Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Public Understanding of Adolescents’ Risks on Facebook in Taiwan
Previous Article in Journal
Sexual and Reproductive Health Needs and Priorities of the Adolescents in Northern Pakistan: A Formative Evaluation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Dysfunction among U.S. High School Students and Its Association with Time Spent on Digital Devices: A Population-Based Study

1
Department of Psychiatry, Brookdale University Hospital Medical Center, New York, NY 11212, USA
2
Port of Spain General Hospital, Port of Spain, Trinidad and Tobago
3
Government Medical College, Patiala 147001, India
4
Dr N. D. Desai Hospital and Research Center, Nadiad 387001, India
5
B J Medical College, Ahmedabad 380016, India
6
Government Medical College, Surat 395001, India
7
Central New York Psychiatric Center, Office of Mental Health, Marcy, NY 13403, USA
8
Government Medical College, Amritsar 143001, India
9
Fulton State Hospital, Fulton, MO 65251, USA
10
Department of Pediatrics, Poznan University of Medical Sciences, 61-701 Poznan, Poland
11
Government Medical College and Hospital Sector 32, Chandigarh 160047, India
12
Department of Pediatrics, St. George’s University School of Medicine, St. George, Grenada
13
Public Health and Neurology, Icahn School of Medicine Mount Sinai, New York, NY 10029, USA
14
IU Health Ball Memorial Hospital, Muncie, IN 47303, USA
15
Ann & Lurie Children’s Hospital of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
16
Department of Pediatrics, University of Illinois Hospital & Health Sciences System, Chicago, IL 60612, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Adolescents 2022, 2(2), 286-295; https://doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2020022
Submission received: 30 March 2022 / Revised: 20 May 2022 / Accepted: 30 May 2022 / Published: 31 May 2022
(This article belongs to the Collection Featured Research in Adolescent Health)

Abstract

:
Introduction: Cognitive dysfunction is a hallmark feature of many psychiatric disorders. We aimed to study the prevalence and predictors of cognitive dysfunction (CD) among U.S. high school students and its association with time spent on digital devices. Methods: We performed a cross-sectional survey study using YRBSS 2019 data of U.S. high school students in grades 9–12. Cognitive dysfunction was defined by difficulties with remembering, concentrating, and making decisions due to emotional, physical, or mental problems. Digital screen time was described by daily time spent on TV, computers, tablets, and phone. We performed univariate and multivariable survey logistic regression analysis to identify the prevalence of cognitive dysfunction and its association with time spent on digital devices. Results: Out of 10,317 total participants, 3914 (37.9%) reported CD. The prevalence of CD was higher in females compared to males (46.0% vs. 29.9%). Compared to participants with no CD, participants with CD reported substance abuse, such as alcohol (35.8% vs. 26.6%), marijuana (28.3% vs. 17.6%), cigarette (8.1% vs. 4.7%), and illicit drugs (18.9% vs. 9.0%) and they reported a higher prevalence (p < 0.0001 for all substances). Participants who felt sad and hopeless (62.8 vs. 22.1%) reported a high prevalence of CD, whereas participants with adequate sleep reported low prevalence (15.7% vs. 25.6%). In a regression, daily video game/internet use for non-work-related activities for 4 h (aOR:1.27; p = 0.03) and ≥5 h (aOR:1.70; p < 0.0001) demonstrated higher odds of CD, compared to participants with no daily use. Female sex, substance use, and depressed mood were additional predictors of CD. Conclusion: The prevalence of CD is high in U.S. high-school students. Female sex, substance abuse, depressed mood, and excessive VG/PC use is associated with high odds of cognitive dysfunction. Further research is needed to explore the complex relationship between screen time and cognitive dysfunction.

1. Introduction

Cognitive dysfunction refers to any deficits in attention, verbal and nonverbal learning, short-term and working memory, visual and auditory processing, problem-solving, processing speed, and motor functioning [1]. Cognitive dysfunction is a secondary manifestation of many psychiatric illnesses. In the pediatric population, ADHD, behavior problems, anxiety, and depression are the most commonly diagnosed psychiatric illnesses. In children aged between 2 and 17 years (~6.1 million), 9.4% of them reported ADHD, and between 3 and 17 years (approximately 1.9 million), 3.2% of them experienced depression [2]. Many of these conditions are associated with secondary cognitive dysfunction as a feature. Research has shown that ADHD is a prevalent condition associated with cognitive dysfunction, especially impaired inhibitory control [3]. Adolescent major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with impulsivity and cognitive dysfunction [4].
With a steady increase in screen time in the past decade, the focus has been on studying the effects of screen time on mental health, and developmental and educational outcomes. For example, a study by Twenge et al. found that adolescents who spent more time on screen media were at increased risk of depression and suicide [5]. A study by Hu et al. found that excessive passive screen time in preschool children was negatively associated with executive functioning and social skills [6]. In 2019, teenagers (13–18 years) spent an average of seven hours per day on screen media; 18% of them reported more than 10 h of usage. Of the total daily screen time, teens spent an average of 2.4 h watching TV/DVDs/Videos, 1.2 h playing mobile, computer, or video games, and 1.1 h on social media [7]. Additional studies have explored the effects of problematic internet use and video game addictions on mental health, and cognitive and educational outcomes. Problematic internet use is defined as repetitive impairing behaviors, such as excessive video game playing, cybersex, online buying, streaming, social media use, and the inability to control the amount of time spent on the internet [8]. Problematic internet use is associated with decrements across a range of neuropsychological domains, irrespective of geographical location, supporting its cross-cultural and biological validity [9]. Individuals with severe ADHD symptoms may be at great risk of developing symptoms of video game addiction, regardless of the type of video game played or preferred [10]. Increased screen time is associated with a sedentary lifestyle and high depression rates [11]. Specifically, research has shown that frequent daily cellphone and tablet use causes sleep problems in adolescents [12]. Thus, the relationship between screen time, mental health, and cognitive and educational outcomes needs further exploration.
Many studies have used data from youth risk behavior surveys to determine the impact of screen time, video games, and internet usage on various health outcomes, including obesity, suicide, depression, and violence. Messias et al. used YRBSS 2007 and 2009 data to find associations between sadness, suicide, and excessive internet/video game use [13]. However, Lee et al. found that the association between video game/internet use and mental health varied by sex [14]. To our knowledge, no studies have been done using the YRBSS database to access the prevalence and predictors of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students. To further explore the relationship between screen time and cognitive dysfunction while adjusting for mental illness as a potential confounder, we decided to perform a retrospective cross-sectional study using the YRBSS 2019 database to identify the prevalence and predictors of cognitive dysfunction in U.S. high school students and its association with digital screen time.

2. Methods

2.1. Details of Data

The Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS) contains statistics collected by the CDC to monitor health behaviors that contribute to the leading cause of morbidity and mortality among youth and adults. The six main categories monitored by the YRBSS include: (1) behaviors that contribute to unintentional injury and violence; (2) tobacco use; (3) alcohol and other drug use; (4) sexual behaviors that contribute to unintended pregnancy and STD/HIV infection; (5) dietary behaviors; and (6) physical inactivity. The YRBSS also monitors the prevalence of asthma, obesity, and other health factors. The YRBSS uses a 3-stage cluster sample design to produce a national representative sample of 9th to 12th-grade students. A new YRBSS database is released every 2 years. The latest YRBSS database at the time of this study was the YRBSS 2019 database.

2.2. Study Population/Study Type/Patient Characteristics

Using the YRBSS 2019 database, we performed a retrospective cross-sectional study to evaluate the prevalence of self-reported cognitive dysfunction in U.S. high school students. Participants in grades 9 to 12 were closely examined, excluding participants with missing or unknown grades (Q3) from the study. Variables, such as age, sex, race/ethnicity, grades, substance abuse, sleep duration, sadness or hopelessness, and self-reported cognitive impairment, were included in the study. Participants with missing information in these variables were excluded from the analysis.

2.3. Outcomes (Definitions) Primary, Secondary

The primary aim of this study was to evaluate the prevalence and predictors of self-reported cognitive impairment amongst 9–12th grade participants. The secondary aim of this study was to identify the association between digital screen time and self-reported cognitive impairment.

3. Measures

3.1. Digital Device Use & Screen Time

To identify the amount of time spent on digital devices, we used the following questions from the YRBSS 2019 database: “Q79. On an average school day, how many hours do you watch TV?” and “Q80. On an average school day, how many hours do you play video or computer games or use a computer for something that is not school work? (Count time spent playing games, watching videos, texting, or using social media on your smartphone, computer, Xbox, PlayStation, iPad, or other tablets)”. We believe that these two questions helped us gather conclusive information to quantify the digital screen time factor appropriately.

3.2. Cognitive Dysfunction

To identify cognitive dysfunction, we used the following question from the YRBSS 2019 database: “Q98. Because of a physical, mental, or emotional problem, do you have serious difficulty concentrating, remembering, or making decisions?”.

3.3. Adequate Sleep

Although adequate sleep can include a range of hours, for our study, we decided to use a precomputed sleep variable from the YRBSS data that divides participants into less than or greater than eight hours of sleep per night. To identify whether this criterion was met, we used the following question from the YRBSS 2019 database. “Q88. On an average school night, how many hours of sleep do you get?”. Responses indicating ≥8 h and those <8 h were dichotomized as “yes” or “no” to separate individuals who met the 8 h of sleep criterion vs. individuals who did not.

3.4. Substance Use

To identify current substance use, we used the following questions from the YRBSS 2019 database. For current cigarette smoking: “Q32. During the past 30 days, how many days did you smoke cigarettes?”. For current alcohol use: “Q41. During the past 30 days, on how many days did you have at least one drink of alcohol?”. For current marijuana use: “Q47. During the past 30 days, how many times did you use marijuana?”. For illicit drug use during lifetime: “QNILLICT” was used corresponding to participants who reported using cocaine, inhalants, heroin, methamphetamine, ecstasy, or hallucinogens during their lifetime. All participants’ reported responses to current substance use questions were dichotomized as “yes” or “no.”

3.5. Depressed Mood

To identify depressed mood, we used the following question from the YRBSS 2019 database. “Q25. During the past 12 months, did you ever feel so sad or hopeless almost every day for 2 weeks or more in a row that you stopped doing some usual activities?”. Responses were dichotomized as “yes” or “no.”

3.6. Covariates and Confounders

Demographic characteristics included sex, age, grade, and race/ethnicity. Substance use, sleep duration, and feeling sad and hopeless were added to the analysis as predictors and potential confounders.

3.7. Statistical Analysis

All the analyses were performed using IBM SPSS, version 25. To account for the complex survey design of the YRBSS 2019 database, a complex sample analysis method in SPSS was used, accounting for strata, clusters, and sample weight. Descriptive statistics were derived using complex sample crosstabs with a chi-square test to determine a statistically significant association. Strata, clusters, and weight-accounted-for multivariable logistic regression analysis were used to determine the association between time spent on digital devices and self-reported cognitive dysfunction after adjusting for previously defined covariates and confounders. All statistical tests used were 2-tailed t-tests. The alpha level was set at 0.05, which means that p-values had to be equal to or less than 0.05 to indicate significance. c-index (area under the ROC curve) to evaluate the goodness of fit was calculated for the regression model.

4. Results

4.1. Demographic Characteristics

Of the total 10,317 U.S. high school students from YRBSS 2019 included in the analysis, 3914 (37.9%) of them reported cognitive dysfunction. Females reported cognitive dysfunction in higher percentages compared to males (46.0% vs. 29.9%, p < 0.0001). Prevalence of cognitive dysfunction was higher in Hispanic (9.5% vs. 9.0%, p = 0.001) and Multiple-Hispanic (19.7% vs. 17.5%, p = 0.001). Among the participants with concurrent conditions, current alcohol users (35.8% vs. 26.6%, p < 0.0001), current marijuana users (28.3% vs. 17.6%, p < 0.0001), current cigarette smokers (8.1% vs. 4.7%, p < 0.0001), and individuals who had ever tried illicit drugs (18.9% vs. 9.0%, p < 0.0001) reported high frequencies of cognitive dysfunction. Participants feeling sad or hopeless reported high frequencies of cognitive dysfunction (62.8% vs. 22.1%, p < 0.0001) (Table 1).

4.2. Digital Screen Time

Participants who watched TV daily for 4 h (4.9% vs. 3.9%, p = 0.002) and 5 h or more (7.1% vs. 5.0%, p = 0.002) reported high prevalence of cognitive dysfunction. Among the participants who played video games/used computer for non-work-related activities every day, individuals with 4 h (11.1% vs. 9.4%, p < 0.0001) and five or more hours of usage (28.2% vs. 16.8%, p < 0.0001) reported cognitive dysfunction in high frequencies (Table 2).

4.3. Multivariable Regression Analysis

The multivariate regression analysis showed that the odds of reporting cognitive dysfunction was high in participants who played video games/used computer for non-work-related activities daily for 4 h (aOR: 1.27, 95% CI 1.02–1.58%; p = 0.035) and five or more hours (1.70, 1.39–2.08; p < 0.0001). Female participants were at high odds of reporting cognitive dysfunction (1.65, 1.49–1.82; p < 0.0001). Substance abuse and depressed mood were additional significant predictors of cognitive dysfunction (Table 3).

5. Discussion

We found that 37.9% of U.S. high school students reported cognitive dysfunction. Females and participants with concurrent substance abuse reported high frequencies of cognitive dysfunction. The prevalence of cognitive dysfunction is significantly higher in participants currently feeling sad and hopeless. In the adjusted regression model, besides the excessive screen time spent on video games/internet, female sex, substance abuse, and depressed mood were significant predictors of cognitive dysfunction.
Regarding the association between screen time and cognitive dysfunction, our results based on the adjusted regression model indicate that the relationship between digital screen time and cognitive dysfunction depends on the amount of time spent and the type of device used. For example, we found that playing video games or using the internet on computers/tablets for non-work-related activities for four or more hours per day is associated with increased odds of reporting cognitive dysfunction. However, we did not find such an association for daily TV use. In support of our findings, an NIH funded cohort study performed by Vohr et al. found that high screen time was independently associated with defects in executive functioning and adverse behavioral outcomes at the age of 6 to 7 years in children born at less than 28 weeks [15]. Similarly, McHarg et al. found that screen time at the age of 2 was negatively associated with the development of executive functioning in toddlerhood from age 2 to 3 [16]. Both of these studies explored the effects of excessive screen time during the critical brain development period and hypothesized that sensory deprivation and social exclusion from excessive screen time could be the reason behind adverse cognitive and behavioral development. While there is some evidence to link excessive screen time directly to cognitive dysfunction, there is plenty of evidence and theories to explain how excessive screen time could lead to mental illness directly or indirectly, which could then lead to cognitive dysfunction as a result of mental illness. Thus, both cognitive impairment and adverse behavioral outcomes remain inseparable when studying the effects of excessive screen time.
While there is no cutoff separating healthy video game/internet use from pathological use, based on our results, 4 or more hours of daily video game/internet use on computers/tablets for non-work-related activities might fall under pathological or excessive usage. A study by Gentile et al. found that pathological video game use was associated with poor school performance in children aged 8–18 [17]. Poor school performance in children could be explained by cognitive impairment secondary to pathological video game usage. Similarly, previous studies have shown that problematic internet use in adolescents is also associated with impaired decision-making, impulsivity, and memory deficit. Such a deficit in decision-making areas of the brain among people with an Internet gaming disorder could potentially increase the risk of substance abuse and subsequent depression [18,19,20]. Our results support this notion as we found that, along with excessive usage of video games/internet (four hours or more per day), concurrent substance abuse and currently feeling sad and hopeless were associated with high odds of cognitive dysfunction. Another possibility is that excessive screen time could lead to a sedentary lifestyle that could increase the odds of developing depression and subsequent cognitive dysfunction [11]. The argument against this theory is that pathological TV use is not associated with cognitive dysfunction as per our results. Similarly, Messias et al. found an association between excessive video game use, sadness, and suicidality, but they reported no statistically significant association between TV use, sadness, and suicidality using the YRBSS 2007 and 2009 data [13]. These results suggest that excessive video games/internet use on computers/tablets for non-work-related activities are uniquely associated with sadness, suicidality, and cognitive dysfunction. The high odds of cognitive dysfunction in females could be explained by higher social media usage, making females more vulnerable to cyberbullying, and increasing insecurities about body image, leading to depression and eventually cognitive dysfunction as a result [21,22,23].
Our results indicate that only pathological use of video games/internet use on computers/tablets for non-work-related activities were associated with increased odds of cognitive dysfunction. The high prevalence rate of self-reported cognitive dysfunction in U.S. high school students necessitates further screening of these individuals. Cognitive dysfunction associated with many pediatric psychiatric disorders is either reversible with treatment or reduced symptomatically with treatment. Thus, it is crucial to identify them early to prevent the adverse effects of cognitive dysfunction on academic and social settings and relationships.

Strengths and Limitations

The strength of our study is that we have used nationally representative data with a large sample size; thus, improving the generalizability of our results. Despite these strengths, our study has some limitations. First, the YRBSS questionnaire asks about the number of hours spent daily on TV, video games/internet use on computers/tablets, but it does not ask about the type of content seen and activities performed on these devices. Second, YRBSS groups video game use and internet use on computers/tablets together. Thus, it was not possible to study the individual effects of each activity. Third, the type of cognitive dysfunction that we used for our study was self-reported (subjective), and no objective neurocognitive test was used to identify cognitive dysfunction. Finally, due to our study’s cross-sectional design, we could not establish a temporal relationship between cognitive dysfunction and digital screen time.

6. Conclusions

There is a high prevalence of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students. Female sex, substance use, depressed mood, and video game/internet use for 4 or more hours daily were significant predictors of cognitive dysfunction. Further screening of such students is needed to identify reversible causes of cognitive dysfunction, identify and treat underlying psychiatric illnesses, and substance abuse, and reduce such impairment’s academic and psycho-social impact. The association between digital screen time and cognitive dysfunction is complex. Only those who played video games/used computers for non-work-related activities for four hours or more per day were at higher odds of reporting cognitive dysfunction. We found no association between TV use and cognitive dysfunction. Further research is needed to explore the relationship between digital screen time and cognitive dysfunction.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.D.; data curation, P.S. (Puneet Singla) and R.S.; formal analysis, S.P. (Sejal Patel); investigation A.M. and M.T.; methodology, T.S.; software, P.S. (Prerna Sharma) and A.A.B.; validation, S.G. and M.C.; project administration, B.D.; resources, T.P.; supervision R.P.M.P. and S.P. (Saurabhkumar Patel); writing–original draft, S.D.; writing–review and editing, Y.-C.H. and U.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Our study utilized deidentified and publicly available data, thus, IRB approval was not required.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System at https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/index.htm (accessed on 30 May 2022). Software and code available upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lam, R.W.; Kennedy, S.; McIntyre, R.S.; Khullar, A. Cognitive Dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: Effects on Psychosocial Functioning and Implications for Treatment. Can. J. Psychiatry 2014, 59, 649–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Data and Statistics on Children’s Mental Health | CDC. (n.d.). Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/childrensmentalhealth/data.html (accessed on 29 May 2022).
  3. Tremblay, L.K.; Hammill, C.; Ameis, S.H.; Bhaijiwala, M.; Mabbott, D.J.; Anagnostou, E.; Lerch, J.P.; Schachar, R.J. Tracking Inhibitory Control in Youth with ADHD: A Multi-Modal Neuroimaging Approach. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Maalouf, F.T.; Brent, D.; Clark, L.; Tavitian, L.; McHugh, R.M.; Sahakian, B.J.; Phillips, M.L. Neurocognitive impairment in adolescent major depressive disorder: State vs. trait illness markers. J. Affect. Disord. 2011, 133, 625–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Twenge, J.M.; Joiner, T.E.; Rogers, M.L.; Martin, G.N. Increases in Depressive Symptoms, Suicide-Related Outcomes, and Suicide Rates among U.S. Adolescents after 2010 and Links to Increased New Media Screen Time. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 6, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Hu, B.Y.; Johnson, G.K.; Teo, T.; Wu, Z. Relationship Between Screen Time and Chinese Children’s Cognitive and Social Development. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2020, 34, 183–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Tweens and Teens, 2019 | Common Sense Media. (n.d.). Available online: https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-tweens-and-teens-2019 (accessed on 29 May 2022).
  8. Ko, C.-H.; Yen, J.-Y.; Yen, C.-F.; Chen, C.-S.; Chen, C.C. The association between Internet addiction and psychiatric disorder: A review of the literature. Eur. Psychiatry 2012, 27, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ioannidis, K.; Hook, R.; Goudriaan, A.E.; Vlies, S.; Fineberg, N.A.; Grant, J.E.; Chamberlain, S.R. Cognitive deficits in problematic internet use: Meta-analysis of 40 studies. Br. J. Psychiatry 2019, 215, 639–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Mathews, C.L.; Morrell, H.E.R.; Molle, J.E. Video game addiction, ADHD symptomatology, and video game reinforcement. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abus. 2019, 45, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wang, X.; Li, Y.; Fan, H. The associations between screen time-based sedentary behavior and depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cabré-Riera, A.; Torrent, M.; Donaire-Gonzalez, D.; Vrijheid, M.; Cardis, E.; Guxens, M. Telecommunication devices use, screen time and sleep in adolescents. Environ. Res. 2019, 171, 341–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Messias, E.; Castro, J.; Saini, A.; Usman, M.; Peeples, D. Sadness, Suicide, and Their Association with Video Game and Internet Overuse among Teens: Results from the Youth Risk Behavior Survey 2007 and 2009. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2011, 41, 307–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lee, H.H.; Sung, J.H.; Lee, J.-Y.; Lee, J.E. Differences by Sex in Association of Mental Health with Video Gaming or Other Nonacademic Computer Use among US Adolescents. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2017, 14, E117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Vohr, B.R.; McGowan, E.C.; Bann, C.; Das, A.; Higgins, R.; Hintz, S. Association of High Screen-Time Use with School-age Cognitive, Executive Function, and Behavior Outcomes in Extremely Preterm Children. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, 1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. McHarg, G.; Ribner, A.D.; Devine, R.T.; Hughes, C. Screen Time and Executive Function in Toddlerhood: A Longitudinal Study. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 570392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gentile, D. Pathological Video-Game Use among Youth Ages 8 to 18: A national study. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 594–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Kim, J.Y. The Nonlinear Association Between Internet Using Time for Non-Educational Purposes and Adolescent Health. J. Prev. Med. Public Health 2012, 45, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wang, Y.; Wu, L.; Wang, L.; Zhang, Y.; Du, X.; Dong, G. Impaired decision-making and impulse control in Internet gaming addicts: Evidence from the comparison with recreational Internet game users. Addict. Biol. 2017, 22, 1610–1621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Park, M.; Jung, M.H.; Lee, J.; Choi, A.R.; Chung, S.J.; Kim, B.; Kim, D.J.; Choi, J.-S. Neurophysiological and Cognitive Correlates of Error Processing Deficits in Internet Gaming Disorder. Cereb. Cortex 2020, 30, 4914–4921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Park, S. Concentration of internet usage and its relation to exposure to negative content: Does the gender gap differ among adults and adolescents? Women’s Stud. Int. Forum 2009, 32, 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Thorisdottir, I.E.; Sigurvinsdottir, R.; Asgeirsdottir, B.B.; Allegrante, J.P.; Sigfusdottir, I.D. Active and Passive Social Media Use and Symptoms of Anxiety and Depressed Mood among Icelandic Adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2019, 22, 535–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Houghton, S.; Hunter, S.C.; Rosenberg, M.; Wood, L.; Zadow, C.; Martin, K.; Shilton, T. Virtually impossible: Limiting Australian children and adolescents daily screen based media use. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Epidemiological characteristics and weighted prevalence of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students-YRBSS 2019.
Table 1. Epidemiological characteristics and weighted prevalence of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students-YRBSS 2019.
Cognitive Dysfunction
N = 3914 (37.9%)
No Cognitive Dysfunction
N = 6403
(62.1%)
Total
N = 10,317
(100%)
p-Value
Age0.754
14 years old or younger464 (11.9% *)763 (11.9%)1227 (11.9%)
15 years old945 (24.2)1618 (25.3)2563 (24.9)
16 years old1035 (26.5)1607 (25.1)2642 (25.6)
17 years old944 (24.2)1506 (23.5)2450 (23.8)
18 years old or older519 (13.3)902 (14.1)1420 (13.8)
Sex<0.0001
Female2341 (60.1)2753 (43.1)5094 (49.6)
Male1553 (39.9)3633 (56.9)5186 (50.4)
Race/Ethnicity0.001
Am Indian/Alaska Native28 (0.7)31 (0.5)59 (0.6)
Asian185 (4.8)305 (4.8)490 (4.8)
Black or African American301 (7.8)658 (10.4)958 (9.4)
Native Hawaiian/Other PI6 (0.2)25 (0.4)31 (0.3)
White1995 (51.9)3385 (53.7)5380 (53.0)
Hispanic/Latino364 (9.5)567 (9.0)931 (9.2)
Multiple-Hispanic757 (19.7)1103 (17.5)1860 (18.3)
Multiple-Non-Hispanic210 (5.5)232 (3.7)443 (4.4)
Grade0.176
9th grade1000 (25.6)1797 (28.1)2797 (27.1)
10th grade1027 (26.2)1593 (24.9)2619 (25.4)
11th grade980 (25.0)1475 (23.0)2454 (23.8)
12th grade907 (23.2)1539 (24.0)2446 (23.7)
Concurrent conditions
Current alcohol use1296 (35.8)1595 (26.6)2891 (30.0)<0.0001
Current cigarette smoking310 (8.1)298 (4.7)608 (6.0)<0.0001
Current marijuana use1088 (28.3)1108 (17.6)2195 (21.6)<0.0001
Ever illicit drug use730 (18.9)570 (9.0)1300 (12.8)<0.0001
Currently feeling sad or hopeless2433 (62.8)1403 (22.1)3836 (37.5)<0.0001
Currently having adequate sleep609 (15.7)1626 (25.6)2235 (21.8)<0.0001
* The percentage (%) in the table above is column %, describing a comparison between cognitive dysfunction vs. no cognitive dysfunction.
Table 2. Prevalence of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students with daily digital screen time-YRBSS 2019.
Table 2. Prevalence of cognitive dysfunction among U.S. high school students with daily digital screen time-YRBSS 2019.
Cognitive Dysfunction
N = 3914 (37.9%)
No Cognitive Dysfunction
N = 6403
(62.1%)
Total
N = 10,317
(100%)
p-Value
Current Video Game/Non-Work-Related Computer Use<0.0001
No playing video/computer game617 (15.9)1138 (18.0)1755 (17.2)
<1 h per day365 (9.4)671 (10.6)1037 (10.2)
1 h per day277 (7.2)723 (11.4)1001 (9.8)
2 h per day531 (13.7)1139 (18.0)1670 (16.4)
3 h per day561 (14.5)1001 (15.8)1562 (15.3)
4 h per day432 (11.1)595 (9.4)1027 (10.1)
5 h or more per day1096 (28.2)1065 (16.8)2161 (21.2)
Current TV Use0.002
No TV on use1135 (29.2)1721 (27.1)2857 (27.9)
<1 h per day774 (19.9)1383 (21.8)2157 (21.1)
1 h per day499 (12.8)987 (15.5)1485 (14.5)
2 h per day647 (16.7)1084 (17.1)1732 (16.9)
3 h per day361 (9.3)604 (9.5)966 (9.4)
4 h per day190 (4.9)250 (3.9)440 (4.3)
5 h or more per day278 (7.1)316 (5.0)594 (5.8)
Table 3. Multivariable logistic regression establishes an association of cognitive dysfunction with digital screen time.
Table 3. Multivariable logistic regression establishes an association of cognitive dysfunction with digital screen time.
ParameterAdjusted Odds RatioConfidence Interval Lower LimitConfidence Interval Upper Limitp-Value
Current Video Game/Non-Work-Related Computer Use
No useReference
<1 h per day1.110.871.4000.399
1 h per day0.800.611.050.099
2 h per day1.010.821.230.957
3 h per day1.050.821.350.676
4 h per day1.271.021.580.035
5 h or more per day1.701.392.08<0.0001
Current TV Use
No TV useReference
<1 h per day0.980.811.180.832
1 h per day0.980.791.220.861
2 h per day1.010.811.260.908
3 h per day0.880.681.130.295
4 h per day1.050.821.350.676
5 h or more per day1.050.791.390.727
Age
14 years old or youngerReference
15 years old0.990.751.300.932
16 years old0.990.711.370.930
17 years old1.000.671.500.983
18 years old or older0.990.611.600.95
Sex
Female1.651.491.82<0.0001
MaleReference
Race/Ethnicity
Am Indian/Alaska Native1.490.703.160.289
Asian1.250.911.730.166
Black or African American0.780.620.990.039
Native Hawaiian/Other PI0.440.151.330.140
Hispanic/Latino0.960.751.240.759
Multiple-Hispanic1.090.921.280.320
Multiple-Non-Hispanic1.361.101.700.007
WhiteReference
Grade
9thReference
10th1.130.891.420.303
11th1.050.731.510.790
12th0.850.551.320.465
Concurrent conditions
Current Alcohol Use (Yes vs. No)0.990.831.180.876
Current Cigarette Smoking (Yes vs. No)0.960.701.320.798
Current Marijuana Use (Yes vs. No)1.431.221.68<0.0001
Ever Illicit Drug Use (Yes vs. No)1.451.121.880.006
Currently Feeling Sad or Hopeless (Yes vs. No)4.954.125.95<0.0001
Currently Having Adequate Sleep (Yes vs. No)0.770.650.910.003
C-Value (area under the ROC curve)0.759
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Desai, S.; Satnarine, T.; Singla, P.; Mistry, A.; Gadiwala, S.; Patel, S.; Das, B.; Sharma, P.; Telsem, M.; Stuart, R.; et al. Cognitive Dysfunction among U.S. High School Students and Its Association with Time Spent on Digital Devices: A Population-Based Study. Adolescents 2022, 2, 286-295. https://doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2020022

AMA Style

Desai S, Satnarine T, Singla P, Mistry A, Gadiwala S, Patel S, Das B, Sharma P, Telsem M, Stuart R, et al. Cognitive Dysfunction among U.S. High School Students and Its Association with Time Spent on Digital Devices: A Population-Based Study. Adolescents. 2022; 2(2):286-295. https://doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2020022

Chicago/Turabian Style

Desai, Saral, Travis Satnarine, Puneet Singla, Ayushi Mistry, Salika Gadiwala, Sejal Patel, Bibhuti Das, Prerna Sharma, Muna Telsem, Robert Stuart, and et al. 2022. "Cognitive Dysfunction among U.S. High School Students and Its Association with Time Spent on Digital Devices: A Population-Based Study" Adolescents 2, no. 2: 286-295. https://doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2020022

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop