Next Article in Journal
Development of an Evidence-Informed Solution to Emotional Distress in Public Safety Personnel and Healthcare Workers: The Social Support, Tracking Distress, Education, and Discussion CommunitY (STEADY) Program
Previous Article in Journal
Hypericum perforatum and Its Potential Antiplatelet Effect
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of Abdominal Microcurrent in the Consumption and Proportion of Energy Substrates during Aerobic Exercise: A Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Neglected Factor in the Relationship between Executive Functioning and Obesity: The Role of Motor Control

1
Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 80138 Naples, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 81100 Caserta, Italy
3
Neurological Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN “Ospedali dei Colli”, 80131 Naples, Italy
4
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Foggia, 71122 Foggia, Italy
5
Department of Movement Sciences and Wellbeing, University of Naples “Parthenope”, 80133 Naples, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Healthcare 2022, 10(9), 1775; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10091775
Submission received: 30 July 2022 / Revised: 1 September 2022 / Accepted: 12 September 2022 / Published: 15 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Obesity and Metabolic Disease)

Abstract

:
Background: The association between obesity and executive functions (EFs) is highly controversial. It has been suggested that waist circumference (WC), compared to body mass index (BMI), is a better indicator of fat mass and EFs in obesity. Moreover, according to the viewpoint that the brain’s functional architecture meets the need for interactive behavior, we hypothesize that the relationship between EFs and body weight might be mediated by the motor performance. Methods: General executive functioning (frontal assessment battery-15), additional cognitive subdomains (trail making test and digit span backward), and motor performance (finger tapping task) were assessed in a sample that included 330 volunteers (192 females, M age = 45.98 years, SD = 17.70, range = 18–86 years). Results: Hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the FAB15 score and FTT negatively predicted WC but not BMI. A subsequent mediation analysis highlighted that the indirect effect of FAB15 on WC through finger tapping was statistically significant. Conclusions: Our results suggest that WC, as compared to BMI, is a more effective measure for studying the association between EFs and body weight. Still, we found that the motor domain partially mediates the dynamics of such a relationship.

1. Introduction

In recent decades, a growing number of studies have investigated the relationship between obesity and cognition [1,2,3,4]. In particular, special emphasis was given to executive functions (EFs) [5,6]. Traditionally, they are considered a family of top–down cognitive processes (e.g., attention, processing speed, set-shifting, inhibitory control, working memory, concept formation, planning) supporting forethought and goal-directed actions [7,8,9]. In particular, EFs allow individuals to cope adaptively with environmental demands, especially in unfamiliar and conflictual contexts [7,9] through inhibition of dominant responses, blunting interfering stimuli, or resisting temptations [10].
Interestingly, it has been suggested that differences in EFs may predict body weight variability [11]. For instance, decreased EFs have been associated with reduced food intake inhibition [12,13,14], high-fat food ingestion [15], lower appetite regulation [16], decreased energy expenditure [17], increased emotional eating [18], inability to learn from past experiences [19], delayed weight loss [12], poor adherence to healthy nutrition [20,21], and poor outcomes to weight loss interventions [22]. In line with these findings, some studies have found poor executive performance in patients with obesity [11,23,24,25]; furthermore, neuroimaging evidence exists on the relationship between decreased neural activity within the frontal cortex—the main cortical substrate of EFs—and obesity [26,27,28]. This evidence might be supported by studies finding a significant relationship between adipose tissue and systemic inflammation interacting with the physiology of the blood–brain barrier, likely affecting cognitive performance [29,30,31,32,33].
Nevertheless, it is important to stress that conflicting results are also available. Indeed, some studies have reported that obese subjects show equal or better executive performance than normal-weight subjects [34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41]. These latter observations, while counterintuitive, do concur with the phenomenon known as the “obesity paradox” [42,43,44,45], which refers to the possible positive association between obesity and health outcomes, especially in the elderly population.
Reiterating what has been stated in previous research [11,41,46], we argue that the above-mentioned inconsistencies could be attributed to several factors, including the assessment techniques employed to measure obesity, the presence of confounding covariates contaminating the relationship between cognitive performance and obesity, and the inhomogeneous methods used to assess EFs. Furthermore, few studies exploring the relationship between obesity and EFs are longitudinal; therefore, many questions on the direction of such a relationship, i.e., on the causality between obesity and EFs, remain unanswered [47].
Although body mass index (BMI) is currently the most widely used obesity index, it has received several criticisms [48,49]. Among them, BMI does not discriminate between muscle and adipose tissues and cannot directly assess regional adiposity [50]. Moreover, the use of BMI as a measure of obesity results in estimation biases involving obesity-related effects [49]. Alternatively, it has been suggested that waist circumference (WC) should be preferred to BMI as it proved to be a measure more sensitive to visceral obesity. Moreover, WC is a reliable predictor of several health risk factors, such as cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and increased mortality [51,52,53]. Further, it has been suggested that WC is a better indicator than BMI for studying the relationship between obesity and cognitive functions [46].
In addition, many studies did not disclose the role exerted by relevant covariates characterizing the study sample. For instance, obesity is often accompanied by a number of comorbidities, such as high blood pressure, diabetes, and metabolic syndrome, which are clinical conditions that may independently affect cognition [54,55,56]. Still, sociodemographic variables (sex, age, education) are strictly related to cognitive performance and their role is often overlooked [57]. This lack of transparency and methodological rigor makes it unclear whether obesity predisposes to cognitive impairment, or vice versa, because of the possible confounding effects of covariates.
Finally, the relationship between obesity and executive functioning could be mediated by the motor performance, a variable poorly addressed in the neuropsychological literature. Indeed, although some cognitive tasks investigating EFs are based on a verbal response set (e.g., verbal fluency, Stroop test), most executive tests require hand motor responses to be properly performed. Furthermore, it is well-known that motor and executive domains share common neural mechanisms [58,59].
Theoretically speaking, the sensorimotor interaction model by Koziol and Lutz [60,61] may further explain the link between EFs and the motor domain. Although EFs are traditionally considered top–down mental processes, the functional architecture of the brain evolved from childhood to adulthood in order to meet the needs of interactive behavior. This would generate both procedural and declarative knowledge supporting executive functioning [61]. For instance, the fronto-parieto-cerebellar network is involved in the detection of discrepancies between expected and actual sensorimotor feedback. This mechanism has considerable evolutionary relevance, as it may be used to correct future predictions and allows more effective interaction with the environment [62]. Moreover, the above network interacts with basal ganglia to plan and control motor sequences, as well as to predict their resulting outcomes [61,62].
There is some evidence highlighting an inverse relationship between body weight and speed or dexterity during movement execution [63,64,65,66]. Adiposity has been related to the muscle quality ratio that is associated, in turn, to hand strength [67] and motor conduction speed during gross repetitive hand movements [68]. Obese children and adolescents would show reduced motor skills, e.g., fine and gross motor control, and delayed motor development [69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80]. In middle-aged and older adults, higher adiposity levels seem to be correlated with lower motor speed and manual dexterity [81]. More generally, in controlling movements, obese individuals would invest greater attentional resources to compensate for their motor deficits [82,83]. In line with these behavioral observations, individuals with obesity appear to show reduced motor cortex plasticity as compared to normal weight controls [84]. Structural brain mapping studies identified three neural networks strongly related to body weight, i.e., the default mode network, the fronto-executive network, and the fronto-parietal network. Interestingly, recent evidence has suggested that the default mode network’s functional connectivity significantly mediated the relationship between body weight and executive functioning during the execution of the finger tapping, a simple motor task requiring subjects to perform as many finger taps as possible within a given time unit [85].
In line with the above, the current cross-sectional study sought to re-examine the relationship between obesity and EFs. Based on previous research [46], we hypothesize that WC is a more sensitive measure than BMI in capturing executive blunting. Moreover, we hypothesize that the relationship between EFs and body weight could be mediated by gross motor function evaluated by the finger tapping task. In this regard, obesity seems to undermine both motor and frontal/executive abilities [25,26,27,28,46,63,64,65,66]. In addition, in line with the sensorimotor interaction model [60,61], the integrity of the executive processes might depend on bottom-up interactive processes supported by motor behavior. Importantly, the potential confounding effects of sociodemographic covariates were taken into account.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were 365 (212 females) Italian volunteers recruited through a convenience sampling method across different districts of the Campania, Calabria, and Puglia regions (Southern Italy). Eligible participants met the following inclusion criteria: age ≥ 18 years, formal education ≥ 5 years (i.e., primary school), and adjusted score greater than or equal to 23.8 on the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) [86]. According to normative datasets, deficient scores on the administered cognitive tests (see materials and procedure) were considered additional exclusion criteria. Exclusion criteria were: previous or current history of intellectual and/or linguistic deficits, neurocognitive, psychiatric, or psychopathological disorders, and presence of serious health conditions, including cancer and severe obesity. No participant had a history of alcohol or substance abuse/addiction. Participants were not treated with drugs interfering with cognitive processes. Individuals with pharmacologically-compensated chronic medical illnesses, such as hypertension, type II diabetes, or cardiovascular diseases were not excluded to prevent the construction of a hyper-normal sample. According to the exclusion criteria, data from 35 individuals were excluded from the dataset (final N = 330).

2.2. Materials and Procedure

Testing was performed individually in a soundproof room. After sociodemographic data acquisition (e.g., sex, age, and years of formal education), anthropometric measurements, i.e., weight, height, and WC, were measured. BMI values were calculated according to Quetelet’s formula (kg/m2). WC was measured by placing a measuring tape in a horizontal plane around the abdomen, 2 cm above the belly button. Following the administration of MMSE, participants were administered a brief neuropsychological assessment protocol including the frontal assessment battery-15 [87], the trail making test [88], the digit span backward [89], and the finger tapping task [90].
Frontal assessment battery-15 (FAB15). The FAB15 is a screening battery devised to assess general executive/frontal functions. It consists of five subtests exploring abstraction abilities, generativity, planning, sensitivity to interference, and inhibitory control. Three out of the five subtests are based on a motor response set while the remaining ones require a verbal response [87,91]. The administration time is around slightly less than 10 min.
Trail making test (TMT). The TMT is a pencil-and-paper task consisting of two parts, A and B. In the first part (TMT-A), the participant is required to connect, through a pencil, 25 numbered circles in ascending order (i.e., 1–2–3 and so on), while in the second part (TMT-B), the participant is required to connect numbered (from 1 to 13) and lettered circles (from A to N) in alternated numerical vs. alphabetical ascending order (i.e., first a number and then a letter, 1–A–2–B–3–C, and so on). Participants are instructed to connect the circles as quickly as possible without taking the pencil off the paper on which the circles are printed. For both tests, the execution time (in seconds) is recorded. In particular, the time needed to complete the TMT-A is widely used as a measure of processing/psychomotor speed, visual search, and attention skills, whereas the time needed to complete the TMT-B as a measure of set-shifting and inhibition/interference control processes [88]. Overall, the TMT (TMT-A + TMT-B) takes approximately 7–10 min to be administered. In addition to TMT-A and TMT-B scores, we also calculated the B-A difference in order to remove the processing speed component from the performance on part B, thus achieving a measure of mere cognitive flexibility/set-shifting [88,92].
Digit span backward (DSB). The DSB is the most commonly used test in clinical neuropsychology for assessing auditory–verbal working memory (phonological loop and central executive) [93,94]. The examiner pronounces a sequence of digits (e.g., 6, 2, 9) at a rate of approximately one digit per second and the participant is asked to immediately reproduce the sequence of digits in the reverse order (e.g., 9, 2, 6). The test includes a total of six lengths (from 3 to 8 digits) and the span backward corresponds to the length of the longest list correctly repeated [89]. The DSB takes about 3 min to be completed.
Finger tapping task (FTT). This task provides a quantitative assessment of gross upper-extremity motor functions [95] including speed, coordination, and rhythm [96,97]. It is considered an indirect measure of the integrity of the frontal and parietal lobes, basal ganglia, and cerebellum [62,90,98]. To perform this test, participants sat on a comfortable chair in front of a desk where a counting machine was positioned. Participants were asked to place their dominant hand palm down, fingers extended, with the index finger suspended on the screen of the digital counting device. Then, participants were instructed to repetitively tap their index finger on the counting device as quickly as possible for 10 s, being careful to keep the hand and arm stationary. Five finger-tapping trials were recorded, with a 10-s rest interval between each trial in order to decrease the effect of fatigue (average administration time = 90 s). The mean of movements recorded in the five trials (FTTm) was used in the analyses as a measure of movement speed. In the context of the present study, coordination and rhythm were not directly evaluated since we did quantify neither inter-tap intervals nor sensorimotor synchronization processes. Handedness was detected by the Edinburgh handedness inventory-short form [99]. The FTT was administered about 5 min after completion of the cognitive battery.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

After checking the assumptions of linear regression analysis, two four-stage hierarchical multiple regression models were constructed with BMI and WC as the dependent variables, respectively. Sociodemographic variables (i.e., sex, age, and years of education) were entered at stage 1, FAB15 was entered at stage 2, TMT (i.e., TMT-A, TMT-B, TMT(B-A)) and DSB were entered at stage 3, and FTTm was entered at stage 4. This entry order was applied, at first, to control for the confounding effects of sociodemographic variables. Then, we tested the contribution of the FAB15 score as a measure of general executive functioning. Finally, we loaded the FAB15 with additional core executive subdomains that are poorly addressed by the FAB15, i.e., set-shifting (TMT) and working memory (DSB). Lastly, FTTm entered the last block in order to assess the contribution of motor performance.
To investigate if and how motor performance mediated the relationship between executive functioning and body weight, a mediation analysis was run based on the results of the multiple regression analyses. To evaluate the significance of direct and indirect effects, bootstrap-based confidence intervals (95% CI) were constructed [100]. Statistical analyses were conducted by means of IBM SPSS Statistics v. 26. The SPSS Macro PROCESS was employed to perform the mediation analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Data from 330 right-handed volunteers (192 females) were analyzed. Mean age was 45.98 years (SD = 17.70, range = 18–86 years) while mean education was 15.67 years (SD = 3.85, range = 5–24 years). As for anthropometric variables, mean BMI was 24.67 (SD = 3.98, range = 18.73–39.51) and mean WC was 83.35 cm (SD = 13.77, range = 58–120 cm). There were no identified differences in age, years of education or BMI between males and females (all p-values > 0.05); conversely, males had a larger WC compared to females (males, mean WC = 88.45, SD = 12.45; females, mean WC = 79.91, SD = 13.57; F(1, 328) = 32.779, p < 0.001). Descriptive statistics on cognitive tests and FTT are summarized in Table 1.

3.2. Construction of Hierarchical Regression Models

For both models, relevant statistical assumptions of linear regression were tested and satisfied, i.e., the relationship between each predictor and the outcome variable was linear, no violations of the homoscedasticity principle were detected, and standardized residuals showed no significant deviation from normality based on inspection of both P–P plots and histograms. A sample size of 330 was considered adequate based on a priori power analysis (α = 0.05, 1–β = 0.80, f2 = 0.15, maximum number of predictors = 9, required sample size according to G*Power 3.1.9.4 = 114). Furthermore, we computed the variance inflation factors (VIFs) and tolerance values for each predictor in order to determine whether and how much the variance of regression coefficients could be inflated. The following rules were employed to interpret the VIF values: VIF = 1, no collinearity; VIF = 1 to 5, moderate collinearity; VIF > 5, high collinearity. Tolerance values equal to or greater than 0.10 were deemed acceptable, indicating the absence of statistically significant multicollinearity [101,102]. Unsurprisingly, when entered simultaneously into regression models, TMT-A, TMT-B, and TMT (B-A) demonstrated severe multicollinearity (VIF values > 350, tolerance values ≤ 0.003). Therefore, we decided to include only the TMT (B-A) score since it is considered a refined measure of executive control and cognitive flexibility/set-shifting [88]. R2 and R2 changes at each step in the two hierarchical regression models are reported in Table 2. The results of regression analyses are summarized in Table 3. VIF and tolerance values for the included predictors at each stage are also reported in Table 3.

3.3. Regression on BMI

At stage 1, when sociodemographic variables entered the regression model as predictors, a significant predictive capability (F(3, 326) = 21.704, p < 0.001) was found; the model accounted for a significant amount of BMI variance (R2 = 0.22). In the following stages, the addition of the FAB15 (stage 2: F(4, 325) = 16.498, p < 0.001), TMT (B-A), SDB (stage 3: F(6, 323) = 11.122, p < 0.001), and FTTm (stage 4: F(7, 322) = 9.929, p < 0.001) did not lead to a significant increase in the variance explained (stage 2: R2 change = 0.003, p = 0.34; stage 3: R2 change = 0.004, p = 0.36; stage 4: R2 change = 0.008, p = 0.06). All regression models were statistically significant for the sole contribution of sociodemographic variables.

3.4. Regression on WC

The hierarchical multiple linear regression revealed that, at stage 1, sociodemographic variables contributed significantly in predicting WC (F(3, 326) = 54.895, p < 0.001) and explained a large proportion of its variance (R2 = 0.41). Adding the FAB15 score to the regression model (F(4, 325) = 44.363, p < 0.001) explained an additional 0.02% of the variance in WC (stage 2: R2 change, F(1, 325) = 7.967, p < 0.001). The addition of TMT(B-A) and SDB (F(6, 323) = 30.151, p < 0.001) makes a negligible non-significant contribution to the regression model (stage 3: R2 change = 0.007, F(2, 323) = 1.417, p = 0.24). Conversely, introducing the FTTm variable at stage 4 (F(7, 322) = 28.353, p < 0.001), R2 significantly increased by 0.02 (F(1, 322) = 10.378, p < 0.001). The final model, including all seven predictors, accounted for approximately 46% of the variance in WC. Neither TMT (B-A) (B = −0.01, p = 0.62) nor DSB (B = −0.84, p = 0.09) were significant predictors of WC.

3.5. Mediation Analysis

Based on the results from the regression analyses, a mediation model was constructed to test the mediating role of motor performance (FTTm) in the relationship between general executive functioning (FAB15) and body fat (WC) (see Figure 1). Results indicated that FAB15 score was a significant predictor of FTTm (a = 2.282, 95% CI [1.392, 3.171], SE = 0.452, t = 5.409, p < 0.001). Furthermore, FTTm negatively predicted WC (b = −0.295, 95% CI [−0.422, −0.169], SE = 0.064, t = −4.600, p < 0.001). The indirect effect (ab) of FAB15 on WC through FTTm was statistically significant (ab = −0.674, 95% CI [−1.039, −0.332], SE = 0.181, ZSobel = −3.364, p < 0.001). Since the estimated direct effect (controlled for the mediator) of FAB15 on WC was still significant (c′ = −2.198, 95% CI [−3.197, −1–200], SE = 0.507, t = −4.335, p < 0.001), motor performance partially mediated the relationship between general executive functioning and body fat. Specifically, FAB15 showed an indirect negative effect on WC partially conveyed by its positive association with FTTm. Thus, a higher executive functioning predicted a decrease in WC explained, in part, by the positive linear relationship between EFs and motor performance.

4. Discussion

In the present cross-sectional study, we investigated the relationship between EFs, motor skills, and body weight in a sample of healthy individuals from the general population. In particular, we firstly tested the hypothesis that EFs better explained the variance of WC than BMI. Subsequently, we explored the mediating role of motor skills in the relationship between EFs and body weight. In line with our expectations, we found that WC is a more effective measure for studying the association between EFs and body weight as compared to BMI. Still, we found that the motor domain played a non-negligible role in the dynamics of such a relationship. In a scientific context in which the link between EFs and obesity needs to be further investigated [11,16,23,24,25,103,104], these findings may contribute to extending the debate on the matter.
Variability in WC (visceral body fat) was explained by both general executive functioning (FAB15) and motor skills (FTT). More specifically, our results showed that WC displayed an inverse relationship with both the FAB15 score, which covers different executive domains, such as abstraction abilities, generativity, planning, susceptibility to interference, and inhibitory control [87], and the FTTm score, a measure of upper limb gross motor function [95]. In addition, results from the mediation analysis highlighted that FTTm score partially mediated the relationship between general executive functioning and visceral body fat. The indirect effect of FAB15 on WC through FTTm is likely the result of the activity of different brain regions involving frontal and parietal lobes, basal ganglia, and cerebellum [62,90,98]. According to the sensorimotor interaction model of behavior [60], sensorimotor integration processes guarantee the development of action control abilities via interaction with the environment, which enhances, in turn, the maturation and efficiency of cognitive processes [61]. In line with this vein, noteworthy is the role of the cerebellum that assists in the adjustment and automation of movements according to environmental demands and contextual changes for promoting adaptive behaviors. It is worthy of equal interest the anatomo-functional interaction between the occipito-temporal cortex and the reward centers in the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex. The activity of this network contributes to evaluating the reward value of an object and anticipating/predicting reward-related expectancies and outcomes of the action [61]. In line with these claims, it might be misleading to ascribe EFs to the sole prefrontal cortex. Indeed, EFs are also related to sensorimotor circuits responsible for processing sensory information, associating this information with reward values, and programming and executing appropriate actions.
Neuroimaging evidence has suggested that obese subjects show decreased cortical thickness in the superior frontal gyrus and medial orbitofrontal cortex; in these patients, the volumes of the ventral diencephalon, brainstem [105], and cerebellum [106] also appear to be reduced. Moreover, obese patients would show decreased neural plasticity within the frontal cortex [84]. These findings tend to suggest that obese patients may show reduced sensorimotor integration processes, which are strictly related to visuomotor and cognitive processes.
The visuomotor system uses spatial representations generated within the fronto-parieto-cerebellar network, in which the occipitoparietal/dorsal stream and cerebellum play a crucial role [107,108,109,110,111]. The dorsal stream, combining retinotopic and spatiotopic/body-centered coordinates, supports the planning and execution of real-time visually-guided actions [112,113,114,115,116,117,118]. The cerebellum is responsible for the muscular coordination of the distal portions of the limbs, with particular reference to hands and digits. It guarantees harmonious progression and timing of every single movement during the execution of coordinated motor sequences. The cerebellum is the structural core of the feedforward network involved in the predictive phase of the action [119,120,121]. Before starting a movement, the cerebellum receives a copy of the motor command from the frontal cortex and sensory inputs from the parietal cortex [122]. Based on sensory feedback (i.e., vestibular and proprioceptive signals) from the distal body parts, the cerebellum executes a kind of “quality control” and can send rapid corrective signals to the neocortex and spinal cord [62]. Importantly, the cerebellum acts in concert with the nigrostriatal pathway including the substantia nigra pars compacta and the dorsal striatum. The substantia nigra pars compacta supply dopamine to the striatum [123], which enhances—via the frontal cortex—adaptive motor programs and higher cognitive control of hand movements [62,124].
To further disentangle the relationship between visceral body fat and cognition, the functional interaction between body weight and brain physiology needs to be taken into account. There is speculation in the literature on the role exerted by inflammatory processes.
Higher levels of white adipose tissue might increase systemic inflammation. It is widely acknowledged that adipocytes, lymphocytes, and macrophages lead to the production of pro-inflammatory cytokines [125,126,127,128,129,130,131]. In addition, a diet high in fatty acids and sugar may undermine the integrity of the blood–brain barrier [29], which plays a key role in protecting the central nervous system from bloodborne toxins.
Some studies have shown that central inflammation is often observed after high-fat feeding, especially in the hypothalamus region [2,132,133,134,135], and might predict cognitive decline and/or the onset of major neurocognitive disorders [30,31,32,33]. Interestingly, adipose tissue may modify β-amyloid metabolism [136,137]. Indeed, some neuropathological features of Alzheimer’s disease, such as amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, appear to be most observed in elderly obese people as compared to normal-weight older adults [138]. Finally, a relationship between midlife obesity, blood–brain barrier dysfunction, and primary and/or secondary dementias, has been hypothesized [28,139].
The main limitation of the current study is its cross-sectional nature, which does not allow to draw conclusions about causality.

5. Conclusions

Our results highlight a relationship between general executive functioning and visceral body fat, with the gross motor function, which partially mediates such a relationship. The indirect effects of EFs on body fat through motor function may be explained by shared neural substrates mainly embracing the fronto-parieto-cerebellar network. Since most of the tasks assessing EFs in both clinical and experimental practices require a motor response to be performed, future studies could further investigate executive performance in the obese population by administering both motor- and non-motor-based executive tasks.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L.M., A.M., C.R.I. and I.V.; methodology, M.L.M. and C.R.I.; software, C.R.I.; validation, A.M. and I.V.; formal analysis, M.L.M. and C.R.I.; investigation, M.L.M., C.R.I., G.V., N.E., S.T., A.O. and R.A.; resources, A.M., G.D.M., I.V. and V.M.; data curation, C.R.I., G.V., N.E., S.T. and G.D.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L.M., A.M., C.R.I. and I.V.; writing—review and editing, C.R.I., G.V., A.O., G.D.M. and I.V.; visualization, N.E., S.T., A.O. and R.A.; supervision, M.L.M., C.R.I., I.V., G.M. and V.M.; project administration, M.L.M., G.M., I.V. and V.M.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank Alessandro Iavarone and Sergio Chieffi because they taught us that “There is nothing more cognitive than movement”.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Benito-León, J.; Mitchell, A.J.; Hernández-Gallego, J.; Bermejo-Pareja, F. Obesity and Impaired Cognitive Functioning in the Elderly: A Population-Based Cross-Sectional Study (NEDICES). Eur. J. Neurol. 2013, 20, 899-e77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Miller, A.A.; Spencer, S.J. Obesity and Neuroinflammation: A Pathway to Cognitive Impairment. Brain Behav. Immun. 2014, 42, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Prickett, C.; Brennan, L.; Stolwyk, R. Examining the Relationship between Obesity and Cognitive Function: A Systematic Literature Review. Obes. Res. Clin. Pract. 2015, 9, 93–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bove, R.M.; Gerweck, A.V.; Mancuso, S.M.; Bredella, M.A.; Sherman, J.C.; Miller, K.K. Association between Adiposity and Cognitive Function in Young Men: Hormonal Mechanisms: Obesity, Hormones, and Cognition in Men. Obesity 2016, 24, 954–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sellaro, R.; Colzato, L.S. High Body Mass Index Is Associated with Impaired Cognitive Control. Appetite 2017, 113, 301–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Smith, E.; Hay, P.; Campbell, L.; Trollor, J.N. A Review of the Association between Obesity and Cognitive Function across the Lifespan: Implications for Novel Approaches to Prevention and Treatment: Obesity and Cognitive Function across Lifespan. Obes. Rev. 2011, 12, 740–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Burgess, P.W.; Simons, J.S. Theories of Frontal Lobe Executive Function: Clinical Applications. In The Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits; Halligan, P.W., Wade, D.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2005; pp. 211–231. ISBN 978-0-19-852654-4. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gilbert, S.J.; Burgess, P.W. Executive Function. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R110–R114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ardila, A. On the Evolutionary Origins of Executive Functions. Brain Cogn. 2008, 68, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Yang, Y.; Shields, G.S.; Guo, C.; Liu, Y. Executive Function Performance in Obesity and Overweight Individuals: A Meta-Analysis and Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dassen, F.C.M.; Houben, K.; Allom, V.; Jansen, A. Self-Regulation and Obesity: The Role of Executive Function and Delay Discounting in the Prediction of Weight Loss. J. Behav. Med. 2018, 41, 806–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lyke, J.A.; Spinella, M. Associations among Aspects of Impulsivity and Eating Factors in a Nonclinical Sample. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2004, 36, 229–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Steward, T.; Mestre-Bach, G.; Vintró-Alcaraz, C.; Lozano-Madrid, M.; Agüera, Z.; Fernández-Formoso, J.A.; Granero, R.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Vilarrasa, N.; García-Ruiz-de-Gordejuela, A.; et al. Food Addiction and Impaired Executive Functions in Women with Obesity. Eur. Eat. Disorders. Rev. 2018, 26, 574–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Powell, D.J.H.; McMinn, D.; Allan, J.L. Does Real Time Variability in Inhibitory Control Drive Snacking Behavior? An Intensive Longitudinal Study. Health Psychol. 2017, 36, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gluck, M.E.; Viswanath, P.; Stinson, E.J. Obesity, Appetite, and the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2017, 6, 380–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Riggs, N.; Chou, C.-P.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Pentz, M.A. Executive Cognitive Function as a Correlate and Predictor of Child Food Intake and Physical Activity. Child Neuropsychol. 2010, 16, 279–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sims, R.C.; Bennett, N.-K.; Mwendwa, D.T.; Ali, M.K.; Levy, S.-A.T.; Callender, C.O.; Campbell, A.L. Executive Function and Negative Eating Behaviors in Severely Obese African Americans. Ethn. Dis. 2014, 24, 328–334. [Google Scholar]
  19. Rodrigue, C.; Ouellette, A.-S.; Lemieux, S.; Tchernof, A.; Biertho, L.; Bégin, C. Executive Functioning and Psychological Symptoms in Food Addiction: A Study among Individuals with Severe Obesity. Eat. Weight. Disord 2018, 23, 469–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Beth Spitznagel, M.; Galioto, R.; Limbach, K.; Gunstad, J.; Heinberg, L. Cognitive Function Is Linked to Adherence to Bariatric Postoperative Guidelines. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 2013, 9, 580–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. La Marra, M.; Caviglia, G.; Perrella, R. Using Smartphones When Eating Increases Caloric Intake in Young People: An Overview of the Literature. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 587886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Witbracht, M.G.; Laugero, K.D.; Van Loan, M.D.; Adams, S.H.; Keim, N.L. Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task Is Related to Magnitude of Weight Loss and Salivary Cortisol in a Diet-Induced Weight Loss Intervention in Overweight Women. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 106, 291–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Fitzpatrick, S.; Gilbert, S.; Serpell, L. Systematic Review: Are Overweight and Obese Individuals Impaired on Behavioural Tasks of Executive Functioning? Neuropsychol. Rev. 2013, 23, 138–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gettens, K.M.; Gorin, A.A. Executive Function in Weight Loss and Weight Loss Maintenance: A Conceptual Review and Novel Neuropsychological Model of Weight Control. J. Behav. Med. 2017, 40, 687–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. La Marra, M.; Ilardi, C.R.; Villano, I.; Carosella, M.; Staiano, M.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Polito, R.; Scarinci, A.; et al. Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. O’Brien, P.D.; Hinder, L.M.; Callaghan, B.C.; Feldman, E.L. Neurological Consequences of Obesity. Lancet Neurol. 2017, 16, 465–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ho, A.J.; Raji, C.A.; Becker, J.T.; Lopez, O.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Hua, X.; Lee, S.; Hibar, D.; Dinov, I.D.; Stein, J.L.; et al. Obesity Is 442 Linked with Lower Brain Volume in 700 AD and MCI Patients. Neurobiol. Aging 2010, 31, 1326–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Gustafson, D.; Lissner, L.; Bengtsson, C.; Bjorkelund, C.; Skoog, I. A 24-Year Follow-up of Body Mass Index and Cerebral Atro- 445 phy. Neurology 2004, 63, 1876–1881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Nguyen, J.C.D.; Killcross, A.S.; Jenkins, T.A. Obesity and Cognitive Decline: Role of Inflammation and Vascular Changes. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Odegaard, J.I.; Chawla, A. Pleiotropic Actions of Insulin Resistance and Inflammation in Metabolic Homeostasis. Science 2013, 339, 172–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bruun, J.M.; Pedersen, S.B.; Kristensen, K.; Richelsen, B. Effects of Pro-Inflammatory Cytokines and Chemokines on Leptin Production in Human Adipose Tissue in Vitro. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 2002, 190, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Trayhurn, P.; Wood, I.S. Adipokines: Inflammation and the Pleiotropic Role of White Adipose Tissue. Br. J. Nutr. 2004, 92, 347–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Pradhan, A.D. C-Reactive Protein, Interleukin 6, and Risk of Developing Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. JAMA 2001, 286, 327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Boeka, A.; Lokken, K. Neuropsychological Performance of a Clinical Sample of Extremely Obese Individuals. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2008, 23, 467–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gunstad, J.; Lhotsky, A.; Wendell, C.R.; Ferrucci, L.; Zonderman, A.B. Longitudinal Examination of Obesity and Cognitive Function: Results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuroepidemiology 2010, 34, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hartanto, A.; Yong, J.; Toh, W. Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Skinner, J.S.; Abel, W.M.; McCoy, K.; Wilkins, C.H. Exploring the “Obesity Paradox” as a Correlate of Cognitive and Physical Function in Community-Dwelling Black and White Older Adults. Ethn. Dis. 2017, 27, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.-J.; Fowler, J.S.; Telang, F. Overlapping Neuronal Circuits in Addiction and Obesity: Evidence of Systems Pathology. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2008, 363, 3191–3200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Monica, D.; Paulo, M.; Appolinário, J.C.; de Freitas, S.R.; Coutinho, G.; Santos, C.; Coutinho, W. Assessment of Executive Functions in Obese Individuals with Binge Eating Disorder. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2010, 32, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Casagrande, M. The Executive Functions in Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Studies. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. La Marra, M.; Villano, I.; Ilardi, C.R.; Carosella, M.; Staiano, M.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Polito, R.; Porro, C.; et al. Executive Functions in Overweight and Obese Treatment-Seeking Patients: Cross-Sectional Data and Longitudinal Perspectives. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Artham, S.M.; Lavie, C.J.; Milani, R.V.; Ventura, H.O. The Obesity Paradox: Impact of Obesity on the Prevalence and Prognosis of Cardiovascular Diseases. Postgrad. Med. 2008, 120, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Childers, D.K.; Allison, D.B. The ‘Obesity Paradox’: A Parsimonious Explanation for Relations among Obesity, Mortality Rate and Aging? Int. J. Obes. 2010, 34, 1231–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Flegal, K.M.; Kit, B.K.; Orpana, H.; Graubard, B.I. Association of All-Cause Mortality With Overweight and Obesity Using Standard Body Mass Index Categories: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA 2013, 309, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Monda, V.; La Marra, M.; Perrella, R.; Caviglia, G.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Carotenuto, M.; Monda, M.; Messina, A. Obesity and Brain Illness: From Cognitive and Psychological Evidences to Obesity Paradox. DMSO 2017, 10, 473–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Hartanto, A.; Yong, J.C. Measurement Matters: Higher Waist-to-Hip Ratio but Not Body Mass Index Is Associated with Deficits in Executive Functions and Episodic Memory. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Reinert, K.R.S.; Po’e, E.K.; Barkin, S.L. The Relationship between Executive Function and Obesity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Literature Review. J. Obes. 2013, 2013, 820956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Gallagher, D.; Visser, M.; Sepulveda, D.; Pierson, R.N.; Harris, T.; Heymsfield, S.B. How Useful Is Body Mass Index for Comparison of Body Fatness across Age, Sex, and Ethnic Groups? Am. J. Epidemiol. 1996, 143, 228–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Rothman, K.J. BMI-Related Errors in the Measurement of Obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, S56–S59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Stevens, J.; McClain, J.E.; Truesdale, K.P. Selection of Measures in Epidemiologic Studies of the Consequences of Obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, S60–S66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Janssen, I.; Katzmarzyk, P.T.; Ross, R. Waist Circumference and Not Body Mass Index Explains Obesity-Related Health Risk. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 379–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Price, G.M.; Uauy, R.; Breeze, E.; Bulpitt, C.J.; Fletcher, A.E. Weight, Shape, and Mortality Risk in Older Persons: Elevated Waist-Hip Ratio, Not High Body Mass Index, Is Associated with a Greater Risk of Death. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 84, 449–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Zhu, S.; Heymsfield, S.B.; Toyoshima, H.; Wang, Z.; Pietrobelli, A.; Heshka, S. Race-Ethnicity–Specific Waist Circumference Cutoffs for Identifying Cardiovascular Disease Risk Factors. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 81, 409–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yoshitake, T.; Kiyohara, Y.; Kato, I.; Ohmura, T.; Iwamoto, H.; Nakayama, K.; Ohmori, S.; Nomiyama, K.; Kawano, H.; Ueda, K.; et al. Incidence and Risk Factors of Vascular Dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease in a Defined Elderly Japanese Population: The Hisayama Study. Neurology 1995, 45, 1161–1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Segura, B.; Jurado, M.Á.; Freixenet, N.; Albuin, C.; Muniesa, J.; Junqué, C. Mental Slowness and Executive Dysfunctions in Patients with Metabolic Syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 462, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Dye, L.; Boyle, N.B.; Champ, C.; Lawton, C. The Relationship between Obesity and Cognitive Health and Decline. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2017, 76, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Ilardi, C.R.; Garofalo, E.; Chieffi, S.; Gamboz, N.; La Marra, M.; Iavarone, A. Daily Exposure to Digital Displays May Affect the Clock-Drawing Test: From Psychometrics to Serendipity. Neurol. Sci. 2020, 41, 3683–3690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Rycroft, S.S.; Quach, L.T.; Ward, R.E.; Pedersen, M.M.; Grande, L.; Bean, J.F. The Relationship Between Cognitive Impairment and Upper Extremity Function in Older Primary Care Patients. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 2019, 74, 568–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Monda, V.; Valenzano, A.; Moscatelli, F.; Salerno, M.; Sessa, F.; Triggiani, A.I.; Viggiano, A.; Capranica, L.; Marsala, G.; De Luca, V.; et al. Primary Motor Cortex Excitability in Karate Athletes: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Koziol, L.F.; Budding, D.E.; Chidekel, D. From Movement to Thought: Executive Function, Embodied Cognition, and the Cerebellum. Cerebellum 2012, 11, 505–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Koziol, L.F.; Lutz, J.T. From Movement to Thought: The Development of Executive Function. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2013, 2, 104–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Ilardi, C.R.; Iavarone, A.; La Marra, M.; Iachini, T.; Chieffi, S. Hand Movements in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Clinical Implications and Insights for Future Research. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 21, 067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Slining, M.; Adair, L.S.; Goldman, B.D.; Borja, J.B.; Bentley, M. Infant Overweight Is Associated with Delayed Motor Development. J. Pediatrics 2010, 157, 20–25.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Kemp, C.; Pienaar, A.E. Relationship between the Body Composition and Motor and Physical Competence of Grade 1 Learners in South Africa. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 2013, 53, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  65. D’Hondt, E.; Segers, V.; Deforche, B.; Shultz, S.P.; Tanghe, A.; Gentier, I.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; De Clercq, D.; Lenoir, M. The Role of Vision in Obese and Normal-Weight Children’s Gait Control. Gait Posture 2011, 33, 179–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. D’Hondt, E.; Deforche, B.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Lenoir, M. Childhood Obesity Affects Fine Motor Skill Performance under Different Postural Constraints. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 440, 72–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Mehta, R.K.; Shortz, A.E. Obesity-Related Differences in Neural Correlates of Force Control. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2014, 114, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Moore, A.Z.; Caturegli, G.; Metter, E.J.; Makrogiannis, S.; Resnick, S.M.; Harris, T.B.; Ferrucci, L. Difference in Muscle Quality over the Adult Life Span and Biological Correlates in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2014, 62, 230–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Southall, J.E.; Okely, A.D.; Steele, J.R. Actual and Perceived Physical Competence in Overweight and Nonoverweight Children. Pediatric Exerc. Sci. 2004, 16, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Poulsen, A.A.; Desha, L.; Ziviani, J.; Griffiths, L.; Heaslop, A.; Khan, A.; Leong, G.M. Fundamental Movement Skills and Self-Concept of Children Who Are Overweight. Int. J. Pediatric Obes. 2011, 6, e464–e471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Roberts, D.; Veneri, D.; Decker, R.; Gannotti, M. Weight Status and Gross Motor Skill in Kindergarten Children. Pediatric Phys. Ther. 2012, 24, 353–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Mond, J.M.; Stich, H.; Hay, P.J.; Kraemer, A.; Baune, B.T. Associations between Obesity and Developmental Functioning in Pre-School Children: A Population-Based Study. Int. J. Obes. 2007, 31, 1068–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Krombholz, H. Motor and Cognitive Performance of Overweight Preschool Children. Percept. Mot. Skills 2013, 116, 40–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Gentier, I.; D’Hondt, E.; Shultz, S.; Deforche, B.; Augustijn, M.; Hoorne, S.; Verlaecke, K.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Lenoir, M. Fine and Gross Motor Skills Differ between Healthy-Weight and Obese Children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 4043–4051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Cawley, J.; Spiess, C.K. Obesity and Skill Attainment in Early Childhood. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2008, 6, 388–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Vanden Bosch, M.L.; Robbins, L.B.; Pfeiffer, K.A.; Kazanis, A.S.; Maier, K.S. Demographic, Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Variables Associated with Overweight and Obesity in Low-Active Girls. J. Pediatric Nurs. 2014, 29, 576–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Bryant, E.S.; Duncan, M.J.; Birch, S.L. Fundamental Movement Skills and Weight Status in British Primary School Children. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2014, 14, 730–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Cliff, D.P.; Okely, A.D.; Morgan, P.J.; Jones, R.A.; Steele, J.R.; Baur, L.A. Proficiency Deficiency: Mastery of Fundamental Movement Skills and Skill Components in Overweight and Obese Children. Obesity 2012, 20, 1024–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Precenzano, F.; Ruberto, M. Sleep Habits in Children Affected by Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Preliminary Case-Control Study. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2017, 33, 405–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Okely, A.D.; Booth, M.L.; Chey, T. Relationships between Body Composition and Fundamental Movement Skills among Children and Adolescents. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2004, 75, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Waldstein, S.R.; Katzel, L.I. Interactive Relations of Central versus Total Obesity and Blood Pressure to Cognitive Function. Int. J. Obes. 2006, 30, 201–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Mignardot, J.-B.; Olivier, I.; Promayon, E.; Nougier, V. Obesity Impact on the Attentional Cost for Controlling Posture. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e14387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Wang, C.; Chan, J.S.Y.; Ren, L.; Yan, J.H. Obesity Reduces Cognitive and Motor Functions across the Lifespan. Neural Plast. 2016, 2016, 2473081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Sui, S.X.; Ridding, M.C.; Hordacre, B. Obesity Is Associated with Reduced Plasticity of the Human Motor Cortex. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Hsu, C.L.; Voss, M.W.; Best, J.R.; Handy, T.C.; Madden, K.; Bolandzadeh, N.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Elevated Body Mass Index and Maintenance of Cognitive Function in Late Life: Exploring Underlying Neural Mechanisms. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Measso, G.; Cavarzeran, F.; Zappalà, G.; Lebowitz, B.D.; Crook, T.H.; Pirozzolo, F.J.; Amaducci, L.A.; Massari, D.; Grigoletto, F. The Mini-mental State Examination: Normative Study of an Italian Random Sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1993, 9, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Ilardi, C.R.; Chieffi, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gamboz, N.; Galeone, F.; Sannino, M.; Garofalo, E.; La Marra, M.; Ronga, B.; Iavarone, A. The Frontal Assessment Battery 20 Years Later: Normative Data for a Shortened Version (FAB15). Neurol. Sci. 2022, 43, 1709–1719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Siciliano, M.; Chiorri, C.; Battini, V.; Sant’Elia, V.; Altieri, M.; Trojano, L.; Santangelo, G. Regression-Based Normative Data and Equivalent Scores for Trail Making Test (TMT): An Updated Italian Normative Study. Neurol. Sci. 2019, 40, 469–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Monaco, M.; Costa, A.; Caltagirone, C.; Carlesimo, G.A. Forward and Backward Span for Verbal and Visuo-Spatial Data: Standardization and Normative Data from an Italian Adult Population. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 34, 749–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Schatz, P. Finger Tapping Test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Kreutzer, J.S., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 1050–1051. ISBN 978-0-387-79947-6. [Google Scholar]
  91. Dubois, B.; Slachevsky, A.; Litvan, I.; Pillon, B. The FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at Bedside. Neurology 2000, 55, 1621–1626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Jacobson, S.C.; Blanchard, M.; Connolly, C.C.; Cannon, M.; Garavan, H. An FMRI Investigation of a Novel Analogue to the Trail-Making Test. Brain Cogn. 2011, 77, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Baddeley, A. Working Memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Baddeley, A. Exploring the Central Executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1996, 49, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Kluger, A.; Gianutsos, J.G.; Golomb, J.; Ferris, S.H.; George, A.E.; Franssen, E.; Reisberg, B. Patterns of Motor Impairment in Normal Aging, Mild Cognitive Decline, and Early Alzheimer’ Disease. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 1997, 52, P28–P39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Jobbágy, Á.; Harcos, P.; Karoly, R.; Fazekas, G. Analysis of Finger-Tapping Movement. J. Neurosci. Methods 2005, 141, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Suzumura, S.; Osawa, A.; Nagahama, T.; Kondo, I.; Sano, Y.; Kandori, A. Assessment of Finger Motor Skills in Individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease: Relationship between Finger-to-Thumb Tapping and Cognitive Function. JJCRS 2016, 7, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Witt, S.T.; Laird, A.R.; Meyerand, M.E. Functional Neuroimaging Correlates of Finger-Tapping Task Variations: An ALE Meta-Analysis. NeuroImage 2008, 42, 343–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Veale, J.F. Edinburgh Handedness Inventory—Short Form: A Revised Version Based on Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2014, 19, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Preacher, K.J.; Hayes, A.F. Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediator Models. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 879–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Daoud, J.I. Multicollinearity and Regression Analysis. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2017, 949, 012009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Ilardi, A.; Chieffi, S.; Ilardi, C.R. Predictive Role of Population Density and Use of Public Transport for Major Outcomes of SARS-CoV-2 Infection in the Italian Population: An Ecological Study. J. Res. Health Sci. 2021, 21, e00518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Vainik, U.; Dagher, A.; Dubé, L.; Fellows, L.K. Neurobehavioural Correlates of Body Mass Index and Eating Behaviours in Adults: A Systematic Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 279–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  104. Emery, R.L.; Levine, M.D. Questionnaire and Behavioral Task Measures of Impulsivity Are Differentially Associated with Body Mass Index: A Comprehensive Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 868–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Marqués-Iturria, I.; Pueyo, R.; Garolera, M.; Segura, B.; Junqué, C.; García-García, I.; José Sender-Palacios, M.; Vernet-Vernet, M.; Narberhaus, A.; Ariza, M.; et al. Frontal Cortical Thinning and Subcortical Volume Reductions in Early Adulthood Obesity. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2013, 214, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Mueller, K.; Sacher, J.; Arelin, K.; Holiga, Š.; Kratzsch, J.; Villringer, A.; Schroeter, M.L. Overweight and Obesity Are Associated with Neuronal Injury in the Human Cerebellum and Hippocampus in Young Adults: A Combined MRI, Serum Marker and Gene Expression Study. Transl. Psychiatry 2012, 2, e200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Gaul, D.; Mat, A.; O’Shea, D.; Issartel, J. Impaired Visual Motor Coordination in Obese Adults. J. Obes. 2016, 2016, 6178575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Moscatelli, F.; Messina, G.; Valenzano, A.; Petito, A.; Triggiani, A.I.; Ciliberti, M.A.P.; Monda, V.; Messina, A.; Tafuri, D.; Capranica, L.; et al. Relationship between RPE and Blood Lactate after Fatiguing Handgrip Exercise in Taekwondo and Sedentary Subjects. Biol. Med. 2015, 1, S3008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Milner, A.D.; Goodale, M.A. Two Visual Systems Re-Viewed. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 774–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Committeri, G.; Galati, G.; Paradis, A.-L.; Pizzamiglio, L.; Berthoz, A.; LeBihan, D. Reference Frames for Spatial Cognition: Different Brain Areas Are Involved in Viewer-, Object-, and Landmark-Centered Judgments About Object Location. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1517–1535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Zaehle, T.; Jordan, K.; Wüstenberg, T.; Baudewig, J.; Dechent, P.; Mast, F.W. The Neural Basis of the Egocentric and Allocentric Spatial Frame of Reference. Brain Res. 2007, 1137, 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Villano, I.; Messina, A.; Ilardi, C.R.; Monda, M.; Salerno, M.; Sessa, F.; Mollica, M.P.; Cavaliere, G.; et al. Hemispheric Asymmetries in Radial Line Bisection: Role of Retinotopic and Spatiotopic Factors. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Chieffi, S.; Castaldi, C.; Di Maio, G.; La Marra, M.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Villano, I. Attentional Bias in the Radial and Vertical Dimensions of Space. Comptes Rendus. Biol. 2019, 342, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; La Marra, M.; Iavarone, A.; Viggiano, A.; De Luca, V.; Monda, M. Distractor interference in visual motor tasks. Hor. Neurosci. Res. 2014, 13, 151–160. [Google Scholar]
  115. Ilardi, C.R.; Iavarone, A.; Villano, I.; Rapuano, M.; Ruggiero, G.; Iachini, T.; Chieffi, S. Egocentric and Allocentric Spatial Representations in a Patient with Bálint-like Syndrome: A Single-Case Study. Cortex 2021, 135, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Astafiev, S.V.; Shulman, G.L.; Stanley, C.M.; Snyder, A.Z.; Van Essen, D.C.; Corbetta, M. Functional Organization of Human Intraparietal and Frontal Cortex for Attending, Looking, and Pointing. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 4689–4699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Connolly, J.D.; Andersen, R.A.; Goodale, M.A. FMRI Evidence for a “parietal Reach Region” in the Human Brain. Exp. Brain Res. 2003, 153, 140–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Karnath, H.-O.; Perenin, M.-T. Cortical Control of Visually Guided Reaching: Evidence from Patients with Optic Ataxia. Cereb. Cortex 2005, 15, 1561–1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Wolpert, D.M.; Goodbody, S.J.; Husain, M. Maintaining Internal Representations: The Role of the Human Superior Parietal Lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 1998, 1, 529–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Wolpert, D.M.; Miall, R.C.; Kawato, M. Internal Models in the Cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998, 2, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Bastian, A.J. Learning to Predict the Future: The Cerebellum Adapts Feedforward Movement Control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006, 16, 645–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Chieffi, S.; Iavarone, A.; La Marra, M.; Messina, G.; Villano, I.; Ranucci, S.; Messina, A.; Piombino, L.; Dalia, C.; Monda, M. Memory for proprioceptive targets in bulimia nervosa. J. Psychiatry 2015, 18, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Villano, I.; Ilardi, C.R.; Arena, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gleijeses, M.G.; Messina, G.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Monda, M.; Iavarone, A.; et al. Obese Subjects without Eating Disorders Experience Binge Episodes Also Independently of Emotional Eating and Personality Traits among University Students of Southern Italy. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Chieffi, S.; Messina, A.; Villano, I.; Valenzano, A.A.; Nigro, E.; La Marra, M.; Cibelli, G.; Monda, V.; Salerno, M.; Tafuri, D.; et al. The Use of Velocity Information in Movement Reproduction. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  125. Hotamisligil, G.S.; Shargill, N.S.; Spiegelman, B.M. Adipose Expression of Tumor Necrosis Factor-α: Direct Role in Obesity-Linked Insulin Resistance. Science 1993, 259, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Visser, M. Elevated C-Reactive Protein Levels in Overweight and Obese Adults. JAMA 1999, 282, 2131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Yudkin, J.S.; Stehouwer, C.D.A.; Emeis, J.J.; Coppack, S.W. C-Reactive Protein in Healthy Subjects: Associations with Obesity, Insulin Resistance, and Endothelial Dysfunction: A Potential Role for Cytokines Originating From Adipose Tissue? ATVB 1999, 19, 972–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Ouchi, N.; Parker, J.L.; Lugus, J.J.; Walsh, K. Adipokines in Inflammation and Metabolic Disease. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 2011, 11, 85–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Monda, M.; Messina, G.; Mangoni, C.; De Luca, B. Resting Energy Expenditure and Fat-Free Mass Do Not Decline during Aging in Severely Obese Women. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 27, 657–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. De Fusco, C.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Viggiano, E.; Moscatelli, F.; Valenzano, A.; Esposito, T.; Sergio, C.; Cibelli, G.; Monda, M.; et al. Osteopontin: Relation between Adipose Tissue and Bone Homeostasis. Stem Cells Int. 2017, 2017, 4045238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Chieffi, S.; Villano, I.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; La Marra, M.; Messina, G.; Monda, M. Involvement of orexin in sleep disorders and neurodegenerative diseases. Curr. Top. Pept. 2015, 16, 49–54. [Google Scholar]
  132. Cavaliere, G.; Viggiano, E.; Trinchese, G.; De Filippo, C.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Valenzano, A.; Cincione, R.I.; Zammit, C.; Cimmino, F.; et al. Long Feeding High-Fat Diet Induces Hypothalamic Oxidative Stress and Inflammation, and Prolonged Hypothalamic AMPK Activation in Rat Animal Model. Front. Physiol. 2018, 9, 818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Anstey, K.J.; Cherbuin, N.; Budge, M.; Young, J. Body Mass Index in Midlife and Late-Life as a Risk Factor for Dementia: A Meta-Analysis of Prospective Studies: BMI and Risk of Dementia. Obes. Rev. 2011, 12, e426–e437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Avola, R.; Moscatelli, F.; Avalenzano, A.N.; Villano, I.; Ruberto, M.; Monda, E.; Marra, M.L.; Tafuri, D.; et al. Role Of The Orexin System On Arousal, Attention, Feeding Behaviour And Sleep Disorders. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2017, 33, 645–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Villano, I.; La Marra, M.; Di Maio, G.; Monda, V.; Chieffi, S.; Guatteo, E.; Messina, G.; Moscatelli, F.; Monda, M.; Messina, A. Physiological Role of Orexinergic System for Health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Lee, Y.-H.; Martin, J.M.; Maple, R.L.; Tharp, W.G.; Pratley, R.E. Plasma Amyloid-β Peptide Levels Correlate with Adipocyte Amyloid Precursor Protein Gene Expression in Obese Individuals. Neuroendocrinology 2009, 90, 383–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Jahangiri, A.; Wilson, P.G.; Hou, T.; Brown, A.; King, V.L.; Tannock, L.R. Serum Amyloid A Is Found on ApoB-Containing Lipoproteins in Obese Humans with Diabetes: SAA Lipoprotein Distribution. Obesity 2013, 21, 993–996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Serrano-Pozo, A.; Frosch, M.P.; Masliah, E.; Hyman, B.T. Neuropathological Alterations in Alzheimer Disease. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2011, 1, a006189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Skoog, I.; Wallin, A.; Fredman, P.; Hesse, C.; Aevarsson, O.; Karlsson, I.; Gottfries, C.G.; Blennow, K. A Population Study on Blood-Brain Barrier Function in 85-Year-Olds: Relation to Alzheimer’s Disease and Vascular Dementia. Neurology 1998, 50, 966–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Graphical representation of the mediation analysis output. Note: FAB15: frontal assessment battery-15; FTTm: finger tapping task mean; WC: waist circumference; a: relationship between (X) and (M); b: relationship between (M) and (Y); c′: direct effect of (X) on (Y); *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Graphical representation of the mediation analysis output. Note: FAB15: frontal assessment battery-15; FTTm: finger tapping task mean; WC: waist circumference; a: relationship between (X) and (M); b: relationship between (M) and (Y); c′: direct effect of (X) on (Y); *** p < 0.001.
Healthcare 10 01775 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the neurocognitive measures employed.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the neurocognitive measures employed.
MeanSDRange
MMSE a29.211.2223–30
FAB15 a13.631.547–15
TMT-A, seconds a37.8918.7915–100
TMT-B, seconds a95.1042.5320–239
TMT(B-A), seconds a57.9131.382–151
DSB a6.051.434–8
FTTm63.4212.2316.80–91.40
MMSE: Mini-mental state examination; FAB15: frontal assessment battery-15; TMT-A: trail making test-A; TMT-B: trail making test-B; TMT (B-A): trail making test (B-A); DSB: digit span backward; FTTm: finger tapping task mean; a raw scores.
Table 2. R2 change at each step hierarchical regressions on body mass index and waist circumference.
Table 2. R2 change at each step hierarchical regressions on body mass index and waist circumference.
PredictorsR2 for ModelF For ModelR2 ChangeF for R2 Change
Outcome: BMI
1Sex, age, education0.223F(3, 326) = 21.704 ***0.223F(3, 326) = 21.704 ***
2Sex, age, education, FAB150.226F(4, 325) = 16.498 ***0.003F(1, 325) = 0.906
3Sex, age, education, FAB15, TMT(B-A), DSB0.230F(6, 323) = 11.122 ***0.004F(2, 323) = 0.512
4Sex, age, education, FAB15, TMT(B-A), DSB, FTTm0.238F(7, 322) = 9.929 ***0.008F(1, 322) = 0.126
Outcome: WC
1Sex, age, education0.408F(3, 326) = 54.895 ***0.408F(3, 326) = 54.895 ***
2Sex, age, education, FAB150.427F(4, 325) = 44.363 ***0.019F(1, 325) = 7.967 ***
3Sex, age, education, FAB15, TMT(B-A), DSB0.434F(6, 323) = 30.151 ***0.007F(2, 323) = 1.417
4Sex, age, education, FAB15, TMT(B-A), DSB, FTTm0.458F(7, 322) = 28.353 ***0.024F(1, 322) = 10.378 ***
BMI: body mass index; FAB15: frontal assessment battery-15; TMT (B-A): trail making test (B-A); DSB: digit span backward; FTTm: finger tapping task mean; WC: waist circumference. *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Results of the hierarchical regression analysis on BMI and WC.
Table 3. Results of the hierarchical regression analysis on BMI and WC.
PredictorsB95% CI for BSEtp-ValueVIFTol.
LLUL
Outcome: BMI
Stage 1Sex−1.533−2.363−0.7030.421−3.640<0.0010.9911.009
Age0.0610.0360.0860.0134.834<0.0010.9901.010
Education−0.323−0.451−0.1960.065−4.994<0.0010.9911.009
Stage 2Sex−1.564−2.396−0.7310.423−3.701<0.0010.9851.015
Age0.0660.0390.0930.0144.812<0.0010.8331.200
Education−0.351−0.490−0.2110.071−4.950<0.0010.8291.207
FAB15−0.175−0.1580.5390.184−0.9520.3420.7041.420
Stage 3Sex−1.585−2.421−0.7500.424−3.738<0.0010.9821.018
Age0.0600.0300.0900.0153.897<0.0010.6691.494
Education−0.327−0.479−0.1760.077−4.260<0.0010.7081.412
FAB15−0.221−0.1680.6100.197−1.1190.2640.6171.621
TMT(B-A)0.006−0.0100.0210.0080.7270.4680.6851.459
DSB−0.126−0.4650.2130.172−0.7310.4660.7591.318
Stage 4Sex−1.945−2.897−0.9930.483−4.025<0.0010.7521.329
Age0.0500.0170.0830.0173.0250.0030.5721.750
Education−0.326−0.477−0.1750.077−4.257<0.0010.7081.412
FAB15−0.215−0.1730.6030.197−1.0910.2760.6171.622
TMT(B-A)0.004−0.0120.0190.0080.4600.6460.6661.502
DSB−0.116−0.4540.2230.172−0.6730.5020.7581.320
FTTm−0.042−0.0950.0120.027−1.5380.1260.5681.760
Outcome: WC
Stage 1Sex−10.369−12.831−7.9071.250−8.296<0.0011.0070.993
Age0.2940.2210.3670.0377.937<0.0011.0030.997
Education−0.988−1.377−0.6000.197−5.011<0.0011.0080.992
Stage 2Sex−10.465−12.893−8.0371.233−8.491<0.0011.0080.992
Age0.3340.2570.4120.0398.527<0.0011.1550.866
Education−1.233−1.653−0.8140.213−5.793<0.0011.2090.827
FAB15−1.503−2.553−0.4540.533−2.8230.0051.3710.729
Stage 3Sex−10.486−12.924−8.0491.237−8.477<0.0011.0190.982
Age0.3250.2380.4120.0447.349<0.0011.4690.681
Education−1.217−1.676−0.7580.233−5.221<0.0011.4540.688
FAB15−1.827−2.942−0.7120.566−3.2290.0011.5530.644
TMT(B-A)−0.003−0.0490.0430.023−0.1300.8971.5020.666
DSB−0.848−1.8560.1610.512−1.6570.0991.3350.749
Stage 4Sex−12.642−15.371−9.9121.385−9.125<0.0011.3280.753
Age0.2540.1590.3500.0485.249<0.0011.8410.543
Education−1.168−1.619−0.7170.229−5.099<0.0011.4610.685
FAB15−1.746−2.841−0.6520.556−3.1440.0021.5560.643
TMT(B-A)−0.011−0.0570.0340.023−0.4940.6221.5210.658
DSB−0.840−1.8290.1490.502−1.6730.0961.3350.749
FTTm−0.255−0.410−0.0990.079−3.2210.0011.7880.559
BMI: body mass index; FAB15: frontal assessment battery-15; TMT (B-A): trail making test (B-A); DSB: digit span backward; FTTm: finger tapping task mean; WC: waist circumference.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

La Marra, M.; Messina, A.; Ilardi, C.R.; Verde, G.; Amato, R.; Esposito, N.; Troise, S.; Orlando, A.; Messina, G.; Monda, V.; et al. The Neglected Factor in the Relationship between Executive Functioning and Obesity: The Role of Motor Control. Healthcare 2022, 10, 1775. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10091775

AMA Style

La Marra M, Messina A, Ilardi CR, Verde G, Amato R, Esposito N, Troise S, Orlando A, Messina G, Monda V, et al. The Neglected Factor in the Relationship between Executive Functioning and Obesity: The Role of Motor Control. Healthcare. 2022; 10(9):1775. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10091775

Chicago/Turabian Style

La Marra, Marco, Antonietta Messina, Ciro Rosario Ilardi, Giuseppe Verde, Raffaella Amato, Nadia Esposito, Simona Troise, Antonella Orlando, Giovanni Messina, Vincenzo Monda, and et al. 2022. "The Neglected Factor in the Relationship between Executive Functioning and Obesity: The Role of Motor Control" Healthcare 10, no. 9: 1775. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10091775

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop