of life and stress in postmenopausal women. For. Sci. Technol. 2016, 12, 125–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Eriksson, T.; Westerberg, Y.; Jonsson, H. Experiences of women with stress-related ill health in a therapeutic gardening program. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 2011, 78, 273–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Korpela, K.; Hartig, T. Restorative qualities of favorite places. J. Environ. Psychol. 1996, 16, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hartig, T.; Korpela, K.; Evans, G.W.; Gärling, T. A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scand. Hous. Plan. Res. 1997, 14, 175–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect—The panas scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Goldberg, D.P.; Hillier, V.F. Scaled version of the general health questionnaire. Psychol. Med. 1979, 9, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Stewart-Brown, S.; Tennant, A.; Tennant, R.; Platt, S.; Parkinson, J.; Weich, S. Internal construct validity of the warwick-edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): A Rasch analysis using data from the scottish health education population survey. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2009, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Ware, J.E.; Sherbourne, C.D. The mos 36-item short-form health survey (sf-36):1. Conceptual-framework and item selection. Med. Care 1992, 30, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Beck, A.T.; Rial, W.Y.; Rickels, K. Short form of depression inventory: Cross-validation. Psychol. Rep. 1974, 34, 1184–1186. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  72. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A. Internal consistencies of the original and revised beck depression inventory. J. Clin. Psychol. 1984, 40, 1365–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Beck, A.T.; Ward, C.H.; Mendelson, M.; Mock, J.; Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1961, 4, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Grove, J.R.; Prapavessis, H. Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of an abbreviated profile of mood states. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1992, 23, 93–109. [Google Scholar]
  75. Mcnair, D.M. Citation classic-manual for the profile of mood states. Curr. Contents/Soc. Behav. Sci. 1984, 27, 20. [Google Scholar]
  76. McNair, D.M. Profile of mood states (poms) in evaluation of antianxiety and antidepressant drugs. J. Pharmacol. 1974, 5, 10. [Google Scholar]
  77. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M. The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R.E. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologist Press: Mountain View, CA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  79. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Cimprich, B. Development of an intervention to restore attention in cancer-patients. Cancer Nurs. 1993, 16, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  82. Kessler, R.C.; Andrews, G.; Colpe, L.J.; Hiripi, E.; Mroczek, D.K.; Normand, S.L.T.; Walters, E.E.; Zaslavsky, A.M. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol. Med. 2002, 32, 959–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Diener, E.; Emmons, R.A.; Larsen, R.J.; Griffin, S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Personal. Assess. 1985, 49, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. The Whoqol Group. The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): Development and general psychometric properties. Soc. Sci. Med. 1998, 46, 1569–1585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Lovibond, P.F.; Lovibond, S.H. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 1995, 33, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B.W.; Primary, P.H.Q. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD—The PHQ primary care study. JAMA 1999, 282, 1737–1744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Melamed, S.; Ugarten, U.; Shirom, A.; Kahana, L.; Lerman, Y.; Froom, P. Chronic burnout, somatic arousal and elevated salivary cortisol levels. J. Psychosom. Res. 1999, 46, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Ryan, R.M.; Frederick, C. On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Personal. 1997, 65, 529–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Sheikh, J.I.; Yesavage, J.A. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. J. Aging Ment. Health 1986, 5, 165–173. [Google Scholar]
  90. Jenkinson, C.; Layte, R.; Coulter, A.; Wright, L. Evidence for the sensitivity of the SF-36 health status measure to inequalities in health: Results from the Oxford healthy lifestyles survey. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 1996, 50, 377–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Jenkinson, C.; Layte, R.; Jenkinson, D.; Lawrence, K.; Petersen, S.; Paice, C.; Stradling, J. A shorter form health survey: Can the SF-12 replicate results from the SF-36 in longitudinal studies? J. Public Health Med. 1997, 19, 179–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Williams, A. Euroqol—A new facility for the measurement of health-related quality-of-life. Health Policy 1990, 16, 199–208. [Google Scholar]
  93. Schultz, P.W. Inclusion with nature: The psychology of human-nature relations. In Psychology of Sustainable Development; Schmuck, P., Schultz, W.P., Eds.; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Norwell, MA, USA, 2002; pp. 61–78. [Google Scholar]
  94. Ryff, C.D. Happiness is everything, or is it—Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 57, 1069–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Elo, A.-L.; Dallner, M.; Gamberale, F.; Hottinen, V.; Knardahl, S.; Lindström, K.; Skogstad, A.; Ørhede, E. Validation of the nordic questionnaire for psychological and social factors at work—QPSNordic. In Innovative Theories, Tools, and Practices in Work and Organizational Psychology; Vartiainen, M., Avallone, F., Anderson, N., Eds.; Hogrefe & Huber Publishers: Ashland, OH, USA, 2000; pp. 47–57. [Google Scholar]
  96. Beck, A.T.; Brown, G.; Epstein, N.; Steer, R.A. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety—Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988, 56, 893–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Rumpf, H.J.; Meyer, C.; Hapke, U.; John, U. Screening for mental health: Validity of the MHI-5 using DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders as gold standard. Psychiatry Res. 2001, 105, 243–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Pangman, V.C.; Sloan, J.; Guse, L. An examination of psychometric properties of the mini-mental state examination and the standardized mini-mental state examination: Implications for clinical practice. Appl. Nurs. Res. 2000, 13, 209–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Sonnentag, S.; Fritz, C. The recovery experience questionnaire: Development and validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2007, 12, 204–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Trapnell, P.D.; Campbell, J.D. Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 284–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Schaufeli, W.B.; Bakker, A.B.; Salanova, M. The measurement of work engagement with a short questionnaire—A cross-national study. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2006, 66, 701–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Brown, K.W.; Ryan, R.M. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 822–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Hardy, C.J.; Rejeski, W.J. Not what, but how one feels—The measurement of affect during exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1989, 11, 304–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Hamilton, M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1960, 23, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Smith, J.A. Beyond the divide between cognition and discourse: Using interpretative phenomenological analysis in health psychology. Psychol. Health 1996, 11, 261–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Eriksen, H.R.; Ihlebaek, C.; Ursin, H. A scoring system for Subjective Health Complaints (SHC). Scand. J. Public Health 1999, 27, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Neugarten, B.L.; Havighurst, R.J.; Tobin, S.S. The measurement of life satisfaction. J. Gerontol. 1961, 16, 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Diener, E.; Biswas-Diener, R. Happiness: Unlocking the Mysteries of Psychological Wealth; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  109. Zuckerman, M. Development of a situation-specific trait-state test for prediction and measurement of affective responses. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1977, 45, 513–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Russell, J.A.; Ward, L.M.; Pratt, G. Affective quality attributed to environments—A factor analytic study. Environ. Behav. 1981, 13, 259–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Kyle, G.; Graefe, A.; Manning, R. Testing the dimensionality of place attachment in recreational settings. Environ. Behav. 2005, 37, 153–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Abele-Brehm, A.; Brehm, W. Zur konzeptualisierung und messung von befindlichkeit. Die entwicklung der “befindlichkeitsskalen” (BFS) [the conceptualization and measurement of mood: The development of the ‘Mood Survey.’]. Diagnostica 1986, 32, 209–228. [Google Scholar]
  113. Zhang, J.X.; Schwarzer, R. Measuring optimistic self-beliefs—A Chinese adaptation of the general self-efficacy scale. Psychologia 1995, 38, 174–181. [Google Scholar]
  114. Clifton, R.A.; Etcheverry, E.; Hasinoff, S.; Roberts, L.W. Measuring the cognitive domain of the quality of life of university students. Soc. Indic. Res. 1996, 38, 29–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Roberts, L.W.; Clifton, R.A. Measuring the affective quality-of-life of university-students—The validation of an instrument. Soc. Indic. Res. 1992, 27, 113–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Becker, P. Der Trier Personlichkeitsfragebogen [Trier Personality Questionnaire]; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  117. Bradburn, N.M. Bradburn Scale of Psychological Well-Being; Aldine: Chicago, IL, USA, 1969. [Google Scholar]
  118. Cimprich, B.; Visovatti, M.; Ronis, D.L. The attentional function index-a self-report cognitive measure. Psychooncology 2011, 20, 194–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Cosco, N.G.; Moore, R.C.; Islam, M.Z. Behavior mapping: A method for linking preschool physical activity and outdoor design. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2010, 42, 513–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Saucier, G. Mini-markers: A brief version of goldberg’s unipolar Big-Five markers. J. Personal. Assess. 1994, 63, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Derogatis, L.R.; Melisaratos, N. The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychol. Med. 1983, 13, 595–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Treynor, W.; Gonzalez, R.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognit. Ther. Res. 2003, 27, 247–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Buss, A.H.; Perry, M. The aggression questionnaire. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 63, 452–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Cohen-Mansfield, J.; Lipson, S.; Werner, P.; Billig, N.; Taylor, L.; Woosley, R. Withdrawal of haloperidol, thioridazine, and lorazepam in the nursing home—A controlled, double-blind study. Arch. Intern. Med. 1999, 159, 1733–1740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Cox, R.H.; Martens, M.P.; Russell, W.D. Measuring anxiety in athletics: The revised competitive state anxiety inventory-2. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2003, 25, 519–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Connor, K.M.; Davidson, J.R.T. Development of a new resilience scale: The Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depress. Anxiety 2003, 18, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Alexopoulos, G.S.; Abrams, R.C.; Young, R.C.; Shamoian, C.A. Cornell scale for depression in dementia. Biol. Psychiatry 1988, 23, 271–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Brantley, P.J.; Waggoner, C.D.; Jones, G.N.; Rappaport, N.B. A daily stress inventory—Development, reliability, and validity. J. Behav. Med. 1987, 10, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Brod, M.; Stewart, A.L.; Sands, L.; Walton, P. Conceptualization and measurement of quality of life in dementia: The dementia quality of life instrument (DQOL). Gerontologist 1999, 39, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Steer, R.A. Modal profile analyses of the SCL-90-r for 7 mental-disorders. Multivar. Exp. Clin. Res. 1983, 6, 55–67. [Google Scholar]
  131. Maloney, M.P.; Ward, M.P.; Braucht, G.N. Psychology in action—Revised scale for measurement of ecological attitudes and knowledge. Am. Psychol. 1975, 30, 787–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Vincenti, G.E. Edinburgh post-natal depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 1987, 151, 865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Huta, V.; Ryan, R.M. Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 735–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Gauvin, L.; Rejeski, W.J. The exercise-induced feeling inventory—Development and initial validation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1993, 15, 403–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Gauvin, L.; Szabo, A. Application of the experience sampling method to the study of the effects of exercise withdrawal on weil-being. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1992, 14, 361–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Svebak, S.; Murgatroyd, S. Metamotivational dominance: A multimethod validation of reversal theory constructs. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1985, 48, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Gagnon Thompson, S.C.; Barton, M.A. Ecocentric and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 1994, 14, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Kaiser, F.G. General measure of ecological behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 28, 395–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Kozma, A.; Stones, M.J. The measurement of happiness—Development of the memorial-university-of-newfoundland scale of happiness (munsh). J. Gerontol. 1980, 35, 906–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Zigmond, A.S.; Snaith, R.P. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983, 67, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Seo, H.M.; Hah, Y.S. A study of factors influencing on health promiting lifestyle in the elderly-application of pender’s health promotion model. J. Korean Acad. Nurs. 2004, 34, 1288–1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Thake, C.L.; Bambling, M.; Edirippulige, S.; Marx, E. A psychoevolutionary approach to identifying preferred nature scenes with potential to provide restoration from stress. HERD—Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2017, 10, 111–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Cohen, S.; Mermelstein, R.; Kamarck, T.; Hoberman, H. Measuring the functional components of social support. In Social Support: Theory, Research and Applications; Sarason, I.G., Sarason, B.R., Eds.; Martinus Nijhoff Publishers: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1985; pp. 73–94. [Google Scholar]
  144. Vagg, P.R.; Spielberger, C.D. The job stress survey: Assessing perceived severity and frequency of occurrence of generic sources of stress in the workplace. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1999, 4, 288–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Olsen, L.R.; Mortensen, E.L.; Bech, P. The SCL-90 and SCL-90R versions validated by item response models in a Danish community sample. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2004, 110, 225–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Forsell, Y. The major depression inventory versus schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry in a population sample. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2005, 40, 209–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Priebe, S.; Huxley, P.; Knight, S.; Evans, S. Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA). Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 1999, 45, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Crowne, D.P.; Marlowe, D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J. Consult. Psychol. 1960, 24, 349–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Maslach, C.; Jackson, S.E.; Leiter, M.P. Maslach burnout inventory. In Evaluating Stress: A Book of Resources, 3rd ed.; Zalaquett, C.P., Wood, R.J., Eds.; Scarecrow Press: Lanham, MD, USA, 1997; pp. 191–218. [Google Scholar]
  150. Sheehan, D.V.; Janavs, J.; Baker, R.; Harnett-Sheehan, K.; Knapp, E.; Sheehan, M.; Lecrubier, Y.; Weiller, E.; Hergueta, T.; Amorim, P.; et al. MINI—Mini international neuropsychiatric interview—English version 5.0.0—DSM-IV. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1998, 59, 34–57. [Google Scholar]
  151. Sheehan, D.V.; Lecrubier, Y.; Sheehan, K.H.; Amorim, P.; Janavs, J.; Weiller, E.; Hergueta, T.; Baker, R.; Dunbar, G.C. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1998, 59, 22–33. [Google Scholar]
  152. Steyer, R.; Schwenkmezger, P.; Notz, P.; Eid, M. Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF) [the Multidimensional Comfort Questionnaire (MDBF)]; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  153. Terasaki, M.; Kishimoto, Y.; Koga, A. Construction of a multiple mood scale. Jpn. J. Psychol. 1992, 62, 350–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Largo-Wight, E.; Chen, W.W.; Dodd, V.; Weiler, R. The nature contact questionnaire: A measure of healthy workplace exposure. Work 2011, 40, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  155. Scott, B.; Brandberg, M.; Ohman, A. Measuring the negative mood component of stress experiences: Description and psychometric properties of a short adjective check-list of stress responses. Scand. J. Psychol. 2001, 42, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Francis, L.J.; Brown, L.B.; Lester, D.; Philipchalk, R. Happiness as stable extraversion: A cross-cultural examination of the reliability and validity of the Oxford happiness inventory among students in the UK, USA, Australia, and Canada. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 24, 167–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Campbell, A.; Converse, P.E.; Rodgers, W.L. The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
  158. Robitschek, C. Personal growth initiative: The construct and its measure. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 1998, 30, 183–198. [Google Scholar]
  159. Lafortezza, R.; Carrus, G.; Sanesi, G.; Davies, C. Benefits and well-being perceived by people visiting green spaces in periods of heat stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2009, 8, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Dupuy, H. The psychological general wellbeing (PGWB) index. In Assessment of Quality of Life in Clinical Trials of Cardiovascular Therapies; Wenger, N.K., Mattson, M.E., Furberg, C.D., Elinson, J., Eds.; Le Jacq Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 1984; pp. 170–183. [Google Scholar]
  161. Andereck, K.L.; Nyaupane, G. Development of a tourism and quality-of-life instrument. Soc. Indic. Res. Ser. 2011, 43, 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Andereck, K.L.; Nyaupane, G.P. Exploring the nature of tourism and quality of life perceptions among residents. J. Travel Res. 2011, 50, 248–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Pang, S.M.C.; Chan, K.S.; Chung, B.P.M.; Lau, K.S.; Leung, E.M.F.; Leung, A.W.K.; Chan, H.Y.L.; Chan, T.M.F. Assessing quality of life of patients with advanced chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in the end of life. J. Palliat. Care 2005, 21, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Pang, S.M.C.; Tse, C.; Chan, K.; Chung, B.P.M.; Leung, A.K.A.; Leung, E.M.F.; Ko, S.K.K. An empirical analysis of the decision-making of limiting life-sustaining treatment for patients with advanced chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in Hong Kong, China. J. Crit. Care 2004, 19, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Burckhardt, C.S.; Woods, S.L.; Schultz, A.A.; Ziebarth, D.M. Quality of life of adults with chronic illness—A psychometric study. Res. Nurs. Health 1989, 12, 347–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Dunbar, G.C.; Stoker, M.J.; Hodges, T.C.P.; Beaumont, G. The development of SBQUL: A unique scale for measuring quality of life. Br. J. Med. Econ. 1992, 2, 65–74. [Google Scholar]
  167. Kim, K.Y.; Chun, B.Y.; Kam, S.; Lee, S.W.; Park, K.S.; Chae, S.C. Development of measurement scale for the quality of life in hypertensive patients. J. Prev. Med. Public Health 2005, 38, 61–70. [Google Scholar]
  168. Waterman, A.S.; Schwartz, S.J.; Zamboanga, B.L.; Ravert, R.D.; Williams, M.K.; Agocha, V.B.; Kim, S.Y.; Donnellan, M.B. The questionnaire for eudaimonic well-being: Psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evidence of validity. J. Posit. Psychol. 2010, 5, 41–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  169. Hays, R.D.; Sherbourne, C.D.; Mazel, R.M. User’s Manual for the Medical Outcomes Study (MOS) Core Measures of Health-Related Quality of Life; RAND Corporation: Santa Monica, CA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  170. McDowell, I.; Newell, C. Measuring Health: A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  171. Crompton, J.L. Motivations for pleasure vacation. Ann. Tour. Res. 1979, 6, 408–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  172. Mill, R.C.M.; Morrison, A.M. The Tourism System: An Introductory Text; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  173. Pearce, P.L.; Lee, U.-I. Developing the travel career approach to tourist motivation. J. Travel Res. 2005, 43, 226–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Tinsley, H.E.A.; Kass, R.A.; Driver, B.L. Reliability and concurrent validity of the recreation experience preference scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1981, 41, 897–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Korpela, K.M.; Ylen, M.; Tyrvainen, L.; Silvennoinen, H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place 2008, 14, 636–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  176. Korpela, K.M.; Ylen, M.P. Effectiveness of favorite-place prescriptions a field experiment. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2009, 36, 435–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. Rizzo, J.R.; House, R.J.; Lirtzman, S.I. Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations. Adm. Sci. Q. 1970, 15, 150–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. Ryan, R.M.; Weinstein, N.; Bernstein, J.; Brown, K.W.; Mistretta, L.; Gagne, M. Vitalizing effects of being outdoors and in nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Ericsson, A.; Nystrom, C.; Mannerkorpi, K. Psychometric properties of the stress and crisis inventory (SCI-93) in females with fibromyalgia and chronic widespread pain. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2015, 69, 28–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  180. Nyström, C.; Nyström, O. Skattad stress verifierar autonom dysfunktion [estimated stress verifies autonomic dysfunction]. Lakartidningen 1996, 93, 2583–2584. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  181. Mcauley, E.; Courneya, K.S. The Subjective Exercise Experiences Scale (sees)—Development and preliminary validation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1994, 16, 163–177. [Google Scholar]
  182. Tangney, J.P.; Baumeister, R.F.; Boone, A.L. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Personal. 2004, 72, 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Han, K.T. A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2003, 64, 209–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  184. Mackay, C.; Cox, T.; Burrows, G.; Lazzerini, T. Inventory for measurement of self-reported stress and arousal. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1978, 17, 283–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  185. Lundberg, O.; Peck, N.M. A simplified way of measuring sense of coherence experiences from a population survey in Sweden. Eur. J. Public Health 1995, 5, 56–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Salkovskis, P.M.; Rimes, K.A.; Warwick, H.M.C.; Clark, D.M. The health anxiety inventory: Development and validation of scales for the measurement of health anxiety and hypochondriasis. Psychol. Med. 2002, 32, 843–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  187. Abdel-Khalek, A.M. Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2006, 34, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Sampson, R.J.; Raudenbush, S.W.; Earls, F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 1997, 277, 918–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  189. Cutrona, C.E.; Russell, D.W. The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. In Advances in Personal Relationships; JAI Press: Greenwich, CT, USA, 1987; pp. 37–67. [Google Scholar]
  190. Koh, K.B.; Park, J.K.; Kim, C.H.; Cho, S. Development of the stress response inventory and its application in clinical practice. Psychosom. Med. 2001, 63, 668–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Suzuki, S.-I.; Shimada, H.; Miura, M.; Katayanagi, K.; Umano, R.; Sakano, Y. Development of a new psychological Stress Response Scale (SRS-18) and investigation of the reliability and the validity. Jpn. J. Behav. Med. 1997, 4, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Taylor, S.P. Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and tendency to inhibit aggression. J. Personal. 1967, 35, 297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Thayer, R.E. The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  194. Sherman, G.D.; Haidt, J.; Coan, J.A. Viewing cute images increases behavioral carefulness. Emotion 2009, 9, 282–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  195. Lese, K.P.; MacNair-Semands, R.R. The therapeutic factors inventory: Development of a scale. Group 2000, 24, 303–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. Chern, J.S.; Kielhofner, G.; delasHeras, C.G.; Magalhaes, L.C. The volitional questionnaire: Psychometric development and practical use. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1996, 50, 516–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Choi, S.M.; Kang, T.Y.; Woo, J.M. Development and validation of a modifiedform of the stress response inventory for workers. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc. 2006, 45, 541–553. [Google Scholar]
  198. Ferris, D.L.; Brown, D.J.; Berry, J.W.; Lian, H.W. The development and validation of the workplace ostracism scale. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 1348–1366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  199. Hendee, J.C.; Catton, W.R.; Marlow, L.D.; Brockman, C.F. Wilderness Users in the Pacific Northwest—Their Characteristics, Values, and Management Preferences; USDA Forest Service research paper PNW-61; Pacific Northwest Forest and Range Experiment Station: Portland, OR, USA, 1968.
  200. Zung, W.W.K. A self-rating depression scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1965, 12, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  201. Beute, F.; de Kort, Y.A.W. The natural context of wellbeing: Ecological momentary assessment of the influence of nature and daylight on affect and stress for individuals with depression levels varying from none to clinical. Health Place 2018, 49, 7–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  202. Bratman, G.N.; Daily, G.C.; Levy, B.J.; Gross, J.J. The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 138, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  203. Hinds, J.; Sparks, P. The affective quality of human-natural environment relationships. Evol. Psychol. 2011, 9, 147470491100900314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  204. Poon, K.T.; Teng, F.; Wong, W.Y.; Chen, Z.S. When nature heals: Nature exposure moderates the relationship between ostracism and aggression. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 48, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  205. Wang, Y.; She, Y.; Colarelli, S.M.; Fang, Y.; Meng, H.; Chen, Q.; Zhang, X.; Zhu, H. Exposure to nature counteracts aggression after depletion. Aggress. Behav. 2018, 44, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  206. DeWolfe, J.; Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M. The relationship between levels of greenery and landscaping at track and field sites, anxiety, and sports performance of collegiate track and field athletes. HortTechnology 2011, 21, 329–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  207. Mackay, G.J.; Neill, J.T. The effect of “green exercise” on state anxiety and the role of exercise duration, intensity, and greenness: A quasi-experimental study. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2010, 11, 238–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  208. Gidlow, C.J.; Randall, J.; Gillman, J.; Smith, G.R.; Jones, M.V. Natural environments and chronic stress measured by hair cortisol. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 148, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Berman, M.G.; Kross, E.; Krpan, K.M.; Askren, M.K.; Burson, A.; Deldin, P.J.; Kaplan, S.; Sherdell, L.; Gotlib, I.H.; Jonides, J. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 140, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  210. Beyer, K.M.M.; Szabo, A.; Nattinger, A.B. Time spent outdoors, depressive symptoms, and variation by race and ethnicity. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016, 51, 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  211. Christensen, K.M.; Holt, J.M.; Wilson, J.F. The relationship between outdoor recreation and depression among older adults. World Leisure J. 2013, 55, 72–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  212. Farber, M.E.; Hall, T.E. Emotion and environment: Visitors’ extraordinary experiences along the dalton highway in Alaska. J. Leisure Res. 2007, 39, 248–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  213. Gonzalez, M.T.; Hartig, T.; Patil, G.G.; Martinsen, E.W.; Kirkevold, M. A prospective study of group cohesiveness in therapeutic horticulture for clinical depression. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2011, 20, 119–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  214. Korpela, K.M.; Stenggård, E.; Jussila, P. Nature walks as a part of therapeutic intervention for depression. Ecopsychology 2016, 8, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  215. McCaffrey, R. The effect of healing gardens and art therapy on older adults with mild to moderate depression. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 2007, 21, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. McCaffrey, R.; Hanson, C.; McCaffrey, W. Garden walking for depression: A research report. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 2010, 24, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  217. McCaffrey, R.; Liehr, P.; Gregersen, T.; Nishioka, R. Garden walking and art therapy for depression in older adults: A pilot study. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 2011, 4, 237–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  218. McEachan, R.R.C.; Prady, S.L.; Smith, G.; Fairley, L.; Cabieses, B.; Gidlow, C.; Wright, J.; Dadvand, P.; van Gent, D.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. The association between green space and depressive symptoms in pregnant women: Moderating roles of socioeconomic status and physical activity. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2016, 70, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  219. Min, K.B.; Kim, H.J.; Kim, H.J.; Min, J.Y. Parks and green areas and the risk for depression and suicidal indicators. Int. J. Public Health 2017, 62, 647–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  220. Mukherjee, D.; Safraj, S.; Tayyab, M.; Shivashankar, R.; Patel, S.A.; Narayanan, G.; Ajay, V.S.; Ali, M.K.; Narayan, K.M.V.; Tandon, N.; et al. Park availability and major depression in individuals with chronic conditions: Is there an association in urban India? Health Place 2017, 47, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  221. Nichani, V.; Dirks, K.; Burns, B.; Bird, A.; Grant, C. Green space and depression during pregnancy: Results from the growing up in New Zealand study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  222. Rappe, E.; Kivelä, S.-L. Effects of garden visits on long-term care residents as related to depression. HortTechnology 2005, 15, 298–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  223. Reklaitiene, R.; Grazuleviciene, R.; Dedele, A.; Virviciute, D.; Vensloviene, J.; Tamosiunas, A.; Baceviciene, M.; Luksiene, D.; Sapranaviciute-Zabazlajeva, L.; Radisauskas, R.; et al. The relationship of green space, depressive symptoms and perceived general health in urban population. Scand. J. Public Health 2014, 42, 669–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  224. Shin, W.; Shin, C.; Yeoun, P. The influence of forest therapy camp on depression in alcoholics. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2012, 17, 73–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  225. Torres, E.R.; Sampselle, C.M.; Ronis, D.L.; Neighbors, H.W.; Gretebeck, K.A. Gardening/yard work and depressive symptoms in African Americans. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2016, 30, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  226. Wilson, J.F.; Christensen, K.M. The relationship between outdoor recreation and depression among individuals with disabilities. J. Leisure Res. 2012, 44, 486–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  227. Han, K.-T. The effect of nature and physical activity on emotions and attention while engaging in green exercise. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 24, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  228. Marselle, M.R.; Irvine, K.N.; Lorenzo-Arribas, A.; Warber, S.L. Moving beyond green: Exploring the relationship of environment type and indicators of perceived environmental quality on emotional well-being following group walks. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 106–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  229. Ambrey, C.L.; Shahni, T.J. Greenspace and wellbeing in Tehran: A relationship conditional on a neighbourhood’s crime rate? Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 27, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  230. Dadvand, P.; Bartoll, X.; Basagaña, X.; Dalmau-Bueno, A.; Martinez, D.; Ambros, A.; Cirach, M.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Gascon, M.; Borrell, C.; et al. Green spaces and general health: Roles of mental health status, social support, and physical activity. Environ. Int. 2016, 91, 161–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  231. de Vries, S.; van Dillen, S.M.E.; Groenewegen, P.P.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Streetscape greenery and health: Stress, social cohesion and physical activity as mediators. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 94, 26–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  232. Stigsdotter, U.K.; Ekholm, O.; Schipperijn, J.; Toftager, M.; Kamper-Jørgensen, F.; Randrup, T.B. Health promoting outdoor environments—Associations between green space, and health, health-related quality of life and stress based on a Danish national representative survey. Scand. J. Public Health 2010, 38, 411–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  233. Takayama, N.; Korpela, K.; Lee, J.; Morikawa, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Tyrvainen, L.; Miyazaki, Y.; Kagawa, T. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 7207–7230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  234. Weimann, H.; Rylander, L.; Albin, M.; Skärbäck, E.; Grahn, P.; Östergren, P.-O.; Björk, J. Effects of changing exposure to neighbourhood greenness on general and mental health: A longitudinal study. Health Place 2015, 33, 48–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  235. Willis, K.; Crabtree, B.; Osman, L.M.; Cathrine, K. Green space and health benefits: A QALY and CEA of a mental health programme. J. Environ. Econ. Policy 2016, 5, 163–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  236. Cini, F.; Kruger, S.; Ellis, S. A model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations on subjective well-being: The experience of overnight visitors to a national park. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 2013, 8, 45–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  237. Frash, R.E., Jr.; Blose, J.E.; Norman, W.C.; Patience, M. Healthy parks, happy people: An exploratory study of a county park system. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2016, 34, 84–102. [Google Scholar]
  238. Saw, L.E.; Lim, F.K.S.; Carrasco, L.R. The relationship between natural park usage and happiness does not hold in a tropical city-state. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0133781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  239. Dzhambov, A.M.; Dimitrova, D.D. Elderly visitors of an urban park, health anxiety and individual awareness of nature experiences. Urban For. Urban Green. 2014, 13, 806–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  240. Shin, W.S. The influence of forest view through a window on job satisfaction and job stress. Scand. J. For. Res. 2007, 22, 248–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  241. Ambrey, C.; Fleming, C. Public greenspace and life satisfaction in urban Australia. Urban Stud. 2014, 51, 1290–1321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  242. Biedenweg, K.; Scott, R.P.; Scott, T.A. How does engaging with nature relate to life satisfaction? Demonstrating the link between environment-specific social experiences and life satisfaction. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 50, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  243. Fleming, C.M.; Manning, M.; Ambrey, C.L. Crime, greenspace and life satisfaction: An evaluation of the New Zealand experience. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 149, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  244. Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M.; Lineberger, R.D. The influence of gardening activities on consumer perceptions of life satisfaction. Hortscience 2005, 40, 1360–1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  245. Alcock, I.; White, M.P.; Wheeler, B.W.; Fleming, L.E.; Depledge, M.H. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2014, 48, 1247–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  246. Ambrey, C.L. Urban greenspace, physical activity and wellbeing: The moderating role of perceptions of neighbourhood affability and incivility. Land Use Policy 2016, 57, 638–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  247. An, M.; Colarelli, S.M.; O’Brien, K.; Boyajian, M.E. Why we need more nature at work: Effects of natural elements and sunlight on employee mental health and work attitudes. PLoS ONE 2016, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  248. Annerstedt, M.; Ostergren, P.-O.; Björk, J.; Grahn, P.; Skärbäck, E.; Währborg, P. Green qualities in the neighbourhood and mental health—Results from a longitudinal cohort study in Southern Sweden. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  249. Astell-Burt, T.; Mitchell, R.; Hartig, T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2014, 68, 578–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  250. Beyer, K.M.M.; Kaltenbach, A.; Szabo, A.; Bogar, S.; Nieto, F.J.; Malecki, K.M. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: Evidence from the survey of the health of wisconsin. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 3453–3472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  251. Bos, E.H.; van der Meulen, L.; Wichers, M.; Jeronimus, B.F. A primrose path? Moderating effects of age and gender in the association between green space and mental health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  252. Cohen-Cline, H.; Turkheimer, E.; Duncan, G.E. Access to green space, physical activity and mental health: A twin study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2015, 69, 523–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  253. Cox, D.T.C.; Shanahan, D.F.; Hudson, H.L.; Plummer, K.E.; Siriwardena, G.M.; Fuller, R.A.; Anderson, K.; Hancock, S.; Gaston, K.J. Doses of neighborhood nature: The benefits for mental health of living with nature. BioScience 2017, 67, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  254. Crust, L.; Henderson, H.; Middleton, G. The acute effects of urban green and countryside walking on psychological health: A field-based study of green exercise. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2013, 44, 160–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  255. Dzhambov, A.; Hartig, T.; Markevych, I.; Tilov, B.; Dimitrova, D. Urban residential greenspace and mental health in youth: Different approaches to testing multiple pathways yield different conclusions. Environ. Res. 2018, 160, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  256. Gubbels, J.S.; Kremers, S.P.J.; Droomers, M.; Hoefnagels, C.; Stronks, K.; Hosman, C.; de Vries, S. The impact of greenery on physical activity and mental health of adolescent and adult residents of deprived neighborhoods: A longitudinal study. Health Place 2016, 40, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  257. Iwata, Y.; Dhubháin, Á.N.; Brophy, J.; Roddy, D.; Burke, C.; Murphy, B. Benefits of group walking in forests for people with significant mental ill-health. Ecopsychology 2016, 8, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  258. Liu, H.; Li, F.; Li, J.; Zhang, Y. The relationships between urban parks, residents’ physical activity, and mental health benefits: A case study from beijing, China. J. Environ. Manag. 2017, 190, 223–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  259. Mitchell, R. Is physical activity in natural environments better for mental health than physical activity in other environments? Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 91, 130–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  260. Mutz, M.; Mueller, J. Mental health benefits of outdoor adventures: Results from two pilot studies. J. Adolesc. 2016, 49, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  261. Nutsford, D.; Pearson, A.L.; Kingham, S. An ecological study investigating the association between access to urban green space and mental health. Public Health 2013, 127, 1005–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  262. Pálsdóttir, A.M.; Persson, D.; Persson, B.; Grahn, P. The journey of recovery and empowerment embraced by nature—Clients’ perspectives on nature-based rehabilitation in relation to the role of the natural environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 7094–7115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  263. Rogerson, M.; Brown, D.K.; Sandercock, G.; Wooller, J.J.; Barton, J. A comparison of four typical green exercise environments and prediction of psychological health outcomes. Perspect. Public Health 2016, 136, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  264. Ruijsbroek, A.; Mohnen, S.M.; Droomers, M.; Kruize, H.; Gidlow, C.; Grazuleviciene, R.; Andrusaityte, S.; Maas, J.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; Triguero-Mas, M.; et al. Neighbourhood green space, social environment and mental health: An examination in four european cities. Int. J. Public Health 2017, 62, 657–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  265. Sturm, R.; Cohen, D. Proximity to urban parks and mental health. J. Ment. Health Policy Econ. 2014, 17, 19–24. [Google Scholar]
  266. Sugiyama, T.; Leslie, E.; Giles-Corti, B.; Owen, N. Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: Do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2008, 62, e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  267. Sugiyama, T.; Villanueva, K.; Knuiman, M.; Francis, J.; Foster, S.; Wood, L.; Giles-Corti, B. Can neighborhood green space mitigate health inequalities? A study of socio-economic status and mental health. Health Place 2016, 38, 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  268. van den Berg, M.; van Poppel, M.; Smith, G.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Andrusaityte, S.; van Kamp, I.; van Mechelen, W.; Gidlow, C.; Gražulevičiene, R.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; et al. Does time spent on visits to green space mediate the associations between the level of residential greenness and mental health? Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 25, 94–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  269. van den Berg, M.; van Poppel, M.; van Kamp, I.; Andrusaityte, S.; Balseviciene, B.; Cirach, M.; Danileviciute, A.; Ellis, N.; Hurst, G.; Masterson, D.; et al. Visiting green space is associated with mental health and vitality: A cross-sectional study in four european cities. Health Place 2016, 38, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  270. van den Bosch, M.A.; Östergren, P.-O.; Grahn, P.; Skärbäck, E.; Währborg, P. Moving to serene nature may prevent poor mental health-results from a Swedish longitudinal cohort study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 7974–7989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  271. Vujcic, M.; Tomicevic-Dubljevic, J.; Grbic, M.; Lecic-Tosevski, D.; Vukovic, O.; Toskovic, O. Nature based solution for improving mental health and well-being in urban areas. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 385–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  272. White, M.P.; Alcock, I.; Wheeler, B.W.; Depledge, M.H. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 920–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  273. Wilson, N.W.; Jones, R.; Fleming, S.; Lafferty, K.; Knifton, L.; Cathrine, K.; McNish, H. Branching out: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy program. Ecopsychology 2011, 3, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  274. Wilson, N.W.; Fleming, S.; Jones, R.; Lafferty, K.; Cathrine, K.; Seaman, P.; Knifton, L. Green shoots of recovery: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy programme. Ment. Health Rev. J. 2010, 15, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  275. Wood, L.; Hooper, P.; Foster, S.; Bull, F. Public green spaces and positive mental health—Investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place 2017, 48, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  276. Ambrey, C.L.; Cartlidge, N. Do the psychological benefits of greenspace depend on one’s personality? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 116, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  277. Budruk, M.; Lee, W. Importance of managing for personal benefits, hedonic and utilitarian motivations, and place attachment at an urban natural setting. Environ. Manag. 2016, 58, 504–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  278. Gilchrist, K.; Brown, C.; Montarzino, A. Workplace settings and wellbeing: Greenspace use and views contribute to employee wellbeing at pen-urban business sites. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 138, 32–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  279. Henderson-Wilson, C.; Sia, K.L.; Veitch, J.; Staiger, P.K.; Davidson, P.; Nicholls, P. Perceived health benefits and willingness to pay for parks by park users: Quantitative and qualitative research. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  280. Houlden, V.; Weich, S.; Jarvis, S. A cross-sectional analysis of green space prevalence and mental wellbeing in England. BMC Public Health 2017, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  281. Jakubec, S.L.; Den Hoed, D.C.; Ray, H.; Krishnamurthy, A. Mental well-being and quality-of-life benefits of inclusion in nature for adults with disabilities and their caregivers. Landsc. Res. 2016, 41, 616–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  282. Barton, J.; Griffin, M.; Pretty, J. Exercise-, nature- and socially interactive-based initiatives improve mood and self-esteem in the clinical population. Perspect. Public Health 2012, 132, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  283. Joye, Y.; Bolderdijk, J.W. An exploratory study into the effects of extraordinary nature on emotions, mood, and prosociality. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  284. McCaffrey, R.; Liehr, P. The effect of reflective garden walking on adults with increased levels of psychological stress. J. Holist. Nurs. 2016, 34, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  285. Astell-Burt, T.; Feng, X.; Kolt, G.S. Mental health benefits of neighbourhood green space are stronger among physically active adults in middle-to-older age: Evidence from 260,061 australians. Prev. Med. 2013, 57, 601–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  286. Hansmann, R.; Hug, S.-M.; Seeland, K. Restoration and stress relief through physical activities in forests and parks. Urban For. Urban Green. 2007, 6, 213–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  287. Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Strumse, E. Psychological restoration in nature as a source of motivation for ecological behaviour. Environ. Conserv. 2007, 34, 291–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  288. Calogiuri, G.; Evensen, K.; Weydahl, A.; Andersson, K.; Patil, G.; Ihlebæk, C.; Raanaas, R.K. Green exercise as a workplace intervention to reduce job stress. Results from a pilot study. Work 2016, 53, 99–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  289. Barnicle, T.; Midden, K.S. The effects of a horticulture activity program on the psychological well-being of older people in a long-term care facility. HortTechnology 2003, 13, 81–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  290. Fruhauf, A.; Niedermeier, M.; Elliott, L.R.; Ledochowski, L.; Marksteiner, J.; Kopp, M. Acute effects of outdoor physical activity on affect and psychological well-being in depressed patients—A preliminary study. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2016, 10, 4–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  291. Herzog, T.R.; Strevey, S.J. Contact with nature, sense of humor, and psychological well-being. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 747–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  292. Hug, S.-M.; Hansmann, R.; Monn, C.; Krütli, P.; Seeland, K. Restorative effects of physical activity in forests and indoor settings. Int. J. Fit. 2008, 4, 25–37. [Google Scholar]
  293. Kamitsis, I.; Francis, A.J.P. Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  294. Lawton, E.; Brymer, E.; Clough, P.; Denovan, A. The relationship between the physical activity environment, nature relatedness, anxiety, and the psychological well-being benefits of regular exercisers. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  295. Martens, D.; Gutscher, H.; Bauer, N. Walking in “wild” and “tended” urban forests: The impact on psychological well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  296. Wolsko, C.; Lindberg, K. Experiencing connection with nature: The matrix of psychological well-being, mindfulness, and outdoor recreation. Ecopsychology 2013, 5, 80–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  297. Bricker, K.S.; Hendricks, W.W.; Aschenbrenner, C.A. Californians’ perceptions of the influence of parks and recreation on quality of life. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2016, 34, 64–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  298. Camargo, D.M.; Ramirez, P.C.; Fermino, R.C. Individual and environmental correlates to quality of life in park users in Colombia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  299. Lai, C.K.Y.; Lau, C.K.Y.; Kan, W.Y.; Lam, W.M.; Fung, C.Y.Y. The effect of horticultural therapy on the quality of life of palliative care patients. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 2017, 35, 278–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  300. McFarland, A.L.; Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M. The relationship between student use of campus green spaces and perceptions of quality of life. HortTechnology 2008, 18, 232–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  301. Shafer, C.S.; Lee, B.K.; Turner, S. A tale of three greenway trails: User perceptions related to quality of life. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2000, 49, 163–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  302. Sommerfeld, A.J.; Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M. Growing minds: Evaluating the effect of gardening on quality of life and physical activity level of older adults. HortTechnology 2010, 20, 705–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  303. Tiyarattanachai, R.; Hollmann, N.M. Green campus initiative and its impacts on quality of life of stakeholders in green and non-green campus universities. SpringerPlus 2016, 5, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  304. de Bloom, J.; Sianoja, M.; Korpela, K.; Tuomisto, M.; Lilja, A.; Geurts, S.; Kinnunen, U. Effects of park walks and relaxation exercises during lunch breaks on recovery from job stress: Two randomized controlled trials. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  305. Bodin, M.; Hartig, T. Does the outdoor environment matter for psychological restoration gained through running? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2003, 4, 141–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  306. Chen, Z.; He, Y.; Yu, Y. Enhanced functional connectivity properties of human brains during in-situ nature experience. PeerJ 2016, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  307. Hartig, T.; Mang, M.; Evans, G.W. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  308. Korpela, K.M.; Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Fuhrer, U. Restorative experience and self-regulation in favorite places. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 572–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  309. Weng, P.-Y.; Chiang, Y.-C. Psychological restoration through indoor and outdoor leisure activities. J. Leisure Res. 2014, 46, 203–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  310. Sonntag-Ostrom, E.; Nordin, M.; Lundell, Y.; Dolling, A.; Wiklund, U.; Karlsson, M.; Carlberg, B.; Jarvholm, L.S. Restorative effects of visits to urban and forest environments in patients with exhaustion disorder. Urban For. Urban Green. 2014, 13, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  311. von Lindern, E.; Bauer, N.; Frick, J.; Hunziker, M.; Hartig, T. Occupational engagement as a constraint on restoration during leisure time in forest settings. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 118, 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  312. Cervinka, R.; Schwab, M.; Schönbauer, R.; Hämmerle, I.; Pirgie, L.; Sudkamp, J. My garden—My mate? Perceived restorativeness of private gardens and its predictors. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 16, 182–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  313. Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Bowler, P.A. Psychological restoration in nature as a positive motivation for ecological behavior. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 590–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  314. Hipp, J.A.; Gulwadi, G.B.; Alves, S.; Sequeira, S. The relationship between perceived greenness and perceived restorativeness of university campuses and student-reported quality of life. Environ. Behav. 2016, 48, 1292–1308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  315. Twedt, E.; Rainey, R.M.; Proffitt, D.R. Designed natural spaces: Informal gardens are perceived to be more restorative than formal gardens. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  316. Abkar, M.; Kamal, M.S.M.; Maulan, S.; Mariapan, M.; Davoodi, S.R. Relationship between the preference and perceived restorative potential of urban landscapes. HortTechnology 2011, 21, 514–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  317. Nordh, H.; Alalouch, C.; Hartig, T. Assessing restorative components of small urban parks using conjoint methodology. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011, 10, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  318. Tenngart Ivarsson, C.; Hagerhall, C.M. The perceived restorativeness of gardens—Assessing the restorativeness of a mixed built and natural scene type. Urban For. Urban Green. 2008, 7, 107–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  319. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Hahn, K.S.; Daily, G.C.; Gross, J.J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8567–8572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  320. Hoffman, A.J.; Knight, L.F.M.; Wallach, J. Gardening activities, education, and self-esteem—Learning outside the classroom. Urban Educ. 2007, 42, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  321. Wood, C.J.; Pretty, J.; Griffin, M. A case-control study of the health and well-being benefits of allotment gardening. J. Public Health 2016, 38, E336–E344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  322. Fan, Y.; Das, K.V.; Chen, Q. Neighborhood green, social support, physical activity, and stress: Assessing the cumulative impact. Health Place 2011, 17, 1202–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  323. Grahn, P.; Stigsdotter, U.A. Landscape planning and stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2003, 2, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  324. Largo-Wight, E.; Wlyudka, P.S.; Merten, J.W.; Cuvelier, E.A. Effectiveness and feasibility of a 10-minute employee stress intervention: Outdoor booster break. J. Workplace Behav. Health 2017, 32, 159–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  325. Lottrup, L.; Grahn, P.; Stigsdotter, U.K. Workplace greenery and perceived level of stress: Benefits of access to a green outdoor environment at the workplace. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 110, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  326. Morita, E.; Fukuda, S.; Nagano, J.; Hamajima, N.; Yamamoto, H.; Iwai, Y.; Nakashima, T.; Ohira, H.; Shirakawa, T. Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health 2007, 121, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  327. Orsega-Smith, E.; Mowen, A.J.; Payne, L.L.; Godbey, G. The interaction of stress and park use on psycho-physiological health in older adults. J. Leisure Res. 2004, 36, 232–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  328. Roe, J.J.; Aspinall, P.A.; Thompson, C.W. Coping with stress in deprived urban neighborhoods: What is the role of green space according to life stage? Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  329. Roe, J.J.; Thompson, C.W.; Aspinall, P.A.; Brewer, M.J.; Duff, E.I.; Miller, D.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A. Green space and stress: Evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 4086–4103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  330. Sidenius, U.; Stigsdotter, U.K.; Poulsen, D.V.; Bondas, T. “I look at my own forest and fields in a different way”: The lived experience of nature-based therapy in a therapy garden when suffering from stress-related illness. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-Being 2017, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  331. Stigsdotter, U.K.; Grahn, P. Stressed individuals’ preferences for activities and environmental characteristics in green spaces. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011, 10, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  332. Thompson, C.W.; Roe, J.; Aspinall, P.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A.; Miller, D. More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 105, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  333. Thompson, C.W.; Aspinall, P.; Roe, J.; Robertson, L.; Miller, D. Mitigating stress and supporting health in deprived urban communities: The importance of green space and the social environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  334. Tyrvainen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  335. Van Den Berg, A.E.; Custers, M.H.G. Gardening promotes neuroendocrine and affective restoration from stress. J. Health Psychol. 2010, 16, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  336. Olafsdottir, G.; Cloke, P.; Vogele, C. Place, green exercise and stress: An exploration of lived experience and restorative effects. Health Place 2017, 46, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  337. Bjornstad, S.; Patil, G.G.; Raanaas, R.K. Nature contact and organizational support during office working hours: Benefits relating to stress reduction, subjective health complaints, and sick leave. Work—J. Prev. Assess. Rehabilit. 2016, 53, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  338. Hull, R.B.; Michael, S.E. Nature-based recreation, mood change, and stress restoration. Leisure Sci. 1995, 17, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  339. Ambrey, C.L. An investigation into the synergistic wellbeing benefits of greenspace and physical activity: Moving beyond the mean. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 19, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  340. Bertram, C.; Rehdanz, K. The role of urban green space for human well-being. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 120, 139–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  341. Carrus, G.; Scopelliti, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Colangelo, G.; Ferrini, F.; Salbitano, F.; Agrimi, M.; Portoghesi, L.; Sernenzato, P.; Sanesi, G. Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting, urban and peri-urban green areas. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 134, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  342. Cervinka, R.; Röderer, K.; Hefler, E. Are nature lovers happy? On various indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. J. Health Psychol. 2011, 17, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  343. Korpela, K.; De Bloom, J.; Sianoja, M.; Pasanen, T.; Kinnunen, U. Nature at home and at work: Naturally good? Links between window views, indoor plants, outdoor activities and employee well-being over one year. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 160, 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  344. Korpela, K.; Nummi, T.; Lipiainen, L.; De Bloom, J.; Sianoja, M.; Pasanen, T.; Kinnunen, U. Nature exposure predicts well-being trajectory groups among employees across two years. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 52, 81–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  345. Kothencz, G.; Kolcsar, R.; Cabrera-Barona, P.; Szilassi, P. Urban green space perception and its contribution to well-being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  346. Krekel, C.; Kolbe, J.; Wuestemann, H. The greener, the happier? The effect of urban land use on residential well-being. Ecol. Econ. 2016, 121, 117–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  347. Leck, C.; Upton, D.; Evans, N. Growing well-beings: The positive experience of care farms. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2015, 20, 745–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  348. Loureiro, A.; Veloso, S. Outdoor exercise, well-being and connectedness to nature. PSICO 2014, 45, 299–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  349. Luck, G.W.; Davidson, P.; Boxall, D.; Smallbone, L. Relations between urban bird and plant communities and human well-being and connection to nature. Conserv. Biol. 2011, 25, 816–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  350. Marselle, M.R.; Irvine, K.N.; Warber, S.L. Examining group walks in nature and multiple aspects of well-being: A large-scale study. Ecopsychology 2014, 6, 134–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  351. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. Happiness is in our nature: Exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2011, 12, 303–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  352. Holli-Anne, P.; Andrew, J.H. Nature involvement increases hedonic and eudaimonic well-being: A two-week experimental study. Ecopsychology 2014, 6, 148–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  353. Richardson, M.; Cormack, A.; McRobert, L.; Underhill, R. 30 days wild: Development and evaluation of a large-scale nature engagement campaign to improve well-being. PLoS ONE 2016, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  354. Scopelliti, M.; Carrus, G.; Adinolfi, C.; Suarez, G.; Colangelo, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Panno, A.; Sanesi, G. Staying in touch with nature and well-being in different income groups: The experience of urban parks in bogota. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 148, 139–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  355. Wang, X.; Rodiek, S.; Wu, C.; Chen, Y.; Li, Y. Stress recovery and restorative effects of viewing different urban park scenes in Shanghai, China. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 15, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  356. Webber, J.; Hinds, J.; Camic, P.M. The well-being of allotment gardeners: A mixed methodological study. Ecopsychology 2015, 7, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  357. McClimens, A.; Doel, M.; Ibbotson, R.; Partridge, N.; Muscroft, E.; Lockwood, L. How do the ‘peace gardens’ make you feel? Public space and personal wellbeing in city centre Sheffield. J. Urban Des. 2012, 17, 117–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  358. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M. Underestimating nearby nature: Affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 1101–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  359. Akobeng, A.K. Understanding randomised controlled trials. Arch. Dis. Child. 2005, 90, 840–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  360. Stoney, C.M.; Johnson, L.L. Design of clinical studies and trials. In Principles and Practice of Clinical Research, 3rd ed.; Gallin, J.I., Ognibene, F.P., Eds.; Academic Press: London, UK, 2012; pp. 225–242. [Google Scholar]
  361. South, E.C.; Kondo, M.C.; Cheney, R.A.; Branas, C.C. Neighborhood blight, stress, and health: A walking trial of urban greening and ambulatory heart rate. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, 909–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  362. Tilley, S.; Neale, C.; Patuano, A.; Cinderby, S. Older people’s experiences of mobility and mood in an urban environment: A mixed methods approach using electroencephalography (EEG) and interviews. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  363. Fleuret, S.; Atkinson, S. Wellbeing, health and geography: A critical review and research agenda. N. Z. Geogr. 2007, 63, 106–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  364. Gesler, W.M.; Kearns, R.A. Culture/Place/Health; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  365. Heslop, P.; Blair, P.; Flemming, P.; Hoghton, M.; Marriot, A.; Russ, L. Confidential Inquiry into Premature Deaths for People with Learning Disabilities (CIPOLD); Final Report; Norah Fry Research Centre, University of Bristol: Bristol, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  366. Honold, J.; Lakes, T.; Beyer, R.; van der Meer, E. Restoration in urban spaces: Nature views from home, greenways, and public parks. Environ. Behav. 2016, 48, 796–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  367. Ng, K.S.T.; Sia, A.; Ng, M.K.W.; Tan, C.T.Y.; Chan, H.Y.; Tan, C.H.; Rawtaer, I.; Feng, L.; Mahendran, R.; Larbi, A.; et al. Effects of horticultural therapy on asian older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  368. Liu, Y.; Ho, R.C.-M.; Mak, A. Interleukin (il)-6, tumour necrosis factor alpha (tnf-α) and soluble interleukin-2 receptors (sil-2r) are elevated in patients with major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis and meta-regression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 139, 230–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Article screening and selection process.
Figure 1. Article screening and selection process.
Ijerph 16 02081 g001
Table 1. Concept 1: Green space. Concept 2: Mental health and wellbeing.
Table 1. Concept 1: Green space. Concept 2: Mental health and wellbeing.
Concept 1
Alternative Terms/Synonyms (combined with OR)“green space*”, green*, “green environ*”, “green infrastruct*”, outdoor*, “outdoor experience*”, “nature experience*”, “natural space*”, “natural infrastruct*” “green health*”
Broader terms (combined with OR)“wilderness experience*”, “adventure therapy”, “outdoor therapy”, “nature therapy”, “nature connect*”, “near nature*”, ecotherap*, eco-therap*, “eco therap”, “green therap*”, “green-therap*”, “green exercis*”, “green-exercis*”
Narrower terms (combined with OR)ecopsychology, eco-psychology, “eco psychology”, “environmental psychology”, park, parks, forest*, horticultur*, “horticulture therap*”, garden*, allotment*, landscap*, highland*, wasteland*
Concept 2
Alternative Terms/Synonyms (combined with OR)“mental wellbeing”, “mental well-being”, “mental well being”, “mental health”, “emotional wellbeing”, “emotional well-being”, “emotional well being”, “emotional health”, “psychological wellbeing”, “psychological well-being”, “psychological well being”, “psychological health”
Broader terms (combined with OR)“self-concept”, “self concept”, “self-esteem”, “self esteem”, “self-image”, “self image”, “sense of coherence”, “sense of personal control”, “social wellbeing”, “social well-being”, “social well being”, “psychological issue*”, ruminat*, restorative
Narrower terms (combined with OR)“well-being”, wellbeing, “well being” “quality of life”, “life satisfaction”, emotion, depress*, anxi*, stress*, fear*, frustrate*, agress*, lonely, loneliness, isolation, happy, happiness, resilien*, optimis*, hope*, empower*
Table 2. The literature was divided into thematic groups based on the type of GS investigated (literature reviews are not included).
Table 2. The literature was divided into thematic groups based on the type of GS investigated (literature reviews are not included).
Type of Green Space
Group 1Horticulture, garden, allotment (n = 43)
Group 2Urban and mixed green space (n = 140)
Group 3Wild, natural or rural green space (n = 34)
Group 4Virtual or indoor green space (n = 24)
Table 3. The included studies were divided into groups based on their study design (some papers are represented in more than one group, i.e., a cross-sectional study with an RCT design).
Table 3. The included studies were divided into groups based on their study design (some papers are represented in more than one group, i.e., a cross-sectional study with an RCT design).
Type of Study# of Studies
Cross-sectional227
Longitudinal13
Review22
Historic, secondary narrative analysis1
Total263
RCT with crossover9
RCT, no crossover21
Non-randomized CT, cross over2
Non-randomized CT, no crossover6
Table 4. The studies were grouped, based on the continent where the study took place (not including reviews, n = 22).
Table 4. The studies were grouped, based on the continent where the study took place (not including reviews, n = 22).
Continent# of Studies
North America64
South America3
Asia29
South Africa1
New Zealand3
Australia18
Europe123
 -east5
 -west56
 -north38
 -south6
 -central14
 -across regions4
Total241
Table 5. The literature was charted based on the type of participant included in the study.
Table 5. The literature was charted based on the type of participant included in the study.
Participant Type# of Studies
General public, parents, twins79
University students, undergraduates, college students, students, graduate students, university students, healthy and physically inactive, male university students, pupils37
Psychiatric patients, individuals with clinical depression, mental health patients, stress-related mental health patients, adults with depression, adults with increased psychological stress, adults with mental health issues, individuals with burnout, exhaustion disorder, individual with stress, patients with depression, individuals with burnout, diagnosed with depression/anxiety/stress, females diagnosed with exhaustion disorder, mental disorder clients, individuals with stress injuries, people with mental health problems, people with significant mental ill-health, people diagnosed with chronic mental illness32
Older adults, over 65’s, elderly women15
Office workers, science park employees, university office staff, employees, workers11
Park users, allotment gardeners, recreational walkers, botanical garden visitors, forest users, GS users, greenway trail users, forest users/volunteers18
Female healthcare workers, health care workers, caregivers, rehabilitation team members, practitioners/decision-makers, public sector employees6
Athletes, physically active, active runners, experienced runners6
Dementia sufferers, cancer patients, palliative care patients, individuals with hypertension, chronic stroke patients7
Deprived communities, vulnerable, homeless women, female prisoners, deprived urban neighborhoods9
Rural elders, rural population, local residents (predominantly farmers), local residents (farmers and visitors)4
Adults with disabilities, individuals with disabilities, individuals with learning difficulties, people with disability4
Postmenopausal women, pregnant women, women4
Tourists, experienced physically fit backpackers3
Forest workers, woodland workers3
African Americans1
People building houses1
Alcoholics1
Table 6. The studies were grouped according to the primary mental HWB outcome investigated in the study. Some studies investigate more than one primary outcome.
Table 6. The studies were grouped according to the primary mental HWB outcome investigated in the study. Some studies investigate more than one primary outcome.
Primary Mental HWB Outcome# of Times Used
Mental health37
Wellbeing35
Stress34
Restorativeness20
Depression19
Quality of life13
Psychological wellbeing12
General health11
Mental wellbeing8
Life satisfaction6
Aggression4
Affect3
General wellbeing3
Anxiety, cognition, emotion, happiness, mood, psychological distress, self-esteem, stress reduction16 (2 papers for each of the health endpoints)
Chronic stress, clinical depression, emotional wellbeing, general preference for GS, health anxiety, job stress, mental stress, personal development, psychological health, psychological restoration, psychological stress, rumination, severe stress, social integration, stress-related mental illness, stress restoration, stressful life events17 (1 paper for each of the health endpoints)
Table 7. An overview of the tools used to measure mental HWB and the number of times each tool has been used (where the available primary reference for each tool is added in brackets).
Table 7. An overview of the tools used to measure mental HWB and the number of times each tool has been used (where the available primary reference for each tool is added in brackets).
AbbreviationHealth Outcome Measure# of Times Used
DOQDeveloped own questions and questionnaires38
PRSPerceived Restoration Scale [64,65]19
PANASPositive and Negative Affect Schedule [66]17
PSSPerceived Stress Scale [67]16
GHQGeneral Health Questionnaire [68]15
PSPopulation survey with incorporated health and wellbeing assessments14
WEMWBSWarwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale [69]13
HS SF-36Health Survey (SF-36) [70]10
BDIBeck Depression Inventory [71,72,73].9
POMSProfile of Mood States [74,75,76]8
CNConnected to nature [77]8
STAIState-Trait Anxiety Inventory [78]8
CES-DCentre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale for Research in the general population [79]7
NCPCNecker Cube Pattern Control [80]6
RSERosenberg self-esteem scale [81]5
K10Kessler Psychological Distress Scale [82]5
SWLSSatisfaction with Life Scale [83]4
WHOQOLWHO Quality of Life Questionnaire [84]4
DASSDepression Anxiety Stress Scale [85]4
PHQPatient Health Questionnaire [86]4
SMBQShirom-Melmed Burnout Questionnaire [87]4
SVSSubjective Vitality Scale [88]4
GDSGeriatric Depression Scale [89]4
HS SF-12Health Survey (SF-12) [90,91]3
EQ-5DEuroQol-5Dimensions [92]3
INSInclusion of Nature in Self scale [93]3
ICDThe International Classification of Diseases (WHO)3
PWBPsychological Wellbeing Scale [94]2
QPSQPSNordic-ADW; Nordic Questionnaire for Monitoring the Age Diverse Workforce [95]3
BAIBeck Anxiety Inventory [96]2
MHI-5Mental Health Inventory [70,97]2
MMSEMini-Mental state examination (Folstein test) [98]2
REQRecovery Experience Questionnaire [99]2
RRQRumination-Reflection Questionnaire [100]2
UWESUtrecht Work Engagement Scale [101]2
MAASMindful Attention and Awareness Scale [102]2
FSFeeling Scale, affective valence assessed by the FS [103]2
HAM-17Hamilton Depression Rating Scale [104]2
IPAInterpretative Phenomenological Analysis [105]2
SHCISubjective Health Complaints Inventory [106]2
LSIALife satisfaction inventory A [107]2
SPNEScale of Positive and Negative Experience [108]2
ZIPERSInventory of Personal Reactions, measuring affect [109]2
PAQPlace attachment questionnaire [110,111]2
MSSMood Survey Scale [112]1
GSESGeneral Self-efficacy Scale [113]1
CRC-QOLInstrument developed by [114,115] to measure quality of life1
TPITrier Personality Inventory [116]1
ABSAffect Balance Scale [117]1
AFIAttentional function index [118]1
BMBehaviour mapping [119]1
BRFSSBehavioural Risk Factor Surveillance System. (A United States health survey that looks at behavioural risk factors. It is run by Centre for Disease Control and Prevention and conducted by the individual state health departments and is the world’s largest such survey).1
BFBig Five [120]1
BSIBrief Symptom Inventory (anxiety) [121]1
BSBrooding Scale (Rumination) [122]1
BPAQBuss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire [123]1
CMAICohen-Mansfield agitation inventory [124]1
CSAI-2Competitive state anxiety inventory-2 [125]1
CD-RSConnor-Davidson resilience scale [126]1
CS-DDCornell scale for depression in Dementia [127]1
DSIDaily Stress Inventory [128]1
DEMQOLDementia quality of life instrument [129]1
SCL-90-RSymptom Check List [130]1
ES-SFEcology Scale, Short-Form [131]1
EPDSEdinburgh postnatal Depression Scale [132]1
EESElevating Experience Scale [133]1
EFIExercise-Induced Feeling Inventory [134,135]1
FASFelt Arousal Scale [136]1
ESEcocentrism scale. Use of natural environments for psychological restoration [137]1
GEBSGeneral Ecological Behaviour Scale [138]1
MUNSHHappiness Scale based on Memorial University of Newfoundland Scale of happiness [139]1
Urban HEART-2Health Equity Assessment and Response Tool-2 (Urban HEART-2) (http://sdh.umin.jp/heart/)1
HADHospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [140]1
HPLP-IIHealth promoting Lifestyle Profile II [141]1
ISSImportance for Survival Scale [142]1
IWG-2006International Wellbeing Group 2006. Used to evaluate self-reported, subjective well-being1
ISELInterpersonal Support Evaluation List [143]1
JSS-NJob Stress Survey [144]1
MDIMajor Depression inventory [145,146]1
MANSAManchester Short Assessment of Quality of life [147]1
MC-SDSMarlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale [148]1
MBI-GSKorean version of Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey [149]1
MINIMini International Neuropsychiatric Interview [150,151]1
MDBFMultidimensional Comfort Questionnaire [152]1
MMS-SFMultiple Mood Scale-Short form [153]1
NCQNature Contact Questionnaire [154]1
NMSNegative Mood Scale [155]1
OHIOxford Happiness Inventory [156]1
OHSOverall Happiness scale [157]1
PGISPersonal Growth Initiative Scale [158]1
PPWBPhysical and Psychological Wellbeing questionnaire [159]1
PGWBPsychological General Well-Being Index [160]1
PRQOLInfluence of parks and recreation on quality of life [161,162]1
QLCELQQuality of Life Concern in End of Life Questionnaire [163,164]1
QOLIQuality of Life Inventory (Frisch, 2009). [165]1
QLSQuality of Life Scale [166]1
QLS-ACIQuality of Life Scale in adults with chronic illness [165]1
QOLTQuality of Life tool [167]1
QEWBQuestionnaire for Eudaimonic Well-Being [168]1
QEACLQuestionnaires measuring Eudemonia, Apprehension, childhood location. Environments and experiential states. Eliciting participants feelings about place [65]1
MOS SF-20Rand medical Outcomes Study Health survey (MOS SF-20) [169,170]1
RVPReason for Visiting the Park, 23-item scale [171,172,173]1
REPRecreation Experience Preference scales [174]1
ROSRestorative Outcome Scale [175,176]1
RQERestorative quality in environments [65]1
RCASRole conflict and ambiguity scales [177]1
SMSSense of Meaning Scale [178]1
SCI-93Stress and crisis inventory [179,180]1
SEESSubjective Exercise Experiences Scale [181]1
BSCSSelf-Control Scale [182]1
SRRSSelf-rating restoration scale [183]1
SRSASelf-reported stress and arousal [184]1
SOCSense of coherence [185]1
SHAIShort Health Anxiety Inventory [186]1
SI-happyMeasuring happiness with a single-item scale [187]1
SCTSSocial Cohesion and Trust Scale [188]1
SPSSocial Provisions Scale [189]1
SWSStress at Work Scale by the Behavioural Science Institute, Korea university (1999), occupational stress1
SRI-MFStress response inventory-modified form [190]1
SRS-18Stress Response Scale [191]1
TAPTaylor Aggression Paradigm [192]1
TMMModel of mood [193]1
CMMSCurrent mood measurement scale (The best/worst ever; scale taken from [194]1
TFI-CSTherapeutic Factors Inventory–Cohesiveness Scale [195]1
VQVolitional Questionnaire [196]1
WSRIWorkers Stress Response Inventory; an extended version of the Stress Response Inventory-Modified from [190,197]1
WOSWorkplace Ostracism Scale [198]1
WUSWildernism-Urbanism Scale [199]1
ZSDSZung self-rating depression scale [200]1
Table 8. Health outcome measure; the most commonly used tools included in studies assessing the associations between GS and mental HWB.
Table 8. Health outcome measure; the most commonly used tools included in studies assessing the associations between GS and mental HWB.
Paper NumberPrimary Health OutcomeHealth Outcome Measure
1[201]AffectBDI, SCL-90-R,
2[202]AffectSTAI, RRQ, PANAS
3[203]AffectQEACL
4[204]AggressionWOS
5[205]AggressionTAP, BPAQ, BSCS, PRS; PANAS
6[206]AnxietyCSAI-2,
7[207]AnxietySTAI
8[208]Chronic stressPSS
9[209]DepressionBDI
10[210]DepressionPHQ
11[211]DepressionBRFSS, PHQ
12[60]DepressionBDI, HAM-17, STAI
13[212]DepressionDOQ
14[39]DepressionBDI, AFI, BS, PRS
15[213]DepressionBDI, STAI, PANAS, PSS, TFI-CS
16[214]DepressionIBD, ROS, WEMWBS
17[215]DepressionGDS
18[216]DepressionGDS
19[217]DepressionGDS
20[218]DepressionGHQ
21[219]DepressionCES-D, DOQ
22[220]DepressionMINI, ICD, PSS, WHOQOL
23[221]DepressionEPDS
24[222]DepressionZSDS
25[223]DepressionCES-D
26[224]DepressionBDI
27[225]DepressionCES-D
28[226]DepressionPHQ
29[227]EmotionPOMS
30[228]Emotional wellbeingUrban HEART-2
31[229]General healthGHQ
32[230]General healthGHQ, POPS
33[231]General healthHS SF-36, PSS, DOQ
34[232]General healthHS SF-36, PSS
35[233]General healthPOMS, PANAS, ROS, SVS
36[24]General healthPOPS, HS SF-36, GHQ,
37[234]General healthPOPS
38[235]General healthHS SF-12,
39[236]General wellbeingRVP, RQE, SWLS, SPNE
40[237]General wellbeingOHI
41[28]General wellbeingEQ-5D, GHQ, DOQ, RSE, POMS
42[238]HappinessSWLS, PANAS
43[239]Health anxietySHAI
44[240]Job stressSWS
45[241]Life satisfactionDOQ
46[242]Life satisfactionDOQ
47[243]Life satisfactionDOQ
48[244]Life SatisfactionLSIA
49[245]Mental healthGHQ
50[25]Mental healthGHQ
51[246]Mental healthHS SF-36, K10
52[247]Mental healthDOQ, CES-D, BAI, RCAS
53[248]Mental healthGHQ
54[249]Mental healthPOPS, GHQ
55[1]Mental healthHPLP-II, BDI
56[250]Mental healthDASS
57[251]Mental healthDASS, MANSA
58[252]Mental healthPHQ, PSS, BSI
59[253]Mental healthDASS,
60[254]Mental healthPANAS, RSE
61[255]Mental healthGHQ
62[256]Mental healthCES-D
63[257]Mental healthPANAS, HAM-17
64[258]Mental healthDOQ
65[259]Mental healthPS, GHQ, WEMWBS
66[260]Mental healthPSQ, GSES, MAAS
67[261]Mental healthPS
68[262]Mental healthIPA
69[41]Mental healthVQ
70[263]Mental healthRSE, PSS, POMS
71[264]Mental healthMHI-5
72[265]Mental healthMHI-5
73[266]Mental healthHS SF-12,
74[267]Mental healthPS, K10,
75[268]Mental healthHS SF-36, DOQ,
76[269]Mental healthHS SF-36
77[270]Mental healthGHQ
78[271]Mental healthDASS
79[272]Mental healthGHQ, DOQ
80[273]Mental healthWEMEBS, HS SF-12
81[274]Mental healthIPA
82[275]Mental healthWEMWBS
83[52]Mental stressSRS-18
84[276]Mental wellbeingPS, HS SF-36, K10, BF, DOQ
85[277]Mental wellbeingREP, PAQ
86[278]Mental wellbeingDOQ, WEMWBS,
87[279]Mental wellbeingPSS, WEMWBS
88[280]Mental wellbeingWEMWBS
89[281]Mental wellbeingQOLI, BDI
90[282]MoodRSE, TMD
91[283]MoodTBES, DOQ
92[284]Personal developmentPGIS, QLS-ACI
93[285]Psychological distressK10
94[286]Psychological distressDOQ
95[54]Psychological healthMMSE, GDS, PS
96[287]Psychological restorationES
97[288]Psychological stressPRS, CN
98[289]Psychological wellbeingABS
99[290]Psychological wellbeingFS, FAS, MSS
100[291]Psychological wellbeingPRS, PANAS, PSS, CES-D, MUNSH, SPW
101[292]Psychological wellbeingDOQ
102[293]Psychological wellbeingCN, WHOQOL
103[294]Psychological wellbeingSTAI, PWB
104[295]Psychological wellbeingSRSMS
105[55]Psychological wellbeingDOQ, GHQ, SCTS, SHCI
106[296]Psychological wellbeingCN, MAAS, FS, SPNE, SVS
107[297]Quality of lifePRQOL
108[298]Quality of lifeEQ-5D
109[299]Quality of lifeQLCELQ
110[300]Quality of lifeCRC-QOL
111[301]Quality of lifeDOQ
112[302]Quality of lifeLSIA
113[61]Quality of lifeQOLT
114[303]Quality of lifeDOQ
115[62]Quality of lifeQLS
116[43]Quality of lifeDEMQOL, CS-DD, CMAI, MMSE
117[304]RecoveryREQ, DOQ
118[305]RestorativenessEFI, NMS,
119[306]RestorativenessPOMS, PRS, NCPC
120[50]RestorativenessZIPERS, NCPC
121[50]RestorativenessSRRS
122[307]RestorativenessWUS, ZIPERS, OHS
123[308]RestorativenessDOQ, PRS
124[59]RestorativenessDOQ, SRSA
125[309]RestorativenessNCPC, STAI
126[310]RestorativenessPRS, SMBQ, HAD, NCPC, DOQ
127[142]RestorativenessISS, PRS,
128[311]RestorativenessDOQ
129[33]RestorativenessPRS
130[312]RestorativenessPRS, INS
130[313]RestorativenessPRS, GEBS, MC-SDS,
131[314]RestorativenessWHOQOL, PRS,
132[315]RestorativenessDOQ
133[316]RestorativenessPRS
134[317]RestorativenessDOQ
135[318]RestorativenessPRS
135[306]RestorativenessPOMS, PRS, NCPC
136[319]RuminationRRQ
137[320]Self-esteemDOQ
138[321]Self-esteemRSE, POMS, GHQ
139[63]StressICD, SMBQ
140[322]StressPS
141[323]StressDOQ
142[53]StressPSS, SPS, HS SF-36
143[57]StressSRI-MF
144[58]StressMBI-GS, WSRI, REQ
145[32]StressCES-D
146[324]StressPSQ
147[154]StressPSQ, BRFSS
148[325]StressDOQ
149[326]StressMMS-SF, STAI
150[327]StressDSI, MOS SF-20
151[328]StressPSS, WEMWBS
152[329]StressPSS, WEMWBS, PS
153[37]StressDOQ, SMBQ
154[330]StressICD, BM
155[331]StressSCI-93, EQ-5D,
156[332]StressPSS, WEMWBS, PS
157[333]StressPSS, WEMWBS, PS
158[334]StressROS, PRS, PANAS
159[335]StressPANAS
160[333]StressPSS, WEMWBS, PS
161[336]Stress reductionPANAS, NCPC
162[337]Stress reductionNCQ, QPS, JSS-N, SHCI, DOQ
163[36]Stress related mental illnessSMBQ, BDI, BAI, PGWB
164[338]Stress restorationTMM
165[56]Stress restorationPS
166[246]WellbeingHS SF-36, DOQ
167[339]WellbeingHS SF-36, K10
168[340]WellbeingPS
169[45]WellbeingDOQ
170[341]WellbeingPRS
171[342]WellbeingMDBF, SWLS, TPI, HS SF-36
172[343]WellbeingSVS, UWES, QPS
173[344]WellbeingSVS, UWES, QPS
174[345]WellbeingDOQ
175[346]WellbeingPS
176[347]WellbeingWEMWBS, CD-RS, SOC, PS, DOQ
177[348]WellbeingCN, PANAS, SEES
178[349]WellbeingDOQ, IWG-2006, CN
179[350]WellbeingMDI, PSS, PANAS, WEMWBS, ISEL
180[351]WellbeingPWB, PANAS, SWLS, ES-SF
181[352]WellbeingPANAS, EES, SMS, CN
182[353]WellbeingDOQ, OHI, INS
183[354]WellbeingCN, PRS, PAQ, PANAS, PPWB
184[355]WellbeingSTAI, PRS
185[356]WellbeingCN, QEWB, WHOQOL
186[357]WellbeingDOQ
187[358]WellbeingPANAS, INS
Table 9. Overview of the ten most used health endpoints and the tools most commonly used to assess them – the number next to the tool is the number of studies where it was used.
Table 9. Overview of the ten most used health endpoints and the tools most commonly used to assess them – the number next to the tool is the number of studies where it was used.
Mental HealthWellbeingStressRestorativenessDepressionQuality of LifePsychological WellbeingGeneral HealthMental WellbeingLife Satisfaction
GHQ8DOQ7PSS6PRS11BDI5DOQ2DOQ2GHQ3WEMWBS3DOQ3
DASS4PANAS6PS5DOQ6CES-D3CS-DD1CN2POPS3DOQ2LSIA1
DOQ4CN5WEMWBS5NCPC5GDS3CMAI1FS2HS SF-363BDI1
HS SF-363HS SF-363DOQ3POMS2PHQ3CRC-QOL1ABS1PSS2BF1
PS3PRS3ICD2ZIPERS2DOQ2DEMQOL1CES-D1DOQ1HS SF-361
RSE2PS3PANAS2EFI1PSS2EQ-5D1FAS1HS SF-121K101
WEMWBS2INS2PSQ2HAD1PHQ2LSIA1GHQ1PANAS1PAQ1
CES-D2QPS2SMBQ2ISS1STAI2MMSE1MSS1POMS1PS1
HS SF-122SVS2BM1NMS1AFI1QLCELQ1MUNSH1ROS1PSS1
IPA2SWLS2BRFSS1SMBQ1BRFSS1QLS1PANAS1SVS1QOLI1
K102UWES2CES-D1SRRS1BS1QOLT1PRS1 REP1
MHI-52WEMWBS2DSI1SRSA1EPDS1 PSS1
PANAS2CD-RS1EQ-5D1STAI1GHQ1 PWB1
PSS2EES1HS SF-361 IBD1 SCTS1
BAI1ES-SF1MBI-GS1 ICD1 SHCI1
POMS1ISEL1MMS-SF1 MINI1 SPW1
BSI1IWG-20061MOS SF-201 PANAS1 SRSMS1
GSES1K101PRS1 PRS1 STAI1
HPLP-II1MDBF1REQ1 ROS1 SVS1
MANSA1MDI1ROS1 WEMWBS1 WHOQOL1
MAAS1OHI1SCI-931 WHOQOL1
PHQ1PAQ1SPS1 ZSDS1
POPS1PPWB1SRI-MF1
PSQ1PSS1STAI1
RCAS1PWB1WSRI1
VQ1QEWB1
SEES1
SMS1
SOC1
STAI1
TPI1
WHOQOL1
Table 10. Randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of GS on mental health.
Table 10. Randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of GS on mental health.
Paper NumberCountry/Green SpaceParticipant Type# of Subjects/Male/FemaleMean Age/min/maxPositive/NegativeHealth OutcomeHealth AssessmentGreen Space AssessmentQuantitative/Qualitative Study MethodsIntervention/Control GroupComments
2-arm randomised controlled crossover design
[50] Study 2 *UK/NaturalUniversity students17/7/1023.18 (±6.23)/18/43Mixed/NegativeRestorativenessQuestionnaire, heart ratePhotos, videos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQuantitativeWalk through low prospect-high refuge natural environment/Walk through high prospect-low refuge environmentSmall study with indication the GS is restorative only when there is an open aspect and few places where someone might hide.
[361] USA/UrbanGeneral public12/8/4x/x/xPositiveStress, HealthHeart rateObservations. No quantitative assessmentQuantitativeSelf-paced walk in local neighbourhood past sites receiving greening treatment/Self-paced walk in local neighbourhood past sites not receiving greening treatmentSmall study only measuring heart rate. Based on heart rate only, the results indicated that in-view proximity to a greened vacant lot decreased heart rate compared to in-view proximity to a non-greened vacant lot.
[57]Korea/NaturalUniversity students41/14/27x/18/35PositiveStressCytokine serum levels, questionnaireNo quantitative or qualitative assessmentQuantitative/Qualitative2 h exposure to a forest environment/2 h exposure to an urban environmentSmall study indicating the level of somatic and depressive symptoms decrease significantly after exposure to forest environments. Weak design; no before-and-after measurements allowing for comparison.
[57]Japan/NaturalMale university students12/12/021.3 (±1.1)/20/23Mixed/NegativeRestorativenessCortisol, blood pressure, pulse rate, questionnaireNo quantitative or qualitative assessmentQuantitative/Qualitative15 min visits to forest environments/15 min visits to urban environmentsSmall study with no clear conclusion from the quantitative data about the effect of GS. Subjective evaluation data showed significantly more positive responses after exposure to forest environments.
[326]Japan/NaturalGeneral public498/244/25456.2 (±10.6)/20/xPositiveStress, Mental healthQuestionnaireNo quantitative or qualitative assessmentQualitative2 x forest walks/2 days where a forest was not visitedRelatively large number of participants, but no quantitative objective data. The study concluded that a forest environment significantly reduces hostility and depression. The largest benefit was seen for the most stressed participants.
[318]Sweden/Virtual, indoorUndergraduates74/x/xx/x/xPositiveRestorativenessQuestionnaire852 colour photos of two gardens were sampled. Final sample consisted of 12 photos for each garden.Qualitative2 ha large spacious garden with large as well as small garden rooms and many views without buildings/Small and detailed courtyard garden of 13 × 17 m. Views at eye-level always include buildings.A study using only one qualitative measure and no quantitative data. The data showed that gardens are likely to be restorative to varying degrees, depending on the design and the surroundings of the garden.
[233]Japan/UrbanUndergraduates45/45/021.13 (±1.25)/x/xPositiveGeneral healthQuestionnairePhotos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeA 15 min walk in a forest environment/A 15 min walk in an urban environmentRelatively small study looking only at young men. Four different validated questionnaires were used; some revealed significant positive effects of the forest environment, some did not. The results indicated the combination of activity and GS results in greater psychological benefits. The feelings of vigour, positive effects, subjective recovery, and vitality were stronger in the forest environment.
[209]USA/UrbanPeople with depression20/8/1226/x/xMixed/NegativeCognition, AffectInterview, questionnaireSatellite GPS images. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitative50 min walk in natural setting/50 min walk in urban settingSmall study with no clear conclusions about the effect of GS.
2-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
AuthorCountry/Green spaceParticipant type# of subjects/male/femaleMean age/min/maxPositive/NegativeHealth outcomeHealth assessmentGreen space assessmentQuantitative/QualitativeIntervention/Control groupComments
[60]Korea/NaturalPatients with history of stroke59/40/1960.8 (±9.1)/36/79PositiveDepression. anxietyQuestionnaire, physiological measurement (Reactive oxygen metabolites (dROM). Biological antioxidant potential (BAPs))No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQuantitative/QualitativePatients randomly assigned to a forest therapy group or an urban control group.The study found that forest therapy can significantly lower oxidative stress and improve anti-oxidative capacity for patients with a history of stroke. High levels of oxidative stress and reduced anti-oxidative capacity are indicative of depression and anxiety.
[306]China/UrbanCollege students32/16/1620.6 (±1.6)/x/xPositiveCognition, restorativenessQuestionnaire, EEGPhotos. Quantification of green elements, buildings and paved areas of the two environments used.Quantitative/Qualitative20 min exposure to one of two environments: A wooded campus garden/A traffic island under an elevated highwayPositive EEG results identified from a brief exposure to photographs of nature compared to urban environment (20 min).
[319]USA/UrbanGeneral public30/14/1626.6/x/xPositiveRuminationQuestionnaire, neural activity in the sgPFCDetailed description of the two walk.Quantitative/Qualitative5.3 km nature walk/5.3 km urban walkThe study found a significant reduction in self-reported rumination driven by a decreased cerebral blood flow in the sgPFC for the nature group, but not for the urban group.
[335]Netherlands/Horticulture, gardenGeneral public30/8/2257.6 (±8.49)/38/79Positive/mixedStressQuestionnaire, salivary cortisolVery brief description of allotment complex but no in depth description or quantitative measures of the GS investigatedQuantitative/QualitativeAfter stress induction: 30 min of outdoor gardening in own allotment/30 min of indoor reading in allotment home with no view of nature (popular magazines chosen by researcher)The small study found that both reading and gardening showed a significant reduction in cortisol levels after stress. Cortisol levels were lower after gardening compared to reading, but the difference was not significant. Positive mood was significantly higher after gardening compared to reading. There were indications that gardening is more restorative after stress than reading.
[204] – study 1USA/Virtual, indoorGeneral public86/22/6435.47 (±14.05)/x/xPositiveAggressionQuestionnaire QualitativeOstracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature picturesThe qualitative study found that among participants with a high feeling of ostracism, those who viewed nature pictures reported a significantly lower level of aggression than those who viewed urban pictures. The authors concluded that nature exposure can counteract the relationship between ostracism and aggression.
[289]USA/Virtual, indoorOlder adults62/6/56x/x/xMixed, negativePhysiological wellbeingQuestionnaireNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeHorticultural activity program in a care home, once a week for 7 weeks/Normal daily activities in a care home, over 7 weeksThe study found no statistically significant differences in the effect of a horticultural activity program on physiological wellbeing of older adults in a care home. However, there were some indications that horticultural activities may have a positive effect on wellbeing.
[202]USA/UrbanGeneral public60/27/3322.9/x/xMixedAffect, cognitionQuestionnairePhotos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeA nature walk/An urban walkThe study found significant evidence that a nature walk improves affect, but no clear evidence that it improves cognition.
[324]USA/UrbanUniversity office staff37/34/348.8/x/xPositiveStressQuestionnaireNo description or quantitative measures of the GS investigated.QualitativeWork breaks over 4 weeks: 10–15 min outdoor booster break/standard work breakThe small qualitative study found that a 10–15 min outdoor booster break during the work day results in a significantly greater reduction in stress than an indoor work break.
[320]USA/Horticulture, gardenUndergraduates32/x/xx/18/32Mixed/NegativeSelf-esteemQuestionnaireNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitative4 h of gardening work over a period of 3 weeks/No gardening activitiesThis small qualitative study found no significant differences regarding ethnocentrism and self-esteem, in relation to the effects of GS. There were indications that gardening can positively affect self-esteem.
[271]Serbia/Horticulture, gardenPsychiatric patients30/9/2145.35 (±10.16)/25/65MixedMental healthQuestionnaireMap, photos and short description. No in-depth quality assessment or quantitative measuresQualitativeFour weeks (12 sessions) of horticultural therapy/Four weeks of occupational art therapyThe small qualitative study found a significantly larger reduction in stress after horticultural therapy compared to occupational art therapy. However, no significant differences were identified for anxiety or depression after the two treatments.
3-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
AuthorCountry/Green spaceParticipant type# of subjects/male/femaleMean age/min/maxPositive/NegativeHealth outcomeHealth assessmentGreen space assessmentQuantitative/QualitativeIntervention/Control groupComments
[307]-Study 2 **USA/UrbanCollege students34/17/1720/x/xPositiveRestorativenessQuestionnaire, physiological measurements (blood pressure and pulse)Very brief description of GSQuantitative/QualitativeCollege students were randomly assigned to a nature walk, an urban walk, or a relaxation conditionSmall study showing that happiness and positive affect significantly increase and anger and aggression significantly decreased after being in a natural environment compared to an urban environment.
[304]Finland/UrbanOffice workers153/137/2047.2/x/xMixedStress, wellbeingQuestionnaire (paper format and mobile text messages)No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativePark walk/relaxation exercises/usual break activitiesThe study found no clear conclusions about the effect of park walks on employees’ wellbeing. The effects on wellbeing were of a small magnitude and short duration.
[338]USA/UrbanPark visitors108/48/6022/x/xMixed, negativeStress reduction, restorativenessQuestionnaireNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitative
[282]UK/UrbanAdults with mental health issues53/20/3353 (±15.4)/21/83MixedMood, self-esteemQuestionnaireNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeThree health-promoting interventions: Walking in GS/Swimming/Quizzes, bingo, games, crafts and musicThe study found that green exercise was as health-promoting for people experiencing mental ill health as existing non-green interventions. There was no conclusive evidence that GS activity was more health-promoting than other activities.
[217]USA/Horticulture, gardenOlder adults with mild to moderate depression39/16/2374.3 (±6.40)/x/xPositiveDepressionQuestionnaire, focus groupsNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeThe participants were randomly assigned to one of 3 treatments; walk alone, guided imagery, or art therapy.Small study surmising that GS as well as art interventions were helpful in improving mood and overall attitude. However, only subjective, anecdotal evidence was explored.
[215]USA/Horticulture, gardenOlder adults with mild to moderate depression39/x/x75/x/xPositive/MixedDepressionQuestionnaire, focus groupsNo in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeThe participants were randomly assigned to one of 3 treatments; walk alone, group walking, or art therapy.The study found that assisted and unassisted GS walks as well as art therapy interventions can significantly reduce symptoms of depression.
[336]Iceland/UrbanUniversity students18/9/9 ***x/x/xMixed/negativeStressInterviews, observationsPhotos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeThree treatments for alleviation of stress: Walking in the gym/Walking in nature/Watching nature on TVThe very small study using personal narratives involving restoration found no clear conclusions about the effect of GS on stress.
4-arm randomised controlled crossover design
AuthorCountry/Green spaceParticipant type# of subjects/male/femaleMean age/min/maxPositive/NegativeHealth outcomeHealth assessmentGreen space assessmentQuantitative/QualitativeIntervention/Control groupComments
[310]Sweden/NaturalFemales diagnosed with exhaustion disorder20/0/2041.6 (±7.3)/24/55PositiveRestorativenessQuestionnaire, heart rate, blood pressure, heart rate recoveryPhotos, detailed description. No quantitative assessment. Quantitative/Qualitative90 min test procedure in 3 different forest environments/and in 1 city environmentSmall study indicating significantly higher perceived restorativeness in the forest environments compared to the city.
4-arm randomised controlled design, no crossover
AuthorCountry/Green spaceParticipant type# of subjects/male/femaleMean age/min/maxPositive/NegativeHealth outcomeHealth assessmentGreen space assessmentQuantitative/qualitativeIntervention/Control groupComments
[227]Taiwan/UrbanCollege students116/52/6420.85 (±1.14)/x/xMixed/NegativeEmotion, attentionQuestionnairePhotos used to quantify the level of greenness, aerial mapsQuantitative/QualitativeWalking or jogging in natural environment/Walking or jogging in built environmentThe study found no clear conclusions about the effect of GS and exercise on emotion and attention. The key finding is the indication that walking in a setting with at least 40% visible greenness elicits the largest benefits.
[204] – study 2USA/Virtual, indoorGeneral public150/48/10236.87 (±13.30)/x/xMixedAggressionQuestionnairePhotos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeOstracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature picturesThe study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing nature photos to moderate the relationship between ostracism and aggression. There were some indications that viewing nature photos can alleviate aggressive responses following ostracism.
[204] – study 3USA/Virtual, indoorGeneral public144/47/9735.47 (±11.99)/x/xMixedAggressionQuestionnairePhotos. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeOstracised individuals exposed to urban or nature pictures/Non-ostracised individuals exposed to urban or nature picturesThe study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing nature photos to moderate the relationship between ostracism and aggression. There were some indications that viewing nature photos can alleviate aggressive responses following ostracism.
[205]China/Virtual, indoorUndergraduate students118/25/9321.23 (±2.26)Mixed, negativeAggression, moodQuestionnaireVideo. No in-depth quality assessment, description, or quantitative measuresQualitativeDepleted individuals exposed to a natural or urban video/Non-depleted individuals exposed to a natural or urban videoThe study found no clear conclusions about the effect of viewing a natural video to counteract aggression after depletion. The study suggests that watching a natural video helps to restore self-control after depletion.
x = data missing. ± = standard deviation around the mean. * This paper consists of three small studies; only one of which is presented in this table (study 2). ** This paper consists of two studies; only study 2 is presented in this table. *** Only three participants are described in the results; one for each treatment.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wendelboe-Nelson, C.; Kelly, S.; Kennedy, M.; Cherrie, J.W. A Scoping Review Mapping Research on Green Space and Associated Mental Health Benefits. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2081. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122081

AMA Style

Wendelboe-Nelson C, Kelly S, Kennedy M, Cherrie JW. A Scoping Review Mapping Research on Green Space and Associated Mental Health Benefits. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(12):2081. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122081

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wendelboe-Nelson, Charlotte, Sarah Kelly, Marion Kennedy, and John W. Cherrie. 2019. "A Scoping Review Mapping Research on Green Space and Associated Mental Health Benefits" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 12: 2081. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122081

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop