Next Article in Journal
Impacts of Mir146a Genotypes on Bladder Cancer Risk in Taiwan
Previous Article in Journal
An Insight into Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms Underlying the Pathogenesis of Neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s Disease
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Electrophysiological and Behavioral Effects of Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Neural Mechanisms and Clinical Applications

by
Jessica Gallina
1,2,†,
Gianluca Marsicano
1,2,†,
Vincenzo Romei
1,2 and
Caterina Bertini
1,2,*
1
Centre for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Bologna, Via Rasi e Spinelli 176, 47521 Cesena, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat 5, 40121 Bologna, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Biomedicines 2023, 11(5), 1399; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11051399
Submission received: 30 March 2023 / Revised: 28 April 2023 / Accepted: 4 May 2023 / Published: 8 May 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Neurobiology and Clinical Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Alpha-band (7–13 Hz) activity has been linked to visuo-attentional performance in healthy participants and to impaired functionality of the visual system in a variety of clinical populations including patients with acquired posterior brain lesion and neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. Crucially, several studies suggested that short uni- and multi-sensory rhythmic stimulation (i.e., visual, auditory and audio-visual) administered in the alpha-band effectively induces transient changes in alpha oscillatory activity and improvements in visuo-attentional performance by synchronizing the intrinsic brain oscillations to the external stimulation (neural entrainment). The present review aims to address the current state of the art on the alpha-band sensory entrainment, outlining its potential functional effects and current limitations. Indeed, the results of the alpha-band entrainment studies are currently mixed, possibly due to the different stimulation modalities, task features and behavioral and physiological measures employed in the various paradigms. Furthermore, it is still unknown whether prolonged alpha-band sensory entrainment might lead to long-lasting effects at a neural and behavioral level. Overall, despite the limitations emerging from the current literature, alpha-band sensory entrainment may represent a promising and valuable tool, inducing functionally relevant changes in oscillatory activity, with potential rehabilitative applications in individuals characterized by impaired alpha activity.

1. Alpha-Band Oscillations and Neural Entrainment Mechanisms

1.1. Alpha-Band Oscillations Shape Visual Perception

Over the past decades, extensive research suggested that periodic fluctuations in brain activity are strongly associated with different cognitive and sensory processes and that the spontaneous brain rhythms are susceptible to functional variations, strongly affecting behavior. Among all spontaneous brain frequencies, alpha neuronal oscillations (~7–13 Hz) have been hypothesized to have a direct influence on visual processing, and its oscillatory parameters (i.e., frequency, power and phase) have been consistently associated with different aspects of visual performance [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13], suggesting that the neural oscillatory activity in this frequency band may be a reliable neurophysiological hallmark of the functionality of the visual system [14]. Specifically, variations in alpha power and phase have been strongly associated with fluctuations in the excitability of the visual cortex, with the concurrent rhythmic modulation of visual performance [3,14,15,16], suggesting that neural oscillations in this frequency band actively shape visual perception modulating cortical excitability and determining the probability of perceptual reports [3,17,18]. In addition, it has been widely documented that the speed of the alpha oscillatory activity regulates the temporal resolution of the processing of visual information [16,19,20], indicating a link between alpha oscillations and the natural attitude of the visual system for processing sensory input within separate temporal windows. Overall, these extensive findings highlight the pivotal role of neuronal alpha oscillations in various aspects of visual cognition and demonstrate that modifications in alpha amplitude, phase and speed, together with changes in the perceptual performance [1,15,21], emerge during an experimental session, depending on the task requirements and cognitive state. This strongly suggests that oscillatory and perceptual activity can be considered as dynamic rather than static processes, making it possible to hypothesize that the spontaneous neuronal oscillations in the alpha frequency band can be externally modulated and facilitated.

1.2. Entrainment Mechanisms of Alpha-Band Neuronal Oscillations

Based on the previously mentioned notion highlighting that oscillatory patterns can be externally modulated, a recent line of research aimed at driving functionally relevant changes in alpha oscillatory activity by directly stimulating the neural oscillating networks, through rhythmic stimulation protocols, exploiting mechanisms of neural plasticity in the visual system [22,23,24,25,26,27]. Indeed, it has been consistently documented that administering a rhythmical external force, such as Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) e.g., [12,28,29,30], transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) e.g., [24,31,32,33,34,35,36,37] or rhythmic sensory stimulation [15,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47], effectively induces a temporal alignment of the endogenous brain oscillations to the external rhythmical event [29,30,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57]. Hence, the stimulated neural substrate synchronizes its intrinsic oscillatory activity to the rhythm of the driving oscillatory force [28,44,58], typically resulting in a phase alignment [58] and power enhancement [28,39,58] of the ongoing neural oscillations, a mechanism described as neural entrainment [59] (see Figure 1). The effects of the entrainment on the ongoing oscillatory activity were demonstrated to strongly depend on different stimulation parameters e.g., [60,61]. More specifically, given the causal relationship between brain rhythms and perceptual processing, to be effective, the frequency of the entrainment should fall into a narrow bandwidth and, most specifically, the frequency of the stimulation should match the brain rhythms that are perceptually relevant for the targeted area [62]. Indeed, the mechanism of neural entrainment is based on the principle of the Arnold Tongue phenomena [60,61], which predicts that the phase synchronization between two dynamical properties of two coupled rhythmic forces occurs when the driving oscillators (i.e., stimulation frequency) approach the preferential rhythm of the targeted oscillatory system (i.e., endogenous brain oscillations). In this respect, the majority of the experimental paradigms based on entrainment protocols in vision studies showed that the periodical stimulation of parieto-occipital brain regions leads to selective and immediate changes in alpha oscillatory indices, with concurrent variations in visual performance, when the frequency of the stimulation is tuned to the preferential frequency of the visual system. Specifically, given such frequency-dependence, these studies demonstrated that a rhythmical entrainment administered at a frequency included in the alpha-band (7–13 Hz) has strong effects on both electroencephalography (EEG) activity, commonly in the posterior brain areas contralateral to the stimulation [12,28,63], and visual performance [48,64], which can last for several cycles after the entrainment offset [12,28,29,63]. In addition, it has been argued that the observed entrainment-related effects may be maximal (i.e., maximal enhancement in alpha amplitude at the stimulation offset) when the frequency of the entrainment corresponds to the participants’ individual alpha frequency (IAF; [28,31,65]). Nonetheless, it is still not completely understood whether small or large deviations from the IAF stimulation frequency may possibly result in different functional outcomes. However, a further key parameter described in the Arnold Tongue phenomena suggests that the strength of the neural entrainment outcomes might correspond to the intensity of the stimulation. In detail, it has been demonstrated that higher stimulation intensities (i.e., higher amplitude) result in a synchronization of a larger range of intrinsic frequencies around the preferential endogenous oscillatory rhythm [37,60,61,65]. Overall, these converging findings have strongly proved the efficacy of the alpha-band entrainment in driving visual performance by modulating the ongoing alpha oscillatory dynamics. Importantly, although most of the entrainment studies so far have employed non-invasive neurostimulation techniques (e.g., TMS, tACS; Figure 1, panel A), in more recent years, evidence coming from entrainment paradigms based on sensory stimulation modality has given new insights into the entrainment-related dynamics and effects on neural processing and perceptual performance. Interestingly, those studies allowed for highlighting the efficacy of the alpha-band entrainment in modulating visual abilities through intra- and cross-modal interactions between sensory systems, suggesting a potential advantage of the sensory rhythmic stimulation in inducing functionally relevant changes in neural and perceptual activity [40,41].

2. Physiological and Behavioral Effects of Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment

Alpha-band unisensory (i.e., visual, auditory) and multisensory (i.e., audio-visual) entrainment has been successfully employed in a number of recent experimental investigations and was shown to be effective in inducing modulations in alpha rhythm parameters in functionally interconnected networks, both within and between the visual and the auditory systems [48,66,67,68,69,70,71], with concurrent improvements in perceptual performance [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,58]. Specifically, entrainment protocols are based on the principle that the first stimulus of a rhythmical train of sensory stimuli is capable of inducing a phase reset of the ongoing alpha oscillations in the visual system, synchronizing endogenous oscillatory activity to the rhythm of the external stimulation, with a consequent increase in visual detection when visual stimuli are presented in a phase relative to stimulation frequency ([38,70]; for a graphical representation of alpha-band sensory entrainment mechanisms, see Figure 1, panel B). Overall, these experimental paradigms provide strong evidence for a causal role of sensory rhythmic stimulation in enhancing neural and perceptual activity, both intra- and across-modality, and allow for clarifying how different sensory systems interact to shape perceptual performance when an external rhythmical force is administered in the range of alpha.

2.1. Alpha-Band Visual Entrainment

The majority of the studies probing the effects of alpha-band sensory entrainment on neuronal and perceptual activity have commonly used short trains of visual rhythmic stimulation of ~0.5–5 s ([38,39,42,43,46,58,72]; see Table 1). The short-term entrainment protocols were consistently shown to result in phase synchronization [42,43,46,58], power enhancement [38,39,42,46,58] or a shift in the frequency [44] of the ongoing alpha oscillations during the rhythmic stimulation ([65]; i.e., online entrainment), with the maximum effects being observed over the posterior scalp sites [32,39,58], supporting the idea that brain oscillating networks can be efficiently entrained when stimulated at their preferential frequency. In addition, it was shown that the entrainment-driven alpha oscillations do not return to baseline power immediately after the entrainment offset. Indeed, the alpha power exhibits a relatively high increase for approximately 2/3 consecutive alpha cycles after the stimulation ([38,39,42,46,58,65]; i.e., offline entrainment), which is frequently accompanied by an improvement in visual performance [38,39,42,43,46,58]. For instance, in a series of EEG studies, a 12 Hz visual entrainment of ~1 s in duration was effective in inducing a phase-locking and a power increase of alpha band activity in parieto-occipital scalp sites, which persisted for ~200 ms [42,58], and a clear increase in detection accuracy [42,58] and perceptual sensitivity ([15]; i.e., d’) for targets presented in-phase with the preceding stimulation in metacontrast masking paradigms. Consistent with that, in another study, a visual 10.6 Hz entrainment, administered for ~500 ms, induced a phase-locking of alpha-band oscillations in parieto-occipital areas, lasting for ~300 ms after the stimulation, and an improvement in discrimination rates for invalid trials in a cueing paradigm for in-phase visual targets [38]. In addition, in another investigation, a 10 Hz visual entrainment of 1.5 s in duration was shown to selectively induce an alpha power increase in contra-lateral early visual areas, recorded for ~300 ms after the stimulation offset, and an improvement in visual detection rates [39]. However, in contrast with the aforementioned evidence, such improvements in visual performance were observed for targets presented out-of-phase with respect to the stimulation. Therefore, the phase relationship between the driving stimulation and the subsequent target presentation has not been fully understood yet. Recently, Wiesman & Wilson [46] employed an alpha-band stimulation protocol administered at 10 Hz for 1.5 s, revealing a greater alpha event-related synchronization (ERS) and inter-trials phase coherence (ITPC) compared to a 30 Hz stimulation, persisting for ~550 ms after the offset of the stimulation. Importantly, these alpha band entrainment-induced aftereffects were accompanied by a reduced congruency effect (i.e., a smaller difference in discrimination rates between congruent and incongruent trials) in an adapted version of the arrow-based Erikson “flanker” paradigm, suggesting a robust relationship between alpha entrainment and the active inhibition of distractor stimuli. However, contrary to previous evidence, in a recent investigation, Gray and Emmanouil [73] documented no effect of ~1 s of rhythmic visual stimulation delivered at the upper (12.5 Hz) and lower (8.3 Hz) frequencies of the alpha-band on a temporal integration/segregation task of visual stimuli. Similarly, De Graaf and Duecker [72] recently documented no effect of alpha-band visual entrainment (10 Hz) on visual performance. In their web-based study, the authors employed a brief (~1 s) alpha-band visual entrainment (10 Hz), investigating whether the alpha entrainment administered on the contralateral hemifield relative to the visual target could negatively impact visual performance. Surprisingly, their results showed that alpha-band visual entrainment did not affect visual performance. However, as proposed by the authors, in the absence of M/EEG data, it is difficult to clarify whether the stimulation was effective in entraining endogenous alpha oscillations and thus in modulating visual performance.
Overall, these converging results provided evidence that short trains of visual alpha-band entrainment can induce transient, functionally relevant modulations in alpha oscillatory activity, leading to improved visual abilities and visual awareness, although some inconsistent findings suggest that the stimulation protocols, task requirements and behavioral measures used to sample visual performance can be critical for disclosing entrainment-induced effects.

2.2. Alpha-Band Auditory Entrainment

It has been recently hypothesized that both visual and auditory stimuli are capable of inducing a phase reset of ongoing neuronal oscillations not only within ([70]; i.e., from visual stimuli to visual areas) but also across sensory modalities ([48,71]; i.e., from auditory stimuli to the visual areas), enhancing the activity of endogenous alpha oscillations in the primary sensory visual cortices via subthreshold depolarization, synchronizing ongoing oscillatory activity across functionally interconnected nodes of sensory cortices [66,67,68,71]. Indeed, several pieces of evidence demonstrated that cross-modal phase reset (i.e., auditory to visual) of the intrinsic neuronal oscillations of the visual system results in an increase in visual detection performance and in the cyclic fluctuations of occipital alpha oscillations when visual stimuli were phase-locked to the auditory reset stimulus [48,68,70,74,75,76,77]. However, to the best of our knowledge, only three studies employed a systematic auditory alpha-band entrainment protocol with the aim of investigating its effects on performance in the visual domain ([40,41,47]; see Table 1). In detail, Ronconi and colleagues [40] investigated, in two different experiments, whether a brief (~2 s) auditory or visual entrainment administered in the pre-targets period at the alpha-band frequency (10 Hz) were able to reduce the Attentional Blink (AB) magnitude effect for reviews of AB, see: [78,79,80,81] and thus increase the accuracy detection of a second visual target (T2) presented after a first visual target (T1) in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream. Intriguingly, their findings demonstrated that only alpha-band entrainment administered in the auditory modality—and not in the visual modality—was effective in increasing the detection of the visual target. In line with previous findings demonstrating an auditory-to-visual phase reset of endogenous alpha oscillation in the visual system [48,76,77], the auditory rhythmic stimulation facilitated target detection by synchronizing the ongoing alpha oscillations over visual cortices. Ronconi and colleagues [40] did not observe modulations of visual performance following visual stimulation. The authors proposed that auditory entrainment may be more effective in inducing stronger neuronal entrainment effects through auditory-to-visual cross-modal phase reset mechanisms of neuronal oscillations, with functional modulations of visual performance, due to the higher sensitivity of the auditory system to rhythmicity [82,83,84,85,86,87,88]. In line with this hypothesis, other evidence demonstrated that auditory rhythmic temporal sequences are capable of modulating the perception of flickering visual stimuli [89,90], taking advantage of cross-modal auditory-to-visual phase reset phenomena [66,67,68,71]. Accordingly, in a subsequent study, Ronconi and colleagues [41] confirmed the efficacy of alpha-band auditory entrainment in reducing the AB magnitude, providing EEG evidence of an association between increased posterior and frontal alpha activity during the rhythmic stream of auditory stimuli and the detection of the visual target. Furthermore, Kawashima and colleagues [47] recently employed an auditory alpha-band entrainment (10 Hz) in a web-based modality in order to replicate the findings of Ronconi and colleagues [40,41], increasing the duration of pre-target auditory entrainment (5 s) and showing that auditory rhythmic stimulation worked to increase visual detection performance in an AB task.
Overall, these converging findings underline the efficacy of the alpha-band auditory entrainment in modulating visual performance via cross-modal phase reset phenomena of alpha neuronal oscillatory activity, potentially suggesting further advantages of the auditory rhythmic stimulation in inducing entrainment, with functional consequences on visual performance [40,41].

2.3. Alpha-Band Audiovisual Entrainment

Intriguingly, it is conceivable that alpha-band multisensory entrainment administered through audiovisual (AV) modality may maximize the modulatory effects of the alpha neuronal oscillations with stronger consequences on various domains of the visual system, taking advantage of the cross-modal phase reset mechanisms between the functional interconnected nodes of the auditory and visual sensory systems [48,68,70,74,75,76,77]. However, to date, only two investigations have probed the modulatory efficacy of AV alpha-band entrainment regarding visual performance, employing brief (~2 s) rhythmic stimulation protocols characterized by the combination of synchronized visual and auditory stimuli ([44,45]; see Table 1). In detail, Ronconi and Melcher [44] investigated whether an AV entrainment administered in the pre-stimulus interval at the alpha (upper alpha: 11.5 Hz; lower alpha: ~8.5 Hz), theta (~6.5 Hz) and beta (~15 Hz) frequencies could shape visual temporal perception, affecting the temporal sampling of the visual system in a visual temporal integration/segregation task. Their findings demonstrated that an AV rhythmical stream delivered at the alpha band improved the temporal segregation of visual stimuli, enhancing the temporal sampling of the visual system, with respect to sensory entrainment administered at theta and beta frequencies. However, the comparison between lower and upper alpha AV entrainment revealed no significant difference in the temporal segregation/integration performance. This result appears in contrast with the previous findings highlighting that faster alpha oscillations are typically associated [91,92,93,94,95] and causally linked [96,97] with an increase in the temporal sampling of sensory systems, thus improving the temporal segregation of sensory inputs as compared to slower alpha oscillations. A potential explanation for such null result may rely on the fact that the investigators did not administer the stimulations based on the participants’ IAF, and thus, the lower and/or upper alpha stimulation frequencies were potentially set out of the participant’s IAF range, leading to an inability of the AV rhythmic stimulation to synchronize ongoing alpha oscillations via entrainment mechanisms [37,60,61,65]. However, to disambiguate this potential methodological issue, in a subsequent study, Ronconi and colleagues [45] administered an AV entrainment settled at the IAF − 2 Hz and IAF + 2 Hz of participants during the prestimulus interval in a visual temporal integration/segregation task. In line with previous evidence [91,93,96,97,98], their findings highlighted that faster and slower alpha AV stimulations accounted for opposite effects on visual temporal resolution: the AV entrainment delivered at IAF + 2 Hz improved segregation performance, whereas the IAF − 2 Hz stimulation improved integration performance. In addition, the accuracy of the segregation/integration of participants’ performance was densely sampled in time to probe whether the stimulation frequency may induce a shift of the alpha peak. Accordingly, their findings revealed a shift of the averaged alpha peak toward the stimulation frequency, highlighting faster fluctuations following IAF + 2 Hz stimulation, relative to IAF − 2 Hz.
Overall, despite empirical evidence supporting the efficacy of alpha-band AV entrainment in functionally modulating visual performance is still scarce, preliminary findings seems to support the existence of functional effects following AV entrainment [44,45], potentially opening new scenarios for the identification of optimal sensory entrainment protocols.

2.4. Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Current State-of-the-Art and Critical Aspects

Taken together, the aforementioned evidence outlined an overall consistent effect of brief protocols of sensory entrainment, irrespective of the specific sensory modality used, in synchronizing endogenous alpha oscillatory activity over visual cortices during sensory stimulation, as well as after its offset (~three alpha cycles), with functional consequences on visual performance [38,39,42,43,46,58,99].
As mentioned, sensory entrainment effects on visual performance rely on mechanisms of phase reset of the ongoing alpha oscillations in the visual system, with a subsequent synchronization of endogenous oscillatory activity. In line with this notion, unisensory visual entrainment administered at alpha frequencies has been widely demonstrated as effective in entraining endogenous alpha oscillations of the cortical and subcortical visual pathways [28,100,101], with associated functional improvements in visual abilities and visual awareness [38,39,42,43,46,58,99]. Moreover, despite a more limited range of available empirical evidence, converging findings have recently also documented the effectiveness of auditory sensory entrainment in entraining the ongoing neuronal oscillations of the visual system, with consequent functional modulation of visual performance [40,41,47]. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that auditory stimuli are capable of inducing a phase reset of the endogenous neuronal oscillations of the visual system, synchronizing oscillatory activity through the functionally interconnected nodes between sensory systems [48,66,67,68,69,70,71]. This same mechanism of cross-modal phase reset might also subserve audio-visual entrainment-mediated effects on visual performance, although evidence of the efficacy of this sensory stimulation protocol is scarce so far [44,45].
Regarding the neural bases underlying the effects of sensory entrainment over visual areas, intra-cranial recordings in humans [102,103] and in animal models [104,105] showed that neuronal activity in both the primary visual and auditory cortex is successfully entrained in response to trains of auditory or audio-visual rhythmical stimulation, administered in a wide range of frequencies (e.g., 3–40 Hz), providing evidence for the efficacy of both unisensory auditory and cross-modal entrainment protocols in activating the neural substrate in visual areas, possibly taking advantage of direct connections between the visual and auditory cortices. In this respect, the use of tracers to highlight neural connections in monkeys uncovered the presence of direct projections of the auditory cortex to the primary visual cortex [106] and of the associative auditory cortex to the primary and secondary visual areas [107] that might subserve cortical activation in response to cross-modal rhythmic stimulation.
Overall, the existing literature strongly suggests that, although the neural circuits involved in different forms of sensory entrainment might vary as a function of the sensory modality used, the basic mechanisms orchestrating alpha-band sensory entrainment (i.e., phase reset, synchronization of endogenous alpha oscillations) are similar between sensory modalities (i.e., visual, auditory and audiovisual) of administration and have been demonstrated to be effective in modulating visual performance.
Although sensory entrainment delivered via different sensory modalities has been shown to be effective in synchronizing visual alpha neuronal oscillations and in inducing effects on visual performance, it is still unclear whether the different sensory modalities of sensory stimulation can produce different outcomes. Critically, to date, only a study conducted by Ronconi and colleagues [40] has provided a direct comparison of the potential dissociabilities of the effects on visual detection performance in a rapid serial visual presentation stream (i.e., Attentional Blink task), induced by the alpha-band sensory entrainment administered through visual and auditory modalities. A noteworthy aspect emerging from the study conducted by Ronconi and colleagues [40] is that only auditory and not visual alpha-band entrainment was effective in functionally modulating the temporal mechanisms of visual detection performance. The authors suggested that the dissociable effects of auditory and visual entrainment may potentially rely on the underlying asymmetries in sensitivity to the temporal regularities between the auditory and visual sensory systems. Accordingly, converging evidence suggests that, relative to the visual system, the auditory system is more prone to temporal regularities and thus more sensitive to rhythmic stimulation, likely due to the rhythmical nature of the auditory environment [82,83,84,85,86,87,88]. Indeed, although rhythmicity has been found to be a consistent functional property of the visual system [1,3,14,15,16,108,109,110,111], Zoefel and VanRullen [88] recently proposed that phase synchronization between endogenous oscillations and the external oscillatory rhythm is most crucial for stimulus selection in the auditory system as compared to the visual system. Such hypotheses may suggest greater entrainment modulations of the underlying neuronal oscillatory activity when the stimulation is administered in the auditory domain.
Crucially, it is conceivable that the modulatory effects of sensory entrainment may vary as a function of the sensory modality of the rhythmic sensory stream employed and, potentially, as a result of its interaction with the perceptual/cognitive processes that are investigated. Indeed, the efficacy of alpha-band visual entrainment has been mainly demonstrated in studies aiming at modulating the spatial domain of visual perceptual/attentional performance e.g., [38,39,58,99], whereas there is currently a lack of knowledge regarding its efficacy in entraining alpha oscillations in order to induce modulatory effects of the temporal mechanisms orchestrating visual performance. On the contrary, the effectiveness of alpha-band auditory entrainment has only been documented for the temporal domain of visual perception/attention [40,41,47]. Speculatively, the findings of Ronconi and colleagues [40] highlighting a modulation of visual performance only following auditory—and not visual—alpha band-entrainment seem to be in line with this perspective, suggesting a putative higher intrinsic sensitivity to the rhythmicity of the auditory system and its natural predisposition to orchestrate the temporal mechanisms of perception and cognition. Accordingly, employing an alpha-band entrainment administered via visual modality, Gray and Emmanouil [73] documented no effect on the mechanisms of temporal integration/segregation of visual stimuli. Further studies combining psychophysiological and behavioral techniques will be required to disambiguate potential dissociable effects of the alpha-band sensory entrainment administered in the visual and auditory modalities on the neuronal oscillatory activity orchestrating various aspects of visual perception.
Recently, Ronconi and colleagues [44,45] also employed multisensory AV rhythmic stimulations, exploiting the cross-modal phase reset mechanisms between auditory and visual systems [48,68,70,71,74,75,76,77]. As previously described, their findings highlighted the effectiveness of AV entrainment in entraining the ongoing alpha oscillatory activity, with functional consequences on temporal aspects of visual performance [44,45]. However, whether this modulatory effect is extendable to other domains (i.e., spatial domain) of visual perception is currently unknown, leaving to future investigations the possibility of disambiguating whether the functional modulation of the temporal and spatial aspects of the mechanisms orchestrating the visual performance may depend on the entrained sensory modality.
The examined evidence seems to suggest that the optimal stimulation parameters of alpha-band sensory entrainment that are effective in modulating visual performance have not yet been identified. To address this open question, mathematical models, such as the modified version of the Jansen–Rit neural mass model ([112]; NMM), have been developed to investigate the spectro-temporal features of the entrainment as a function of the stimulation frequency and the resonance frequency and coupling strength of resonant neural circuits in biophysical models [113]. Using this mathematical approach, Otero and colleagues [112] documented that the maximum entrainment effects are achieved only when the frequency of driving stimulation is centered to the resonance frequency of the simulated neural system, and the amplitude of the stimulus-induced oscillation in a particular brain region, as a function of the stimulation frequency, is proportional to the size of the neural population tuned to the frequency of the sensory stimulus. In addition, in the simulated neural system, the propagation of the entrainment-driven effects was restricted to cortical regions sharing similar intrinsic frequencies, hence processing the same type of information. These findings bridge models of neural oscillations and empirical electrophysiology and help shed light on the mechanisms underlying neural entrainment and the use of rhythmic sensory stimulation.
Taken together, the aforementioned evidence highlights a general efficacy of alpha-band sensory entrainment in inducing functional modulations of visual performance by synchronizing the endogenous alpha oscillations, regardless of the sensory modality in which the entrainment is administered (i.e., visual, auditory and audiovisual). At the same time, the literature reviewed suggests that the functional effects of entrainment may vary according to the sensory modality employed and its correspondence with the different aspects of visual perception that are targeted by the stimulation (i.e., temporal and spatial). However, at present, the evidence comparing the potential differences between different sensory modalities of entrainment is scarce, and future investigations are needed to disambiguate such potential differences.
Critically, in order to effectively unveil the effect of the alpha-band sensory entrainment on visual performance, in future investigations, it could also be crucial to characterize the behavioral and electrophysiological measures that may best reflect the modulated perceptual and cognitive mechanisms underlying various aspects of visual performance. The majority of the studies that systematically investigated the modulatory effects of alpha band sensory entrainment on visual performance indexed its effects by analyzing the response accuracy, documenting a general increase in visual detection accuracy following alpha-band rhythmic stimulation [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,58,114]. Although accuracy was found to be an efficient measure in revealing the effects of entrainment on visual performance, it has been recently proposed [115] that the measures of the signal detection theory (SDT) framework i.e., (d’ and criterion) [116], depict a clearer view of the functional dissociabilities of alpha parameters (i.e., phase, power, frequency), thus reflecting different aspects of visual performance, such as the visual temporal resolution and detection accuracy, as well as prior expectations or top-down biases. Accordingly, recent findings highlighted that higher pre-stimulus alpha power may reflect the subjective confidence of visual stimuli detection, affecting the response criterion [8,12,117], whereas alpha frequency may be more related to the spatio-temporal sampling mechanisms of the visual system orchestrating perceptual accuracy and sensitivity [12,33,91,92,96,97,98]. To date, alpha-band entrainment-induced effects were assessed by analyzing perceptual sensitivity (d’) in only one investigation [15], demonstrating higher perceptual sensitivity for visual targets presented in phase with the preceding alpha-band visual stimulation (12 Hz) in a metacontrast masking paradigm. Future investigations will be required to disentangle how sensory entrainment can differently affect perceptual sensitivity, subjective confidence, prior expectations and top-down biases by modulating the underlying parameters of the alpha oscillatory activity (however, see [22] for a first enquiry causally dissociating different oscillatory parameters such as the power and frequency of alpha and confidence and sensitivity, respectively, using neurostimulation entrainment paradigms).
Furthermore, the identification of optimal behavioral indices is of considerable importance to avoiding that the potential entrainment-induced modulations might be masked by a lack of sensitivity of the behavioral measure employed. In this regard, as previously mentioned (see Section 2.1), De Graaf and Duecker [72] recently documented no effect of alpha-band visual entrainment (10 Hz) on visual performance indexing, as a proxy measure of entrainment effects, the Reaction Times (RTs). More specifically, they investigated whether an uninformative (congruent or incongruent) cue administered at the alpha rhythm in the contralateral hemifield relative to the visual target could affect RTs. We might speculate that the task requirements and cognitive load e.g., [118] might, at least in part, have contributed to the observed lack of modulatory effects on RTs of alpha-entrainment cues. Indeed, it is conceivable that attentional costs or facilitations might prevail regarding the modulation of response times, masking visual alpha-band entrainment effects. This suggests that task requirements, behavioral responses and their relative measures might affect the emergence of sensory entrainment effects and that, therefore, these parameters should be further understood and taken in careful consideration when planning entrainment-related experimental designs.
Overall, although alpha-band sensory entrainment was widely documented as effective in modulating visual performance via the synchronization of the alpha oscillatory activity over visual cortices, further investigations are required to unravel the mechanisms underlying the different sensory entrainment paradigms, thus allowing for identifying effective protocols for promoting functional effects on alpha oscillatory activity and visual performance.

3. Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Long-Term Effects and Clinical Applications

A noteworthy aspect that has not yet been elucidated regards whether prolonged and repeated sensory entrainment paradigms may lead to longer-lasting entrainment-induced variations in alpha oscillatory parameters and perceptual outcomes. Indeed, as extensively reported in the previous chapters, the entrainment protocols that have been employed so far were typically characterized by brief trains of periodic stimulation (~0.5–5 s), leading to transient neuronal and perceptual aftereffects, briefly outlasting the entrainment offset (~100–550 ms) [38,39,42,46,58]. A pioneering investigation showed that a prolonged audio-visual entrainment (i.e., 10 Hz) administered for 8 min resulted in either an increase or decrease in alpha power, measured for ~8 min at the entrainment offset [119]. Nonetheless, whether persistent variations in alpha oscillatory activity can be consistently achieved with prolonged sensory rhythmic stimulation remains an open question. Another crucial topic that is currently debated is whether the entrainment-related effects on alpha parameters’ activity reflect focal, rather than diffused, processes. Indeed, based on the aforementioned investigations employing short-term entrainment paradigms, it has been hypothesized that the effects of the entrainment on the ongoing alpha activity are rather local and typically occur in contralateral parieto-occipital brain regions [28,38,39]. Therefore, it still needs to be elucidated whether prolonged trains of sensory entrainment, unlike short-lasting ones, may result in more diffused and long-lasting effects that may extend to ipsilateral brain regions and/or other brain areas. This might be of particular interest in light of well-documented evidence showing alterations in the posterior alpha oscillations [120,121,122], as well as dysfunctions in alpha-band activity in large-scale networks, associated with cognitive and sensory impairments ([123,124,125]; Table 2). Indeed, occipito-parietal alpha rhythm has been consistently shown to reflect the reliable neurophysiological signature of the sensory and cognitive functionality in individuals with neuropsychiatric disorders (for a review, see: [117,123,124]), such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD) [125] or schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD). For instance, individuals with ASD reported a reduction in alpha power [126] and aberrant alpha power modulations [127,128] linked to impairments in the perceptual suppression of irrelevant sensory information. Moreover, individuals with ASD also show alterations in long-range structural and functional alpha connectivity (rather than in a local area; [129,130]), with a prevalence of ascending connections from posterior to anterior areas, pointing to a tendency to convey more bottom-up information [117,129,131]. In the domain of SSD, instead, a consistent slowdown of IAF [130,132,133,134,135,136] and a reduction in alpha power [137,138,139,140,141,142,143] has been observed, together with pervasive dysregulation in the way alpha oscillations instantiate phasic fluctuations and a reduction in long-range alpha coherence at rest and across different cognitive tasks [136,144]. In addition, alterations in alpha rhythm were shown to predict the structural and functional integrity of the visual system in patients affected by posterior brain damage. In particular, recent investigations regarding hemianopic and neglect patients revealed that, although residual alpha activity can be observed in response to stimuli in the blind/neglected field [145,146], these clinical populations show dysfunctional neuronal oscillatory dynamics in the alpha band, characterized by a slowdown of IAF [120,147] and a decrease in the alpha power [120,122] and connectivity [121,148] in the lesioned hemisphere that were associated with impaired visual performance in the blind and in the neglected field [120,121,122,147,148,149], strengthening the notion that post-lesional variations in alpha oscillatory activity could represent a reliable biomarker of the functionality of the visual system. In light of this evidence, prolonged alpha-band sensory entrainment protocols may represent a potential effective tool for modulating the residual impaired alpha oscillatory activity, leading to enhanced visual perceptual performance by means of the plastic reorganization of the underlying oscillating networks in these patients.
In addition to this evidence related to the alteration of alpha parameters in posterior cortices, more diffused alterations in alpha power and connectivity have also been observed in other clinical populations, such as those with dementia [150,154,155,156,157,158]. In particular, patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment [156,157,158] and Alzheimer Disease (AD; [123,124,125]) were shown to exhibit a decrease in alpha power and functional connectivity and a slowdown of the IAF [151,152], which predicted the patients’ performance in a variety of cognitive domains such as working memory, executive functions and language [150]. Importantly, the exposure to repeated sessions of sensory entrainment was shown to be beneficial for the cognitive symptoms of Alzheimer disease by enhancing the ongoing oscillations in these patients [159,160].
In addition, entrainment stimulation has also been applied to clinical populations where alpha modulations might help in ameliorating clinical signs. For instance, alpha-band visual and auditory entrainment was also shown to result in a decrease in pain intensity for acute pain induced by laser stimulation in surgery settings [161,162] and in a reduction in pain intensity ratings in patients with chronic pain [157,163]. Importantly, this reduction in pain perception following alpha-band visual entrainment might be associated with an increase in alpha power in parietal, cingulate and insular cortices [163] involved in noxious stimuli perception. Based on that, it has been hypothesized that alpha sensory entrainment leads to the inhibition of these cortical areas and a subsequent reduction in pain perception [163]. In addition, given that alpha oscillations are also involved in an attentional suppression mechanism [164] and that attention is known to influence pain perception and pain is perceived as more intense when focused on [165], it has also been suggested that the entrainment-induced reduction in pain perception might be related to the suppression of attention towards the painful stimulus, which is subsequently perceived as less intense [163]. Indeed, alpha oscillations have been associated with pain processing [166,167]. Moreover, other findings suggested that alpha-band audio-visual stimulations successfully entrained the alpha oscillations in individuals with anxiety and depressive spectrum disorders, with a beneficial impact on depressive symptoms and cognitive function, possibly due to an increase in alpha power associated with diminishment in cortical activation [168].
Overall, this accumulated evidence strongly points to the potential ameliorative and rehabilitative effects of the alpha-band sensory entrainment, making it a highly valuable tool that can be crosswise employed in various clinical populations and conditions. Based on these findings, providing proof for the efficacy of the alpha-band sensory entrainment protocols in reversing impairment in alpha activity and, in general, cognitive and sensory processing, it can be hypothesized that the administration of repeated and prolonged trains of rhythmic sensory stimulations might also be highly beneficial in the presence of altered alpha-related visual processing. However, in order to fully take advantage of entrainment-driven neural dynamics to benefit cognitive functioning and sensory processing, it should be clarified what makes the entrainment persist or how the entrainment-driven neural signal propagates across neural networks when the stimulation ends. Additionally, the functional relationship between the frequency of the stimulation and the intrinsic oscillatory properties of the targeted regions and the duration of the entrainment aftereffects should be better understood. Therefore, other experiments are required to develop a better understanding regarding the type of sensory modality and the duration and frequency of the rhythmic stimulation to employ sensory entrainment as an effective therapeutic tool that promotes cognitive and sensory enhancement by increasing the ability of distributed cortical networks to coordinate and generate brain rhythms.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.G., G.M., V.R. and C.B.; writing—original draft preparation, J.G. and G.M., writing—review and editing, V.R. and C.B.; supervision, V.R. and C.B.; funding acquisition, V.R. and C.B., J.G. and G.M. equally contributed to this work. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

J.G., G.M. and C.B. were supported by MUR—Ministero Università e Ricerca, grant number PRIN 2017TBA4KS_003. V.R. was supported by Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovacion y Universidades, grant number PID2019-111335GA-100. The funding source had no involvement in the data collection, in the analysis and interpretation, in the writing process or in the decision to submit the paper.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Klimesch, W.; Sauseng, P.; Hanslmayr, S. EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 2007, 53, 63–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Osipova, D.; Hermes, D.; Jensen, O. Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Posterior Alpha Activity. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e3990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Jensen, O.; Mazaheri, A. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Voytek, B.; Canolty, R.T.; Shestyuk, A.; Crone, N.E.; Parvizi, J.; Knight, R.T. Shifts in gamma phase–amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Mo, J.; Liu, Y.; Huang, H.; Ding, M. Coupling between visual alpha oscillations and default mode activity. Neuroimage 2013, 68, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Jensen, O.; Gips, B.; Bergmann, T.O.; Bonnefond, M. Temporal coding organized by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing. Trends Neurosci. 2014, 37, 357–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Van Diepen, R.M.; Miller, L.M.; Mazaheri, A.; Geng, J.J. The role of alpha activity in spatial and feature-based attention. eNeuro 2016, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Limbach, K.; Corballis, P.M. Prestimulus alpha power influences response criterion in a detection task. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 1154–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Benwell, C.S.Y.; Keitel, C.; Harvey, M.; Gross, J.; Thut, G. Trial-by-trial co-variation of pre-stimulus EEG alpha power and visuospatial bias reflects a mixture of stochastic and deterministic effects. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 48, 2566–2584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Samaha, J.; Iemi, L.; Postle, B.R. Prestimulus alpha-band power biases visual discrimination confidence, but not accuracy. Conscious. Cogn. 2017, 54, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gallotto, S.; Duecker, F.; Oever, S.T.; Schuhmann, T.; de Graaf, T.A.; Sack, A. Relating alpha power modulations to competing visuospatial attention theories. Neuroimage 2019, 207, 116429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Di Gregorio, F.; Trajkovic, J.; Roperti, C.; Marcantoni, E.; Di Luzio, P.; Avenanti, A.; Thut, G.; Romei, V. Tuning alpha rhythms to shape conscious visual perception. Curr. Biol. 2022, 32, 988–998.e6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Trajkovic, J.; Di Gregorio, F.; Avenanti, A.; Thut, G.; Romei, V. Two oscillatory correlates of attention control in the alpha-band with distinct consequences on perceptual gain and metacognition. J. Neurosci. 2023, JN-RM-1827-22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Romei, V.; Rihs, T.; Brodbeck, V.; Thut, G. Resting electroencephalogram alpha-power over posterior sites indexes baseline visual cortex excitability. Neuroreport 2008, 19, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mathewson, K.E.; Fabiani, M.; Gratton, G.; Beck, D.M.; Lleras, A. Rescuing stimuli from invisibility: Inducing a momentary release from visual masking with pre-target entrainment. Cognition 2010, 115, 186–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. VanRullen, R. Perceptual cycles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 723–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sokoliuk, R.; VanRullen, R. The Flickering Wheel Illusion: When α Rhythms Make a Static Wheel Flicker. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 13498–13504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Boncompte, G.; Villena-González, M.; Cosmelli, D.; López, V. Spontaneous alpha power lateralization predicts detection performance in an un-cued signal detection task. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0160347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sadaghiani, S.; Scheeringa, R.; Lehongre, K.; Morillon, B.; Giraud, A.L.; Kleinschmidt, A. Intrinsic connectivity networks, alpha oscillations, and tonic alertness: A simultaneous electroencephalography/functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 10243–10250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Morillon, B.; Schroeder, C.E. Neuronal oscillations as a mechanistic substrate of auditory temporal prediction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1337, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Iemi, L.; Chaumon, M.; Crouzet, S.M.; Busch, N.A. Spontaneous Neural Oscillations Bias Perception by Modulating Baseline Excitability. J. Neurosci. 2016, 37, 807–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Hurley, S.; Noë, A. Neural Plasticity and Consciousness. Biol. Philos. 2003, 18, 131–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Espinosa, J.S.; Stryker, M.P. Development and Plasticity of the Primary Visual Cortex. Neuron 2012, 75, 230–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Vossen, A.; Gross, J.; Thut, G. Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (α-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 2015, 8, 499–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Galuske, R.A.W.; Munk, M.H.J.; Singer, W. Relation between gamma oscillations and neuronal plasticity in the visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 23317–23325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Grasso, P.A.; Gallina, J.; Bertini, C. Shaping the visual system: Cortical and subcortical plasticity in the intact and the lesioned brain. Neuropsychologia 2020, 142, 107–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vogeti, S.; Boetzel, C.; Herrmann, C.S. Entrainment and Spike-Timing Dependent Plasticity—A Review of Proposed Mechanisms of Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 827353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Thut, G.; Veniero, D.; Romei, V.; Miniussi, C.; Schyns, P.; Gross, J. Rhythmic TMS Causes Local Entrainment of Natural Oscillatory Signatures. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, 1176–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lin, Y.J.; Shukla, L.; Dugué, L.; Valero-Cabré, A.; Carrasco, M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation entrains alpha oscillatory activity in occipital cortex. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 18562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Coldea, A.; Veniero, D.; Morand, S.; Trajkovic, J.; Romei, V.; Harvey, M.; Thut, G. Effects of Rhythmic Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in the Alpha-Band on Visual Perception Depend on Deviation from Alpha-Peak Frequency: Faster Relative Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Alpha-Pace Improves Performance. Front. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 886342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zaehle, T.; Rach, S.; Herrmann, C.S. Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation Enhances Individual Alpha Activity in Human EEG. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e13766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Helfrich, R.F.; Schneider, T.R.; Rach, S.; Trautmann-Lengsfeld, S.A.; Engel, A.K.; Herrmann, C.S. Entrainment of Brain Oscillations by Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, 333–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Minami, S.; Amano, K. Illusory Jitter Perceived at the Frequency of Alpha Oscillations. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 2344–2351.e4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Borghini, G.; Candini, M.; Filannino, C.; Hussain, M.; Walsh, V.; Romei, V.; Zokaei, N.; Cappelletti, M. Alpha Oscillations Are Causally Linked to Inhibitory Abilities in Ageing. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 4418–4429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Wolinski, N.; Cooper, N.R.; Sauseng, P.; Romei, V. The speed of parietal theta frequency drives visuospatial working memory capacity. PLoS Biol. 2018, 16, e2005348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bender, M.; Romei, V.; Sauseng, P. Slow Theta tACS of the Right Parietal Cortex Enhances Contralateral Visual Working Memory Capacity. Brain Topogr. 2019, 32, 477–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Huang, W.A.; Stitt, I.M.; Negahbani, E.; Passey, D.J.; Ahn, S.; Davey, M.; Dannhauer, M.; Doan, T.T.; Hoover, A.C.; Peterchev, A.V.; et al. Transcranial alternating current stimulation entrains alpha oscillations by preferential phase synchronization of fast-spiking cortical neurons to stimulation waveform. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 3151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. De Graaf, T.A.; Gross, J.; Paterson, G.; Rusch, T.; Sack, A.; Thut, G. Alpha-Band Rhythms in Visual Task Performance: Phase-Locking by Rhythmic Sensory Stimulation. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e60035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Spaak, E.; de Lange, F.P.; Jensen, O. Local Entrainment of Alpha Oscillations by Visual Stimuli Causes Cyclic Modulation of Perception. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 3536–3544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ronconi, L.; Pincham, H.L.; Szűcs, D.; Facoetti, A. Inducing attention not to blink: Auditory entrainment improves conscious visual processing. Psychol. Res. 2015, 80, 774–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ronconi, L.; Pincham, H.L.; Cristoforetti, G.; Facoetti, A.; Szűcs, D. Shaping prestimulus neural activity with auditory rhythmic stimulation improves the temporal allocation of attention. Neuroreport 2016, 27, 487–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kizuk, S.A.D.; Mathewson, K.E. Power and Phase of Alpha Oscillations Reveal an Interaction between Spatial and Temporal Visual Attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 480–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Keitel, C.; Keitel, A.; Benwell, C.S.; Daube, C.; Thut, G.; Gross, J. Stimulus-Driven Brain Rhythms within the Alpha Band: The Attentional-Modulation Conundrum. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 3119–3129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Ronconi, L.; Melcher, D. The role of oscillatory phase in determining the temporal organization of perception: Evidence from sensory entrainment. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 10636–10644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ronconi, L.; Busch, N.A.; Melcher, D. Alpha-band sensory entrainment alters the duration of temporal windows in visual perception. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 11810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Wiesman, A.; Wilson, T.W. Alpha Frequency Entrainment Reduces the Effect of Visual Distractors. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 31, 1392–1403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Kawashima, T.; Shibusawa, S.; Amano, K. Frequency-and phase-dependent effects of auditory entrainment on attentional blink. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2022, 56, 4411–4424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Romei, V.; Thut, G.; Mok, R.M.; Schyns, P.G.; Driver, J. Causal implication by rhythmic transcranial magnetic stimulation of alpha frequency in feature-based local vs. global attention. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 968–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chanes, L.; Quentin, R.; Tallon-Baudry, C.; Valero-Cabré, A. Causal frequency-specific contributions of frontal spatiotemporal patterns induced by non-invasive neurostimulation to human visual performance. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 5000–5005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ruzzoli, M.; Soto-Faraco, S. Alpha Stimulation of the Human Parietal Cortex Attunes Tactile Perception to External Space. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, 329–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Quentin, R.; Chanes, L.; Vernet, M.; Valero-Cabré, A. Fronto-Parietal Anatomical Connections Influence the Modulation of Conscious Visual Perception by High-Beta Frontal Oscillatory Activity. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 25, 2095–2101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Romei, V.; Thut, G.; Silvanto, J. Information-Based Approaches of Noninvasive Transcranial Brain Stimulation. Trends Neurosci. 2016, 39, 782–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Albouy, P.; Weiss, A.; Baillet, S.; Zatorre, R.J. Selective Entrainment of Theta Oscillations in the Dorsal Stream Causally Enhances Auditory Working Memory Performance. Neuron 2017, 94, 193–206.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Amengual, J.L.; Vernet, M.; Adam, C.; Valero-Cabré, A. Local entrainment of oscillatory activity induced by direct brain stimulation in humans. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, srep41908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Albouy, P.; Baillet, S.; Zatorre, R.J. Driving working memory with frequency-tuned noninvasive brain stimulation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1423, 126–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Vernet, M.; Stengel, C.; Quentin, R.; Amengual, J.L.; Valero-Cabré, A. Entrainment of local synchrony reveals a causal role for high-beta right frontal oscillations in human visual consciousness. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 14510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Lakatos, P.; Gross, J.; Thut, G. A New Unifying Account of the Roles of Neuronal Entrainment. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, R890–R905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Mathewson, K.E.; Prudhomme, C.; Fabiani, M.; Beck, D.M.; Lleras, A.; Gratton, G. Making Waves in the Stream of Consciousness: Entraining Oscillations in EEG Alpha and Fluctuations in Visual Awareness with Rhythmic Visual Stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 24, 2321–2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Thut, G.; Schyns, P.G.; Gross, J. Entrainment of Perceptually Relevant Brain Oscillations by Non-Invasive Rhythmic Stimulation of the Human Brain. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Regan, D. Comparison of transient and steady-state methods. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1982, 388, 45–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Pikovsky, A.; Rosenblum, M.; Kurths, J.; Strogatz, S. Books-Synchronization: A Universal Concept in Nonlinear Sciences. Phys. Today 2003, 56, 47. [Google Scholar]
  62. Thut, G.; Miniussi, C.; Gross, J. The Functional Importance of Rhythmic Activity in the Brain. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, R658–R663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Trajkovic, J.; Di Gregorio, F.; Marcantoni, E.; Thut, G.; Romei, V. A TMS/EEG protocol for the causal assessment of the functions of the oscillatory brain rhythms in perceptual and cognitive processes. STAR Protoc. 2022, 3, 101435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Romei, V.; Gross, J.; Thut, G. On the Role of Prestimulus Alpha Rhythms over Occipito-Parietal Areas in Visual Input Regulation: Correlation or Causation? J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 8692–8697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Notbohm, A.; Kurths, J.; Herrmann, C.S. Modification of Brain Oscillations via Rhythmic Light Stimulation Provides Evidence for Entrainment but Not for Superposition of Event-Related Responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Ghazanfar, A.; Schroeder, C. Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 278–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Senkowski, D.; Schneider, T.R.; Foxe, J.J.; Engel, A.K. Crossmodal binding through neural coherence: Implications for multisensory processing. Trends Neurosci. 2008, 31, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lakatos, P.; O’Connell, M.N.; Barczak, A.; Mills, A.; Javitt, D.C.; Schroeder, C.E. The Leading Sense: Supramodal Control of Neurophysiological Context by Attention. Neuron 2009, 64, 419–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Landau, A.N.; Fries, P. Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 1000–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Fiebelkorn, I.C.; Saalmann, Y.B.; Kastner, S. Rhythmic Sampling within and between Objects despite Sustained Attention at a Cued Location. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 2553–2558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bauer, A.-K.R.; Debener, S.; Nobre, A.C. Synchronisation of Neural Oscillations and Cross-modal Influences. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 481–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. de Graaf, T.A.; Duecker, F. No effects of rhythmic visual stimulation on target discrimination: An online alpha entrainment experiment. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2022, 55, 3340–3351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Gray, M.J.; Emmanouil, T.A. Individual alpha frequency increases during a task but is unchanged by alpha-band flicker. Psychophysiology 2019, 57, e13480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lakatos, P.; Karmos, G.; Mehta, A.D.; Ulbert, I.; Schroeder, C.E. Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection. Science 2008, 320, 110–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Naue, N.; Rach, S.; Strüber, D.; Huster, R.J.; Zaehle, T.; Körner, U.; Herrmann, C.S. Auditory Event-Related Response in Visual Cortex Modulates Subsequent Visual Responses in Humans. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 7729–7736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Miller, J.E.; Carlson, L.A.; McAuley, J.D. When what you hear influences when you see: Listening to an auditory rhythm influences the temporal allocation of visual attention. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Mercier, M.R.; Foxe, J.J.; Fiebelkorn, I.C.; Butler, J.S.; Schwartz, T.H.; Molholm, S. Auditory-driven phase reset in visual cortex: Human electrocorticography reveals mechanisms of early multisensory integration. Neuroimage 2013, 79, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Shapiro, K.L.; Raymond, J.E.; Arnell, K.M. The attentional blink. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1997, 1, 291–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Enns, J.T.; Di Lollo, V. What’s new in visual masking? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 345–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Keysers, C.; Perrett, D.I. Visual masking and RSVP reveal neural competition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 120–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Martens, S.; Wyble, B. The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 947–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Glenberg, A.M.; Jona, M. Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Mem. Cogn. 1991, 19, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Guttman, S.E.; Gilroy, L.A.; Blake, R. Hearing what the eyes see: Auditory encoding of visual temporal sequences. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 228–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Repp, B.; Penel, A. Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychol. Res. 2003, 68, 252–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Grahn, J.A. See what I hear? Beat perception in auditory and visual rhythms. Exp. Brain Res. 2012, 220, 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Nozaradan, S. Exploring how musical rhythm entrains brain activity with electroencephalogram frequency-tagging. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Merchant, H.; Grahn, J.; Trainor, L.; Rohrmeier, M.; Fitch, W.T. Finding the beat: A neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2015, 370, 20140093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Zoefel, B.; VanRullen, R. Oscillatory Mechanisms of Stimulus Processing and Selection in the Visual and Auditory Systems: State-of-the-Art, Speculations and Suggestions. Front. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Shipley, T. Auditory Flutter-Driving of Visual Flicker. Science 1964, 145, 1328–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Escoffier, N.; Herrmann, C.S.; Schirmer, A. Auditory rhythms entrain visual processes in the human brain: Evidence from evoked oscillations and event-related potentials. Neuroimage 2015, 111, 267–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Samaha, J.; Postle, B.R. The Speed of Alpha-Band Oscillations Predicts the Temporal Resolution of Visual Perception. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 2985–2990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Cooke, J.; Poch, C.; Gillmeister, H.; Costantini, M.; Romei, V. Oscillatory Properties of Functional Connections Between Sensory Areas Mediate Cross-Modal Illusory Perception. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 5711–5718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Bastiaansen, M.; Berberyan, H.; Stekelenburg, J.J.; Schoffelen, J.M.; Vroomen, J. Are alpha oscillations instrumental in multisensory synchrony perception? Brain Res. 2020, 1734, 146744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Migliorati, D.; Zappasodi, F.; Perrucci, M.G.; Donno, B.; Northoff, G.; Romei, V.; Costantini, M. Individual alpha frequency predicts perceived visuotactile simultaneity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020, 32, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Noguchi, Y. Audio–Visual Fission Illusion and Individual Alpha Frequency: Perspective on Buergers and Noppeney (2022). J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2023, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Cecere, R.; Rees, G.; Romei, V. Individual Differences in Alpha Frequency Drive Crossmodal Illusory Perception. Curr. Biol. 2014, 25, 231–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Venskus, A.; Ferri, F.; Migliorati, D.; Spadone, S.; Costantini, M.; Hughes, G. Temporal binding window and sense of agency are related processes modifiable via occipital tACS. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0256987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Ronconi, L.; Vitale, A.; Federici, A.; Mazzoni, N.; Battaglini, L.; Molteni, M.; Casartelli, L. Neural dynamics driving audio-visual integration in autism. Cereb. Cortex 2022, 33, 543–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Halbleib, A.; Gratkowski, M.; Schwab, K.; Ligges, C.; Witte, H.; Haueisen, J. Topographic analysis of engagement and disengagement of neural oscillators in photic driving: A combined electroencephalogram/magnetoencephalogram study. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012, 29, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Hanslmayr, S.; Axmacher, N.; Inman, C.S. Modulating Human Memory via Entrainment of Brain Oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 2019, 42, 485–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Albouy, P.; Martinez-Moreno, Z.E.; Hoyer, R.S.; Zatorre, R.J.; Baillet, S. Supramodality of neural entrainment: Rhythmic visual stimulation causally enhances auditory working memory performance. Sci. Adv. 2022, 8, eabj9782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Besle, J.; Schevon, C.A.; Mehta, A.D.; Lakatos, P.; Goodman, R.R.; McKhann, G.M.; Emerson, R.G.; Schroeder, C.E. Tuning of the Human Neocortex to the Temporal Dynamics of Attended Events. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 3176–3185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Gomez-Ramirez, M.; Kelly, S.P.; Molholm, S.; Sehatpour, P.; Schwartz, T.H.; Foxe, J.J. Oscillatory Sensory Selection Mechanisms during Intersensory Attention to Rhythmic Auditory and Visual Inputs: A Human Electrocorticographic Investigation. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 18556–18567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Nakao, K.; Nakazawa, K. Brain state-dependent abnormal LFP activity in the auditory cortex of a schizophrenia mouse model. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Li, S.; Ma, L.; Wang, Y.; Wang, X.; Li, Y.; Qin, L. Auditory steady-state responses in primary and non-primary regions of the auditory cortex in neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion rats. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Falchier, A.; Clavagnier, S.; Barone, P.; Kennedy, H. Anatomical Evidence of Multimodal Integration in Primate Striate Cortex. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 5749–5759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Rockland, K.S.; Ojima, H. Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2003, 50, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Shaw, J.C. The Brain’s Alpha Rhythms and the Mind; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2003; pp. 15–32. [Google Scholar]
  109. Niedermeyer, E. The normal EEG of the waking adult. In Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields; Niedermeyer, E., Lopes da Silva, F., Eds.; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2005; pp. 155–164. [Google Scholar]
  110. Buzsaki, G. Rhythms of the Brain; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  111. Mathewson, K.J.; Jetha, M.K.; Drmic, I.E.; Bryson, S.E.; Goldberg, J.O.; Schmidt, L.A. Regional EEG alpha power, coherence, and behavioral symptomatology in autism spectrum disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012, 123, 1798–1809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Otero, M.; Lea-Carnall, C.; Prado, P.; Escobar, M.-J.; El-Deredy, W. Modelling neural entrainment and its persistence: Influence of frequency of stimulation and phase at the stimulus offset. Biomed. Phys. Eng. Express 2022, 8, 045014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Jansen, B.H.; Zouridakis, G.; Brandt, M.E. A neurophysiologically-based mathematical model of flash visual evoked potentials. Biol. Cybern. 1993, 68, 275–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Chota, S.; VanRullen, R. Visual Entrainment at 10 Hz Causes Periodic Modulation of the Flash Lag Illusion. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Buergers, S.; Noppeney, U. The role of alpha oscillations in temporal binding within and across the senses. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2022, 6, 732–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Green, D.M.; Swets, J.A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1966; Volume 1, pp. 1969–2012. [Google Scholar]
  117. Tarasi, L.; Trajkovic, J.; Diciotti, S.; di Pellegrino, G.; Ferri, F.; Ursino, M.; Romei, V. Predictive waves in the autism-schizophrenia continuum: A novel biobehavioral model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 132, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Prinzmetal, W.; McCool, C.; Park, S. Attention: Reaction Time and Accuracy Reveal Different Mechanisms. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2005, 134, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Rosenfeld, J.P.; Reinhart, A.M.; Srivastava, S. The Effects of Alpha (10-Hz) and Beta (22-Hz) “Entrainment” Stimulation on the Alpha and Beta EEG Bands: Individual Differences Are Critical to Prediction of Effects. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 1997, 22, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Pietrelli, M.; Zanon, M.; Làdavas, E.; Grasso, P.A.; Romei, V.; Bertini, C. Posterior brain lesions selectively alter alpha oscillatory activity and predict visual performance in hemianopic patients. Cortex 2019, 121, 347–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Gallina, J.; Pietrelli, M.; Zanon, M.; Bertini, C. Hemispheric differences in altered reactivity of brain oscillations at rest after posterior lesions. Brain Struct. Funct. 2021, 227, 709–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Gallina, J.; Zanon, M.; Mikulan, E.; Pietrelli, M.; Gambino, S.; Ibáñez, A.; Bertini, C. Alterations in resting-state functional connectivity after brain posterior lesions reflect the functionality of the visual system in hemianopic patients. Brain Struct. Funct. 2022, 227, 2939–2956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Uhlhaas, P.J.; Singer, W. Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 100–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Ippolito, G.; Bertaccini, R.; Tarasi, L.; Di Gregorio, F.; Trajkovic, J.; Battaglia, S.; Romei, V. The Role of Alpha Oscillations among the Main Neuropsychiatric Disorders in the Adult and Developing Human Brain: Evidence from the Last 10 Years of Research. Biomedicines 2022, 10, 3189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Tierney, A.L.; Gabard-Durnam, L.; Vogel-Farley, V.; Tager-Flusberg, H.; Nelson, C.A. Developmental Trajectories of Resting EEG Power: An Endophenotype of Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e39127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Machado, C.; Estévez, M.; Leisman, G.; Melillo, R.; Rodriguez-Rojas, R.; DeFina, P.; Hernández, A.; Pérez-Nellar, J.; Naranjo, R.; Chinchilla, M.; et al. QEEG Spectral and Coherence Assessment of Autistic Children in Three Different Experimental Conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 45, 406–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Han, J.; Zeng, K.; Kang, J.; Tong, Z.; Cai, E.; Chen, H.; Ding, M.; Gu, Y.; Ouyang, G.; Li, X. Development of Brain Network in Children with Autism from Early Childhood to Late Childhood. Neuroscience 2017, 367, 134–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Palva, S.; Palva, J.M. Functional Roles of Alpha-Band Phase Synchronization in Local and Large-Scale Cortical Networks. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Ursino, M.; Serra, M.; Tarasi, L.; Ricci, G.; Magosso, E.; Romei, V. Bottom-up vs. top-down connectivity imbalance in individuals with high-autistic traits: An electroencephalographic study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 932128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Murphy, M.; Öngür, D. Decreased peak alpha frequency and impaired visual evoked potentials in first episode psychosis. NeuroImage Clin. 2019, 22, 101693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Tarasi, L.; Magosso, E.; Ricci, G.; Ursino, M.; Romei, V. The Directionality of Fronto-Posterior Brain Connectivity Is Associated with the Degree of Individual Autistic Traits. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Yeum, T.-S.; Kang, U.G. Reduction in Alpha Peak Frequency and Coherence on Quantitative Electroencephalography in Patients with Schizophrenia. J. Korean Med Sci. 2018, 33, e179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Freche, D.; Naim-Feil, J.; Hess, S.; Peled, A.; Grinshpoon, A.; Moses, E.; Levit-Binnun, N. Phase-Amplitude Markers of Synchrony and Noise: A Resting-State and TMS-EEG Study of Schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex Commun. 2020, 1, tgaa013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Nakhnikian, A.; Oribe, N.; Hirano, S.; Hirano, Y.; Levin, M.; Spencer, K. Lower Peak Alpha Frequency Accounts for Elevated Theta-Alpha Power in Resting State EEG in Schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2020, 87, S411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Koshiyama, D.; Miyakoshi, M.; Tanaka-Koshiyama, K.; Joshi, Y.B.; Sprock, J.; Braff, D.L.; Light, G.A. Abnormal phase discontinuity of alpha- and theta-frequency oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2021, 231, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Ramsay, I.S.; Lynn, P.A.; Schermitzler, B.; Sponheim, S.R. Individual alpha peak frequency is slower in schizophrenia and related to deficits in visual perception and cognition. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 17852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Başar, E.; Schmiedt-Fehr, C.; Mathes, B.; Femir, B.; Emek-Savaş, D.D.; Tülay, E.E.; Tan, D.; Düzgün, A.; Güntekin, B.; Özerdem, A.; et al. What does the broken brain say to the neuroscientist? Oscillations and connectivity in schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease, and bipolar disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2016, 103, 135–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Garakh, Z.; Zaytseva, Y.; Kapranova, A.; Fiala, O.; Horacek, J.; Shmukler, A.; Gurovich, I.Y.; Strelets, V.B. EEG correlates of a mental arithmetic task in patients with first episode schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015, 126, 2090–2098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Goldstein, M.R.; Peterson, M.J.; Sanguinetti, J.L.; Tononi, G.; Ferrarelli, F. Topographic deficits in alpha-range resting EEG activity and steady state visual evoked responses in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2015, 168, 145–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Kim, J.W.; Lee, Y.S.; Han, D.H.; Min, K.J.; Lee, J.; Lee, K. Diagnostic utility of quantitative EEG in un-medicated schizophrenia. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 589, 126–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Canali, P.; Sarasso, S.; Rosanova, M.; Casarotto, S.; Sferrazza-Papa, G.; Gosseries, O.; Fecchio, M.; Massimini, M.; Mariotti, M.; Cavallaro, R.; et al. Shared Reduction of Oscillatory Natural Frequencies in Bipolar Disorder, Major Depressive Disorder and Schizophrenia. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 184, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Candelaria-Cook, F.T.; Schendel, M.E.; Ojeda, C.J.; Bustillo, J.R.; Stephen, J.M. Reduced Parietal Alpha Power and Psychotic Symptoms: Test-Retest Reliability of Resting-State Magnetoencephalography in Schizophrenia and Healthy Controls. Schizophr. Res. 2020, 215, 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Mitra, S.; Nizamie, S.H.; Goyal, N.; Tikka, S.K. Electroencephalogram alpha-to-theta ratio over left fronto-temporal region correlates with negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Asian J. Psychiatry 2017, 26, 70–76. [Google Scholar]
  144. Trajkovic, J.; Di Gregorio, F.; Ferri, F.; Marzi, C.; Diciotti, S.; Romei, V. Resting state alpha oscillatory activity is a valid and reliable marker of schizotypy. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 10379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Sanchez-Lopez, J.; Pedersini, C.A.; Di Russo, F.; Cardobi, N.; Fonte, C.; Varalta, V.; Prior, M.; Smania, N.; Savazzi, S.; Marzi, C.A. Visually evoked responses from the blind field of hemianopic patients. Neuropsychologia 2019, 128, 127–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Grasso, P.A.; Pietrelli, M.; Zanon, M.; Làdavas, E.; Bertini, C. Alpha oscillations reveal implicit visual processing of motion in hemianopia. Cortex 2020, 122, 81–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Di Gregorio, F.; Petrone, V.; Casanova, E.; Lullini, G.; Romei, V.; Piperno, R.; La Porta, F. Hierarchical psychophysiological pathways subtend perceptual asymmetries in Neglect. Neuroimage 2023, 270, 119942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Allaman, L.; Mottaz, A.; Guggisberg, A.G. Disrupted resting-state EEG alpha-band interactions as a novel marker for the severity of visual field deficits after brain lesion. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2021, 132, 2101–2109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Lasaponara, S.; Pinto, M.; Aiello, M.; Tomaiuolo, F.; Doricchi, F. The Hemispheric Distribution of α-Band EEG Activity During Orienting of Attention in Patients with Reduced Awareness of the Left Side of Space (Spatial Neglect). J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 4332–4343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Babiloni, C.; Ferri, R.; Moretti, D.V.; Strambi, A.; Binetti, G.; Forno, G.D.; Ferreri, F.; Lanuzza, B.; Bonato, C.; Nobili, F.; et al. Abnormal fronto-parietal coupling of brain rhythms in mild Alzheimer’s disease: A multicentric EEG study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004, 19, 2583–2590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Meghdadi, A.H.; Stevanović Karić, M.; McConnell, M.; Rupp, G.; Richard, C.; Hamilton, J.; Salat, D.; Berka, C. Resting state EEG biomarkers of cognitive decline associated with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0244180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. López-Sanz, D.; Bruña, R.; Garcés, P.; Camara, C.; Serrano, N.; Rodríguez-Rojo, I.C.; Delgado, M.L.; Montenegro, M.; López-Higes, R.; Yus, M.; et al. Alpha band disruption in the AD-continuum starts in the Subjective Cognitive Decline stage: A MEG study. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 37685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Khan, S.; Gramfort, A.; Shetty, N.R.; Kitzbichler, M.G.; Ganesan, S.; Moran, J.M.; Lee, S.M.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Tager-Flusberg, H.B.; Joseph, R.M.; et al. Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 3107–3112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Bhattacharya, B.S.; Coyle, D.; Maguire, L.P. Alpha and Theta Rhythm Abnormality in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Study Using a Computational Model. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2011, 718, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Brueggen, K.; Fiala, C.; Berger, C.; Ochmann, S.; Babiloni, C.; Teipel, S.J. Early Changes in Alpha Band Power and DMN BOLD Activity in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Simultaneous Resting State EEG-fMRI Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Garcés, P.; Vicente, R.; Wibral, M.; Pineda-Pardo, J.; López, M.E.; Aurtenetxe, S.; Marcos, A.; de Andrés, M.E.; Yus, M.; Sancho, M.; et al. Brain-wide slowing of spontaneous alpha rhythms in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2013, 5, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Lopez-Diaz, K.; Henshaw, J.; Casson, A.J.; Brown, C.A.; Taylor, J.R.; Trujillo-Barreto, N.J.; Arendsen, L.J.; Jones, A.K.P.; Sivan, M. Alpha entrainment drives pain relief using visual stimulation in a sample of chronic pain patients: A proof-of-concept controlled study. Neuroreport 2021, 32, 394–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Lejko, N.; Larabi, D.I.; Herrmann, C.S.; Aleman, A.; Ćurčić-Blake, B. Alpha Power and Functional Connectivity in Cognitive Decline: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020, 78, 1047–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Iaccarino, H.F.; Singer, A.C.; Martorell, A.J.; Rudenko, A.; Gao, F.; Gillingham, T.Z.; Mathys, H.; Seo, J.; Kritskiy, O.; Abdurrob, F.; et al. Gamma frequency entrainment attenuates amyloid load and modifies microglia. Nature 2016, 540, 230–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Martorell, A.J.; Paulson, A.L.; Suk, H.-J.; Abdurrob, F.; Drummond, G.T.; Guan, W.; Young, J.Z.; Kim, D.N.-W.; Kritskiy, O.; Barker, S.J.; et al. Multi-sensory Gamma Stimulation Ameliorates Alzheimer’s-Associated Pathology and Improves Cognition. Cell 2019, 177, 256–271.e22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Ecsy, K.; Jones, A.; Brown, C. Alpha-range visual and auditory stimulation reduces the perception of pain. Eur. J. Pain 2016, 21, 562–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Ölçücü, M.T.; Yılmaz, K.; Karamık, K.; Okuducu, Y.; Özsoy, Ç.; Aktaş, Y.; Çakır, S.; Ateş, M. Effects of Listening to Binaural Beats on Anxiety Levels and Pain Scores in Male Patients Undergoing Cystoscopy and Ureteral Stent Removal: A Randomized Placebo-Controlled Trial. J. Endourol. 2021, 35, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Maddison, R.; Nazar, H.; Obara, I.; Vuong, Q.C. The efficacy of sensory neural entrainment on acute and chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Pain 2022, 17, 126–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. Foxe, J.J.; Snyder, A.C. The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  165. Garland, E.L. Pain Processing in the Human Nervous System: A Selective Review of Nociceptive and Biobehavioral Pathways. Prim. Care Clin. Off. Pr. 2012, 39, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Furman, A.J.; Meeker, T.J.; Rietschel, J.C.; Yoo, S.; Muthulingam, J.; Prokhorenko, M.; Keaser, M.L.; Goodman, R.N.; Mazaheri, A.; Seminowicz, D.A. Cerebral peak alpha frequency predicts individual differences in pain sensitivity. Neuroimage 2018, 167, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  167. Furman, A.J.; Prokhorenko, M.; Keaser, M.L.; Zhang, J.; Chen, S.; Mazaheri, A.; Seminowicz, D. Sensorimotor Peak Alpha Frequency Is a Reliable Biomarker of Prolonged Pain Sensitivity. Cereb. Cortex 2020, 30, 6069–6082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Pino, O. A randomized controlled trial (RCT) to explore the effect of audio-visual entrainment among psychological disorders. Acta Biomed. 2022, 92, e2021408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Graphical representation depicting the alpha-band neural entrainment mechanisms. (A) An external rhythmic force (i.e., TMS, tACS; here depicted as TMS), delivered in the frequencies of the alpha-band (here depicted as a 10 Hz entrainment), induces a phase reset of the endogenous alpha brain oscillations of the targeted visual areas, with a subsequent phase alignment, an increase in amplitude and synchronization to the external stimulation frequency. Such entrainment effects typically persist after stimulation offset for three alpha cycles (entrainment after-effect; gray shaded area). (B) A sensory stimulus (i.e., visual, auditory, audio-visual; here, a square, representing visual entrainment) streamed in the alpha-band (here depicted as a 10 Hz entrainment) induces a phase reset of the endogenous alpha brain oscillations of the sensory cortices, with a subsequent phase alignment, an increase in amplitude and synchronization to the external stimulation frequency. Such entrainment effects typically persist after the stimulation offset for three alpha cycles (entrainment after-effect; gray shaded area).
Figure 1. Graphical representation depicting the alpha-band neural entrainment mechanisms. (A) An external rhythmic force (i.e., TMS, tACS; here depicted as TMS), delivered in the frequencies of the alpha-band (here depicted as a 10 Hz entrainment), induces a phase reset of the endogenous alpha brain oscillations of the targeted visual areas, with a subsequent phase alignment, an increase in amplitude and synchronization to the external stimulation frequency. Such entrainment effects typically persist after stimulation offset for three alpha cycles (entrainment after-effect; gray shaded area). (B) A sensory stimulus (i.e., visual, auditory, audio-visual; here, a square, representing visual entrainment) streamed in the alpha-band (here depicted as a 10 Hz entrainment) induces a phase reset of the endogenous alpha brain oscillations of the sensory cortices, with a subsequent phase alignment, an increase in amplitude and synchronization to the external stimulation frequency. Such entrainment effects typically persist after the stimulation offset for three alpha cycles (entrainment after-effect; gray shaded area).
Biomedicines 11 01399 g001
Table 1. Report of the main electrophysiological and behavioral findings on alpha-band sensory entrainment.
Table 1. Report of the main electrophysiological and behavioral findings on alpha-band sensory entrainment.
Authors, Year, JournalEntrainment ModalityEntrainment DurationFrequencyVisual Domain/TaskBehavioral MeasuresEntrainment-Induced Effects
Mathewson et al., 2010; Cognition [15]; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.010Unisensory: Visual0.800 s12 HzSpatial/Metacontrast Masking paradigmAccuracy and Signal Detection Theory (d’)Alpha Power increase and phase-locking lasting for ~200 ms. Increased perceptual sensitivity (d’) for the in-phase target.
Mathewson et al., 2012; Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience [58]; https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00288Unisensory: Visual0.576 s12 HzSpatial/Metacontrast Masking paradigmAccuracyAlpha Power increase and phase-locking lasting for ~200 ms. Increased detection accuracy for the in-phase target.
De Graaf et al., 2013; Plos One [38]; https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060035Unisensory: Visual0.516 s10.6 HzSpatial/Cueing paradigm (Visual discrimination task)AccuracyAlpha phase-locking lasting for ~300 ms and improvments in discrimination rates for in-phase targets.
Spaak et al., 2014; Journal of Neuroscience [39]; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4385-13.2014Unisensory: Visual1.5 s10 HzSpatial/Visual detectiont taskAccuracyAlpha power increase lasting for ~300 ms and improvements in detection rates for anti-phase targets.
Kizuk & Mathewson, 2017; Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience [42]; https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01058Unisensory: Visual1 s12 HzSpatial/Metacontrast Masking paradigmAccuracyAlpha Power increase and phase-locking lasting for ~200 ms. Increased detection accuracy for the in-phase target.
Wiesman & Wilson, 2019 [46]; Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience; https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01422Unisensory: Visual1.5 s10 HzSpatial/Adapted version of the arrow-based Erikson “flanker” paradigmRTsIncreased alpha ERS and ITPC lasting for ~550 ms, with reduced discrimination rates for distractor stimuli.
Gray & Emmanouil, 2020;
Psychophysiology [73];
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13480
Unisensory: Visual1 sLower Alpha:
8.3 Hz; Upper Alpha:
12.5 Hz
Temporal/Two-flash fusion taskAccuracy and psychometric thresholdsNo effects of alpha-band visual entrainment on the visual temporal integration/segregation task. Increased alpha power and ITPC during and following alpha entrainment.
De Graaf & Duecker, 2022; European Journal of Neuroscience [72]; https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15483Unisensory: Visual0.717 s10 HzSpatial/Cueing paradigm (Visual discrimination task)RTsNo effects of alpha-band visual entrainment on visual detection performance (RTs).
Ronconi et al., 2016; Psych. Res. [40]; https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0691-8Unisensory: Visual and Auditory2 s10 HzTemporal/Attentional BlinkAccuracyOnly auditory—but not visual—alpha-band entrainment improved visual target detection accuracy, reducing the AB magnitude.
Ronconi et al., 2016; NeuroReport [41]; 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000565Unisensory: Auditory2 s10 HzTemporal/Attentional BlinkAccuracyIncrease in the alpha power during auditory entrainment, associated with improvements in the visual detection of the target.
Kawashima et al., 2022; European Journal of Neuroscience [47]; https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15760Unisensory: Auditory5 s10 HzTemporal/Attentional BlinkAccuracyAuditory alpha-band entrainment improved visual target detection accuracy, reducing the AB magnitude.
Ronconi & Melcher, 2017; J. Neurosc. [44]; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1704-17.2017Multisensory: Audiovisual2 sLower Alpha: ~8.5 Hz; Upper Alpha: ~12 HzTemporal/Integration and Segregation taskAccuracyPhase alignment of the perceptual oscillation, with two different power spectra that peaked toward the entrainment stimulation frequency.
Ronconi et al., 2018; Scientific Reports [45]; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-29671-5Multisensory: Audiovisual2 sIAF − 2 Hz, IAF + 2 HzTemporal/Integration and Segregation taskAccuracyIAF + 2 Hz entrainment improved the segregation performance of visual stimuli, whereas IAF − 2 Hz improved integration. Behavioral oscillations were almost in anti-phase between the two tasks: the highest integration performance corresponded to the lowest segregation performance.
Table 2. Summary of the alterations of the oscillatory parameters in the alpha-band that could represent a valuable target for rhythmic sensory stimulations administered in the alpha-band in different clinical populations. (AD: Alzheimer Disease; MCI: Mild Cognitive Impairment; ASD: Autism Spectrum Disorder; SSD: Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorder.)
Table 2. Summary of the alterations of the oscillatory parameters in the alpha-band that could represent a valuable target for rhythmic sensory stimulations administered in the alpha-band in different clinical populations. (AD: Alzheimer Disease; MCI: Mild Cognitive Impairment; ASD: Autism Spectrum Disorder; SSD: Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorder.)
Cognitive ImpairmentAcquired Posterior Brain LesionPsychiatric Conditions
AD
  • Reduced alpha functional connectivity in bilateral temporal regions at rest [150].
  • Slowdown of the IAF and reduced alpha power all over the scalp at rest [151].
MCI
  • Slowdown of the IAF and reduced alpha power in anterior, temporal and parietal areas at rest [152].
Hemianopia
  • Slowdown of IAF and power imbalance in favor of the intact hemisphere in parieto-occipital regions at rest [120].
  • Reduced alpha desynchronization in anterior, parietal and posterior areas at rest [122].
  • Reduced intra-hemispheric alpha connectivity and reduced alpha short-range connections in the lesioned hemisphere at rest [121].
  • Reduced alpha ERD and functional connectivity in the visual areas of the lesioned hemisphere during tasks [148].
Neglect
  • Slowdown of the IAF in fronto-parieto-occipital regions, alpha power asymmetry in favor of the intact hemisphere in parietal areas, reduced alpha functional connectivity between lesioned fronto-parietal areas and reduced inter-hemispheric alpha connectivity in parietal areas [147].
ASD
  • Reduced alpha power in parieto-occipital areas [127].
  • Altered alpha connectivity at rest in long-scale networks [127,153].
SSD
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gallina, J.; Marsicano, G.; Romei, V.; Bertini, C. Electrophysiological and Behavioral Effects of Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Neural Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 1399. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11051399

AMA Style

Gallina J, Marsicano G, Romei V, Bertini C. Electrophysiological and Behavioral Effects of Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Neural Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Biomedicines. 2023; 11(5):1399. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11051399

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gallina, Jessica, Gianluca Marsicano, Vincenzo Romei, and Caterina Bertini. 2023. "Electrophysiological and Behavioral Effects of Alpha-Band Sensory Entrainment: Neural Mechanisms and Clinical Applications" Biomedicines 11, no. 5: 1399. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11051399

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop