Next Article in Journal
Extracorporeal Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation: A Narrative Review and Establishment of a Sustainable Program
Previous Article in Journal
High Percentage of Complications and Re-Operations Following Dynamic Locking Plate Fixation with the Targon® FN for Intracapsular Proximal Femoral Fractures: An Analysis of Risk Factors
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Qualitative Verbal Fluency Components as Prognostic Factors for Developing Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Population-Based HELIAD Cohort

1
Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Larissa, School of Medicine, University of Thessaly, Mezourlo Hill, 41110 Larissa, Greece
2
Lab of Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, University Campus, 54124 Thessaloniki, Greece
3
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Harokopio University, 70 El. Venizelou, 17671 Athens, Greece
4
Athens Alzheimer’s Association, 89 M. Mousourou & 33 Stilponos St, 11636 Athens, Greece
5
School of Medicine, University of Cyprus, 93 Agiou Nikolaou St, Engomi, Nicosia 2408, Cyprus
6
1st Department of Neurology, Aiginition Hospital, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Medical School, 72-74 Vassilissis Sofias Ave, 11528 Athens, Greece
7
Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, 710 West 168th St, New York, NY 10032, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work as co-first authors.
These authors contributed equally to this work as co-senior authors.
Medicina 2022, 58(12), 1814; https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58121814
Submission received: 4 November 2022 / Revised: 29 November 2022 / Accepted: 6 December 2022 / Published: 9 December 2022

Abstract

:
Background and Objectives: The aim of the present study was to investigate the prognostic value of the qualitative components of verbal fluency (clustering, switching, intrusions, and perseverations) on the development of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. Materials and Methods: Participants were drawn from the multidisciplinary, population-based, prospective HELIAD (Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet) cohort. Two participant sets were separately analysed: those with normal cognition and MCI at baseline. Verbal fluency was assessed via one category and one letter fluency task. Separate Cox proportional hazards regressions adjusted for important sociodemographic parameters were performed for each qualitative semantic and phonemic verbal fluency component. Results: There were 955 cognitively normal (CN), older (72.9 years ±4.9), predominantly female (~60%) individuals with available follow-up assessments after a mean of 3.09 years (±0.83). Among them, 34 developed dementia at follow-up (29 of whom progressed to Alzheimer’s dementia (AD)), 160 developed MCI, and 761 remained CN. Each additional perseveration on the semantic condition increased the risk of developing all-cause dementia and AD by 52% and 55%, respectively. Of note, participants with two or more perseverations on the semantic task presented a much more prominent risk for incident dementia compared to those with one or no perseverations. Among the remaining qualitative indices, none were associated with the hazard of developing all-cause dementia, AD, and MCI at follow-up. Conclusions: Perseverations on the semantic fluency condition were related to an increased risk of incident all-cause dementia or AD in older, CN individuals.

1. Introduction

Spontaneous language production and semantic memory can be concomitantly assessed via verbal fluency (VF), which typically comprises a semantic (SVF) and a phonemic (PVF) task [1,2]. VF is widely used during neuropsychological evaluations, owing to its simple and quick administration and sensitivity in revealing cognitive dysfunction at early clinical stages [3,4]. Both VF conditions (SVF and PVF) rely on frontal lobe operations (for the initiation, organization, and monitoring of word production), while SVF additionally demands a constrained search of exemplars from a superordinate category [5,6]. Therefore, SVF is additionally heavily dependent on semantic memory/temporal lobe-based processes [5,6]. PVF performance is instead accomplished through a less constrained lexical search from a broader set of vocabular exemplars [5,6]. VF scoring typically refers to the total word production on each condition [7,8]. Word production in the SVF condition has been suggested to be of crucial clinical importance in the preclinical stages of cognitive dysfunction. Specifically, it has been consistently associated with the risk of incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia, especially of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) type [9,10]. These associations are mainly attributed to the potential presence of premorbid neurodegenerative alterations, prior to the formal identification of dementia or MCI [9,10].
Apart from word production, however, useful prognostic information may also arise from the qualitative assessment of VF tasks, i.e., clustering and switching techniques, numbers of intrusions, and perseverations. These indices have even been proposed to be more sensitive than total word production in the early detection and differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment [11,12,13,14]. Clustering refers to the production of related words within a given semantic or phonemic subcategory, while switching refers to the efficient shifting between clusters [13]. The former relies heavily on temporal-lobe-based processes (semantic storage), while the latter depends mainly on frontal lobe operations (executive function—cognitive flexibility) [13]. Clustering is considered a relatively unconscious and automatic process, particularly vital in SVF [14,15]. Switching, on the other hand, is a more deliberate operation based on conscious decisions to shift from one subcategory to another and is equally useful in SVF and PVF [14,15]. In addition to clustering and switching, errors constitute another important qualitative component of VF. Errors are typically divided into two subcategories: out-of-category words (intrusions) and repeated words (perseverations). It has been proposed that recurrent perseverations and intrusions are caused by frontal dysfunction and particularly by deficits in working memory, attention, and inhibition [16]. Even though their use is limited in clinical practice, previous research has occasionally reported some utility in the assessment of several neurological conditions including dementia [17,18,19,20].
To date, there are only a few studies that have indicated a potential role for qualitative VF indices in the preclinical stages of dementia or cognitive impairment. Specifically, previous research has associated perseverations on the SVF condition with an increased risk of incident MCI or dementia [19]. Of note, semantic perseverations have been found to increase steeply during the last two years prior to the onset of AD [11]. Among the remaining qualitative variables, only the mean semantic cluster size has been reported to possess predictive properties regarding the development of AD among participants with memory complaints (its predictive ability was comparable to the total word production on the SVF) [21]. Consequently, the aim of the present study was to investigate the prognostic value of clustering and switching techniques and number of intrusions and perseverations in the preclinical stages of MCI and dementia. For this purpose, we capitalized on a large data set from the population-based HELIAD (Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet) cohort [22]. The prognostic value of the aforementioned qualitative variables was explored after adjusting for global cognitive status (to account for the confounding of concomitant cognitive dysfunctions) and for important sociodemographic parameters that might confound language impairment and progression to MCI-dementia [23,24].

2. Materials and Methods

The present study conformed with the STROBE reporting guidelines (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) [25]. Participants were drawn from the population-based HELIAD (Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet) cohort. The rationale, objectives, and key elements of the HELIAD study have been reported previously in detail [26,27,28]. In short, HELIAD is a multidisciplinary, population-based, prospective cohort primarily exploring the epidemiology of dementia, cognitive impairment, and other neuropsychiatric entities in the aging Greek population. It comprises a generational cohort including both urban and rural dwellers who grew up during World War II and the Greek Civil War, which disrupted normal operations of many services including schools for extended periods of time, leaving children with intermittent educational experiences, at best. The majority of these individuals were poorly educated in terms of formal schooling, having only received elementary school education. The Institutional Ethics Review Boards of the University of Thessaly and the Kapodistrian University of Athens approved all procedures prior to the initiation of the study. Informed consent was acquired from all participants prior to participation.
Participants were selected through random sampling from the older rosters (≥65 years) of two Greek municipalities: Marousi (suburb of Athens) and Larissa (an urban–rural area in the province of Thessaly). Extensive baseline and follow-up evaluations (~2 to 2.5 h long sessions) were carried out at approximately 3-year intervals. For the present study, participants with no dementia at baseline and available follow-up investigations were considered for potential eligibility. Collaborative assessments designated by a consortium of expert neurologists and neuropsychologists were performed at both visits; a description of the evaluations pertinent to the present article is provided below.

2.1. Neuropsychological Assessments and Diagnostic Procedures

A comprehensive neuropsychological assessment was carried out by trained neuropsychologists [29]: non-verbal and verbal memory (Medical College of Georgia—MCG—Complex Figure Test; Greek Verbal Learning Test), language (semantic and phonological verbal fluency; subtests of the Greek version of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination short form, namely, the Boston Naming Test short form, and selected items from the Complex Ideational Material Subtest, to assess verbal comprehension and repetition of words and phrases), visuospatial ability (Judgment of Line Orientation abbreviated form; MCG Complex Figure Test copy condition; Clock Drawing Test), attention-processing speed (Trail Making Test—TMT—Part A), and executive functioning (TMT—Part B; Verbal Fluency; Anomalous Sentence Repetition; Graphical Sequence Test; Motor Programming; months forwards and backwards) were specifically evaluated. A brief screening of global cognition and orientation was performed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). All of the aforementioned tests were capitalized on in the diagnostic classification of the participants.
As part of a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, verbal fluency was evaluated using a category (SVF) and a letter (PVF) fluency task [7,30]. Participants were instructed to immediately begin generating items, following the announcement of the category (objects) or letter ((α alpha), and each trial lasted for 60 s. The SVF condition was administered prior to PVF. Regarding word search and production, no instructions were given, to ensure that any cognitive strategies would be spontaneously employed by the examinees. Participants were specifically told to abstain from reporting proper nouns (on the phonemic test) as well as repetitions and word variations. The number of clusters, switches, intrusions, and perseverations for SVF and PVF were calculated separately. Any identical words or variations of a previously given word were considered as perseverations. All the words that were irrelevant to each given semantic category or letter were considered as intrusions. Finally, the total numbers of clusters and switches were calculated according to the previously reported scoring guidelines: three or more consecutive words belonging to the same semantic subcategory or two consecutive words with a strong association in the Greek language (strong pairs of words) were defined as semantic clusters; semantic switches were estimated by subtracting the total number of related words (all words belonging to a semantic cluster) from the total word production and adding that to the number of semantic clusters; three or more consecutive words beginning with the same two letters and having the same sound, or two consecutive words that differed only in a vowel sound, or words that were homophones were defined as phonemic clusters; phonemic switches were calculated by subtracting the total number of related words (all words belonging to a phonemic cluster) from the total phonemic word production and adding that to the number of phonemic clusters [30,31]. Successive words stemming from the same root (such as act–action–acting) were considered as repetitions. On the other hand, three or more words sharing a part/suffix but having a different meaning (e.g., superman–supermarket–supercilious), were considered as a cluster.
The diagnostic categorization of the participants was performed during expert consensus meetings, involving senior neurologists (E.D., G.M.H., P.S., and N.S) and a neuropsychologist (M.H.K.) [32,33]. In brief, particular focus was placed on identifying potential comorbidities that could affect cognitive performance through screening participants for medical problems and illnesses, current medications, hospitalizations, depression, anxiety, essential tremor, behavioural symptoms, neuropsychiatric symptoms, functional status, Parkinson disease (PD), dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), and personal history of cerebrovascular disease accounting for the onset (abrupt, gradual, or insidious onset) or deterioration (stepwise deterioration, stable condition, continuous decline, or fluctuating cognition) of cognitive decline. The diagnoses of dementia and possible Alzheimer’s disease (AD) were based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-Text Revision criteria [34] and the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria, respectively [35]. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and its subtypes were diagnosed according to the Petersen criteria [36].

2.2. Statistical Analysis and Outcome Measures

All statistical analyses were performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics Software Version 25 (Chicago, IL, USA). Adjusted Cox proportional hazards regressions were conducted to investigate the associations of qualitative VF components with the development of dementia and MCI at follow-up. All analyses were adjusted for the following scale variables: age at baseline (in years), years of education, and standardized MMSE scores (i.e., MMSE scores of illiterate participants were converted to the literate MMSE scale of 30). All analyses were additionally adjusted for the following categorical parameters: sex, main occupation (manual or mental), and socioeconomic status (low or high) [37]. Inclusion of the MMSE score aimed towards controlling for global cognitive function. In other words, we explored the value of qualitative VF variables over and above the general cognitive status of the participants. Time to second visit was employed as the time-to-event variable. In the case of not documenting the event of interest, participants were censored at the second visit.
First, all CN participants at baseline were analysed. Eight consecutive adjusted Cox proportional hazards regressions were performed, involving the eight qualitative VF indices as potential predictors and dementia development at follow-up as the dichotomous outcome (intercorrelations prevented the insertion of all strategy variables in a single model). Owing to the multiple comparisons, the significance threshold was corrected to α = 0.006. Exploratory analyses with AD development at follow-up as the dichotomous outcome were subsequently conducted (using the corrected criterion α = 0.006).
Second, all CN participants at baseline without a dementia diagnosis at follow-up were analysed (i.e., those who either remained CN or developed MCI at follow-up). Eight consecutive adjusted Cox proportional hazards regressions were again performed, involving the eight qualitative VF variables as potential predictors and MCI development at follow-up as the dichotomous outcome. It was not feasible to classify individuals with a follow-up diagnosis of dementia according to the outcome of interest (MCI), in view of the lack of information regarding their transitional conversion to MCI. Therefore, individuals with diagnosis of dementia at follow-up were excluded from our analyses. Again, the corrected α = 0.006 significance threshold was implemented.
Finally, a secondary analysis involving the smaller participant set of MCI individuals at baseline was performed. Eight consecutive adjusted Cox proportional hazards regressions were conducted, involving the eight qualitative VF variables as potential predictors and dementia development at follow-up as the dichotomous outcome (again, using the corrected criterion α = 0.006). Exploratory analyses with AD development at follow-up as the dichotomous outcome were subsequently conducted (according to the corrected criterion α = 0.006).

3. Results

3.1. Baseline Characteristics and Missing Data

Among the 1984 participants of the HELIAD cohort, there were 103 individuals with dementia (79 with AD, 9 with vascular dementia, 8 with dementia with LBD or PD dementia, and 7 with less common entities), 243 with MCI, 1607 who were CN, and 4 with missing data leading to inconclusive cognitive diagnosis. A subgroup of 959 CN, older (mean age = 72.9 years ±4.9), predominantly female (~60%) individuals had completed their follow-up assessments after an average of 3.09 years (±0.83). Among them, 34 had developed dementia at follow-up (29 of whom developed AD), 160 developed MCI, 761 remained CN and four 4 were missing a follow-up cognitive diagnosis (these 4 were excluded from the analysis).
Those who had developed dementia or MCI by the second visit were older at baseline, less educated, and had lower MMSE scores compared to those who remained CN (Table 1 and Table 2). In addition, a greater portion of those who were diagnosed with MCI at first visit were of low socioeconomic status and had a manual occupation compared to those who remained CN at follow-up. Regarding the qualitative VF components, CN participants who had developed dementia by the 2nd visit recorded fewer clusters and switches on the SVF, as well as fewer switches on the PVF condition. On the other hand, CN participants with a follow-up MCI diagnosis documented fewer switches on both conditions as well as more intrusions on the PVF condition.
Regarding those with MCI at baseline (N = 243), 118 older (mean age = 75.4 years ±5.0), predominantly female (~60%) participants had completed their follow-up assessments after a mean of 2.92 years (±0.89). A total of 29 individuals with MCI had developed dementia by the second visit, 25 of whom were diagnosed with AD. Those with dementia at the second visit were older, often of low socioeconomic status, had performed more poorly in MMSE, and had generated fewer related words and clusters in the SVF condition (data not shown).

3.2. Associations between Qualitative Verbal Fluency Indices and Incident Dementia or Mild Cognitive Impairment

Table 3 summarizes the associations of the qualitative VF components with incident all-cause dementia or AD in CN participants. Each additional perseveration in the SVF condition increased the risk of developing dementia and AD by 52% and 55%, respectively. Of note, participants with two or more perseverations at baseline presented a much more prominent risk of incident dementia compared to those who documented one or no repetitions (Figure 1). Among the remaining qualitative variables (on both SVF and PVF), none were found to be associated with the hazard of developing all-cause dementia (or AD) at follow-up.
Table 4 summarizes the associations of the qualitative VF parameters with incident all-cause dementia or AD in MCI participants. Neither SVF nor PVF indices were related to the hazard of future dementia or AD. Similarly, no qualitative parameter was linked to the risk of CN individuals progressing to MCI (Table 5).

4. Discussion

The present study demonstrated that perseverations on the SVF condition are related to an increased risk of incident all-cause dementia or AD in older, CN individuals. These results are in line with the findings of Pakhomov et al., who reported that perseverations on the SVF condition constitute a harbinger of MCI or dementia [19]. No other variable (i.e., numbers of clusters, switches, or intrusions) was associated with the hazard of developing dementia or AD in participants with CN. Moreover, the numbers of clusters, switches, intrusions, or perseverations were not related to a differential risk of progressing to MCI or converting from MCI to dementia or AD.
There are a few studies that have explored the neuropsychological construct of perseverations in AD-related neurodegeneration and highlighted the pivotal implication of working memory and executive control deficits. These conclusions have been mostly deducted from the temporal pattern of perseverations’ occurrence: specifically, the distance/lag between the first occurrence of an exemplar and its later repetition [17,18,38]. It has been proposed that short lags between the first occurrence of a word and its reappearance are common in aphasia and could stem from impairments in lexical selection, i.e., language dysfunction [39]. On the other hand, long intervals are more frequent in patients with AD and could be attributed to an impediment of temporary information holding and self-monitoring, i.e., working memory deficits [17,18,19,39].
Considering that neuropathological alterations tend to precede the clinical onset of AD (or other neurodegenerative entities) for years, perseverations might reflect the clinical equivalent of these early pathological processes in non-demented individuals [40]. Working memory and inhibitory control deficits have been reported to manifest in the preclinical course towards the development of AD, typically within the last 3 years before its onset [41,42,43]. Intriguingly, Auriacombe et al. have specifically investigated the preclinical course of repetitions and intrusions and reported that the rate of repetitions on the SVF task increases steeply shortly (about 2 years) before the diagnosis of AD [11]. Considering that episodic memory, language, and visuo-perceptual impairment tend to manifest as early as 4–8 years prior to the formal identification of AD [44,45,46], the detection of perseverations could represent a pivotal turning point in the preclinical neuropathological courses of older adults towards the development of AD (a prominent implication of frontal operations), ultimately leading to full-blown dementia of the AD type [47].
Although perseverations may constitute a useful preclinical marker of imminent dementia, this marker probably lacks specificity. Working memory impairment is a common feature in the course of many common neurodegenerative entities and, therefore, cannot reveal the predominant underlying neuropathology on its own [48,49]. Inferentially, despite the limited relevant research, perseverations may also manifest in the context of numerous other neurodegenerative entities. Future research ought to focus on the prognostic value of perseverations on the development of other dementia entities (a very small number of non-AD dementia cases were documented in our study, preventing further sub-analyses) as well as other neurodegenerative disorders, for example, Parkinson’s disease. More importantly, the combined value of perseverations with other concomitant cognitive deficits should be explored, since the complete “neuropsychological profile” of an individual might be much more reflective of the exact underlying pattern of neurodegeneration compared to the use of individual cognitive tests.

Strengths and Limitations

HELIAD is a multidisciplinary, population-based, generational cohort involving a fairly representative sample of the older Greek population. The diagnostic classification of the participants was based on comprehensive neurological and neuropsychological evaluations and was established during expert consensus meetings involving senior neurologists and neuropsychologists, according to standard clinical criteria. All analyses accounted for measures of global cognition and important sociodemographic parameters that may confound language impairment and dementia-MCI development.
Despite the unique qualities of our study, it is important to acknowledge the presence of several limitations. First, the diagnosis of dementia was exclusively based on clinical criteria and did not include imaging or biological biomarkers (potential misclassification bias). Second, the non-trivial dropout rate at follow-up may be responsible for the introduction of non-response bias. Furthermore, despite adjusting our analyses for several crucial parameters, residual confounding should be expected. However, given the small number of total ‘‘events’’ at follow-up, the inclusion of additional covariates in our models could underpower our analyses and conceal non-trivial associations. Moreover, the moderate duration of the follow-up period (approximately 3 years) and the relatively small set of participants with MCI might have underpowered several analyses. Finally, the number of incident dementia cases other than AD was particularly small and did not allow their separate investigation.

5. Conclusions

The present study demonstrated that perseverations on the SVF task were associated with an elevated risk of incident all-cause dementia and AD in older, CN individuals. The numbers of clusters, switches, or intrusions were not related to the hazard of developing all-cause dementia or AD in participants with normal cognition. Finally, qualitative VF indices were not linked to the risk of progression to MCI or the hazard of conversion from MCI to all-cause dementia or AD.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.F., E.D. and M.H.K.; Data curation, E.Z. and V.S.; Formal analysis, I.L.; Funding acquisition, N.S.; Resources, M.Y., P.S., G.H., N.S., E.D. and M.H.K.; Writing—original draft, I.L., V.F. and E.Z.; Writing—review and editing, I.L., V.F., E.Z., V.S., M.Y., P.S., G.H., N.S., E.D. and M.H.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the following grants: IIRG-09-133014 from the Alzheimer’s Association; 189 10276/8/9/2011 from the ESPA-EU program Excellence Grant (ARISTEIA), which is co-funded by the European Social Fund and Greek National resources; and DY2b/oik.51657/14.4.2009 from the Ministry for Health and Social Solidarity (Greece). The funders had no role in the design, analysis, or writing of this article.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Institutional Ethics Review Boards of the University of Thessaly (256/10-05-2021) and the Kapodistrian University of Athens (AΔA 9Φ9Π46Ψ8Ν2-ΔΚΘ).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Quaranta, D.; Piccininni, C.; Caprara, A.; Malandrino, A.; Gainotti, G.; Marra, C. Semantic Relations in a Categorical Verbal Fluency Test: An Exploratory Investigation in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Shao, Z.; Janse, E.; Visser, K.; Meyer, A.S. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Henry, J.D.; Crawford, J.R.; Phillips, L. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1212–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Rinehardt, E.; Eichstaedt, K.; Schinka, J.A.; Loewenstein, D.A.; Mattingly, M.; Fils, J.; Duara, R.; Schoenberg, M.R. Verbal fluency patterns in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2014, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Birn, R.M.; Kenworthy, L.; Case, L.; Caravella, R.; Jones, T.B.; Bandettini, P.A.; Martin, A. Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: A self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 1099–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Stuss, D.T.; Alexander, M.P.; Hamer, L.; Palumbo, C.; Dempster, R.; Binns, M.; Levine, B.; Izukawa, D. The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998, 4, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Folia, V.; Liampas, I.; Ntanasi, E.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Longitudinal trajectories and normative language standards in older adults with normal cognitive status. Neuropsychology 2022, 36, 626–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Liampas, I.; Folia, V.; Morfakidou, R.; Siokas, V.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Scarmeas, N.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Language Differences among Individuals with Normal Cognition, Amnestic and Non-Amnestic MCI, and Alzheimer’s Disease. Arch. Clin Neuropsychol. 2022, acac080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Sutin, A.R.; Stephan, Y.; Terracciano, A. Verbal fluency and risk of dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2019, 34, 863–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Folia, V.; Liampas, I.; Siokas, V.; Silva, S.; Ntanasi, E.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; et al. Language performance as a prognostic factor for developing Alzheimer’s Clinical Syndrome and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the population-based HELIAD cohort. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2022, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Auriacombe, S.; Lechevallier, N.; Amieva, H.; Harston, S.; Raoux, N.; Dartigues, J.F. A Longitudinal Study of Quantitative and Qualitative Features of Category Verbal Fluency in Incident Alzheimer’s Disease Subjects: Results from the PAQUID Study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2006, 21, 260–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Gomez, R.; White, D. Using verbal fluency to detect very mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2006, 21, 771–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Troyer, A.K.; Moscovitch, M.; Winocur, G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology 1997, 11, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Troyer, A.K.; Moscovitch, M.; Winocur, G.; Leach, L.; Freedman, M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998, 4, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Clark, D.G.; Wadley, V.G.; Kapur, P.; DeRamus, T.P.; Singletary, B.; Nicholas, A.P.; Blanton, P.D.; Lokken, K.; Deshpande, H.; Marson, D.; et al. Lexical factors and cerebral regions influencing verbal fluency performance in MCI. Neuropsychologia 2014, 54, 98–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Azuma, T. Working Memory and Perseveration in Verbal Fluency. Neuropsychology 2004, 18, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fischer-Baum, S.; Miozzo, M.; Laiacona, M.; Capitani, E. Perseveration during verbal fluency in traumatic brain injury reflects impairments in working memory. Neuropsychology 2016, 30, 791–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Itaguchi, Y.; Castro-Chavira, S.A.; Waterloo, K.; Johnsen, S.H.; Rodríguez-Aranda, C. Evaluation of Error Production in Animal Fluency and Its Relationship to Frontal Tracts in Normal Aging and Mild Alzheimer’s Disease: A Combined LDA and Time-Course Analysis Investigation. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2022, 13, 710938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pakhomov, S.V.S.; Eberly, L.E.; Knopman, D.S. Recurrent perseverations on semantic verbal fluency tasks as an early marker of cognitive impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2018, 40, 832–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pekkala, S.; Albert, M.L.; Spiro III, A.; Erkinjuntti, T. Perseveration in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2008, 25, 109–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fagundo, A.B.; López, S.; Romero, M.; Guarch, J.; Marcos, T.; Salamero, M. Clustering and switching in semantic fluency: Predictors of the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 1007–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Liampas, I.; Folia, V.; Ntanasi, E.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Longitudinal episodic memory trajectories in older adults with normal cognition. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2022, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Karp, A.; Kåreholt, I.; Qiu, C.; Bellander, T.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. Relation of education and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2004, 159, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Letenneur, L.; Gilleron, V.; Commenges, D.; Helmer, C.; Orgogozo, J.-M.; Dartigues, J.-F. Are sex and educational level independent predictors of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease? Incidence data from the PAQUID project. JNNP 1999, 66, 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. von Elm, E.; Altman, D.G.; Egger, M.; Pocock, S.J.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Vandenbroucke, J.P. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int. J. Surg. 2014, 12, 1495–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Yannakoulia, M.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Scarmeas, N. The Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet (HELIAD): Rationale, Study Design, and Cohort Description. Neuroepidemiology 2014, 43, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Liampas, I.; Hatzimanolis, A.; Siokas, V.; Yannakoulia, M.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E. Antihypertensive medication class and the risk of dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: A secondary analysis of the prospective HELIAD cohort. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2022, 89, 709–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Liampas, I.; Siokas, V.; Kyrozis, A.; Sakoutis, G.; Yannakoulia, M.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Sakka, P.; Sakkas, G.K.; Giannaki, C.D.; Stefanidis, I.; et al. Prevalence and Determinants of Restless Legs Syndrome (Willis-Ekbom Disease) in an Older Greek Population. Behav. Sleep Med. 2022, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bougea, A.; Maraki, M.I.; Yannakoulia, M.; Stamelou, M.; Xiromerisiou, G.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Ntanasi, E.; Dardiotis, E.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Sakka, P. Higher probability of prodromal Parkinson disease is related to lower cognitive performance. Neurology 2019, 92, e2261–e2272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kosmidis, M.H.; Vlahou, C.H.; Panagiotaki, P.; Kiosseoglou, G. The verbal fluency task in the Greek population: Normative data, and clustering and switching strategies. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 164–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Robert, P.H.; Lafont, V.; Medecin, I.; Berthet, L.; Thauby, S.; Baudu, C.; Darcourt, G.U.Y. Clustering and switching strategies in verbal fluency tasks: Comparison between schizophrenics and healthy adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998, 4, 539–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kosmidis, M.H.; Vlachos, G.S.; Anastasiou, C.A.; Yannakoulia, M.; Dardiotis, E.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Sakka, P.; Ntanasi, E.; Scarmeas, N. Dementia Prevalence in Greece: The Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet (HELIAD). Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2018, 32, 232–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Vlachos, G.S.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Yannakoulia, M.; Dardiotis, E.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Sakka, P.; Ntanasi, E.; Stefanis, L.; Scarmeas, N. Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in the Elderly Population in Greece: Results From the HELIAD Study. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2020, 34, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  35. McKhann, G.; Drachman, D.; Folstein, M.; Katzman, R.; Price, D.; Stadlan, E.M. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group* under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984, 34, 939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Petersen, R.C. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 2004, 256, 183–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Mourtzi, N.; Yannakoulia, M.; Ntanasi, E.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Anastasiou, C.A.; Dardiotis, E.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Megalou, M.; Sakka, P.; Scarmeas, N. History of induced abortions and frailty in older Greek women: Results from the HELIAD study. Eur. Geriatr. Med. 2018, 9, 301–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Bayles, K.A.; Tomoeda, C.K.; McKnight, P.E.; Helm-Estabrooks, N.; Hawley, J.N. Verbal Perseveration in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease. Semin. Speech Lang. 2004, 25, 335–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Miozzo, M.; Fischer-Baum, S.; Caccappolo-van Vliet, E. Perseverations in Alzheimer’s disease: Memory slips? Cortex 2013, 49, 2028–2039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. DeTure, M.A.; Dickson, D.W. The neuropathological diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Molecular Neurodegeneration 2019, 14, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Grober, E.; Hall, C.B.; Lipton, R.B.; Zonderman, A.B.; Resnick, S.M.; Kawas, C. Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008, 14, 266–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hamel, R.; Köhler, S.; Sistermans, N.; Koene, T.; Pijnenburg, Y.; van der Flier, W.; Scheltens, P.; Aalten, P.; Verhey, F.; Visser, P.J.; et al. The trajectory of cognitive decline in the pre-dementia phase in memory clinic visitors: Findings from the 4C-MCI study. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 1509–1519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Williams, O.A.; An, Y.; Armstrong, N.M.; Kitner-Triolo, M.; Ferrucci, L.; Resnick, S.M. Profiles of Cognitive Change in Preclinical and Prodromal Alzheimer’s Disease Using Change-Point Analysis. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020, 75, 1169–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Amieva, H.; Jacqmin-Gadda, H.; Orgogozo, J.M.; Le Carret, N.; Helmer, C.; Letenneur, L.; Barberger-Gateau, P.; Fabrigoule, C.; Dartigues, J.F. The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: A prospective population-based study. Brain 2005, 128, 1093–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Amieva, H.; Mokri, H.; Le Goff, M.; Meillon, C.; Jacqmin-Gadda, H.; Foubert-Samier, A.; Orgogozo, J.-M.; Stern, Y.; Dartigues, J.-F. Compensatory mechanisms in higher-educated subjects with Alzheimer’s disease: A study of 20 years of cognitive decline. Brain 2014, 137, 1167–1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Wilson, R.S.; Leurgans, S.E.; Boyle, P.A.; Bennett, D.A. Cognitive Decline in Prodromal Alzheimer Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2011, 68, 351–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Reinvang, I.; Grambaite, R.; Espeseth, T. Executive Dysfunction in MCI: Subtype or Early Symptom. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2012, 2012, 936272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Kemp, J.; Philippi, N.; Phillipps, C.; Botzung, A.; Blanc, F. Cognitive profile in prodromal disease (dementia) with Lewy bodies. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2017, 15, 434–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kawashima, S.; Shimizu, Y.; Ueki, Y.; Matsukawa, N. Impairment of the visuospatial working memory in the patients with Parkinson’s Disease: An fMRI study. BMC Neurol. 2021, 21, 335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Survival curves for incident dementia according to the perseverations on the semantic task. Owing to their small number, individuals with ≥3 perseverations were clustered together (n = 40).
Figure 1. Survival curves for incident dementia according to the perseverations on the semantic task. Owing to their small number, individuals with ≥3 perseverations were clustered together (n = 40).
Medicina 58 01814 g001
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of cognitively normal participants according to the diagnosis of dementia or not at follow-up.
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of cognitively normal participants according to the diagnosis of dementia or not at follow-up.
 Baseline ParameterWithout Dementia at Follow-Up (n = 921)With Dementia at Follow-Up (n = 34)p-Value (between-Group Differences)
 Age in years at baseline (N = 955)72.75 ± 4.8677.34 ± 4.99<0.001
 Years of education (N = 955)8.39 ± 4.866.18 ± 4.680.009
 Sex (M/F)
(N = 955)
363/558
(39.4%/60.6%)
15/19
(44.1%/55.9%)
0.582
 Main occupation
(Manual/mental) (N = 860)
520/307
(62.9%/37.1%)
24/9
(72.7%/27.3%)
0.250
 Socioeconomic status
(Low/high) (N = 955)
395/526
(42.9%/57.1%)
18/16
(52.9%/47.1%)
0.245
 MMSE (N = 937)27.66 ± 2.1625.62 ± 2.89<0.001
Semantic condition Number of clusters (N = 936)2.42 ± 1.311.48 ± 1.06<0.001
 Number of switches (N = 936)10.19 ± 4.737.06 ± 4.31<0.001
 Number of intrusions (N = 936)0.62 ± 1.540.35 ± 0.800.346
 Number of perseverations (N = 936)0.57 ± 1.030.71 ± 1.010.469
Phonemic
condition
 Number of clusters (N = 914)0.34 ± 0.790.27 ± 0.520.596
 Number of switches (N = 914)6.91 ± 4.184.60 ± 2.880.003
 Number of intrusions (N = 914)0.28 ± 0.760.23 ± 0.630.749
 Number of perseverations (N = 914)0.41 ± 0.880.33 ± 0.840.656
n: total number of participants; N: number of participants with available data per variable; M/F: male/female; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; bold denotes statistical significance.
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of cognitively normal participants based on the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or not at follow-up.
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of cognitively normal participants based on the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or not at follow-up.
 Baseline ParameterWithout Dementia or MCI at Follow-Up (n = 761)With MCI at Follow-Up (n= 160)p-Value (between-Group Differences)
 Age in years at baseline (N = 921)72.46 ± 4.7074.13 ± 5.36<0.001
 Years of education (N = 921)8.74 ± 4.866.72 ± 4.52<0.001
 Sex (M/F)
(N = 921)
297/464
(39.0%61.0%)
66/94
(41.3%/58.7%)
0.601
 Main occupation
(Manual/mental) (N = 827)
414/269
(60.6%/39.4%)
106/38
(73.6%/26.4%)
0.003
 Socioeconomic status
(Low/high) (N = 921)
313/448
(41.1%/58.9%)
82/78
(51.3%/48.7%)
0.019
 MMSE (N = 905)27.78 ± 2.0027.09 ± 2.50<0.001
Semantic condition Number of clusters (N = 905)2.45 ± 1.242.27 ± 1.590.131
 Number of switches (N = 905)10.56 ± 4.778.41 ± 4.09<0.001
 Number of intrusions (N = 905)0.63 ± 1.620.55 ± 1.090.583
 Number of perseverations (N = 905)0.59 ± 1.010.48 ± 1.140.202
Phonemic condition Number of clusters (N = 884)0.35 ± 0.640.32 ± 1.310.714
 Number of switches (N = 884)7.07 ± 4.236.12 ± 3.800.012
 Number of intrusions (N = 884)0.25 ± 0.680.42 ± 1.080.011
 Number of perseverations (N = 884)0.40 ± 0.840.42 ± 1.060.781
n: total number of participants; N: number of participants with available data per variable; M/F: male/female; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; bold denotes statistical significance.
Table 3. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident all-cause dementia (primary outcome) and Alzheimer’s dementia (secondary outcome).
Table 3. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident all-cause dementia (primary outcome) and Alzheimer’s dementia (secondary outcome).
 VariableDementia at Follow-UpAlzheimer’ s Dementia at Follow-Up
 Semantic condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value
 Number of clusters 0.69 (0.49, 0.97), 0.035 0.71 (0.49, 1.03), 0.069
 Number of switches 0.87 (0.79, 0.97), 0.012 0.86 (0.77, 0.97), 0.011
 Number of intrusions 0.91 (0.60, 1.39), 0.660 0.72 (0.38, 1.34), 0.296
 Number of perseverations 1.52 (1.15, 2.01), 0.003 1.55 (1.18, 2.04), 0.002
 Phonemic condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value
 Number of clusters 1.19 (0.73, 1.94), 0.480 1.17 (0.69, 1.98), 0.552
 Number of switches 0.88 (0.77, 1.01), 0.060 0.88 (0.76, 1.01), 0.071
 Number of intrusions 0.99 (0.55, 1.78), 0.975 1.04 (0.59, 1.84), 0.894
 Number of perseverations 1.26 (0.81, 1.96), 0.308 1.32 (0.85, 2.03), 0.217
HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval; bold denotes statistical significance.
Table 4. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident all-cause dementia (primary outcome) and Alzheimer’s dementia (secondary outcome).
Table 4. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident all-cause dementia (primary outcome) and Alzheimer’s dementia (secondary outcome).
 VariableDementia at Follow-UpAlzheimer’ s Dementia at Follow-Up
 Semantic condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value
 Number of clusters 0.64 (0.43, 0.97), 0.037 0.64 (0.41, 1.01), 0.057
 Number of switches 0.94 (0.83, 1.07), 0.364 0.96 (0.84, 1.10), 0.576
 Number of intrusions 0.86 (0.56, 1.31), 0.468 0.89 (0.59, 1.34), 0.567
 Number of perseverations 1.08 (0.71, 1.66), 0.719 1.01 (0.66, 1.55), 0.950
 Phonemic condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-value
 Number of clusters 1.20 (0.41, 3.52), 0.742 1.32 (0.42, 4.09), 0.634
 Number of switches 1.00 (0.85, 1.16), 0.957 0.98 (0.83, 1.16), 0.844
 Number of intrusions 3.05 (0.94, 9.95), 0.062 3.46 (1.07, 11.19), 0.039
 Number of perseverations 1.48 (0.47, 4.66), 0.506 1.49 (0.46, 4.80), 0.502
HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval.
Table 5. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Table 5. Associations between qualitative verbal fluency indices and incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
 VariableMCI at Follow-UpVariableMCI at Follow-Up
 Semantic Condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-ValuePhonemic Condition Adjusted HR (95%CI), p-Value
 Number of clusters 1.03 (0.90, 1.18), 0.689Number of clusters 1.21 (0.96, 1.51), 0.101
 Number of switches 0.95 (0.91, 0.99), 0.014Number of switches 0.98 (0.93, 1.04), 0.544
 Number of intrusions 1.03 (0.89, 1.18), 0.773Number of intrusions 1.27 (1.06, 1.53), 0.009
 Number of perseverations 1.15 (0.96, 1.39), 0.139Number of perseverations 1.14 (0.94, 1.38), 0.186
HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval; MCI: mild cognitive impairment.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Liampas, I.; Folia, V.; Zoupa, E.; Siokas, V.; Yannakoulia, M.; Sakka, P.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H. Qualitative Verbal Fluency Components as Prognostic Factors for Developing Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Population-Based HELIAD Cohort. Medicina 2022, 58, 1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58121814

AMA Style

Liampas I, Folia V, Zoupa E, Siokas V, Yannakoulia M, Sakka P, Hadjigeorgiou G, Scarmeas N, Dardiotis E, Kosmidis MH. Qualitative Verbal Fluency Components as Prognostic Factors for Developing Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Population-Based HELIAD Cohort. Medicina. 2022; 58(12):1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58121814

Chicago/Turabian Style

Liampas, Ioannis, Vasiliki Folia, Elli Zoupa, Vasileios Siokas, Mary Yannakoulia, Paraskevi Sakka, Georgios Hadjigeorgiou, Nikolaos Scarmeas, Efthimios Dardiotis, and Mary H. Kosmidis. 2022. "Qualitative Verbal Fluency Components as Prognostic Factors for Developing Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results from the Population-Based HELIAD Cohort" Medicina 58, no. 12: 1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58121814

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop