Next Article in Journal
The Influence of a Modified 3rd Generation Cementation Technique and Vaccum Mixing of Bone Cement on the Bone Cement Implantation Syndrome (BCIS) in Geriatric Patients with Cemented Hemiarthroplasty for Femoral Neck Fractures
Next Article in Special Issue
Qualitative Verbal Fluency Components as Prognostic Factors for Developing Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Population-Based HELIAD Cohort
Previous Article in Journal
Superior Mesenteric Artery Occlusion Caused by Infective Endocarditis and Worsened by Mycotic Aneurysm and Intracranial Hemorrhage: A Case Report
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition

by
Ioannis Liampas
1,*,
Vasileios Siokas
1,
Constantine George Lyketsos
2 and
Efthimios Dardiotis
1,2
1
Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Larissa, Faculty of Medicine, University of Thessaly, 41100 Larissa, Greece
2
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Medicina 2022, 58(11), 1586; https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58111586
Submission received: 19 August 2022 / Revised: 12 October 2022 / Accepted: 1 November 2022 / Published: 3 November 2022

Abstract

:
Background and Objectives: To explore whether specific Neuropsychiatric Symptoms (NPS) are related to worse performance in particular cognitive domains. Materials and Methods: A cross-sectional analysis of the baseline evaluations of older (≥60 years), cognitively unimpaired (CU) participants from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set was performed. Data were derived from 43 Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers. Cognitively impaired individuals, participants with psychiatric disorders and/or under treatment with antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative, or hypnotic agents were excluded. NPS were assessed using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire. The association of NPS with participants’ performance on episodic memory, semantic memory, language, attention, processing speed and executive function was analysed using an adjusted (considering important demographic and medical factors) multivariate general linear model. Results: A total of 7179 CU, older, predominantly female, Caucasian, and well-educated participants were included in the present analysis. Among them, 1856 individuals had one or more NPS. Our analysis revealed that moderate/severe anxiety was related to worse performance on semantic memory, attention and executive function, the presence of hallucinations was linked to worse processing speed and executive function scores, while the presence of elation/euphoria and aberrant motor behaviour were associated with poorer attention and language performance, respectively. In the context of a secondary, exploratory analysis, the presence of moderate/severe delusions was related to worse processing speed and executive function performance. Conclusions: The relationship between specific NPS and worse performance in particular cognitive domains could inform the formulation of individualized preventive strategies directed to the ‘‘fortification’’ of specific cognitive functions in CU individuals with NPS.

1. Introduction

Neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) are prevalent among individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia [1]. Of note, the frequency and severity of NPS increase in parallel with the severity of cognitive impairment in both conditions [2,3]. In cognitively unimpaired (CU) older individuals, NPS are associated with more rapid cognitive decline and higher risk of incident MCI and dementia [4,5,6]. Of importance, NPS are found in the majority (~60%) of CU individuals who progress to MCI or dementia, often manifesting for some years prior to the diagnosis of any cognitive disorder [7]. Therefore, the identification of NPS in CU individuals ought to increase clinical vigilance regarding the development of incident cognitive impairment.
Unfortunately, the management of NPS has not proven sufficient for altering the course of cognitive decline in older adults. Many traditional medicinal options for NPS, including antipsychotics, benzodiazepines and tricyclic antidepressants, have a well-established detrimental impact on cognition themselves [8,9,10]. Moreover, newer psychotropic agents and namely selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors appear ineffective in improving cognitive outcomes with occasional evidence suggesting that they may even exert a deleterious effect on cognition, as well [11,12,13]. In parallel, there is only sparse and conflicting evidence indicating that psychological-behavioural interventions may improve cognitive performance or alter the course of cognitive decline in older adults with NPS [14,15,16,17,18].
Given the suboptimal efficacy of the available therapeutic options targeting NPS on cognitive outcomes, the implementation of preventive interventions targeting cognition to slow down or preclude cognitive decline, MCI as well as dementia development in CU older adults with NPS is probably warranted. However, it is uncertain which specific cognitive domains (if any) are affected by the presence of particular NPS. Previous researchers have mostly focused on composite neuropsychiatric and cognitive measures while the specific associations between individual NPS and cognitive domains have not been sufficiently explored (only depression, anxiety and sleep disorders have been individually addressed in greater detail) [5,6,19,20,21]. The mapping of the exact correspondence between NPS and affected cognitive domains is, nonetheless, of crucial importance since it could inform the formulation of individualized preventive strategies directed to the ‘‘fortification’’ of cognitive functions specifically affected by the presence of particular NPS.
Therefore, the aim in undertaking the present study was to explore the relationship between NPS and cognitive performance in CU older (≥60 years) individuals and determine whether specific NPS are related to worse performance in particular cognitive domains. For this purpose, we capitalized on cross-sectional data from the Uniform Data Set (UDS), a standardized set of prospectively collected data on individuals from multiple Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers (ADRCs) across the United States.

2. Materials and Methods

The UDS was initiated in 2005 and continues to be stewarded by the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC). It constitutes the central repository of uniform clinical, neuropathologic and genetic data from all National Institute on Aging/NIH—funded ADRCs across the United States. UDS is freely available to research scientists upon request (https://naccdata.org/). The Institutional Review Boards overseeing each ADRC approved all procedures before the initiation of the study. Informed consent is obtained from all participants or surrogates prior to participation. The rationale, design, data collection process, as well as other key methodological features of the UDS have been described elsewhere [22,23,24]. In brief, data are collected at participating ADRCs by trained physicians and clinic personnel during in-person (office or home) visits or telephone calls, using a common, standardized evaluation protocol. Follow-up assessments are conducted on an approximately annual basis. Data collected are primarily focused on Alzheimer’s disease (AD), although information is also gathered about a variety of other neuropsychiatric disorders.

2.1. Eligibility Criteria and Selection of Older Cognitively Unimpaired Participants

The current study was based on data from the baseline (first visit) evaluations of older (≥60 years) CU NACC participants between September 2005 (inception of the UDS) and December 2021 (data freeze) and used data from 43 ADRCs. CU was defined by absence of a cognitive diagnosis of dementia, MCI or cognitive impairment not MCI (see below). Participants being treated with an FDA-approved medication for AD (i.e., tacrine, donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine and memantine) were excluded as well (receiving such medication challenged the credibility of the physician-based diagnosis). Individuals with a clinician-based diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder (depression, anxiety, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder or other psychiatric conditions) were also excluded (to exclude long-standing neuropsychiatric conditions). Finally, participants receiving antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative or hypnotic agents were excluded from the current research due to the potential effect of these medication on neuropsychiatric (improving) and cognitive (impeding) parameters.
CU participants were defined by the absence of dementia, MCI and “cognitive impairment—not MCI,” according to physician-based diagnosis. Cognitive diagnoses were established by either an interdisciplinary consensus team (in the majority of cases) or a single clinician (who conducted the examination), depending on the specific protocol of each ADRC. Diagnoses were based on the personal history, neuropsychological performance, and psychosocial functioning of the participants. MCI and dementia were diagnosed using standard clinical criteria [25,26,27,28,29,30]. Participants with cognitive impairment who did not clearly fit into the categories of CU, MCI, or dementia were diagnosed as cognitively impaired—not MCI.

2.2. Measurement of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms

The Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire (NPI-Q) is a self-administered, widely used tool for the evaluation of NPS in dementia research [31]. NPI-Q evaluates 12 domains namely delusions, hallucinations, agitation/aggression, depression/dysphoria, anxiety, elation/euphoria, apathy/indifference, disinhibition, irritability/lability, aberrant motor behaviour, night-time behaviours, and appetite/eating. Responders are initially requested to report the presence or absence of cardinal symptomatology for each domain in the month preceding the examination and subsequently to rate the severity of these symptoms (if present) according to a 3-point severity scale: mild (noticeable, but not a significant change); moderate (significant, but not a dramatic change); or severe (very marked or prominent; a dramatic change) [32]. For most NPI-Q domains, participants were grouped according to symptoms on a 3-point scale: 0: absent; 1: mild; 2: moderate and severe symptomatology (e.g., irritability: absent, mild, moderate/severe; anxiety: absent, mild, moderate/severe). For the domains of delusions, hallucinations, elation/euphoria and aberrant motor behaviour, owing to the very small number of participants with moderate/severe symptomatology (i.e., from N = 2 with moderate/severe hallucinations to N = 14 with moderate/severe aberrant motor behaviour), participants were dichotomized for presence of these NPS (0: absent; 1: mild, moderate and severe symptomatology).

2.3. Measurement of Cognitive Performance

The UDS focuses on the following cognitive domains: episodic memory, semantic memory, language, attention, processing speed and executive function [24]. In the current analysis, episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall) was assessed on the Logical Memory Test (Story A) from the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (WMS-R) [33], language on the total word production summing the animal and vegetable fluency tasks [34], semantic memory according to the 30-item version of the Boston Naming Test (BNT-30) [35], attention using the Digit Span Test (DST, forward and backward conditions) from the WMS-R [33], processing speed on the Trail Making Test—Part A (TMT-A) and executive function on the Trail Making Test—Part B (TMT-B) [36]. The administration and scoring of these tests have been previously detailed [24].

2.4. Factors and Covariates Considered

Age at the time of the evaluation and education in years of formal schooling were treated as scale variables. Sex, race (Caucasian, African American, American Indian or Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, Asian and multiracial), current use of antidepressant agents and a number of comorbidities that may confound the relationship between neuropsychiatric symptomatology and cognitive performance were treated as categorical variables: history of seizures, traumatic brain injury (TBI), Parkinson’s disease (PD), thyroid disease, vitamin B12 deficiency, alcohol or other substance abuse (with clinically significant impairment occurring over a 12-month period manifested in one of the following areas: work, driving, legal, or social), cardiovascular (heart attack/cardiac arrest or coronal angioplasty/endarterectomy/stent or cardiac bypass procedure or congestive heart failure) and cerebrovascular disease (stroke or transient ischemic attack). These comorbidities were positively assessed according to subject or co-participant reporting of either recent/active or remote/inactive condition. To avoid overadjustment, a statistical criterion was set for the inclusion of each comorbidity in our analysis: only comorbidities that significantly differed between those without and those with one or more NPS were included in our analysis.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Baseline differences between those without and those with one or more NPS were analysed using independent sample t-tests (scale variables) and Pearson’s chi-squared tests (categorical variables). Associations between individual NPS and cognitive performance were examined using a multivariate general linear model (GLM). Multivariate GLM is an extension of univariate GLM which deals with multiple continuous dependent variables at once (jointly) while controlling for one or more independent variables. In this way multivariate GLM provides regression analysis and analysis of variance for multiple dependent variables by several factors and covariates while accounting for the potential intercorrelations of the dependent variables and avoiding the pitfalls posed by multiple independent comparisons. A distinct set of regression parameters is generated for each dependent variable.
In the present analysis, six neuropsychological measures were set as the dependent scale parameters: episodic memory [the sum of items recalled in the immediate (0–25 total items recalled) and delayed recall (0–25 total items recalled) tasks], language (the sum of word production in the animal and vegetable 1-min category fluency tasks), semantic memory [BNT-30 (0–30 items retrieved)], attention [the sum of the longest sequences in DST forward (0–8 digits) and backward (0–7 digits) conditions], processing speed [total time in TMT-A (0–150 s)] and executive function [total time in TMT-B (0–300 s)]. The multivariate model featured all 12 NPI-Q variables as independent categorical variables and was adjusted for the factors and covariates described in the previous section (apart from history of seizures and thyroid disease, which did not fulfil the predefined statistical prerequisite, see Table 1). Six functions adhering to the general form of [ y = b 0 + b 1 X 1 + b 2 X 2   + + b i X i ] were generated, one for each dependent neuropsychological outcome (where y = predicted value of the dependent variable for the given values of the independent variables X1−i, b0 = intercept and b1−i = regression coefficients, i.e., how much we expect y to change as each independent variable—NPS and confounders—increases).
The statistical analysis was performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics Software Version 27 (Chicago, IL, USA). The conventional threshold of α = 0.05 was implemented for the revelation of statistical significance.

2.6. Supplementary Analyses

Apart from the main analysis, two additional sensitivity analyses were performed according to the same general statistical approach. The first one was a confirmatory analysis in which the presence of symptoms for each individual NPI-Q domain was analysed according to a 2-point scale: 0: absent-mild versus 1: moderate-severe symptomatology (a very common practice in dementia research). The exceptions from this rule were (again) the domains of delusions, hallucinations, elation/euphoria and aberrant motor behaviour. The presence of symptoms for these NPS was once again dichotomized as follows: 0: absent; 1: mild, moderate and severe symptomatology (due to the very small number of participants with moderate and severe symptomatology).
The second analysis was exploratory in which every NPS (including delusions, hallucinations, elation/euphoria and aberrant motor behaviour) was analysed according to the following 2-point scale: 0: absent-mild; and 1: moderate-severe symptomatology (despite the formation of very small participant groups with moderate and severe symptomatology).

3. Results

3.1. Patient Characteristics and Missing Data

The beginning database included 17,605 CU individuals with at least one UDS evaluation. After excluding those younger than 60 years old, individuals with a clinician-based diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder and participants receiving FDA-approved medication for AD, antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative or hypnotic agents, a total of 11,882 participants were eligible for the present analyses. Due to the presence of missing data, our adjusted multivariate analysis ultimately involved a total of 7179 older CU participants (Figure 1). The majority of missing data were introduced by the use of alternative neuropsychological tests in the 3rd (last) version of the UDS. Thus, among the total of 11,882 eligible CU participants, there were only 7717, 7712 and 7739 individuals with available neuropsychological assessments according to the Logical Memory Test, BNT-30 and DST.
The baseline characteristics of the included participants are in Table 1 where we compare participants with no NPS and with at least one mild or more severe NPS. Participants were predominantly female, Caucasian, and well-educated. They had a wide range of comorbidities the majority of which were more common in the NPS group (apart from history of seizures and thyroid disease). A total of 1856 individuals had one or more NPS during the month preceding the evaluation: 11 with hallucinations (9 with mild and 2 with moderate), 38 with delusions (29 with mild, 8 with moderate and 1 with severe), 44 with elation/euphoria (31 with mild, 8 with moderate and 5 with severe), 61 with aberrant motor behaviours (47 with mild, 11 with moderate and 3 with severe), 331 with agitation/aggression (252 with mild, 65 with moderate and 14 with severe), 605 with depression/dysphoria (488 with mild, 103 with moderate and 14 with severe), 436 with anxiety (322 with mild, 100 with moderate and 14 with severe), 211 with apathy/indifference (169 with mild, 32 with moderate and 10 with severe), 142 with disinhibition (93 with mild, 31 with moderate and 8 with severe), 658 with irritability/lability (524 with mild, 111 with moderate and 23 with severe), 602 with night-time behaviours (433 with mild, 136 with moderate and 33 with severe) and 332 with appetite/eating disturbances (251 with mild, 68 with moderate and 13 with severe).

3.2. The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Cognitively Unimpaired Adults

Table 2 displays the models of the regressions examining associations between individual NPS with neuropsychological performance on the 6 tests we examined. The table displays betas with their p-values. Betas should be interpreted as follows: regarding the trichotomous variables: how much we expect cognitive performance to differ between those without NPS (0) and those with moderate/severe NPS (2), and between those with mild NPS (1) and those with moderate/severe NPS (2); regarding the dichotomous variables: how much we expect cognitive performance to differ between those without NPS (0) and those with NPS (1).
Anxiety, elation, aberrant motor behaviour and hallucinations were the only NPS associated with worse cognitive performance in these CU older individuals (Table 2). Individuals with moderate/severe anxiety performed worse on semantic memory (BNT-30) compared to both participants with mild anxiety (~0.8 fewer correct responses on average) as well as unaffected individuals (~0.95 fewer correct responses). Moreover, those with moderate/severe anxiety scored worse on attention (~0.4 fewer items scored in the DST forward and backward conditions—sum of the longest sequences) compared to non-anxious controls. With respect to executive function, individuals with moderate/severe anxiety required ~10 more seconds to complete the TMT-B task compared to unaffected individuals.
Elation and aberrant motor behaviour affected only one cognitive domain each, attention and language, respectively. Those reporting euphoria scored ~0.8 fewer items in DST forward and backward conditions in comparison with unaffected participants. Regarding motor complains, aberrant motor behaviour was associated with ~2.1 fewer words generated in the category fluency task. Finally, the presence of hallucinations was related to worse performance in processing speed and executive function with affected individuals requiring ~10 and ~43 more seconds than healthy controls to complete the TMT-A and TMT-B tasks, respectively.
The confirmatory analysis (Table S1 published as supplementary material online) practically reproduced the findings of the main analysis. However, the exploratory analysis provided several inconsistent findings (Table S2 published as supplementary material online). According to the exploratory analysis, elation was associated with both poorer attention (as in the main analysis) and processing speed while delusions were related to worse processing speed and executive function (motor disturbance was no more associated with SVF and hallucinations were no more linked to processing speed and executive function). The associations of anxiety with semantic memory, attention and executive function remained unaltered.

4. Discussion

The present article explored whether specific NPS are related to worse performance on particular cognitive domains in older CU individuals. Our analysis revealed that moderate/severe anxiety was related to worse semantic memory, attention and executive function, hallucinations (moderate/severe delusions in the exploratory analysis) were linked to worse processing speed and executive function, while elation and aberrant motor behaviour were associated with poorer attention and language performance, respectively. The measures of association were relatively small in most cases (except for the relashionship between psychotic symptoms and executive impairment). The remaining NPS were not linked to worse cognitive performance. At this point, it is appropriate to highlight that participants with long-standing neuropsychiatric conditions were excluded from our analysis while NPI-Q focuses on the assessment of NPS that occurred in the month prior to the assessment (ignoring symptoms and behaviours that are usual to the examinee); therefore, the tardive impact of NPS on cognitive performance may have not been captured (our findings probably reflect the earlier cognitive fallout associated with recently occurring NPS).
Late-life anxiety and depression are among the most profoundly studied NPS with respect to cognitive performance. Anxiety has been consistently associated with poorer cognitive performance, and predominantly worse attention and executive functioning [37,38,39]. In fact, anxiety is hypothesized to compromise the very neuropsychological construct of the aforementioned cognitive operations by impeding fixation on task-pertinent stimuli and inhibition of interferences, cognitive flexibility and switching between and within relevant tasks, as well as updating and monitoring of transient information storage [40]. Less often, anxiety has been related to poorer performance in other cognitive functions including semantic memory, but these less consistent associations are more poorly understood and might be a by-product of the generally undermined cognitive functioning [41]. Of note, we failed to reveal an association between depression and cognitive performance. This arguably unexpected finding could be, however, explained by the temporal dynamics of the well-established association between depression and cognitive impairment [16]. Although anxiety is often accompanied by contemporary cognitive ramifications, the overall duration of the disorder and the number of affective episodes appear to be of decisive importance with respect to the effect of depression on cognitive outcomes [42]. As previously mentioned, the tardive impact of NPS on cognitive performance may not have been captured by our study.
Psychotic symptoms, i.e., hallucinations and delusions, are the least frequent NPS among CU individuals [43]. However, they are considered the strongest precursors to cognitive impairment and close surveillance is probably warranted in the presence of late-life psychotic manifestations [6]. Hallucinations and delusions are predominantly associated with incident dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB) and secondarily with frontotemporal degeneration (FTD) [6]. Both dementia entities share a prominent dysfunction in executive functioning [44] while DLB presents a strong association with attention-processing speed deficits, as well [45]. Intriguingly, our findings suggest that psychotic symptoms may be related to worse executive and processing speed performance in CU older adults (a finding probably consistent with their prominent association with incident DLB and FTD). Although these associations have been reproduced in CU, otherwise healthy older adults [6,46], the majority of relevant research has focused on individuals with psychotic disorders. In this specific subgroup, psychotic symptoms have been predominantly related to executive dysfunction as well, and namely impaired inhibitory control that leads to misinterpretation of experiences or false perceptions in the absence of external stimulation [47,48]. Among the remaining cognitive domains, psychotic disorders have mainly been associated with worse processing speed, which (slower processing) also seems to mediate part of the deleterious effect of psychotic disorders on executive function [49,50,51].
With the exception of sleep disturbances, the remaining NPS have been substantially less investigated with respect to their effect on cognitive outcomes in CU older adults. Contrary to the current study, articles exclusively focusing on the impact of sleep disorders on cognition tend to report a detrimental effect on most cognitive functions [52,53]. However, such articles implement more thorough sleep evaluation protocols separately and meticulously addressing the impact of several crucial sleep parameters and most notably sleep quality, efficiency, duration, and daytime sleepiness, on individual cognitive functions. Therefore, the less rigorous NPI-Q-based assessment of sleep disorders along with the potential temporal dynamics governing the sleep-related effects on cognition may account for the lack of an association between sleep disturbances and cognitive performance in the present analysis.
As for the rest NPS (elation/euphoria, appetite disorders, apathy, irritability, disinhibition, agitation and aberrant motor behaviour), to date, their relationship with cognitive performance has been primarily explored in cognitively impaired populations. However, as indicated by Lü and colleagues, estimated associations vary significantly between individuals with different cognitive backgrounds [54]. Therefore, the extrapolation of these findings to CU individuals would be erroneous. On the aforementioned grounds, future research ought to focus on these understudied NPS to shed more light on their effect on individual cognitive functions in CU older individuals.

4.1. Strengths and Limitations

The main strengths of our study are the large sample size of older individuals without cognitive impairment and the rigorous statistical protocol. We were careful to exclude individuals with long-standing psychiatric conditions and/or under treatment with medication targeting neuropsychiatric manifestations while interfering with cognitive performance, as well as to adjust for a number of important confounders. The statistical approach accounted for the potential intercorrelations among the dependent neuropsychological measures while avoided the performance of multiple independent comparisons (minimizing Type I error).
However, it is appropriate to point out that the current analysis presents a number of weaknesses, as well. First, the prevalence of several NPS and most notably of psychotic symptoms was expectedly low. Therefore, as reflected on the low accuracy estimates of the relevant findings, several aspects of our analysis were relatively underpowered. Secondly, NPS were assessed using the NPI-Q, which is a widely used instrument for their evaluation in dementia research. However, the use of more thorough assessment tools (e.g., depression and anxiety scales, sleep quality and efficiency scales, etc.) could potentially be more sensitive in revealing and more accurate in quantifying the severity of NPS. Another limitation of our study stems from its observational, cross-sectional nature, which makes it inappropriate to make any etiologic inferences about NPS and cognitive performance. Therefore, it is important that future longitudinal investigations will complement the findings of this cross-sectional analysis, to better understand the temporal dynamics of NPS and cognitive outcomes. Finally, although we accounted for a number of important confounders, residual confounding cannot be excluded [55,56,57].

4.2. Conclusions

In the present study, moderate/severe anxiety was related to worse semantic memory, attention and executive function, psychotic symptoms were associated with worse processing speed and executive function, while elation and aberrant motor behaviour were associated with poorer attention and language performance, respectively. These findings could inform the formulation of individualized preventive strategies directed to the ‘‘fortification’’ of cognitive functions specifically affected by the presence of particular NPS.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/medicina58111586/s1, Table S1: Associations between neuropsychiatric manifestations and cognitive performance: confirmatory analysis; Table S2: Associations between neuropsychiatric manifestations and cognitive performance: exploratory analysis.

Author Contributions

I.L.: original draft preparation, data curation, formal analysis, design of the study, interpretation of data, and review & editing of manuscript; V.S.: data curation, validation, review & editing of manuscript; C.G.L.: conceptualization, formulation of research question, design of the study, supervision, review & editing; E.D.: conceptualization, formulation of research question, design of the study, supervision; review & editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U24 AG072122. NACC data are contributed by the NIA-funded ADRCs: P30 AG062429 (PI James Brewer, MD, PhD), P30 AG066468 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P30 AG062421 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P30 AG066509 (PI Thomas Grabowski, MD), P30 AG066514 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P30 AG066530 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P30 AG066507 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P30 AG066444 (PI John Morris, MD), P30 AG066518 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG066512 (PI Thomas Wisniewski, MD), P30 AG066462 (PI Scott Small, MD), P30 AG072979 (PI David Wolk, MD), P30 AG072972 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P30 AG072976 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P30 AG072975 (PI David Bennett, MD), P30 AG072978 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P30 AG072977 (PI Robert Vassar, PhD), P30 AG066519 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P30 AG062677 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P30 AG079280 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P30 AG062422 (PI Gil Rabinovici, MD), P30 AG066511 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P30 AG072946 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P30 AG062715 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P30 AG072973 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P30 AG066506 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P30 AG066508 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD), P30 AG066515 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG072947 (PI Suzanne Craft, PhD), P30 AG072931 (PI Henry Paulson, MD, PhD), P30 AG066546 (PI Sudha Seshadri, MD), P20 AG068024 (PI Erik Roberson, MD, PhD), P20 AG068053 (PI Justin Miller, PhD), P20 AG068077 (PI Gary Rosenberg, MD), P20 AG068082 (PI Angela Jefferson, PhD), P30 AG072958 (PI Heather Whitson, MD), P30 AG072959 (PI James Leverenz, MD).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Institutional Review Boards overseeing each ADRC approved all procedures before the initiation of the study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

For further information on access to the NACC database, please contact NACC (contact details can be found at https://naccdata.org/).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lyketsos, C.G.; Lopez, O.; Jones, B.; Fitzpatrick, A.L.; Breitner, J.; DeKosky, S. Prevalence of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Cardiovascular Health Study. JAMA 2002, 288, 1475–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Fernandez-Martinez, M.; Molano, A.; Castro, J.; Zarranz, J.J. Prevalence of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimers Disease, and its Relationship with Cognitive Impairment. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2010, 7, 517–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Siafarikas, N.; Selbaek, G.; Fladby, T.; Benth, J.; Auning, E.; Aarsland, D. Frequency and subgroups of neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment and different stages of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2018, 30, 103–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Geda, Y.E.; Roberts, R.O.; Mielke, M.; Knopman, D.S.; Christianson, T.J.; Pankratz, V.S.; Boeve, B.F.; Sochor, O.; Tangalos, E.G.; Petersen, R.C.; et al. Baseline Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and the Risk of Incident Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study. Am. J. Psychiatry 2014, 171, 572–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Krell-Roesch, J.; Syrjanen, J.A.; Machulda, M.M.; Christianson, T.J.; Kremers, W.K.; Mielke, M.M.; Knopman, D.S.; Petersen, R.C.; Vassilaki, M.; Geda, Y.E. Neuropsychiatric symptoms and the outcome of cognitive trajectories in older adults free of dementia: The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2021, 36, 1362–1369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Liew, T.M. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in cognitively normal older persons, and the association with Alzheimer’s and non-Alzheimer’s dementia. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2020, 12, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Wise, E.A.; Rosenberg, P.B.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Leoutsakos, J. Time course of neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive diagnosis in National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Centers volunteers. Alzheimer’s Dement. Diagn. Assess. Dis. Monit. 2019, 11, 333–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cummings, J.; Ritter, A.; Rothenberg, K. Advances in Management of Neuropsychiatric Syndromes in Neurodegenerative Diseases. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2019, 21, 79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Mulsant, B.H.; Gharabawi, G.M.; Bossie, C.A.; Mao, L.; Martinez, R.A.; Tune, L.E.; Greenspan, A.J.; Bastean, J.N.; Pollock, B.G. Correlates of Anticholinergic Activity in Patients with Dementia and Psychosis Treated with Risperidone or Olanzapine. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2004, 65, 1708–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Stewart, S.A. The effects of benzodiazepines on cognition. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2005, 66 (Suppl. S2), 9–13. [Google Scholar]
  11. Leng, Y.; Diem, S.J.; Stone, K.L.; Yaffe, K. Antidepressant Use and Cognitive Outcomes in Very Old Women. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 2018, 73, 1390–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Lyketsos, C.G.; DelCampo, L.; Steinberg, M.; Miles, Q.; Steele, C.D.; Munro, C.; Baker, A.S.; Sheppard, J.-M.E.; Frangakis, C.; Brandt, J.; et al. Treating Depression in Alzheimer Disease: Efficacy and Safety of Sertraline Therapy, and the Benefits of Depression Reduction: The DIADS. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2003, 60, 737–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Porsteinsson, A.P.; Drye, L.T.; Pollock, B.G.; Devanand, D.P.; Frangakis, C.; Ismail, Z.; Marano, C.; Meinert, C.L.; Mintzer, J.E.; Munro, C.A.; et al. Effect of Citalopram on Agitation in Alzheimer Disease. JAMA 2014, 311, 682–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Cassidy-Eagle, E.; Siebern, A.; Unti, L.; Glassman, J.; O’Hara, R. Neuropsychological Functioning in Older Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Insomnia Randomized to CBT-I or Control Group. Clin. Gerontol. 2018, 41, 136–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kanellopoulos, D.; Rosenberg, P.; Ravdin, L.D.; Maldonado, D.; Jamil, N.; Quinn, C.; Kiosses, D.N. Depression, cognitive, and functional outcomes of Problem Adaptation Therapy (PATH) in older adults with major depression and mild cognitive deficits. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2020, 32, 485–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Morimoto, S.S.; Kanellopoulos, T.; Alexopoulos, G.S. Cognitive Impairment in Depressed Older Adults: Implications for Prognosis and Treatment. Psychiatr. Ann. 2014, 44, 138–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Orgeta, V.; Qazi, A.; Spector, A.E.; Orrell, M. Psychological treatments for depression and anxiety in dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2014, 22, CD009125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Rostamzadeh, A.; Kahlert, A.; Kalthegener, F.; Jessen, F. Psychotherapeutic interventions in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s dementia: A systematic review. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2022, 14, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Burhanullah, M.H.; Tschanz, J.T.; Peters, M.E.; Leoutsakos, J.-M.; Matyi, J.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Nowrangi, M.A.; Rosenberg, P.B. Neuropsychiatric Symptoms as Risk Factors for Cognitive Decline in Clinically Normal Older Adults: The Cache County Study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2020, 28, 64–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Creese, B.; Brooker, H.; Ismail, Z.; Wesnes, K.A.; Hampshire, A.; Khan, Z.; Megalogeni, M.; Corbett, A.; Aarsland, D.; Ballard, C. Mild Behavioral Impairment as a Marker of Cognitive Decline in Cognitively Normal Older Adults. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2019, 27, 823–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Köhler, S.; Allardyce, J.; Verhey, F.R.; McKeith, I.G.; Matthews, F.; Brayne, C.; Savva, G.M. Cognitive Decline and Dementia Risk in Older Adults with Psychotic Symptoms: A Prospective Cohort Study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2013, 21, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Beekly, D.L.; Ramos, E.M.; Lee, W.W.; Deitrich, W.D.; Jacka, M.E.; Wu, J.; Hubbard, J.L.; Koepsell, T.D.; Morris, J.C.; Kukull, W.A. The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Database: The Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2007, 21, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Morris, J.C.; Weintraub, S.; Chui, H.C.; Cummings, J.; DeCarli, C.; Ferris, S.; Foster, N.L.; Galasko, D.; Graff-Radford, N.; Peskind, E.R.; et al. The Uniform Data Set (UDS): Clinical and Cognitive Variables and Descriptive Data from Alzheimer Disease Centers. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2006, 20, 210–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Weintraub, S.; Salmon, D.; Mercaldo, N.; Ferris, S.; Graff-Radford, N.R.; Chui, H.; Cummings, J.; DeCarli, C.; Foster, N.L.; Galasko, D.; et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS). Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2009, 23, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. McKeith, I.G.; Dickson, D.W.; Lowe, J.; Emre, M.; O’Brien, J.T.; Feldman, H.; Cummings, J.; Duda, J.E.; Lippa, C.; Perry, E.K.; et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology 2005, 65, 1863–1872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. McKhann, G.; Drachman, D.; Folstein, M.; Katzman, R.; Price, D.; Stadlan, E.M. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984, 34, 939–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Neary, D.; Snowden, J.S.; Gustafson, L.; Passant, U.; Stuss, D.; Black, S.; Freedman, M.; Kertesz, A.; Robert, P.H.; Albert, M.; et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998, 51, 1546–1554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Petersen, R.C.; Smith, G.E.; Waring, S.C.; Ivnik, R.J.; Tangalos, E.G.; Kokmen, E. Mild Cognitive Impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999, 56, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Roman, G.C.; Tatemichi, T.K.; Erkinjuntti, T.; Cummings, J.L.; Masdeu, J.C.; Garcia, J.H.; Amaducci, L.; Orgogozo, J.-M.; Brun, A.; Hofman, A.; et al. Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies: Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 1993, 43, 250–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Winblad, B.; Palmer, K.; Kivipelto, M.; Jelic, V.; Fratiglioni, L.; Wahlund, L.-O.; Nordberg, A.; Backman, L.J.; Albert, M.S.; Almkvist, O.; et al. Mild cognitive impairment—Beyond controversies, towards a consensus: Report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Intern. Med. 2004, 256, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kaufer, D.I.; Cummings, J.L.; Ketchel, P.; Smith, V.; Macmillan, A.; Shelley, T.; Lopez, O.L.; DeKosky, S.T. Validation of the NPI-Q, a Brief Clinical Form of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Leoutsakos, J.-M.S.; Forrester, S.N.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Smith, G.S. Latent Classes of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in NACC Controls and Conversion to Mild Cognitive Impairment or Dementia. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015, 48, 483–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Wechsler, D. WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  34. Morris, J.C.; Heyman, A.; Mohs, R.C.; Hughes, J.P.; van Belle, G.; Fillenbaum, G.; Mellits, E.D.; Clark, C. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989, 39, 1159–1165. [Google Scholar]
  35. Goodglass, H.; Kaplan, E.; Weintraub, S. BDAE: The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hultsch, D.F.; Strauss, E.; Hunter, M.A.; MacDonald, S.W.S.; Craik, F.I.M.; Salthouse, T.A. The Handbook of Aging and Cognition; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  37. Beaudreau, S.A.; O’Hara, R. The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults. Psychol. Aging 2009, 24, 507–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Parra-Díaz, A.B.; Aibar-Almazán, A.; Martínez-Amat, A.; Jiménez-García, J.D.; Álvarez-Salvago, F.; Hita-Contreras, F. Associations of Sleep Quality, Anxiety, and Depression with Cognitive and Executive Functions among Community-Dwelling Women Aged ≥65 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Healthcare 2021, 9, 1599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Yochim, B.P.; Mueller, A.E.; Segal, D.L. Late life anxiety is associated with decreased memory and executive functioning in community dwelling older adults. J. Anxiety Disord. 2013, 27, 567–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Derakhshan, N.; Eysenck, M.W. Anxiety, Processing Efficiency, and Cognitive Performance: New Developments from Attentional Control Theory. Eur. Psychol. 2009, 14, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wetherell, J.L.; Reynolds, C.A.; Gatz, M.; Pedersen, N.L. Anxiety, Cognitive Performance, and Cognitive Decline in Normal Aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2002, 57, P246–P255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Srivastava, K.; Ryali, V.; Prakash, J.; Bhat, P.S.; Shashikumar, R.; Khan, S. Neuropsychophysiological correlates of depression. Ind. Psychiatry J. 2010, 19, 82–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sannemann, L.; Schild, A.-K.; Altenstein, S.; Bartels, C.; Brosseron, F.; Buerger, K.; Cosma, N.C.; Fliessbach, K.; Freiesleben, S.D.; Glanz, W.; et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in at-risk groups for AD dementia and their association with worry and AD biomarkers—Results from the DELCODE study. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2020, 12, 131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Johns, E.K.; Phillips, N.A.; Belleville, S.; Goupil, D.; Babins, L.; Kelner, N.; Ska, B.; Gilbert, B.; Inglis, G.; Panisset, M.; et al. Executive functions in frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body dementia. Neuropsychology 2009, 23, 765–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Botzung, A.; Mondino, M.; Noblet, V.; Philippi, N.; Blanc, F. Attention processing speed impairment in early dementia with Lewy bodies correlates with white matter damage on volumetric and diffusion tensor imaging: Neuroimaging/multi-modal comparisons. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2020, 16, e042729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Daalman, K.; van Zandvoort, M.; Bootsman, F.; Boks, M.; Kahn, R.; Sommer, I. Auditory verbal hallucinations and cognitive functioning in healthy individuals. Schizophr. Res. 2011, 132, 203–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Michie, P.T.; Badcock, J.C.; Waters, F.A.V.; Maybery, M. Auditory hallucinations: Failure to inhibit irrelevant memories. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2005, 10, 125–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Soriano, M.F.; Jiménez, J.F.; Román, P.; Bajo, M.T. Intentional inhibition in memory and hallucinations: Directed forgetting and updating. Neuropsychology 2009, 23, 61–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Harvey, P.D.; Rosenthal, J.B. Cognitive and functional deficits in people with schizophrenia: Evidence for accelerated or exaggerated aging? Schizophr. Res. 2018, 196, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mathias, S.R.; Knowles, E.E.M.; Barrett, J.; Leach, O.; Buccheri, S.; Beetham, T.; Blangero, J.; Poldrack, R.A.; Glahn, D.C. The Processing-Speed Impairment in Psychosis Is More Than Just Accelerated Aging. Schizophr. Bull. 2017, 43, 814–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Thuaire, F.; Rondepierre, F.; Vallet, G.T.; Jalenques, I.; Izaute, M. Executive deficits in schizophrenia: Mediation by processing speed and its relationships with aging. Psychol. Med. 2020, 52, 1126–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Boeve, A.; Halpin, A.; Michaud, S.; Fagan, M.; MacAulay, R.K. Specific Sleep Health Domains as Predictors of Executive Function in Older Adults. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2022, 34, 422–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dzierzewski, J.M.; Dautovich, N.; Ravyts, S. Sleep and Cognition in Older Adults. Sleep Med. Clin. 2018, 13, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Lü, W.; Duan, J.; Zhang, W.; Yang, W.; Yu, W. Relationship between neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive functions in patients with cognitive impairment. Psychogeriatrics 2021, 21, 773–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Liampas, I.; Hatzimanolis, A.; Siokas, V.; Yannakoulia, M.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E. Antihypertensive Medication Class and the Risk of Dementia and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults: A Secondary Analysis of the Prospective HELIAD Cohort. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2022, 89, 709–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Folia, V.; Liampas, I.; Ntanasi, E.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Longitudinal trajectories and normative language standards in older adults with normal cognitive status. Neuropsychology 2022, 36, 626–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Liampas, I.; Folia, V.; Ntanasi, E.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Longitudinal episodic memory trajectories in older adults with normal cognition. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2022, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flowchart of participant selection.
Figure 1. Flowchart of participant selection.
Medicina 58 01586 g001
Table 1. Baseline differences between cognitively unimpaired individuals without and with one or more neuropsychiatric symptoms.
Table 1. Baseline differences between cognitively unimpaired individuals without and with one or more neuropsychiatric symptoms.
VariableWithout NPS (n = 5323)With NPS (n = 1856)p-Value
Age in years73.63 ± 7.8173.65 ± 7.870.913
Formal education in years15.72 ± 2.9415.42 ± 3.11<0.001
Sex (male/female)1788 (33.6%)/3535 (66.4%)762 (41.1%)/1094 (58.9%)<0.001
Race (Caucasian/African American/American Indian or Alaska Native/Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander/Asian/Multiracial)4130 (77.5%)/916 (17.2%)/14 (0.3%)/3 (0.1%)/120 (2.3%)/140 (2.6%)1580 (85.1%)/190 (10.2%)/8 (0.4%)/1 (0.1%)/22 (2.2%)/55 (3.0%)<0.001
Cardiovascular disease (No/Yes)4748 (89.2%)/575 (10.8%)1608 (86.6%)/248 (13.4%)0.003
Cerebrovascular disease (No/Yes)5028 (94.5%)/295 (5.5%)1706 (91.9%)/150 (8.1%)<0.001
Parkinson’s disease (No/Yes)5293 (99.4%)/30 (0.6%)1810 (97.5%)/46 (2.5%)<0.001
Traumatic brain injury (No/Yes)4896 (92.0%)/427 (8.0%)1662 (89.5%)/194 (10.5%)0.001
History of seizures (No/Yes)5237 (98.5%)/82 (1.5%)1826 (98.5%)/27 (1.5%)0.798
Thyroid disease (No/Yes)4324 (81.4%)/990 (18.6%)1508 (81.3%)/348 (18.7%)0.909
B12 deficiency (No/Yes)5146 (96.7%)/177 (3.3%)1771 (95.4%)/85 (4.6%)0.013
Alcohol abuse (No/Yes)5221 (98.1%)/102 (1.9%)1774 (95.6%)/82 (4.4%)<0.001
Other substance abuse (No/Yes)5289 (99.4%)/34 (0.6%)1835 (98.9%)/21 (1.1%)0.036
Current antidepressant use (No/Yes)4844 (91.0%)/479 (9.0%)1547 (83.4%)/309 (16.6%)<0.001
Episodic memory (sum of items recalled in the immediate and delayed recall tasks)25.66 ± 7.7124.55 ± 7.71<0.001
Language (sum of word production in the animals and vegetables lists)34.61 ± 8.5733.70 ± 8.16<0.001
Semantic Memory (BNT-30)26.94 ± 3.4127.09 ± 3.150.104
Attention (sum of longest sequences in DST forward & backward conditions)11.58 ± 1.9911.50 ± 1.990.139
Processing speed (TMT-A seconds)35.15 ± 15.2735.68 ± 17.200.212
Executive function (TMT-B seconds)93.04 ± 50.8896.25 ± 53.650.021
NPS: neuropsychiatric symptoms; BNT: Boston naming test; DST: digit span test; TMT-A: trails making test—part A; TMT-B: trails making test—part B; n = number of participants with available data per parameter.
Table 2. Associations between neuropsychiatric manifestations and cognitive performance.
Table 2. Associations between neuropsychiatric manifestations and cognitive performance.
NPSEpisodic MemoryLanguage Semantic MemoryAttentio Processing SpeedExecutive Function
BpBpBpBpBpBp
Depression00.1450.8440.3980.610−0.3820.2020.0280.8871.4760.3142.0800.645
1−0.1650.832−0.0180.982−0.1400.6560.1570.4452.8720.0634.3810.357
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Anxiety01.2050.100−0.6790.3790.966 (1)0.0010.387 (3)0.046−1.5330.291−10.220 (4)0.022
10.3450.670−1.6410.0550.816 (2)0.0130.2860.183−1.5300.341−6.1870.211
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Agitation00.4370.619−0.0990.915−0.3230.3650.0590.802−0.5990.731−0.3060.955
10.7080.4540.0790.937−0.1680.6620.0180.944−3.1460.094−7.6690.185
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Apathy00.9220.4371.6870.178−0.0480.9200.1980.529−0.6970.767−1.5100.835
10.2830.8240.7600.573−0.3120.5470.0660.8460.7800.7582.1610.782
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Disinhibition00.4390.724−0.2090.8740.1680.740−0.0230.9441.0120.682−9.8220.197
1−0.2850.838−0.4130.779−0.3750.5080.2600.4820.1420.959−12.4960.143
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Irritability00.5860.403−0.3200.665−0.1130.690−0.1620.384−2.6520.056−2.9180.495
10.4520.538−0.3710.632−0.0440.883−0.2270.244−2.4300.095−1.8930.673
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Night-time00.3730.5190.5520.3660.1340.5700.0760.6211.0330.3684.9380.163
1−0.2920.6550.3870.5750.1950.462−0.0250.8850.7890.5436.1740.122
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Appetite00.2330.7771.3810.1120.6080.0690.1370.529−2.7170.096−9.7080.054
1−0.3360.7151.0930.2600.4280.2510.0890.714−1.2960.477−0.9130.871
2Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
ElationNo0.5650.6130.5090.6660.0690.8800.764 (5)0.010−2.4840.263−3.5930.599
YesRef Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
MotorNo1.3390.1522.129 (6)0.031−0.2250.5540.0850.7321.7520.345−0.1730.976
YesRef Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
DelusionsNo0.8660.4790.8450.5130.4370.3780.4150.200−2.5350.296−4.5240.545
YesRef Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
HallucinationsNo3.6920.0940.0150.9950.3240.7170.1250.830−10.272 (7)0.019−43.103 (8)0.001
YesRef Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Bold denotes statistical significance; Ref: reference group; the numbering of the neuropsychiatric symptoms corresponds to: 0 = absent; 1 = mild; 2 = moderate and severe symptomatology; 95% CIs (1) (0.384, 1.547); (2) (0.172, 1.460); (3) (0.007, 0.767); (4) (−18.980, −1.461); (5) (0.183, 1.345); (6) (0.193, 4.065); (7) (−18.835, −1.710); (8) (−69.479, −16.727).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Liampas, I.; Siokas, V.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Dardiotis, E. The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition. Medicina 2022, 58, 1586. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58111586

AMA Style

Liampas I, Siokas V, Lyketsos CG, Dardiotis E. The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition. Medicina. 2022; 58(11):1586. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58111586

Chicago/Turabian Style

Liampas, Ioannis, Vasileios Siokas, Constantine George Lyketsos, and Efthimios Dardiotis. 2022. "The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition" Medicina 58, no. 11: 1586. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58111586

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop