Next Article in Journal
Goal-Setting among Biology Undergraduates during a Free-Choice Learning Experience at a Regional Zoo
Next Article in Special Issue
Investigating the Effect of Disturbance on Prey Consumption in Captive Congo Caecilians Herpele squalostoma
Previous Article in Journal
How Can India Leverage Its Botanic Gardens for the Conservation and Sustainable Utilization of Wild Food Plant Resources through the Implementation of a Global Strategy for Plant Conservation?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Investigating the Effect of Enrichment on the Behavior of Zoo-Housed Southern Ground Hornbills

by
James Edward Brereton
1,*,
Mark Nigel Geoffrey Myhill
2 and
James Ali Shora
3
1
Zoo and Animal Studies, Higher Education, University Centre Sparsholt, Westley Lane, Sparsholt, Winchester SO21 2NF, UK
2
Beale Wildlife Park, Lower Basildon, Pangbourne, Reading RG8 9NW, UK
3
Battersea Park Children’s Zoo, Battersea Park, Chelsea Bridge, London SW11 4NJ, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2(4), 600-609; https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040043
Submission received: 6 September 2021 / Revised: 16 October 2021 / Accepted: 8 November 2021 / Published: 13 November 2021

Abstract

:
Enrichment is essential for the welfare of many zoo-housed animals, yet the value of enrichment is not well understood for all taxa. As an intelligent, long-lived species, the southern ground hornbill (Bucorvus leadbeateri) is a good model for enrichment research. A pair of southern ground hornbills, housed at Beale Wildlife Park and Gardens, were observed during study periods in 2014, 2018, and 2019. Three types of enrichment were provided for the birds; these enrichment types were developed based on information on the habits of the species as found in natural history papers. The enrichment types consisted of a pile of twigs, small animal carcasses, and plastic mirrors. Overall, the carcass feeds and the mirrors resulted in the greatest changes in behavior, with hornbills engaging in long periods of food manipulation with carcasses. For the mirror condition, hornbills spent time stalking around and pecking at mirrors, similar to the ‘window smashing’ behavior seen in wild hornbills. Overall, the research suggests that not only can enrichment modify the behavior of southern ground hornbills, but non-nutritional enrichment may be equally valuable to the animals. Natural history papers may have some value in inspiring novel enrichment items for zoo-housed animals.

1. Introduction

Enrichment is fundamentally important for the welfare of many animals in zoos, yet there remain gaps in the knowledge of provision of enrichment for some taxa [1]. Many enrichment studies have been conducted for some taxonomic groups, such as the mammalian families, Felidae and Elephantidae [2]. The availability of studies allows researchers to evaluate and compare enrichment strategies, and therefore put in place the most effective plans. For some taxonomic groups, however, information on enrichment is more limited. This reduces the information available to practitioners to help improve the welfare of their animals.
Enrichment is particularly important for highly cognitive species that may otherwise become bored with a predictable or unstimulating environment. Many avian families are particularly susceptible, expressing unnatural behaviors, such as stereotypy, if enrichment is insufficient. Parrots (family Psittacidae) are a good example; birds in this family are typically intelligent, capable problem-solvers who live long lives. Where enrichment or social groups are not provided, feather-plucking and stereotyped behavior commonly occur [3]. Fortunately, enrichment has been well studied for the Psittacidae, and as a result, animal keepers have several effective strategies available to reduce the prevalence of stereotypy [1,2,3].
However, other intelligent, long-lived bird taxa are kept in zoos, some of which have received less focus in terms of enrichment research. One example is the hornbills (Order Bucerotiformes). Hornbills can be found throughout Africa and Asia, and are well-known for their unusual nesting habits, in which females often seal themselves into a tree cavity to incubate their chicks. Hornbills have been shown to be capable problem-solvers [4] and are also sensitive to both visitors and keepers when housed in zoos [5]. With a diverse range of hornbill species held in captivity, each representing different habitats, dietary niches and breeding strategies, there is a need to further investigate enrichment for this group of species.
The southern ground hornbill (Bucorvus leadbeateri) is one of the two largest extant hornbill species, reaching weights of up to 4 kg [6]. In recent years, the species has received some conservation attention in the wild, on account of decreasing population numbers [7,8,9,10,11]. In many parts of its historic range, the species has been persecuted because it is viewed as a bad omen [12]. The southern ground hornbill has a cooperative breeding strategy and a slow reproductive rate, leaving it vulnerable to extinction. As a result, zoo populations for this species are important, potentially providing a ‘safety net’. A July 2021 search of Species360’s [13] database revealed that at least 151 institutions globally keep this species, with an overall population size of over 390 birds. While these numbers are not excessively high in comparison to other zoo-housed birds [14,15,16,17], there is a need to further develop enrichment strategies for this species.
In the wild, southern ground hornbills typically forage on the ground, walking across the savannah [10]. The birds are entirely carnivorous, and feed on a range of foods including carrion, and invertebrates, reptiles, birds, and small mammals that are captured and killed [6]. The birds are particularly intelligent and social communication is advanced for this species, with small groups or ‘mobs’ developing, that work cooperatively to support female birds during incubation.
The purpose of this study was twofold. First, we aimed to develop an activity budget for zoo-housed southern ground hornbills. Second, we aimed to identify which enrichment types were most effective in encouraging exploratory behavior for the pair of birds.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Site and Subjects

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour and approved by the ethics committee of Beale Wildlife Park (A17, 19 January 2014). Following ethical approval from Beale Wildlife Park, the study commenced at Beale Wildlife Park and Gardens in Reading, United Kingdom. Three periods of data collection were undertaken: these were from 27 March 2014 to 30 June 2014, 15 January 2018 to 16 July 2018, and 1 November 2019 to 23 December 2019. Animals were observed during three observation periods: these were 08:00–10:00, 10:30–12:30, 13:00–15:00, and 15:30–17:30. Birds were observed based on the availability of the authors.
The study focused on two (1.1) parent-reared southern ground hornbills who were kept in a large single-species aviary in the ‘Owl Walk’ (Table 1, Figure 1). The exhibit consisted of several elevated perches, and one large barrel (for breeding purposes). The exhibit substrate was a mixture of leaf litter and soil. The birds were not flight restrained, but the exhibit was covered with nylon mesh to prevent escapes.

2.2. Enrichment Types

Enrichment was provided on a randomized schedule. Three enrichment types were provided for the animals; the enrichment types were inspired by the natural history documentation for the species. The first enrichment type consisted of a large pile of twigs and branches, into which several morio worms (Zophobas morio) were presented; this enrichment style was developed to encourage hornbills to forage using their talons and beak. The second enrichment type consisted of an entire rabbit carcass, as the species regularly feeds on large carcasses, and can hunt large prey in the wild [6,18,19]. The final enrichment style consisted of two large, non-shatter mirrors. Mirrors were used because they are frequently applied in bird husbandry to mimic the presence of conspecifics. There is also some evidence to suggest that wild hornbills interact with reflective surfaces such as on water, or in windows, in their native range.
The behavior of the birds was also observed during ‘control’ periods, when food was not present, and during normal feeding hours. Control periods were matched for time of day to the experimental treatments. The normal feed for the hornbills was provided at 16:00 and consisted of either day-old chicks or chunks of rabbit or quail, chopped into small pieces. When enrichment feeds were provided, they were deducted from the normal dietary provision.

2.3. Behavioral Recording

Behavior was recorded using instantaneous focal sampling of both birds simultaneously. One-hour observations were conducted, with the observer recording state behaviors at one-minute intervals. An ethogram was developed, containing behaviors which were adapted from Kemp & Kemp [6] (Table 2).
During observations, the observers partially concealed themselves behind a large oak tree at the front of the exhibit. Data were compiled onto paper observation sheets. In addition to behavioral data, observers also recorded the temperature and humidity (using BBC weather information for Pangbourne), weather conditions (e.g., rain, cloud), and the number of visitors that walked past the exhibit during the hour observation period.

2.4. Enclosure Use

In addition to hornbill behavior, the enclosure use of the two birds was recorded. The location of each bird was recorded using instantaneous focal sampling at one-minute intervals. The enclosure was separated into seven different zones, based on their biological value to the animals. The size of each useable surface per zone was measured using a tape measure (Table 3).
Overall hornbill enclosure use was assessed for each bird using a modified spread of participation index (mSPI) [15]. The equation for mSPI is:
Σ fo fe 2 N femin
Here, N refers to the number of observations; fo and fe refer to the number of observed and expected observations in a given zone, respectively; femin refers to the expected observation in the smallest zone [15]. For mSPI, the maximum value of 1 indicates uneven enclosure use (the animal is using only the smallest zone and avoiding all other zones) whereas the minimum value of 0 indicates that animals are using all zones equally (in proportion to their size) [15].

2.5. Data Analysis

Data were compiled onto a Microsoft Excel™ 2010, Albuquerque, USA spreadsheet and then uploaded to Minitab version 21 for analysis. Analysis was conducted on the effect of the three enrichment types and the control condition on hornbill behavior. For analysis, behavioral data were tested for normality. Where data were normally distributed, one-way ANOVAs with Tukey post hoc tests were used to investigate the impact of enrichment. For non-normally distributed data, Friedman’s ANOVAs with pairwise Wilcoxon tests were used [20].

3. Results

3.1. Behavior

A comparative activity budget was developed to demonstrate the effects of enrichment for the male and female hornbills (Figure 2). As data were non-parametric, Friedman’s ANOVAs were run to determine the effect of enrichment on behavior (Table 4). Two behaviors, enrichment interaction and feeding, were significantly affected by enrichment type.

3.2. Enclosure Use

mSPI values were generated for all observations. A bar chart was developed to demonstrate the effect of enrichment on the mSPI values for the male and female hornbill (Figure 3). Whilst average mSPI scores differed slightly between enrichment types, the difference was not significant (X2(3) = 6.06, p = 0.195).

4. Discussion

Overall, the introduction of enrichment into the hornbill enclosures resulted in significant changes in feeding and enrichment interaction. Carcass provision resulted in hornbills spending much longer periods of time engaged in feeding and food manipulation, and mirrors were highly effective at engaging hornbills. No other significant changes in behavior occurred. While hornbills did appear to use their enclosure more evenly when enrichment was provided, this was not significant.

4.1. Carcass Enrichment

Significant differences in levels of food manipulation were noted when carcass enrichment was provided. Hornbills engaged in movements including stabbing and shaking of the carcass in order to remove pieces of meat. In the normal feed, typically consisting of chopped meat or day-old chicks, little food manipulation was observed. In the wild, southern ground hornbills may feed on large carcasses, and are also known to hunt animals such as hares and medium-sized snakes [6,9,20]. Occasional carcass feeds may therefore allow the birds to express a greater range of feeding-related behaviors. This could be used in tandem with small food items such as live invertebrates, which could be used to simulate hunting. Providing birds with the opportunity to express more natural behaviors is part of the five welfare domains [16].
Whilst some visitors may support this more natural feeding experience, there may also be a negative response from the public to these feeding techniques [17]. Carcass feeds may sometimes be met with disapproval from key visitor demographics, such as families with small children [17]. Whilst there is an educational value to the provision of carcass feeds, visitors may need to be made aware that carcass feeding is taking place.
Feeding whole foods could also reduce aggression between subjects [18] provided all animals are given access to food items simultaneously. In the current study, no aggression was observed between individuals, though this may pose a challenge if hornbills are kept in groups that simulate their wild social grouping [6,7,20]. Providing whole carcasses can save keepers food preparation time, but larger diet items may require significantly more storage, which could be more difficult for smaller collections. Consideration should also be given to exhibit cleaning once a large food item has been offered.

4.2. Mirror Enrichment

It is sometimes challenging to find non-food-related enrichment types for zoo-housed animals. Food-related enrichment may have drawbacks in that it must be deducted from the animal’s normal rations [21]. Non-food-related enrichment, by contrast, can be used for long periods of time without reducing an animal’s appetite or resulting in an imbalanced diet.
Mirrors are a common strategy employed by bird keepers for use as enrichment. The hornbill pair spent significantly longer interacting with mirrors than with any other enrichment type. This significant increase in interactions with mirrors could be considered beneficial, as levels of resting decreased while activity levels increased. The two birds in the study were typically inactive during visitor open hours, so an increase in activity may have a positive impact on physical fitness.
In the wild, hornbills have been noted to interact with reflective objects such as mirrors and windows, and even parts of cars [10]. The hornbill has been persecuted as a result of this behavior. The underlying purpose of the behavior is still not fully understood; the behavior may be related to curiosity or interaction with another hornbill [10]. Whilst the hornbills could in fact be aware that their reflection is harmless, this behavior could also be based around territorial displays, with the hornbills assuming they have another hornbill to defend against or compete with. This could indicate that the birds consider the mirror reflection to be a rival. While this condition could therefore be considered stressful to the birds, it does allow the birds to demonstrate natural behavior and potentially could improve pair bonding. Therefore, mirrors could play a similar role to the playback calls used in zoos for primate species, such as gibbons [21].

4.3. Enclosure Use

There was no significant difference in enclosure use for the southern ground hornbills as a result of enrichment type. Whilst the mSPI scores appeared lower for the enrichment and carcass feeds, this was not significant. This may be due to variability in mSPI scores as a result of other extraneous variables. For example, it is possible that visitor presence influenced the enclosure use of the birds. Other zoo-housed hornbills have been shown to respond to visitor presence [5]. Anecdotally, the southern ground hornbills appeared to favor elevated perches during time periods when visitor numbers were higher. Future studies could consider visitor presence and its influence on behavior.
Enrichment items encouraged the hornbills to use more of the ground substrate, walking around the exhibit. In the wild, southern ground hornbills spend much of their time walking, rather than flying, around grassland and savannah in search of prey [6,11,22,23]. The use of a greater range of zones, rather than primarily the elevated perches, could be beneficial in terms of physical movement for these birds.

4.4. Future Directions

Generally, birds seem to be a neglected taxa for enrichment, despite their prevalence in zoological collections [14]. Finding any objects that have significant impacts on activity is positive for keepers. Many enrichment items create animal interaction, but not for significant periods of time. Hornbills were observed interacting with mirrors for over 40 min, which is an extended period of activity for the animals. The public perception of birds in captivity can often be more negative than other taxa, as many captive birds lack the large amounts of space the public, with little knowledge of husbandry guidelines, believe they need for optimum health. Enrichment can improve public perception of welfare, especially considering birds are viewed much less emotively than other taxa, such as primates.
Future studies should consider use of tests that reduce issues with seudoreplication, such as G-tests. These studies could also utilize the extensive historical records of natural history, in order to identify novel enrichment practices. For example, early records of sightings of animals in their natural habitats, or interactions with other species, may help practitioners to identify novel husbandry practices to trial. In turn, the use of natural history documents may help zoos to provide more informed, evidence-based management for the animals that they keep.

5. Conclusions

Overall, provision of enrichment influenced some, but not all aspects of captive hornbill behavior. Interaction with enrichment varied between items, with the twigs pile receiving little attention and the carcasses resulting in considerable feeding activity. Mirrors were very well utilized by the birds, linking to the behavior of wild southern ground hornbills and their interest in reflective windows. Information on natural history may be useful in developing novel enrichment devices, especially enrichment types that do not involve food. Further inspiration for enrichment practices may be found in natural history books or papers that could, with controlled testing, be used to advance the state of current enrichment practice.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.E.B.; methodology, J.E.B. and M.N.G.M.; software, J.E.B.; validation, J.A.S., J.E.B. and M.N.G.M.; formal analysis, J.E.B.; investigation, J.A.S. and J.E.B.; data curation, J.A.S.; writing—original draft preparation, J.A.S., J.E.B. and M.N.G.M.; writing—review and editing, J.A.S., J.E.B. and M.N.G.M.; visualization, J.A.S., J.E.B. and M.N.G.M.; supervision, J.E.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee of Beale Wildlife Park (A17, 19 January 2014).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy of zoo records.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank staff at Beale Wildlife Park for their support during the project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Swaisgood, R.R.; Shepherdson, D.J. Scientific approaches to enrichment and stereotypies in zoo animals: What’s been done and where should we go next? Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 499–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Dos Santos, J.W.; Correia, R.A.; Malhado, A.C.; Campos-Silva, J.V.; Teles, D.; Jepson, P.; Ladle, R.J. Drivers of taxonomic bias in conservation research: A global analysis of terrestrial mammals. Anim. Conserv. 2020, 23, 679–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Reimer, J.; Maia, C.M.; Santos, E.F. Environmental enrichments for a group of captive macaws: Low interaction does not mean low behavioral changes. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2016, 19, 385–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Danel, S.; von Bayern, A.M.; Osiurak, F. Ground-hornbills (Bucorvus) show means-end understanding in a horizontal two-string discrimination task. J. Ethol. 2019, 37, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rose, P.E.; Scales, J.S.; Brereton, J.E. Why the “visitor effect” is complicated. unraveling individual animal, visitor number, and climatic influences on behavior, space use and interactions with keepers—A case study on captive hornbills. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kemp, A.C.; Kemp, M.I. The biology of the southern ground hornbill Bucorvus leadbeateri (Vigors)(Aves: Bucerotidae). Ann. Transvaal Mus. 1980, 32, 65–100. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/10520/AJA00411752_1057 (accessed on 3 September 2021).
  7. Combrink, L.; Combrink, H.J.; Botha, A.J.; Downs, C.T. Habitat preferences of Southern Ground-hornbills in the Kruger National Park: Implications for future conservation measures. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Theron, N.; Grobler, P.; Kotze, A.; Jansen, R. The home range of a recently established group of Southern ground-hornbill (Bucorvus leadbeateri) in the Limpopo Valley, South Africa. Koedoe Afr. Prot. Area Conserv. Sci. 2013, 55, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Gula, J.; Phiri, C.G. Observations of Southern Ground-hornbill Bucorvus leadbeateri groups in the Kafue National Park, Zambia. Ostrich 2020, 91, 267–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Msimanga, A. Breeding biology of Southern Ground Hornbill Bucorvus leadbeateri in Zimbabwe: Impacts of human activities. Bird Conserv. Int. 2007, 14, 63–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Engelbrecht, D.; Theron, N.; Turner, A.; Van Wyk, J.; Pienaar, K. The status and conservation of Southern Ground Hornbills, Bucorvus leadbeateri, in the Limpopo Province, South Africa. Act. Manag. Hornbills Habitats Conserv. 2007, 1, 252–266. [Google Scholar]
  12. Coetzee, H.; Nell, W.; van Rensburg, L. An exploration of cultural beliefs and practices across the Southern Ground-Hornbill’s range in Africa. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 2014, 10, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Species360. Southern Ground Horn Bill. 2021. Available online: https://zims.species360.org/ (accessed on 26 July 2021).
  14. Brereton, S.R.; Brereton, J.E. Sixty years of collection planning: What species do zoos and aquariums keep? Int. Zoo Yearb. 2020, 54, 131–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Brereton, J.E. Directions in animal enclosure use studies. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2020, 8, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  16. Mellor, D.J. Operational details of the five domains model and its key applications to the assessment and management of animal welfare. Animals 2017, 7, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Gaengler, H.; Clum, N. Investigating the impact of large carcass feeding on the behavior of captive Andean condors (Vultur gryphus) and its perception by zoo visitors. Zoo Biol. 2015, 34, 118–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Shora, J.A.; Myhill, M.G.N.; Brereton, J.E. Should zoo foods be coati chopped? J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2018, 6, 22–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Brereton, J.E. Challenges and directions in zoo and aquarium food presentation research: A review. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2020, 1, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Plowman, A.B. BIAZA statistics guidelines: Toward a common application of statistical tests for zoo research. Zoo Biol. 2008, 27, 226–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Shepherdson, D.; Bemment, N.; Carman, M.; Reynolds, S. Auditory enrichment for Lar gibbons Hylobates lar at London Zoo. Int. Zoo Yearb. 1989, 28, 256–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kemp, L.V.; Kotze, A.; Jansen, R.; Dalton, D.L.; Grobler, P.; Little, R.M. Review of trial reintroductions of the long-lived, cooperative breeding Southern Ground-hornbill. Bird Conserv. Int. 2020, 30, 533–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Galama, W.; King, C.; Brouwer, K. EAZA Hornbill Management and Husbandry Guidelines; EAZA: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2002. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Hornbill enclosure, with male hornbill resting on log.
Figure 1. Hornbill enclosure, with male hornbill resting on log.
Jzbg 02 00043 g001
Figure 2. Activity budget for (a) female and (b) male hornbill.
Figure 2. Activity budget for (a) female and (b) male hornbill.
Jzbg 02 00043 g002
Figure 3. mSPI values for different enrichment types.
Figure 3. mSPI values for different enrichment types.
Jzbg 02 00043 g003
Table 1. Study subjects.
Table 1. Study subjects.
SexDate of BirthStudbook NumberGANMovement into Collection
Male20 May 2000EAZA/73MIG12-2877216512 November 2010
Female11 May 2001EAZA/74MIG12-2877216412 November 2010
Table 2. Hornbill ethogram. Inspired by Kemp & Kemp [6].
Table 2. Hornbill ethogram. Inspired by Kemp & Kemp [6].
BehaviorDescription
AllopreeningThe bird engages in preening of a conspecific.
Enrichment interactionThe bird uses its beak or feet to poke at or scratch an enrichment item.
FeedingThe bird takes food items from the exhibit and swallows them.
FlyingThe bird lifts off the ground by raising and lowering its wings rapidly.
Object in beakThe bird is standing or walking with an item (e.g., food or nest material) in its beak.
PreeningThe bird wipes its bill across its feathers in a repeated fashion.
RestingThe bird is motionless. The eyes may be either open or closed. Includes both standing and perching.
SunbathingThe bird extends its wings in a fan and angles them toward the sun. The bird remains motionless.
WalkingThe bird moves around the exhibit using its feet.
Table 3. Enclosure zones for the Southern ground hornbills.
Table 3. Enclosure zones for the Southern ground hornbills.
ZoneDescriptionSize (m2)
Elevated perches (left)Large logs for perching, 2–2.5 m from ground.12.5
Elevated perches (right)Logs for perching, roughly 1.8–2.4 m from ground.14.1
Central logLong tree trunk, extending between left and right elevated perches, 0.8–1.5 m from ground.15.6
WaterSmall water pool and surrounding concrete.3.5
BarrelBarrel used by female during nesting.2.1
Tree stumpLarge tree stump turned upside down, with roots available for perching.7.8
GroundSubstrate of enclosure, consisting of soil and leaf litter.151.2
Total 206.8
Table 4. Output of Friedman’s ANOVAs on the effect of enrichment on hornbill behavior.
Table 4. Output of Friedman’s ANOVAs on the effect of enrichment on hornbill behavior.
BehaviourTest StatisticpSignificant Post Hoc Tests
AllopreeningX2(3) = 2.310.412
Enrichment interactionX2(3) = 99.62<0.001 *Mirror-None, Mirror-Carcass, Mirror-Twigs
FeedingX2(3) = 75.16<0.001 *Carcass-Mirror, Carcass-Twigs, Carcass-None
FlyingX2(3) = 6.260.096
Object in beakX2(3) = 0.680.718
PreeningX2(3) = 6.990.099
RestingX2(3) = 22.140.127
SunbathingX2(3) = 4.160.180
WalkingX2(3) = 11.410.416
* indicates significant values.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brereton, J.E.; Myhill, M.N.G.; Shora, J.A. Investigating the Effect of Enrichment on the Behavior of Zoo-Housed Southern Ground Hornbills. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2, 600-609. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040043

AMA Style

Brereton JE, Myhill MNG, Shora JA. Investigating the Effect of Enrichment on the Behavior of Zoo-Housed Southern Ground Hornbills. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2021; 2(4):600-609. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040043

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brereton, James Edward, Mark Nigel Geoffrey Myhill, and James Ali Shora. 2021. "Investigating the Effect of Enrichment on the Behavior of Zoo-Housed Southern Ground Hornbills" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 2, no. 4: 600-609. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040043

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop