Next Article in Journal
Associations between Sedentary Time and Sedentary Patterns and Cardiorespiratory Fitness in Chinese Children and Adolescents
Next Article in Special Issue
Evaluating in the Real-World Educational Intervention to Improve Interference Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Evaluation of a Program of Aquatic Motor Games in the Improvement of Motor Competence in Children from 4 to 5 Years Old
Previous Article in Special Issue
Social Humanoid Robots for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Review of Modalities, Indications, and Pitfalls
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Early Neuro-Psychomotor Therapy Intervention for Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Pilot Study

Department of Human Neurosciences, Sapienza University of Roma, 00185 Rome, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(8), 1142; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081142
Submission received: 10 July 2022 / Revised: 26 July 2022 / Accepted: 27 July 2022 / Published: 29 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Autism and Children)

Abstract

:
The aim of the present study is to explore the effect of early neuro-psychomotor therapy to improve theory of mind skills and emotion recognition in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. A pilot study was set up, consisting of in-group training activities based on the neuro-psychomotor approach. Children were evaluated using Neuropsychological Assessment for Child (Nepsy-II), Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC), and Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). For data analysis, one-sample Wilcoxon signed rank test was used with a significance of p < 0.05. Two children with a developmental language disorder and four children with autism spectrum disorders participated in a 3-month training program. Our findings revealed significant improvement in emotion recognition, as measured with Nepsy-II (p = 0.04), while no statistical improvement was found for theory of mind. Despite the limited sample, early neuro-psychomotor therapy improves emotion recognition skills in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. However, considering the explorative nature of the study, findings should be interpreted with caution.

1. Introduction

Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the ability to know and attribute mental states (perceptions, emotions, and thoughts) in oneself and others. It ranges, with a development across time, from understanding simple desires and emotions to more complex mental states [1,2]. As data suggest, children at 2 years of age already manifest reasoning about desire, before the ability to reason about cognition, referring in particular to false beliefs [3]. Moreover, ToM is strongly linked to the concept of empathy, which can be divided into two domains: the social cognitive understanding of other people’s mental states (i.e., theory of mind) and empathic responsiveness, better explained as behavioral aspects of the individuals [4,5,6,7]. Evidence demonstrates that delays of development of ToM skills have been seen in deaf and hard of hearing children born to hearing parents, suggesting that environmental factors, such as language and verbal interaction, can influence its development [8]. Observational studies show that socioeconomic status [9], family size, and number of siblings [10] can have an impact on ToM. A meta-analysis of controlled studies analyzed the effectiveness of ToM training among children with an average age of 63 months [11], highlighting how early training has a positive effect on ToM skills in children. The length of the training period significantly influenced the overall effect size, while the number of sessions and gender moderate it only marginally. Common interventions among the different studies were based on training procedures, including corrective feedback, use of imagination, modeling, and role-play, all of which are similar to ToM tasks. In some studies, ToM training was considered as a part of a broader project aiming to develop social skills. Several studies, regarding individual differences of ToM acquisition, demonstrated that among typically developing children, the use of language is an important instrument in order to draw their attention to mental states [12,13,14]. Children’s language ability, from the perspective that it is needed for everyday conversation [15], and their involvement in pretend play (in which they need to assume the other’s perspective) [16] have been associated with ToM abilities in previous studies.
Facial recognition impacts social functioning, peer relationships, and behavior, and is the ability to determine facial emotions or differentiate familiar and unfamiliar faces [17]. This is important for successful interaction with others and to actively participate in the social environment and is also vital for the development of the perceptual components of ToM [18]. There are six universally recognized basic expressions of facial affect; happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, and disgust [19]. The standardized facial emotional expression set [20] developed by Ekman and coworkers has identified the discrete emotions of happiness, surprise, anger, fear, disgust, and sadness. Contrasting discrete emotional states (happiness vs. sadness, amusement vs. disgust, calmness vs. excitement, calmness vs. anger, and fear vs. anger) revealed high association between emotional arousal and neuro-functional brain connectivity measures [21]. Neural networks underlying this ability involve a set of structures that includes the visual cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, the insula, and the basal ganglia, but mainly the mesial temporal structures, with an important role played by the amygdala [22]. The recognition of others’ emotions and mental states relies on the integration of emotional cues from various channels, including facial expressions, vocal intonation, and body language, as well as contextual information [23]. However, there are differences in the recognition of basic and complex emotions between children with neurodevelopmental disorders and typically developing children [24,25]. Complex mechanisms underlying emotion recognition and ToM should be addressed by specific rehabilitation interventions.
Neuro-psychomotor therapy is a unique rehabilitation approach in Italy and worldwide. It has been referred to as psychomotricity in other European countries and it is also known as play therapy [26]. In the United States, a similar treatment may be represented in developmental therapy [27]. This approach focuses on the development of how children process information through movement during play sessions. Specifically, it proposes the following objectives: to favor the appearance of social signalers (eye contact, reference looks, smiles, etc.), to increase attention time, to facilitate a more appropriate use of objects, to stimulate communication, to enrich vocabulary, and to discourage certain behaviors (hyperactivity, motor stereotypes, self-injurious behaviors, etc.). Furthermore, psychomotricity may be considered a safe and effective therapy for neurodevelopmental disorders [28,29].
The objective of the present pilot study is to evaluate the efficacy of early neuro-psychomotor therapy intervention for the development of ToM skills and emotion recognition in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities.

2. Materials and Methods

The present study was conducted within the Child Neuropsychiatry Unit of the Department of Human Neurosciences of Sapienza University of Rome. This work was carried out on the basis of previous studies conducted by the authors who have great experience in child neuropsychiatry diagnoses and rehabilitation [30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38].

2.1. Sampling

A convenience sample was selected from the Child Neuropsychiatry Unit of the Department of Human Neurosciences of Sapienza University of Rome. Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) were enrolled in the study. Compared with the general language profile of DLD, where difficulties are traditionally thought to center on structural aspects of language, in ASD, language difficulties are wider, affecting areas of pragmatics and social interaction [39]. Although the diagnostic criteria make a clear distinction between DLD and ASD (they exclude each other), co-occurrence and overlap of ASD and DLD in social, communication, and language development are discussed in many studies [40,41,42,43]. To be included in the study, children had to have the following characteristics: aged between 42 and 72 months and a diagnosis of DLD and ASD. Children with cognitive impairment (IQ < 70) and/or sensory disorders were excluded. To evaluate cognitive abilities and global functioning, different tools were used: Griffiths III [44] (Griffiths Mental Development Scales—Extended Revised [45]) and Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) III [46].

2.2. Procedures, Measures, and Data Analysis

Children were tested individually by clinicians for approximately 30–45 min in two different sessions. For measuring theory of mind tasks, emotional behavior, and social communication, the following instruments were used:
Neuropsychological Assessment for Child. (Nepsy-II) [47] is a comprehensive instrument designed to assess neuropsychological development in preschool and school-age children. This tool is composed of three different sessions: S01, made of 15 short stories to analyze first- and second-order beliefs, S01.1, with 8 questions in which the child must guess the character’s facial expression based on the situations depicted in the image, and S02 (16 questions for children aged 3–4, 35 for older children), in which the aim is to couple similar emotions among the pictures represented. This test can be used in children from 3 to 16 years old. NEPSY-II asks children to recognize different facial expressions and to interpret other’s beliefs and intentions and how these can affect their behaviors.
Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC) [48] is an emotional-perspective taking test. It assesses the understanding of emotions in children aged 3–11 years and includes nine components: recognition, external cause, desire, belief, reminder, regulation, hiding, mixed, and morality. It consists of an A4 picture book (versions for boys and girls) presenting a series of cartoon scenarios at the top of each page; the bottom of the same page shows four possible emotional outcomes depicted by facial expressions. The child is read a short story while looking at the cartoon scenario, then the child is asked to point to the appropriate facial expression (the answer is typically nonverbal). The total score is determined by assigning 1 point for each component answered correctly.
Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [49]. The SCQ is a brief, 40-item, parent-report screening measure that focuses on items relating to ASD symptomatology likely to be observed by a primary caregiver. Although the SCQ is a screening tool—and, thus, cannot be used for diagnosis of ASD—it is based on the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADIR) [17], a semi-structured parent interview conducted by a trained clinician or researcher that can be used for diagnostic evaluation of children with suspected ASD. Each item in the SCQ requires a dichotomous “yes”/“no” response, and each scored item receives a value of 1 point for abnormal behavior and 0 points for absence of abnormal behavior/normal behavior.
All these measures were used for evaluation. Children repeated the same tests at the end of the intervention, three months later, while parents were given a similar set of questions from the SCQ [31], focused only on the last three months. For data analysis, one-sample Wilcoxon signed rank test was used with a significance of p < 0.05.

2.3. Neuro-Psychomotor Therapy Protocol

This study involved residents MD in child neurology, supervisors that specialized in Child Neurology and Psychiatry and neuro-psychomotor therapists. The sessions consisted of two hours twice a week, divided as follows: 30 min intervention focused on ToM or emotion understanding, and 1.30 h intervention focused on social interaction, language skills, and autonomy.
In the intervention, among the different activities, software for playing activity, “Autismo e competenze cognitivo-emotive”, was utilized, which focuses on potentiating cognitive and emotional competences in children with difficulties in social relations and emotions. It represents a house with different activities for each room: for example, in the kitchen, the subject was asked to recognize a child’s emotion when he received or did not receive the object of desire; in the bedroom, the activity was focused on second-order beliefs; in the attic, the subject was asked to recognize emotions through the eyes.
Half of the sessions were focused on understanding emotions through the use of drawings, pretend play, and short movies chosen to represent happiness, anger, sadness, fear, and disgust. Toward the end of the training, when the children had already mastered these basic discrete emotions, they were presented with more complex ones, such as shame, hope, disappointment, and avarice. The remaining sessions focused on theory of mind, using pretend play and jokes, favoring the understanding of first- and second-order beliefs.
In Table 1, the different types of interventions focused on either emotions or theory of mind are represented. Since the meetings were biweekly, both types of interventions were performed alternately every week. Children’s responses and behaviors were assessed after two months.

3. Results

A total of 6 children with a mean (SD) age of 4.22 (0.79) participated in this study. Half of the children were female. Two of the six children had an immigrant background and presented a developmental language disorder (DLD), while others had ASD. Main characteristics for each child are presented in Table 2.
Children attended an average of 14.2 sessions out of 17 in three months of training. After treatment, the total scores of the NEPSY, TEC, and SCQ were compared. NEPSY S02 section showed general improvement that reached statistical significance (Table 3), while, for the other outcomes, we did not find statistical differences.

4. Discussion

The aim of our study was to train preschool children with the assumption that early intervention could improve socialization skills and reduce behavioral problems, thus improving the quality of their relationships and the quality of life [35]. The current study focused on improving conceptual understanding of ToM and emotions in children with ASD or DLD. The reasoning behind this group selection was so that children with both diagnoses could benefit from the interaction with each other; in particular, we believed that DLD children could assist in the process of emotions recognition in ASD children, while the language abilities of the latter could enrich the vocabulary of the former.
Regarding the recognition of emotions, in the early phase of training, children were asked to watch a cartoon representing basic discrete emotions and draw the main characters afterwards. They could represent sad, angry, and happy faces. The adoption of role-playing, psychomotor group activities, and the use of a PC game resulted in a modest improvement in emotion recognition and understanding, although this is not consistently observed in all outcome measures. The efficacy of psychomotricity in emotional behavior is reported in De Lourdes Crò study [36], with positive effects in resilience capacity, dealing with problems, and controlling emotions. In addition, the use of computer-assisted training showed promising results; for instance, Rice LM and colleagues [37] demonstrated that children who received computer-assisted training improved their affect recognition and mentalizing skills, as well as their social skills. Our results showed a statistically significant improvement in the NEPSY S02, which measures emotion recognition ability. In this regard, a recent systematic review [38] confirms that the effects of emotion recognition training in ASD showed significant improvement compared to the control group. Measures designed to capture emotion recognition skills and changes in those skills generally assessed the ability to recognize emotions from facial expression, voices, or postures. These studies used a variety of outcome measures intended to evaluate emotion recognition, including the affect recognition subtest of the NEPSY-II, the Face Task, and photographs from Ekman’s Pictures of Facial Affect [38]. However ToM skills did not improve, and this can be explained by different etiologies. A study conducted on ToM training with children aged 8–13 showed, as main benefits of this intervention, improvements in empathic behavior and social understanding concepts that are explicitly addressed in the training, with little generalization to other aspects of social behavior [7]. Fisher and Happe confirmed that ASD trained children compared to a control group did not show a significant improvement in daily life theory of mind use [50]. Another RCT instead showed the effect of a computer program for training emotion recognition. Results suggested that cognitive abilities improved compared to a control group on emotion recognition in cartoons and second-order ToM reasoning, but not on their recognition of facial emotional expressions [51]. However, it is important to consider that ToM development is strongly affected by non-heritable and social/environmental factors [52,53].
Hence, TOM training could have an important role in the improvement of this ability, but perhaps there should be a more lasting, stable intervention that includes individual sessions and psychoeducative training for parents, teachers, and the whole social environment of the child. It is important to point out that children with ASD, DLD, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) show very similar emotion recognition skills and are significantly delayed in their development of these skills [39]. In this regard, evidence suggests that ASD, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and ADHD are classically associated with poor face-processing skills and difficulties in understanding emotions. A recent study, which included these three clinical pediatric groups, compared with typically developing controls, was conducted by using dynamic faces (happy, angry) and dynamic flowers, presented in 18 pseudo-randomized blocks while fMRI data were collected with a 3T MRI. Children with ASD, ADHD, and OCD differentiated less between dynamic faces and dynamic flowers, with most of the effects seen in the occipital and temporal regions, suggesting that emotional difficulties shared in NDDs may be partly attributed to shared atypical visual information processing [54]. Another similar study conducted by using a morphing technique demonstrated a faster response time in neurotypical children compared to ASD and ADHD children, with ADHD participants performing better than ASD participants on the same task [55]. These studies have shown that there is an impairment of emotion recognition in neurodevelopmental disorders, leading to our decision to work on this skill in our neuro-psychomotor training. We choose to use tools such as videos, dynamic faces, drawings, cards, and software because they are easily usable and accessible even for non-verbal children, are easy to provide and comprehend, and are useful in detecting the real abilities of the child in a direct way. Our study provides information about impaired emotion recognition, how to improve this skill in ASD and DLD, and consequently, how to select intervention procedures. At the end of the three-month training, all children could easily understand first-order false beliefs, they could conceptually recognize basic discrete emotions (joy, anger, disgust, sadness, and fear), and, with some guidance, they could recognize more complex ones (shame, hope, disappointment, and greed). The quality of role-play improved in all children, as it was enriched by more details, imaginary objects, and more complex story lines.
Despite the promising results, our study has some limitations. First, the very limited sample size does not allow generalization of findings. Furthermore, different diagnoses can affect the results for ToM skills. However, the explorative nature of the research lays the foundation to set up a more structured study with a larger sample. Furthermore, we did not explore gender differences among children, while heterogeneity of behavior has been documented [18]. In the end, it could be useful to evaluate parents’ and/or teachers’ perspective on children’s behavior. Further studies with a larger population of children should consider these aspects. The present protocol and neuro-psychomotor approach might also be applied in different health conditions, such as children with intellectual disability and ADHD. Further studies should investigate the possibility of applying the present intervention protocol in different neurodevelopmental disorders, involving parents, and promoting a family-centered approach.

5. Conclusions

Early neuro-psychomotor therapy is a promising approach for preschool children with neurodevelopmental disorders. The improvement in socialization skills and the reduction of behavioral problems could ameliorate the quality of the relationship and the quality of life.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.S., E.G. and M.C.V.; methodology, C.S., M.T. and G.G.; investigation, F.A., N.G., F.G. and M.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.T., E.G. and M.C.V.; writing—review and editing, D.V., C.S., N.G. and M.M.; supervision, C.S. and F.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patients to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Flavell, J.H. Theory-of-Mind Development: Retrospect and Prospect. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2004, 50, 274–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Wellman, H.M. Making Minds: How Theory of Mind Develops; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014; ISBN 9780199334919. [Google Scholar]
  3. Wellman, H.M.; Cross, D.; Watson, J. Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth about False Belief. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 655–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lawrence, E.J.; Shaw, P.; Baker, D.; Baron-Cohen, S.; David, A.S. Measuring Empathy: Reliability and Validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychol. Med. 2004, 34, 911–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. de Waal, F.B.M. Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008, 59, 279–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. Theory of Mind and Autism: A Review. Int. Rev. Res. Ment. Retard. 2000, 23, 169–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Holopainen, A.; de Veld, D.M.J.; Hoddenbach, E.; Begeer, S. Does Theory of Mind Training Enhance Empathy in Autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 3965–3972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Stanzione, C.; Schick, B. Environmental Language Factors in Theory of Mind Development Evidence From Children Who Are Deaf/Hard-of-Hearing or Who Have Specific Language Impairment. Top. Lang. Disord. 2014, 34, 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Cutting, A.L.; Dunn, J. Theory of Mind, Emotion Understanding, Language, and Family Background: Individual Differences and Interrelations. Child Dev. 1999, 70, 853–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. McAlister, A.; Peterson, C. A Longitudinal Study of Child Siblings and Theory of Mind Development. Cogn. Dev. 2007, 22, 258–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hofmann, S.G.; Doan, S.N.; Sprung, M.; Wilson, A.; Ebesutani, C.; Andrews, L.A.; Curtiss, J.; Harris, P.L. Training Children’s Theory-of-Mind: A Meta-Analysis of Controlled Studies. Cognition 2016, 150, 200–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Lillard, A.S.; Kavanaugh, R.D. The Contribution of Symbolic Skills to the Development of an Explicit Theory of Mind. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 1535–1551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hughes, C.; Jaffee, S.R.; Happé, F.; Taylor, A.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E. Origins of Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: From Nature to Nurture? Child Dev. 2005, 76, 1278–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Harris, P.L.; de Rosnay, M.; Pons, F. Language and Children’s Understanding of Mental States. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 14, 69–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Deleau, M. Language and Theory of Mind: Why Pragmatics Matter. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 9, 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Taylor, M.; Carlson, S.M.; Mating, B.L.; Gerow, L.; Charley, C.M. The Characteristics and Correlates of Fantasy in School-Age Children: Imaginary Companions, Impersonation, and Social Understanding. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 40, 1173–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Collin, L.; Bindra, J.; Raju, M.; Gillberg, C.; Minnis, H. Facial Emotion Recognition in Child Psychiatry: A Systematic Review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 1505–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Korkmaz, B. Theory of Mind and Neurodevelopmental Disorders of Childhood. Pediatr. Res. 2011, 69, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Thayer, J.; Johnsen, B.H. Sex Differences in Judgement of Facial Affect: A Multivariate Analysis of Recognition Errors. Scand. J. Psychol. 2000, 41, 243–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dodich, A.; Cerami, C.; Canessa, N.; Crespi, C.; Marcone, A.; Arpone, M.; Realmuto, S.; Cappa, S.F. Emotion Recognition from Facial Expressions: A Normative Study of the Ekman 60-Faces Test in the Italian Population. Neurol. Sci. 2014, 35, 1015–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kılıç, B.; Aydın, S. Classification of Contrasting Discrete Emotional States Indicated by EEG Based Graph Theoretical Network Measures. Neuroinformatics 2021, 1, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Nelson, E.E.; McClure, E.B.; Monk, C.S.; Zarahn, E.; Leibenluft, E.; Pine, D.S.; Ernst, M. Developmental Differences in Neuronal Engagement during Implicit Encoding of Emotional Faces: An Event-Related FMRI Study. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2003, 44, 1015–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Foxe, J.J.; Molholm, S. Ten Years at the Multisensory Forum: Musings on the Evolution of a Field. Brain Topogr. 2009, 21, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Paulus, F.W.; Ohmann, S.; Möhler, E.; Plener, P.; Popow, C. Emotional Dysregulation in Children and Adolescents With Psychiatric Disorders. A Narrative Review. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 628252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fridenson-Hayo, S.; Berggren, S.; Lassalle, A.; Tal, S.; Pigat, D.; Bölte, S.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Golan, O. Basic and Complex Emotion Recognition in Children with Autism: Cross-Cultural Findings. Mol. Autism 2016, 7, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Cappellini, A.C.; Mancini, S.; Zuffellato, S.; Bini, F.; Polcaro, P.; Conti, A.A.; Molino, L.; Macchi, C. Environmental Effects on School Age Child Psychomotricity. Minerva Pediatr. 2008, 60, 277–284. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  27. Angeli, J.M.; Harpster, K.L.; Hanson, E.; Sheehan, A.; Schwab, S.M. Patient- and Caregiver-Identified Preferences: Dimensions of Change in Developmental Therapy Treatment Goals. Dev. Neurorehabilit. 2018, 22, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Caliendo, M.; di Sessa, A.; D’alterio, E.; Frolli, A.; Verde, D.; Iacono, D.; Romano, P.; Vetri, L.; Carotenuto, M. Efficacy of Neuro-Psychomotor Approach in Children Affected by Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Multicenter Study in Italian Pediatric Population. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Jolanta, G.; Agata, N.; Anna, R.-T. Psychomotor therapy as a effective method of alleviating the symptoms of child’s non-harmonious development. Pedagog. Psychol. 2015, 19, 44–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Cavalli, G.; Galeoto, G.; Sogos, C.; Berardi, A.; Tofani, M. The Efficacy of Executive Function Interventions in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2022, 22, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tofani, M.; Scarcella, L.; Galeoto, G.; Giovannone, F.; Sogos, C. Behavioral Gender Differences across Pre-School Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Cross-Sectional Study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2022, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Pittella, E.; Piuzzi, E.; Rizzuto, E.; del Prete, Z.; Fioriello, F.; Maugeri, A.; Sogos, C. Wearable Heart Rate Monitoring as Stress Response Indicator in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorder. In Proceedings of the 2018 IEEE International Symposium on Medical Measurements and Applications (MeMeA), Rome, Italy, 11–13 June 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Fioriello, F.; Maugeri, A.; D’Alvia, L.; Pittella, E.; Piuzzi, E.; Rizzuto, E.; del Prete, Z.; Manti, F.; Sogos, C. A Wearable Heart Rate Measurement Device for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 18659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Rocchi, G.; Serio, V.; Carluccio, G.M.; Marini, I.; Meuti, V.; Zaccagni, M.; Giacchetti, N.; Aceti, F. Early Attachement Relationships and Epigenetic Customization. Riv. Psichiatr. 2015, 50, 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Meuti, V.; Marini, I.; Grillo, A.; Lauriola, M.; Leone, C.; Giacchetti, N.; Aceti, F. MMPI-2: Cluster Analysis of Personality Profiles in Perinatal Depression—Preliminary Evidence. Sci. World J. 2014, 2014, 964210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Giacchetti, N.; Roma, P.; Pancheri, C.; Williams, R.; Meuti, V.; Aceti, F. Personality Traits in a Sample of Italian Filicide Mothers. Riv. Psichiatr. 2019, 54, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Mammarella, V.; Arigliani, E.; Giovannone, F.; Cavalli, G.; Tofani, M.; Sogos, C. Is It Hyperlexia? Toward a Deeper Understanding of Precocious Reading Skills in Two Cases of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Clin. Ter. 2022, 173, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Tofani, M.; Galeoto, G.; Cazzetta, D.; Berardi, A.; Sansoni, J.; Valente, D. Validation of the Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory in an Italian Population with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Study. Clin. Ter. 2019, 170, e460–e464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Loukusa, S.; Mäkinen, L.; Kuusikko-Gauffin, S.; Ebeling, H.; Moilanen, I. Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition Skills in Children with Specific Language Impairment, Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development: Group Differences and Connection to Knowledge of Grammatical Morphology, Word-Finding Abilities and Verbal Working Memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2014, 49, 498–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Taylor, L.J.; Maybery, M.T.; Whitehouse, A.J.O. Do Children with Specific Language Impairment Have a Cognitive Profile Reminiscent of Autism? A Review of the Literature. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 2067–2083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Loucas, T.; Charman, T.; Pickles, A.; Simonoff, E.; Chandler, S.; Meldrum, D.; Baird, G. Autistic Symptomatology and Language Ability in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Specific Language Impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 1184–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Bishop, D.V.M. Overlaps Between Autism and Language Impairment: Phenomimicry or Shared Etiology? Behav. Genet. 2010, 40, 618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Leyfer, O.T.; Tager-Flusberg, H.; Dowd, M.; Bruce Tomblin, J.; Folstein, S.E. Overlap between Autism and Specific Language Impairment: Comparison of Autism Diagnostic Interview and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Scores. Autism Res. 2008, 1, 284–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Green, E.M.; Stroud, L.; Marx, C.; Cronje, J. Child Development Assessment: Practitioner Input in the Revision for Griffiths III. Child Care Health Dev. 2020, 46, 682–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Li, H.H.; Feng, J.Y.; Wang, B.; Zhang, Y.; Wang, C.X.; Jia, F.Y. Comparison of the Children Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale and the Griffiths Mental Development Scales When Assessing the Development of Children with Autism. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2019, 12, 973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Gordon, B. Test Review: Wechsler, D. (2002). The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Third Edition (WPPSI-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 2004, 19, 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Brooks, B.L.; Sherman, E.M.S.; Strauss, E. NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, Second Edition. Child Neuropsychol. 2009, 16, 80–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Cavioni, V.; Grazzani, I.; Ornaghi, V.; Pepe, A.; Pons, F. Assessing the Factor Structure and Measurement Invariance of the Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC): A Large Cross-Sectional Study with Children Aged 3–10 Years. J. Cogn. Dev. 2020, 21, 406–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Marvin, A.R.; Marvin, D.J.; Lipkin, P.H.; Law, J.K. Analysis of Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) Screening for Children Less Than Age 4. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2017, 4, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Fisher, N.; Happé, F. A Training Study of Theory of Mind and Executive Function in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 757–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Silver, M.; Oakes, P. Evaluation of a New Computer Intervention to Teach People with Autism or Asperger Syndrome to Recognize and Predict Emotions in Others. Autism 2001, 5, 299–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kiesner, J.; Pastore, M. Differences in the Relations Between Antisocial Behavior and Peer Acceptance Across Contexts and Across Adolescence. Child Dev. 2005, 76, 1278–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Meins, E.; Fernyhough, C.; Wainwright, R.; das Gupta, M.; Fradley, E.; Tuckey, M. Maternal Mind–Mindedness and Attachment Security as Predictors of Theory of Mind Understanding. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 1715–1726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Vandewouw, M.M.; Choi, E.; Hammill, C.; Arnold, P.; Schachar, R.; Lerch, J.P.; Anagnostou, E.; Taylor, M.J. Emotional Face Processing across Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Dynamic Faces Study in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Greco, C.; Romani, M.; Berardi, A.; de Vita, G.; Galeoto, G.; Giovannone, F.; Vigliante, M.; Sogos, C. Morphing Task: The Emotion Recognition Process in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 13273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Interventions focused on emotions and theory of mind.
Table 1. Interventions focused on emotions and theory of mind.
Interventions Focused on EmotionsInterventions Focused on Theory of Mind
  • Cartoons or short movies focused on basic discrete emotions: anger, joy, disgust, fear, and sadness
  • Discussion about the cartoon watched, e.g., what the character felt and why
  • Associate a color with an emotion: yellow for joy, red for anger, green for disgust, violet for fear, and blue for sadness
  • Mime the emotions seen in the cartoon
  • Music video introducing complex emotions: shame, hope, disappointment, and greed.
  • Drawings:
    -
    cartoon’ characters
    -
    another member of the group experiencing an emotion
    -
    emotion felt
    -
    music video character
  • Color a printed thermometer, representing the intensity of an emotion
  • Picture of complex emotions followed by questions
  • Tell when they experienced the emotion represented by a colored hula hoop, following the color code
  • PC game “Autismo e competenze cognitivo-emotive”
  • Cartoons or short movies focused on false beliefs and understanding of behaviors
  • Discussion on cartoon watched, e.g., what is the character doing and why
  • Enactment of the Sally-Anne test
  • Pretend play with toy kitchen, fake tools or fake food according to child’s ability
  • Role play:
    -
    enactment of “Little red riding hood”
    -
    doctor, patient, and parent
    -
    prince, princess, ghost, witch
    -
    False-belief joke using an empty snack package
    -
    Games started autonomously by the children
    -
    pretend play
    -
    Sally-Anne test enactment
  • PC game “Autismo e competenze cognitivo-emotive”
Table 2. Characteristics of participants.
Table 2. Characteristics of participants.
Child AChild BChild CChild DChild EChild F
DiagnosisDLDASDASDDLDASDASD
Age3.114.23.114.104.114.2
EthnicityLatinoItalianItalianRomanianItalianItalian
Family historyNo family history of ASD or DLDNo family history of ASD or DLDNo family history of ASD or DLDNo family history of ASD or DLDFamily history for ASD and DLDNo family history of ASD or DLD
Cognitive evaluationGriffiths III
Development score: 45 months equivalent age;
Griffiths III
Development score: 47 months equivalent age;
Griffiths III
Development score: 29 months equivalent age;
GMDS-ER
Developmental age:
40.5 months equivalent age;
GMDS-ER:
Developmental age:
24.5 months equivalent age
WPPSI-III verbal IQ: 100

Performance IQ: 93

Processing speed index: 94
Table 3. Pre-post differences.
Table 3. Pre-post differences.
Wilcoxon Signed Rank TestNEPSY S01NEPSY S02TECSCQ
z−1.21−1.99−0.44−1.22
Sig (2-tailed)0.220.04 *0.650.22
* p < 0.05; NEPSY Neuropsychological Assessment for Child; TEC Test of Emotion Comprehension; SCQ Social Communication Questionnaire.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Giangiacomo, E.; Visaggi, M.C.; Aceti, F.; Giacchetti, N.; Martucci, M.; Giovannone, F.; Valente, D.; Galeoto, G.; Tofani, M.; Sogos, C. Early Neuro-Psychomotor Therapy Intervention for Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Pilot Study. Children 2022, 9, 1142. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081142

AMA Style

Giangiacomo E, Visaggi MC, Aceti F, Giacchetti N, Martucci M, Giovannone F, Valente D, Galeoto G, Tofani M, Sogos C. Early Neuro-Psychomotor Therapy Intervention for Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Pilot Study. Children. 2022; 9(8):1142. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081142

Chicago/Turabian Style

Giangiacomo, Elisa, Maria Castellano Visaggi, Franca Aceti, Nicoletta Giacchetti, Melania Martucci, Federica Giovannone, Donatella Valente, Giovanni Galeoto, Marco Tofani, and Carla Sogos. 2022. "Early Neuro-Psychomotor Therapy Intervention for Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Pilot Study" Children 9, no. 8: 1142. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081142

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop