Next Article in Journal
A Guide to a Pharmacist-Led Pharmacogenetic Testing and Counselling Service in an Interprofessional Healthcare Setting
Previous Article in Journal
The Resilience of Final-Year Pharmacy Students and Aspects of the Course They Found to Be Resilience-Building
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

The Moderating Power of Impulsivity: A Systematic Literature Review Examining the Theory of Planned Behavior

by
Lindsey A. Hohmann
1 and
Kimberly B. Garza
2,*
1
Department of Pharmacy Practice, Harrison College of Pharmacy, Auburn University, 1330J Walker Building, Auburn, AL 36849, USA
2
Department of Health Outcomes Research and Policy, Harrison College of Pharmacy, Auburn University, 4306B Walker Building, Auburn, AL 36849, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Pharmacy 2022, 10(4), 85; https://doi.org/10.3390/pharmacy10040085
Submission received: 27 May 2022 / Revised: 16 June 2022 / Accepted: 23 June 2022 / Published: 18 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Topic Advancing the Knowledge and Application of Health Behavior Theories)
(This article belongs to the Section Pharmacy Practice and Practice-Based Research)

Abstract

:
The theory of planned behavior (TPB) states that behavioral intention is the best predictor of actual behavior change. However, intention explains only a portion of the variance in behavior. Of specific interest is the question of which moderating or mediating variables can be leveraged to aid health promotion interventions utilizing the tenets of behavioral economics (delay discounting and commitment contracts) in the intention–behavior pathway. Impulsivity has been postulated to fill this role and may be applied to multiple behaviors. We aim to determine if impulsivity moderates the association between intention and actual behavior in the TPB, to discover what other variables may moderate or mediate this association, and to apply the findings to future studies in the field of behavioral economics. To this end, a systematic review was conducted using the PubMed, PsychINFO, and Embase online databases. Eligible studies in peer-reviewed journals published prior to November 2021 were selected. Thirty-three studies were included in the final review, examining physical activity, diet, preventive health, mental health, addiction, and medication adherence behaviors. Three main concepts emerged: (1) impulsivity moderates the association between intention and behavior change; (2) self-efficacy moderates the association between intention and behavior change; and (3) planning and self-efficacy contribute to moderated mediation. This review demonstrates a gap in the literature regarding the application of the TPB to the intention–behavior pathway for health behaviors. Future studies in behavioral economics may leverage the variables of impulsivity, self-efficacy, and planning to predict follow-through in this area and to develop targeted change initiatives.

1. Introduction

The classic theory of planned behavior (TPB) states that behavioral intentions are the best predictors of actual behavior change [1,2,3]. Support for the traditional TPB is found throughout the literature, most often using cross-sectional surveys, pre-post surveys, or small timeseries [4,5,6,7,8]. The bulk of the research has been within the psychology of eating and drinking behavior, as well as physical and sexual activity, gambling, and smoking, where intention has been shown to mediate the association between behavioral perceptions and behavior change [5,6,9,10,11,12,13]. Variables of behavioral perception, including attitude towards behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control, have accounted for a range of 40 to 77% of the variance in subjects’ intentions to change behavior [10,14,15,16]. Attitude towards behavior is defined as a positive or negative individual view of that behavior, while subjective norms are defined as views or behaviors that are generally accepted or expected in the community or by personally important individuals [14]. Similarly, perceived behavioral control is defined as an individual’s belief regarding their ability to change a particular behavior [14]. Furthermore, behavioral intention has been shown to account for 15 to 39% of the variance in actual behavior change [9,15]. This fluctuation may be partially explained by variation in the topic of study (smoking, condom use, etc.) [10,15], but this does not answer the question of what accounts for the remaining variance in actual behavior change. Is intention to change behavior the only mediator between perceptions and behavior change, and is intention alone really a good predictor of behavior change? Is intention itself dependent on some other variable?
This question leads us to consider factors that may influence the relationship between intention to change behavior and actual behavior change. It seems likely that there is some additional variable that mediates (is in the causal pathway between intention and behavior change) or moderates (influences the direction and strength of the intention–behavior relationship) this process [17], especially as we take into consideration the concept of behavioral economics, which combines principles of economics and cognitive psychology and states that people do not always behave rationally [18]. Furthermore, based on the authors’ previous research [19,20], it also seems likely that this intention–behavior pathway is moderated by a level of impulsivity, where impulsivity is a broad concept containing four general domains: urgency, lack of premeditation, sensation seeking, and lack of perseverance [21]. In the realm of behavioral economics, impulsivity is defined as the tendency to place a much higher value on immediate rewards compared to rewards received in the future and is commonly operationalized using various binary, choice-discounting tasks [22]. One might imagine that, even with a strong intention to change behavior, the powerful influence of impulsive choice (or difficulty delaying gratification) can undermine attempts at actual behavior change. One might intend to go to the gym regularly, but when tempted by the prospect of sleeping in or engaging in more pleasurable sedentary behaviors, that intention may not result in frequent gym attendance. Outside of the behavioral economics literature, impulsivity has also been found to moderate the relationship between eating traits and body mass index [23], as well as between negative life events and suicidal behavior [24]. Although impulsivity, thus, seems a likely culprit, the influences of other variables cannot be discounted.
Investigating moderators and mediators of the intention–behavior pathway is, thus, critical in informing future interventions to promote beneficial health behavior and improve health outcomes. With this belief in mind, we begin to pursue leads on potential mediators and moderators of the intention–behavior gap by conducting a short literature review across a wide variety of health behaviors with the goal of informing future studies in the field of behavioral economics.

2. Methods

A systematic literature review was performed using the PubMed, PsychINFO, and Embase online databases according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [25]. The searches were conducted in October 2021, and entries began broadly with “theory of planned behavior”, then were progressively narrowed by adding or modifying terms, with search strings combined using the Boolean operator “OR”. The search strings included “‘Theory of planned behavior’ and intention and behavior and (moderator or mediator)”, “(Impulse or impulsiv*) and mediator and intention”, “(Impulse or impulsiv*) and behavior and moderator and intention”, “Intention and behavior and (Impulse or impulsiv*) AND ‘theory of planned behavior’.” The database hits were then combed through by title and abstract for the most relevant studies, with those mentioning novel mediators and moderators selected for full-text review. Google Scholar was also used with a “snowballing” method, in which additional papers were identified from manually searching the text and reference sections of papers selected from database hits.
Eligibility was determined by two reviewers (LAH and KBG) in an unblinded and standardized manner. The primary author (LH) conducted title and abstract reviews, followed by full-text reviews. Full-text reviews were then independently conducted by a second reviewer (KBG), and discrepancies were resolved via discourse and consensus. Studies written in English and utilizing an experimental, quasi-experimental, or observational design in peer-reviewed journals published prior to November 2021 were included in this review. The variables sought included novel, participant-level moderators or mediators between intention and behavior change in the theory of planned behavior. In order to provide a foundation for future interventions across a variety of disciplines within behavioral economics, studies examining a broad range of health behaviors were eligible for this review. Studies were excluded if they were: unclear whether moderation or mediation was being tested (not explicitly stated by the authors or unable to distinguish by methods); not examining the theory of planned behavior; or not testing the moderation or mediation of variables between intention and a health behavior change. Moderating or mediating variables controlling for study procedures were not of interest. EndNote version X9 (ClarivateTM, Philadelphia, PA, USA) citation management software was used as a data organization tool and to assist with removal of duplicate articles.
The data were extracted by one investigator (LH) using a standardized template, including study design, intervention setting and population, study period, health behavior, measure scales used, statistical significance (yes or no), and risk of bias. Risk of bias (ROB) was assessed within each study using the mixed methods appraisal tool (MMAT) version 2018 [26]. This tool is suitable for a wide variety of study designs and consists of a 5-item measure of methodological quality (e.g., “Is the sample representative of the target population?”) rated as “Yes” (quality criteria are met), “No” (quality criteria are not met), or “Can’t Tell” (not enough information to determine whether quality criteria are met). The quality scores were calculated based on the number of items rated “Yes”, with a score of 0–1 being low quality (high risk of bias), 2–3 being moderate quality (moderate risk of bias), and 4–5 being high quality (low risk of bias) [27]. The findings were independently confirmed by a second reviewer (KBG), with discrepancies resolved via discourse and consensus. Due to the heterogeneous nature of behavioral intervention studies, the results were not pooled, nor were meta-analytic methods used. No registration was filed for this protocol.

3. Results

3.1. Database Hits

The initial search yielded 822 hits, with 698 after the removal of duplicates (Figure 1). After title and abstract review, 41 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility, and 33 studies met the inclusion criteria and were retained in the final review [16,21,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58]. The majority of the studies utilized a noncontrolled timeseries or pre-post design, with follow-up periods ranging from 1 week to 6 months (Table 1). No significant conflicts of interest were noted amongst the studies, with the risks of bias rated as low (n = 22) or moderate (n = 11) (Figure 2a,b). The behaviors of interest included physical activity (n = 17), diet (n = 9), preventive health behaviors (e.g., condom use or sun protection; n = 7), addiction (n = 6), mental health (n = 3), and medication adherence (n = 1) (Table 2a). Most articles reported moderation analyses (n = 30), followed by mediation (n = 5), moderated mediation (n = 4), and moderated moderation (n = 3), with more than one type of analysis or variable of interest per study in some cases (Table 2b). The specific moderating and mediating variables surrounding the intention–behavior gap are presented below.

3.2. Moderators and Mediators with a High Level of Support

Sixteen articles assessing moderators and mediators of the intention–behavior gap contributed to a straightforward picture of variables’ effects, with the majority of these studies (n = 12) reporting statistically significant findings and limited contradictory findings (Table 2c and Figure 3).

3.2.1. Impulsivity Moderates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Six studies showed that impulsivity moderates the relationship between behavioral intention and actual behavior change (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [21,28,29,30,31,32]. These results spanned several disciplines, most notably substance misuse and addiction, diet, and physical activity. The common theme of these studies was that the intention to change behavior leads to an actual behavior change, but the magnitude of this change depends on the level of impulsivity in the individual. Specifically, Moshier and colleagues studied the effect of impulsivity on illicit drug use and found that premeditation and sensation seeking were moderator variables affecting the pathway between the intention to stop drug use and actual drug use in the previous month [21]. Similarly, Mullan et al. found that higher inhibitory control and planning ability, two elements implicit in the concept of impulsivity, resulted in less binge drinking in college students with the intention to drink [30], and Wang [32] found that lower problem-gambling severity (a proxy for impulsivity) led to less unintended sports-gambling behavior. This moderation was echoed by the findings of Churchill [28,29], who demonstrated greater decreases in unintended snacking [28] and increases in intended fruit and vegetable consumption [29] when urgency (or impulsivity) was lower. Furthermore, Pfeffer and colleagues [31] found that self-control moderated the path between intentions and physical activity, such that higher self-control resulted in increased physical activity in those intending to exercise.
Although the majority of the articles demonstrated support for impulsivity as a moderator, three studies assessed the moderating role of impulsivity in the intention–behavior gap but found no statistical significance [33,34,35]. Specifically, Chevance and colleagues’ [33] investigation of physical activity, Crandall’s [34] assessment of mindfulness mobile app usage, and Stevens’ [35] evaluation of alcohol consumption did not demonstrate a significant association.

3.2.2. Self-Efficacy Moderates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Moderation was not only shown to occur via impulsivity, but self-efficacy demonstrated a moderation effect as well (n = 4; Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [16,36,37,38]. Schutz and colleagues showed that HIV-positive men who have sex with men were approximately 21 times more likely to act on their intentions to use condoms when they had higher self-efficacy [36]. Self-efficacy was defined in this case as the perceived ability to surmount barriers that impede the fulfillment of a particular behavior [36,59]. In this way, self-efficacy is related to the concept of perceived behavioral control, or one’s belief in the ability to affect change in a particular behavior [14,60]. Similarly, Koring [37], Lippke [38], and Luszczynska [16] all found that intentions to be physically active were put into practice more often in those with higher versus lower self-efficacy. On the other hand, C.Q. Zhang and colleagues [41] assessed the moderating roles of action self-efficacy (perceived ability to start a behavior) and maintenance self-efficacy (perceived ability to continue a behavior in the face of barriers) [61] on hand-washing and sleep hygiene behaviors, but no statistically significant moderating effects were found. Further pursuit of this thread led to a more complex view of self-efficacy as a part of moderated mediation in the context of an additional variable: planning.

3.2.3. Planning Mediates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Five studies demonstrated a statistically significant mediating effect of planning in the intention–behavior pathway (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [16,37,38,43,44]. Four of these studies focused on behaviors related to physical activity [16,37,38,44]. Koring [37], Luszczynska [16], Lippke [38], and Packel [44] found that the intention to exercise translated into actual physical activity indirectly via planning. While Koring [37] assessed planning in general, other studies have broken planning down into its constituent parts of action planning (making plans to act on intentions) and coping planning (making plans to overcome expected barriers) [61]. Specifically, Luszczynska [16] and Packel [44] found that both action planning and coping planning filled this mediating role, while Lippke [38] found statistical significance for action planning alone. In terms of behaviors other than physical activity, Lin and colleagues [43] found that the intention to adhere to a medication influenced actual medication adherence (to aspirin) indirectly via both action planning and coping planning. However, when planning was viewed in combination with self-efficacy, a more complicated picture of moderated mediation emerged.

3.2.4. Planning and Self-Efficacy Contribute to Moderated Mediation

Moderated mediation between intentions and behavior change was demonstrated via planning and self-efficacy in several studies (n = 3; Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [16,37,38]. Moderated mediation occurs when a variable mediates the relationship between the behavioral intention and the behavior change, and an additional variable acts as the moderator of this process. Koring [37], Luszczynska [16], and Lippke [38] and colleagues independently found that planning mediated the pathway from intentions to actual changes in physical activity. Individuals’ levels of self-efficacy moderated this relationship; in other words, higher self-efficacy increased the likelihood of plans being realized as actual changes in physical activity behavior [16,37,38]. This was a more complicated process than noted by Schutz’s [36], Lin [43], and Packel [44] and opened the consideration of additional moderators and mediators within the theory of planned behavior and the intention–behavior gap, specifically.

3.3. Moderators and Mediators with Less Support

Seventeen articles reported moderators and mediators of the intention–behavior gap that were less clear, with fewer statistically significant findings (n = 10) or mixed findings, which warrant additional investigation in future studies (Table 2c and Figure 3).

3.3.1. Personality Moderates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Of seven studies assessing personality traits as moderators in the intention–behavior pathway, two studies found statistically significant moderators [45,46], while five studies did not [47,48,49,50,51] (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3). De Bruijn et al. [46] assessed neuroticism and conscientiousness, which are two traits from the Five-Factor Model of Personality (FFM), and found that neuroticism moderated the intention–fruit consumption gap, such that lower neuroticism led to a stronger relationship between intention and behavior. Additionally, Gucciardi [45] found mental toughness to be a statistically significant moderator, resulting in increased translation of exercise intentions into actual exercise performance. On the other hand, MacCann [47] and Monds [48] assessed the moderating effect of the HEXACO personality domains (honesty and humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience) on the intention–physical activity and intention–fruit and vegetable consumption pathways, respectively, and found no statistically significant moderators. Similarly, Hannan [50] and Hartson [51] found no moderating roles for mental toughness or mindfulness, respectively, between intention and physical activity, while Cao [49] found that mental toughness served as a moderator in the intention–physical activity gap in Chinese college students but not in adult wage earners.

3.3.2. Socioeconomic Factors Moderate the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Four studies assessed moderation by socioeconomic factors (n = 4 statistically significant), with a wide variety of factors assessed, including age, race, sex, and education level (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [52,53,54,55]. Gibson [53] found that age and race moderated between the intention to social distance and actual social distancing behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic, such that intentions were translated into practice more often in older adults and those identifying as White. Additionally, Schuz [54] found that a higher education level resulted in a greater influence of intention on multiple different behaviors, including fruit and vegetable consumption, physical activity, alcohol consumption, flossing, healthy snack consumption, limiting the amount of time spent sitting daily, low-fat diet, and breast or testicular self-exams. Lange and colleagues [55] examined the role of sex in a moderated mediation model and found that sex moderated the effect of planning in the intention–behavior gap in cases of fruit and vegetable consumption, physical activity, and sun protection behaviors, with planning mediating the intention–behavior pathway for men but not women. On the other hand, Rhodes [52] found a moderating effect of sex within the intention–physical activity pathway, such that intention was successfully translated into physical activity for women but not men. However, when the perceived opportunity for exercise was added to the model, physical activity intentions were successfully realized for men more than women (moderated moderation). With the limited number of studies focused on each socioeconomic factor, it was difficult to draw conclusions about any particular factor.

3.3.3. Perceptions and Beliefs Regarding Stigma and Norms Moderate the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Two studies found particular perceptions or beliefs that moderated the intention–behavior gap (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [56,57]. Specifically, Gaum and colleagues [56] found that lower anticipated stigma resulted in the increased implementation of depression prevention strategies in the workplace. Likewise, Bauman [57] demonstrated that those with higher normative belief incongruence (changes in normative beliefs over time) were more likely to consume alcohol despite the opposite intention. However, multiple studies assessing perceived stigma or belief incongruence are necessary in order to come to a more firm conclusion regarding their roles as moderators.

3.3.4. Environment Moderates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

The evidence was mixed concerning the moderating effect of environment on the intention–behavior gap, with one study finding a statistically significant effect [58] and one study finding no effect [39] (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3). Gourlan and colleagues [58] found that built environment, when combined with planning, contributed to moderated moderation of the intention–physical activity gap, such that those with a greater level of planning and living closer to exercise facilities were more likely to follow through with intentions to exercise. However, R. Zhang [39] found that perceived worksite neighborhood walkability had no moderating role between the intention to walk and actual physical activity.

3.3.5. Habit Moderates the Association between Intention and Behavior Change

Evidence was also mixed for the moderating role of habit (Table 1 and Table 2c, and Figure 3) [40,42]. Specifically, Kothe [40] found that self-reported habit strength increased follow-through on the intention to adhere to a gluten-free diet. On the other hand, Allom et al. [42] found that habit strength did not serve to moderate the intention–physical activity gap.

4. Discussion

The bulk of our findings regarding additional variables in an extended version of the theory of planned behavior suggested the moderation of the intention–behavior pathway by impulsivity [21,28,29,30,31] or self-efficacy [16,36,37,38], mediation by planning [16,37,38,43,44], and some evidence for moderated mediation via planning and self-efficacy [16,37,38]. However, the evidence for moderation by other personality and demographic traits, such as neuroticism, age, and gender, was sparse [46,48,53], with a lack of consistent measurement instruments or definitions for personality traits. Furthermore, there was a paucity of research investigating habit, built environment, and stigmatizing and normative beliefs within the intention–behavior gap, as well as research evaluating the interactions of these variables with impulsivity. These findings align with previous systematic reviews focused solely on physical activity and sun protection behaviors, which have found some evidence for the moderation of the intention–behavior gap by self-efficacy but have shown mixed evidence for sociodemographic variables [62,63]. Future studies assessing the moderators and mediators between intention and behavior should focus on the aforementioned variables, which have a current low level of support in the literature.
When taken as a whole, these studies showed the broad application of the theory of planned behavior throughout various disciplines. Studies investigating physical activity and dietary behaviors were most prominent, as well as substance misuse, mental health, condom use, and other preventive health behaviors. However, using our protocol and inclusion criteria, only one study was found that addressed patient medication adherence. One can imagine that medication-taking behavior is highly related to the level of impulsivity, given that many medications for chronic diseases have immediate costs, including adverse effects, while the benefits of taking them are often not apparent until sometime in the future. Indeed, studies have shown that certain character traits, including impulsivity, predict low medication adherence [64]. However, patient medication adherence remains low despite educational interventions targeting patients and providers [65]. Thus, this is a critical area for future study.

4.1. Implications

This review has implications for future studies in the field of behavioral economics and health outcomes. The concepts of impulsivity and planning are especially relevant to this field, as the theoretical framework of the discipline relies on these individual characteristics. Commitment contracts (financial or social incentives to perform a behavior to which individuals agree ahead of time) in behavioral economics are particularly relevant to the role of planning as a mediational variable. Studies have shown that the use of financial commitment contracts, where participants’ previously deposited money is deducted for not meeting a behavioral goal, are able to overcome high levels of impulsivity to increase follow-through in the desired behavior [66]. Reward substitution, or the provision of short-term financial or social incentives to avoid engaging in an undesirable behavior, has also been used successfully to overcome difficulty in delaying gratification [67]. Future studies may include pre-post or small timeseries surveys examining the role of impulsivity as a moderator between patient intentions and health behaviors. Follow-up studies may use commitment contracts, incentives, or decision aids as a means to investigate behavior follow-through in highly impulsive participants. Further investigation also needs to be conducted on the effects of age and gender in the intention–behavior pathway; if effects are found, this may be significant for the development of targeted and tailored health behavior tools or interventions. The pursuit of such studies can help to fill the knowledge gap demonstrated by this review and may have implications for patient and prescriber incentives in the increasingly outcome-oriented United States healthcare system.

4.2. Limitations

Although this systematic review was performed following the PRISMA guidelines [25], the results must be interpreted with caution in light of several limitations. First, the majority of the studies exploring this topic utilized cross-sectional, pre-post, or small timeseries designs with no more than 6 months of follow up, which may not be sufficient time to effect or maintain a behavioral change, limiting the validity of the findings. However, given the psychological nature of many of these studies, these designs were the most ethical and feasible.
Second, the heterogeneity of the study designs and behaviors studied limited the ability to pool the findings. This precludes the use of meta-analytic methods. However, the findings may still be clinically valuable and informative for behavioral interventions.
Third, studies using the theory of reasoned action (TRA) were not included in this review. The TRA is closely related to the TBP, but the TPB includes one additional construct-perceived behavioral control. In order to focus the review effort, only studies examining the TPB were included; however, the inclusion of “TRA” in the search parameters may have yielded additional studies in other areas of health behavior. This may be included in future studies. However, a preliminary search did not yield appreciably different results.
Fourth, varying definitions and measurements of impulsivity were found amongst the studies. Where the definitions were unclear, the reviewers’ judgement was used; however, this may limit the replicability of the search. Furthermore, planning was found to be a mediational variable in several studies, yet planning may also be related to the concept of impulsivity or self-regulation [30], making this association unclear. Additionally, in some cases, it was difficult to determine whether moderation or mediation was being tested if it was not explicitly stated by the authors.

5. Conclusions

The bulk of the evidence from this review pointed to impulsivity as a moderator between intention and behavior change, with a possible moderating role of self-efficacy. Furthermore, planning, which has been defined in several studies as a composite of both action planning and coping planning [68,69], may play a mediational role between behavioral intention and actual behavior change. However, despite the breadth of the application in topics of psychology, this review demonstrated a gap in the literature regarding the study of moderators and mediators in the theory of planned behavior as it applies to behavioral economics and health outcomes. Future studies assessing moderators and mediators between intentions and health behavior should focus on personality, habit, built environment, stigmatizing and normative beliefs, and sociodemographic variables, which have a current low level of support in the literature.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.A.H. and K.B.G.; Methodology, L.A.H. and K.B.G.; Validation, L.A.H. and K.B.G.; Investigation, L.A.H.; Formal Analysis, L.A.H.; Data Interpretation, L.A.H. and K.B.G.; Data Curation, L.A.H.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, L.A.H..; Writing—Review and Editing, L.A.H. and K.B.G.; Visualization, L.A.H.; Supervision, K.B.G.; Project Administration, L.A.H. and K.B.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ajzen, I. Theories of Cognitive Self-Regulation, the Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Conner, M.; Armitage, C.J. Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 28, 1429–1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior: Frequently asked questions. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2020, 2, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Pearson, M.R.; Henson, J.M. Unplanned drinking and alcohol-related problems: A preliminary test of the model of unplanned drinking behavior. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2013, 27, 584–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Heeren, G.A.; Jemmott, J.B., 3rd; Mandeya, A.; Tyler, J.C. Sub-Saharan African university students’ beliefs about condoms, condom-use intention, and subsequent condom use: A prospective study. AIDS Behav. 2009, 13, 268–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Jemmott, J.B., 3rd; Stephens-Shields, A.; O’Leary, A.; Jemmott, L.S.; Teitelman, A.; Ngwane, Z.; Mtose, X. Mediation of effects of a theory-based behavioral intervention on self-reported physical activity in South African men. Prev. Med. 2015, 72, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Reyes Fernandez, B.; Warner, L.M.; Knoll, N.; Montenegro Montenegro, E.; Schwarzer, R. Synergistic effects of social support and self-efficacy on dietary motivation predicting fruit and vegetable intake. Appetite 2015, 87, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. McDermott, M.S.; Oliver, M.; Simnadis, T.; Beck, E.J.; Coltman, T.; Iverson, D.; Caputi, P.; Sharma, R. The Theory of Planned Behaviour and dietary patterns: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev. Med. 2015, 81, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Di Sarra, L.; Ghezzi, V.; Eastland, T.Y.; Antonini, F.; Scialo, G.; Zega, M.; Alvaro, R. Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Women’s Role in Prostate Cancer Screening. Res. Theory Nurs. Pract. 2015, 29, 200–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Karimy, M.; Zareban, I.; Araban, M.; Montazeri, A. An Extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) Used to Predict Smoking Behavior Among a Sample of Iranian Medical Students. Int. J. High Risk Behav. Addict. 2015, 4, e24715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Martin, R.J.; Usdan, S.; Nelson, S.; Umstattd, M.R.; Laplante, D.; Perko, M.; Shaffer, H. Using the theory of planned behavior to predict gambling behavior. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2010, 24, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Jeong, S.Y.; Kim, K.M. Influencing factors on hand hygiene behavior of nursing students based on theory of planned behavior: A descriptive survey study. Nurse Educ. Today 2016, 36, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Godin, G.; Gagnon, H.; Lambert, L.-D.; Conner, M. Determinants of condom use among a random sample of single heterosexual adults. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2005, 10, 85–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Aldrich, R.S. Using the Theory of Planned Behavior to Predict College Students’ Intention to Intervene with a Suicidal Individual. Crisis 2015, 36, 332–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Asare, M. Using the Theory of Planned Behavior to Determine the Condom Use Behavior Among College Students. Am. J. Health Stud. 2015, 30, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Luszczynska, A.; Sheng Cao, D.; Mallach, N.; Pietron, K.; Mazurkiewicz, M.; Schwarzer, R. Intentions, planning, and self-efficacy predict physical activity in Chinese and Polish adolescents: Two moderated mediation analyses. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2010, 10, 265–278. [Google Scholar]
  17. Scribbr. Mediator vs. Moderator Variables. 2021. Available online: https://www.scribbr.com/methodology/mediator-vs-moderator/ (accessed on 10 June 2022).
  18. Rice, T. The Behavioral Economics of Health and Health Care. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2013, 34, 431–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Messina, B.G.; Silvestri, M.M.; Diulio, A.R.; Murphy, J.G.; Garza, K.B.; Correia, C.J. Alcohol use, impulsivity, and the non-medical use of prescription stimulants among college students. Addict. Behav. 2014, 39, 1798–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Garza, K.B.; Harris, C.V.; Bolding, M.S. Examination of value of the future and health beliefs to explain dietary and physical activity behaviors. Res. Soc. Adm. Pharm. 2013, 9, 851–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Moshier, S.J.; Ewen, M.; Otto, M.W. Impulsivity as a moderator of the intention-behavior relationship for illicit drug use in patients undergoing treatment. Addict. Behav. 2013, 38, 1651–1655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Chapman, G.B.; Elstein, A.S. Valuing the future: Temporal discounting of health and money. Med. Decis. Mak. Int. J. Soc. Med. Decis. Mak. 1995, 15, 373–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Price, M.; Higgs, S.; Lee, M. Self-reported eating traits: Underlying components of food responsivity and dietary restriction are positively related to BMI. Appetite 2015, 95, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Zhang, J.; Lin, L. The moderating effects of impulsivity on Chinese rural young suicide. J. Clin. Psychol. 2014, 70, 579–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009, 151, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Hong, Q.N.; Fàbregues, S.; Bartlett, G.; Boardman, F.; Cargo, M.; Dagenais, P.; Gagnon, M.-P.; Griffiths, F.; Nicolau, B.; O’Cathain, A.; et al. The Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT) version 2018 for information professionals and researchers. Educ. Inf. 2018, 34, 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Hohmann, N.S.; McDaniel, C.C.; Mason, S.W.; Cheung, W.Y.; Williams, M.S.; Salvador, C.; Graves, E.K.; Camp, C.N.; Chou, C. Healthcare providers’ perspectives on care coordination for adults with cancer and multiple chronic conditions: A systematic review. J. Pharm. Health Serv. Res. 2020, 11, 97–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Churchill, S.; Jessop, D. Spontaneous implementation intentions and impulsivity: Can impulsivity moderate the effectiveness of planning strategies? Br. J. Health Psychol. 2010, 15, 529–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Churchill, S.; Jessop, D.C. Too impulsive for implementation intentions? Evidence that impulsivity moderates the effectiveness of an implementation intention intervention. Psychol. Health 2011, 26, 517–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mullan, B.; Wong, C.; Allom, V.; Pack, S.L. The role of executive function in bridging the intention-behaviour gap for binge-drinking in university students. Addict. Behav. 2011, 36, 1023–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Pfeffer, I.; Strobach, T. Physical activity automaticity, intention, and trait self-control as predictors of physical activity behavior—A dual-process perspective. Psychol. Health Med. 2022, 27, 1021–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wang, X.; Won, D.; Jeon, H.S. Predictors of Sports Gambling among College Students: The Role of the Theory of Planned Behavior and Problem Gambling Severity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chevance, G.; Stephan, Y.; Héraud, N.; Boiché, J. Interaction between self-regulation, intentions and implicit attitudes in the prediction of physical activity among persons with obesity. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2018, 37, 257–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Crandall, A.; Cheung, A.; Young, A.; Hooper, A.P. Theory-Based Predictors of Mindfulness Meditation Mobile App Usage: A Survey and Cohort Study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2019, 7, e10794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Stevens, A.K.; Littlefield, A.K.; Talley, A.E.; Brown, J.L. Do individuals higher in impulsivity drink more impulsively? A pilot study within a high risk sample of young adults. Addict. Behav. 2017, 65, 147–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Schutz, M.; Godin, G.; Kok, G.; Vézina-Im, L.-A.; Naccache, H.; Otis, J.; MAYA Study Group. Determinants of condom use among HIV-positive men who have sex with men. Int. J. STD AIDS 2011, 22, 391–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Koring, M.; Richert, J.; Lippke, S.; Parschau, L.; Reuter, T.; Schwarzer, R. Synergistic effects of planning and self-efficacy on physical activity. Health Educ. Behav. Off. Publ. Soc. Public Health Educ. 2012, 39, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lippke, S.; Wiedemann, A.U.; Ziegelmann, J.P.; Reuter, T.; Schwarzer, R. Self-efficacy moderates the mediation of intentions into behavior via plans. Am. J. Health Behav. 2009, 33, 521–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Zhang, R.; Zhang, C.Q.; Gan, Y.; Li, D.; Rhodes, R.E. Predicting Transport-Related Walking in Chinese Employees by Integrating Worksite Neighbourhood Walkability and Social Cognition. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2019, 11, 484–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kothe, E.J.; Sainsbury, K.; Smith, L.; Mullan, B.A. Explaining the intention–behaviour gap in gluten-free diet adherence: The moderating roles of habit and perceived behavioural control. J. Health Psychol. 2015, 20, 580–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Zhang, C.Q.; Fang, R.; Zhang, R.; Hagger, M.S.; Hamilton, K. Predicting Hand Washing and Sleep Hygiene Behaviors among College Students: Test of an Integrated Social-Cognition Model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Allom, V.; Mullan, B.; Cowie, E.; Hamilton, K. Physical Activity and Transitioning to College: The Importance of Intentions and Habits. Am. J. Health Behav. 2016, 40, 280–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Lin, C.Y.; Broström, A.; Nilsen, P.; Pakpour, A.H. Using extended theory of planned behavior to understand aspirin adherence in pregnant women. Pregnancy Hypertens. 2018, 12, 84–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Packel, L.B.; Prehn, A.W.; Anderson, C.L.; Fisher, P.L. Factors influencing physical activity behaviors in colorectal cancer survivors. Am. J. Health Promot. AJHP 2015, 30, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Gucciardi, D.F. Mental toughness as a moderator of the intention-behaviour gap in the rehabilitation of knee pain. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 454–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. de Bruijn, G.-J.; Brug, J.; Van Lenthe, F.J. Neuroticism, conscientiousness and fruit consumption: Exploring mediator and moderator effects in the theory of planned behaviour. Psychol. Health 2009, 24, 1051–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. MacCann, C.; Todd, J.; Mullan, B.A.; Roberts, R.D. Can personality bridge the intention-behavior gap to predict who will exercise? Am. J. Health Behav. 2015, 39, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Monds, L.A.; MacCann, C.; Mullan, B.A.; Wong, C.; Todd, J.; Roberts, R.D. Can personality close the intention-behavior gap for healthy eating? An examination with the HEXACO personality traits. Psychol. Health Med. 2016, 21, 845–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cao, Z.; Yang, Y.; Ding, W.; Huang, Z. From Physical Activity Intention to Behavior: The Moderation Role of Mental Toughness Among College Students and Wage Earners. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 584760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hannan, T.E.; Moffitt, R.L.; Neumann, D.L.; Thomas, P.R. Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior to Physical Activity: The Moderating Role of Mental Toughness. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 37, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hartson, K.R.; Avila, M.; Gance-Cleveland, B.; Harpin, S.B.; Tigges, B.; Cook, P.F. Predictors of Physical Activity in a Predominantly Hispanic Sample of Adolescents. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2020, 42, 514–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rhodes, R.E.; Beauchamp, M.R.; Quinlan, A.; Symons Downs, D.; Warburton, D.E.R.; Blanchard, C.M. Predicting the physical activity of new parents who participated in a physical activity intervention. Soc. Sci. Med. 2021, 284, 114221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Gibson, L.P.; Magnan, R.E.; Kramer, E.B.; Bryan, A.D. Theory of Planned Behavior Analysis of Social Distancing During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Focusing on the Intention-Behavior Gap. Ann. Behav. Med. A Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2021, 55, 805–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Schüz, B.; Brick, C.; Wilding, S.; Conner, M. Socioeconomic Status Moderates the Effects of Health Cognitions on Health Behaviors within Participants: Two Multibehavior Studies. Ann. Behav. Med. A Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2020, 54, 36–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Lange, D.; Barz, M.; Baldensperger, L.; Lippke, S.; Knoll, N.; Schwarzer, R. Sex differential mediation effects of planning within the health behavior change process. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 211, 137–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Gaum, P.M.; Brey, F.; Kraus, T.; Lang, J. Does stigmatization moderate the association between intention and implementation of learned prevention-strategies at work after a depressive episode?—A cross-sectional pilot study. J. Occup. Med. Toxicol. 2019, 14, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Baumann, S.; Gaertner, B.; Schnuerer, I.; Haberecht, K.; John, U.; Freyer-Adam, J. Belief incongruence and the intention-behavior gap in persons with at-risk alcohol use. Addict. Behav. 2015, 48, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gourlan, M.; Bord, A.; Cousson-Gélie, F. From intentions formation to their translation into behavior: An extended model of theory of planned behavior in the exercise domain. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2019, 8, 317–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. St-Pierre, R.A.; Temcheff, C.E.; Derevensky, J.L.; Gupta, R. Theory of Planned Behavior in School-Based Adolescent Problem Gambling Prevention: A Conceptual Framework. J. Prim. Prev. 2015, 36, 361–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Norman, P.; Conner, M. Health Behavior. In Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Rhodes, R.E.; Dickau, L. Moderators of the intention-behaviour relationship in the physical activity domain: A systematic review. Br. J. Sports Med. 2013, 47, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Starfelt Sutton, L.C.; White, K.M. Predicting sun-protective intentions and behaviours using the theory of planned behaviour: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Health 2016, 31, 1272–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Verdoux, H.; Lengronne, J.; Liraud, F.; Gonzales, B.; Assens, F.; Abalan, F.; Os, J.V. Medication adherence in psychosis: Predictors and impact on outcome. A 2-year follow-up of first-admitted subjects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2000, 102, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Viswanathan, M.; Golin, C.E.; Jones, C.D.; Ashok, M.; Blalock, S.J.; Wines, R.C.; Coker-Schwimmer, E.J.; Rosen, D.L.; Sista, P.; Lohr, K.N. Interventions to improve adherence to self-administered medications for chronic diseases in the United States: A systematic review. Ann. Intern. Med. 2012, 157, 785–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Halpern, S.D.; Asch, D.A.; Volpp, K.G. Commitment contracts as a way to health. BMJ 2012, 344, e522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Reimann, M.; Bechara, A.; MacInnis, D. Leveraging the happy meal effect: Substituting food with modest nonfood incentives decreases portion size choice. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2015, 21, 276–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Scholz, U.; Schuz, B.; Ziegelmann, J.P.; Lippke, S.; Schwarzer, R. Beyond behavioural intentions: Planning mediates between intentions and physical activity. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2008, 13, 479–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Caudroit, J.; Boiche, J.; Stephan, Y. The role of action and coping planning in the relationship between intention and physical activity: A moderated mediation analysis. Psychol. Health 2014, 29, 768–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA Diagram. Database hits and stepwise inclusion or exclusion of studies.
Figure 1. PRISMA Diagram. Database hits and stepwise inclusion or exclusion of studies.
Pharmacy 10 00085 g001
Figure 2. (a,b) Risk of bias assessment using mixed methods appraisal tool (MMAT): (a) frequency of article quality scores in risk of bias assessment (n = 33) and (b) frequency of article risk of bias ratings (n = 33).
Figure 2. (a,b) Risk of bias assessment using mixed methods appraisal tool (MMAT): (a) frequency of article quality scores in risk of bias assessment (n = 33) and (b) frequency of article risk of bias ratings (n = 33).
Pharmacy 10 00085 g002aPharmacy 10 00085 g002b
Figure 3. Summary of statistically significant literature review findings as applied to an extended view of the theory of planned behavior [5,6,9,10,11,12,14,15,16,21,29,30,31,32,36,37,38,39,40,43,44,45,52,53,54,57].
Figure 3. Summary of statistically significant literature review findings as applied to an extended view of the theory of planned behavior [5,6,9,10,11,12,14,15,16,21,29,30,31,32,36,37,38,39,40,43,44,45,52,53,54,57].
Pharmacy 10 00085 g003
Table 1. Final study characteristics.
Table 1. Final study characteristics.
Article Author, YearStudy DesignIntervention PopulationFollow-Up
Period
Behavior of InterestModerating or Mediating Variables (Scales)Statistical Significance (p-Value)
Allom, 2016 [42]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 101
Australian college students
Mean age: 19.60 yrs
Female: 81.40%
1 weekPhysical activity Moderation
  • Habit strength (12-item Self-Report Habit Index)
No
Baumann, 2015 [57]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 433
German adult job agency clients
Mean age: 30.6 yrs
Female: 36%
3 monthsAt-risk alcohol useModeration
  • Belief incongruence
 ○ Normative belief incongruence (4-item measure)Yes (<0.05)
 ○ Behavioral belief incongruence (6-item measure)No
 ○ Control belief incongruence (4-item measure)No
Cao, 2021 [49]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 591
Chinese college students
Mean age: not reported (range 19–24 yrs)
Female: 57.53%; and
n = 285
Chinese adult wage earners
Mean age: not reported (range 27–58 yrs)
Female: 44.56%
1 weekPhysical activityModeration
  • Mental toughness (8-item Mental Toughness Inventory (MTI))
 ○ Among college studentsYes (<0.01)
 ○ Among wage earnersNo
Chevance, 2018 [33]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 76
French adults
Mean age: 56 yrs
Female: not reported
4 monthsPhysical activityModeration
  • Trait impulsivity (20-item UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale)
No
 ○ Lack of conscientiousnessNo
  • Executive functions (Computerized Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test)
No
Churchill, 2010 [28]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2 n = 256
UK adults
Mean age: 33.05 yrs
Female: 79.36%
2 weeksAvoidance of snackingModeration
  • Impulsivity at T2 (45-item UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale)
 ○ Urgency Yes (<0.05)
 ○ Lack of premeditationNo
 ○ Lack of perseveranceNo
 ○ Sensation seekingNo
Churchill, 2011 [29]Controlled timeseries—T1, T2, T3n = 323
UK adults
Mean age: 32.8 yrs
Female: 81.42%
Students: 52.94%
2 weeks (1-week intervals)Fruit and vegetable consumptionModeration
  • Impulsivity at T1 (45-item UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale)
 ○ Urgency Yes (<0.05)
 ○ Lack of premeditationNo
 ○ Lack of perseverance No
 ○ Sensation seekingNo
Crandall, 2019 [34]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2 n = 85
Undergraduate college students in Utah, US
Mean age: 21.81 yrs
Female: 53%
2 weeksMindfulness meditation mobile app useModeration
  • Executive functioning (three tasks from the NIH Toolbox (NIH-TB)
No
 ○ Cognitive shifting (NIH-TB Dimensional Change Card-Sort Test)No
 ○ Inhibitory control and attention (NIH-TB Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test)No
 ○ Working memory (NIH-TB List-Sorting Working Memory Test)No
De Bruijn, 2009 [46]Cross-sectional interviewer-administered surveyn = 405
Dutch adults
Mean age: 60.25 yrs
Female: 57.53%
No follow-upFruit consumptionModeration
  • Five-Factor Model of Personality (FFM)
 ○ Neuroticism (6-item measure)Yes (<0.001)
 ○ Conscientiousness (6-item measure)No
Gaum, 2019 [56]Cross-sectional surveyn = 112
German adults with history of depression
Mean age: 42.3 yrs
Female: 75%
No follow-upImplementation of depression prevention strategies at workModeration
  • Anticipated stigmatization (2-item German Inventory of Subjective Stigma Experience)
Yes (0.003)
  • Experienced stigmatization (2-item German Inventory of Subjective Stigma Experience)
No
Gibson, 2021 [53]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 507
US adults
Mean age: 50.39 yrs
Female: 50.9%
3 monthsSocial distancing during COVID-19Moderation
  • Age
Yes (<0.001)
  • Race
Yes (0.002)
  • Intention stability over time
Yes (<0.001)
Gourlan, 2019 [58]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 219
French adults
Mean age: 41.28
Female: 52.51%
3 monthsPhysical activityModeration
  • Planning (5-item French Action-Planning Scale)
No
  • Perceived built environment (3-item measure)
No
Moderated Moderation
  • Planning*Environment
Yes (0.02)
Gucciardi, 2016 [45]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 193
Australian adults
Mean age: 30.79 yrs
Female: 55.44%
2 weeksRehabilitation exercises for knee painModeration
  • Mental toughness (8-item index)
Yes (0.013)
Hannan, 2015 [50]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 117
Australian adults and undergraduate students
Mean age: 28.29 yrs
Female: 73.50%
1 weekPhysical activityModeration
  • Mental toughness (8-item Mental Toughness Index (MTI))
No
Hartson, 2020 [51]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 232
US Hispanic adolescents
Mean age: 15.23 yrs
Female: 51.3%
2 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Mindfulness (10-item Child and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure)
No
Koring, 2012 [37]Noncontrolled timeseries—T1, T2, T3 n = 290
German adults
Mean age: 41.9 yrs
Female: 77%
6 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Self-efficacy (2-item measure)
Yes (<0.05)
Mediation
  • Planning (2-item measure)
Yes (<0.05)
Moderated Mediation
  • Planning*Self-efficacy
Yes (<0.05)
Kothe, 2015 [40]Cross-sectional survey n = 228
Australian adults with Celiac disease
Mean age: 45.2 yrs
Female: 89.5%
No follow-upGluten-free diet adherenceModeration
  • Perceived behavioral control (PBC) (17-item TPB Celiac Disease Questionnaire)
No
  • Habit (12-item Self-Reported Habit Index)
Yes (0.013)
Moderated Moderation
  • PBC*Habit
Yes (<0.001)
Lange, 2018 [55]Study I. Noncontrolled timeseries—T1, T2, T3n = 461
German adults
Mean age: 38.2 yrs
Female: 81.6%
4 monthsFruit and vegetable intakeModerated Mediation
  • Planning*Sex (3-item planning measure)
Yes (0.040)
Study II. Noncontrolled timeseries—T1, T2, T3n = 193
German university students
Mean age: 24.5 yrs
Female: 80.8%
2 weeksPhysical activity
  • Planning*Sex (4-item planning measure)
Yes (0.022)
Study III. Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 166
German adults
Mean age: 37.6 yrs
Female: 49.3%
2 weeksSun protection
  • Planning*Sex (2-item planning measure)
Yes (0.014)
Lin, 2018 [43]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 535
Iranian women with high-risk pregnancies
Mean age: 32.29 yrs
Female: 100%
8 weeksMedication adherence (aspirin) Mediation
  • Planning
 ○ Action planning (4-item measure)Yes (<0.01)
 ○ Coping planning (5-item measure)Yes (<0.01)
Lippke, 2009 [38]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2 n = 812
German adults
Mean age: 36.69 yrs
Female: 74.4%
4 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Self-efficacy (3-item measure)
Yes (<0.01)
Mediation
  • Action planning (3-item measure)
Yes (<0.01)
Moderated Mediation
  • Action planning*Self-efficacy
Yes (<0.01)
Luszczynska, 2010 [16]Study I. Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 534
Chinese adolescents, grades 7–12
Mean age: 13.8 yrs
Female: 54%
4 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Self-efficacy (3-item scale)
Yes (<0.01)
Mediation
  • Coping planning (4-item measure)
Yes (<0.01)
Moderated Mediation
  • Coping planning*Self-efficacy
Yes (<0.01)
Study II. Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 620
Polish high school adolescents
Mean age: 16.46 yrs
Female: 62%
10 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Maintenance of self-efficacy (7-item measure developed via elicitation study)
Yes (<0.05)
Mediation
  • Action planning (5-item measure)
Yes (<0.05)
Moderated Mediation
  • Action planning*Self-efficacy
Yes (<0.05)
MacCann, 2015 [47]Cross-sectional surveyn = 1017
US college students
Mean age: 23.1 yrs
Female: 63.9%
No follow-upPhysical activityModeration
  • Personality (6 HEXACO personality domains measured via 96-item International Personality Item Pool (IPIP))
 ○ Honesty and humility (16 items)No
 ○ Emotionality (16 items)No
 ○ Extraversion (16 items)No
 ○ Agreeableness (16 items)No
 ○ Conscientiousness (16 items)No
 ○ Openness to Experience (16 items) No
Monds, 2016 [48]Cross-sectional surveyn = 1036
US college students
Mean age: 23.08 yrs
Female: 63.9%
No follow-up Fruit and vegetable consumptionModeration
  • Personality (6 HEXACO personality domains measured via 96-item International Personality Item Pool (IPIP))
 ○ Honesty and humility (16 items)No
 ○ Emotionality (16 items)No
 ○ Extraversion (16 items)No
 ○ Agreeableness (16 items)No
 ○ Conscientiousness (16 items)No
 ○ Openness to Experience (16 items)No
Moshier, 2013 [21]Cross-sectional surveyn = 84
Adults receiving methadone maintenance treatment from 2 outpatient clinics in Boston, US
Mean age: 40 yrs
Female: 56%
No follow-upIllicit drug useModeration
  • Impulsivity (44-item UPPS Impulsivity Scale)
 ○ Urgency No
 ○ Lack of premeditationYes (0.015)
 ○ Lack of perseverance No
 ○ Sensation seekingYes (0.007)
Mullan, 2011 [30]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 153
Australian university students
Mean age: 20.1 yrs
Female: 73.86%
1 weekBinge drinking of alcoholModeration
  • Impulsivity and Self-Regulation (Executive Function measures)
 ○ Planning ability (the Tower of Hanoi task)Yes (0.03)
 ○ Inhibitory control (the Stroop Task)Yes (0.035)
 ○ Decision making (the Iowa Gambling Task)No
 ○ Cognitive flexibility (the Wisconsin Card-Sorting Task)No
Packel, 2015 [44]Cross-sectional surveyn = 96
Adults with colorectal cancer in Pennsylvania, US
Mean age: 65.6 yrs
Female: % not reported
No follow-upPhysical activityMediation
  • Planning
 ○ Action planning (4-item Action-Planning and Coping-Planning Scale—Physical Exercise)Yes (0.007)
 ○ Coping planning (5-item Action-Planning and Coping-Planning Scale—Physical Exercise)Yes (0.001)
Pfeffer, 2020 [31]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 118
University students
Mean age: 24.74 yrs
Female: 70.3%
4 weeksPhysical activityModeration
  • Trait self-control (13-item Brief Self-Control Scale)
Yes (0.033)
Rhodes, 2021 [52]2-arm parallel randomized trial (groups collapsed)—T1, T2, T3, T4n = 254
Canadian adults who were new parents
Mean age: 31.94 yrs
Female: 50%
Baseline, 6 weeks, 12 weeks, 6 months Physical activityModeration
  • Gender
Yes (<0.01)
Moderated Moderation
  • Gender*Action control (M-PAC model)
 ○ Gender*Affective attitudeNo
 ○ Gender*Perceived opportunityYes (<0.05)
 ○ Gender*PlanningNo
 ○ Gender*HabitNo
 ○ Gender*IdentityNo
Schutz, 2011 [36]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2 n = 237
HIV-positive men who have sex with men in Montreal, Canada
Mean age: 42.5 yrs
6 monthsCondom useModeration
  • Self-efficacy (11-item interview measure)
Yes a
  • Perceived behavioral control (3-item interview measure)
No
  • Past behavior (3-item interview measure)
No
  • Moral norm (3-item interview measure)
No
  • Anticipated regret (3-item interview measure)
No
  • Role beliefs (3-item interview measure)
No
  • Sociodemographics
No
  • Context
No
  • Life experience
No
Schüz, 2020 [54]Study I. Cross-sectional surveyn = 1005
US adults
Mean age: 33.6 yrs
Female: 47.1%

No follow-up1. Fruit and vegetable consumption
2. Physical activity
3. Low-fat diet
4. Alcohol consumption
5. Flossing daily
6. Testicular or breast self-exams
Moderation
  • Socioeconomic status (SES)
 ○ Education level (categorical multiple choice based on the US Census Current Population Survey and International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED))Yes (<0.05)
 ○ Income (categorical multiple choice)No
 ○ Occupation status (percentage unemployment level matched to zip code based on American Community Survey; area-level SES measure)No
 ○ Zip code (text entry; area-level SES measure) No
 ○ Subjective SES (10-point ladder subjective SES scale) No
Study II. Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 1273
International adults
Mean age: 31.57 yrs
Female: 50.5%
4 weeks1. Fruit and vegetable consumption
2. Physical activity
3. Alcohol consumption
4. Flossing daily
5. Not sitting for extended periods
6. Healthy snack consumption
Moderation
  • Socioeconomic status (SES)
 ○ Education level (categorical multiple choice based on the US Census Current Population Survey and International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED))Yes (<0.01)
 ○ Income (categorical multiple choice)No
 ○ Occupation status (personal employment)No
 ○ Subjective SES (10-point ladder subjective SES scale)Yes (<0.05)
Stevens, 2017 [35]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2 n = 77
US young adults
Mean age: 20.8 yrs
Female: 60.5%
10 daysAlcohol consumption Moderation
  • Impulsivity
 ○ Lack of planning (59-item UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale)No
 ○ Lack of perseverance (59-item UPPS-P)No
 ○ Negative urgency (59-item UPPS-P)No
 ○ Positive urgency (59-item UPPS-P)No
 ○ Sensation seeking (59-item UPPS-P)No
 ○ Response inhibition (Go–Stop Impulsivity Paradigm)No
 ○ Response initiation (Immediate memory Task (IMT))No
 ○ Delay discounting (27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ) and Two-Choice Impulsivity Paradigm (TCIP))No
Wang, 2021 [32]Cross-sectional survey n = 334
US college students.
Mean age: 21 yrs
Female: 32.3%
No follow-upSports gamblingModeration
  • Problem-gambling severity (4-item scale)
Yes (0.003)
Zhang C.Q., 2020 [41]Noncontrolled timeseries—T1, T2, T3n = 297
College students in China
Mean age: not reported (range: 18-35 yrs)
Female: 82.49%
2 monthsHand washing and sleep hygieneModeration (intention—hand washing)
  • Action planning (3-item scale)
Yes (<0.001)
  • Coping planning (3-item scale)
No
  • Action self-efficacy (3-item scale)
No
  • Maintenance self-efficacy (3-item scale)
No
Moderation (intention—sleep hygiene)
  • Action planning (3-item scale)
No
  • Coping planning (3-item scale)
No
  • Action self-efficacy (3-item scale)
No
  • Maintenance self-efficacy (3-item scale)
No
Zhang R., 2019 [39]Noncontrolled timeseries (pre-post)—T1, T2n = 157
Office employees in China
Mean age: 33.26 yrs
Female: 64.97%
1 monthTransport-related walking Moderation
  • Worksite neighborhood walkability index (abbreviated Neighborhood Environment Walkability Scale (Chinese NEWS-A))
No
a p-value not reported. * Variables interact within moderated mediation or moderated moderation analyses.
Table 2. (a) Number of articles describing a particular behavior, (b) number of articles describing moderation or mediation analyses within the intention–behavior gap, and (c) number of articles describing a particular moderator or mediator within the intention–behavior gap.
Table 2. (a) Number of articles describing a particular behavior, (b) number of articles describing moderation or mediation analyses within the intention–behavior gap, and (c) number of articles describing a particular moderator or mediator within the intention–behavior gap.
(a)
Behavior CategorySpecific Behaviors Included in Categoryn
Physical ActivityGeneral physical activity, transport-related walking, and knee pain rehabilitation exercises17
DietFruit and vegetable consumption, snacking, low-fat diet, and gluten-free diet9
Preventive Health BehaviorsFlossing, hand washing, social distancing, limited sitting, condom use, breast or testicular self-exam, and sun protection7
AddictionAlcohol use, illicit drug use, and sports gambling6
Mental HealthMindfulness meditation app, depression prevention strategies, and sleep hygiene3
Medication AdherenceAspirin adherence1
(b)
Type of Analysisn
Statistically SignificantNot Statistically SignificantTotal
Moderation191130
Mediation505
Moderated Mediation404
Moderated Moderation303
(c)
Moderator or Mediator CategoryModerator and Mediator Variablesn
Statistically
Significant
Not Statistically SignificantTotal
More SupportImpulsivity Moderation639
Self-Efficacy Moderation415
Planning Mediation505
Planning*Self-Efficacy-Moderated Mediation303
Less SupportPersonality Moderation257
Socioeconomics Moderation404
Perceptions and Beliefs Moderation202
Environment Moderation112
Habit Moderation112
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hohmann, L.A.; Garza, K.B. The Moderating Power of Impulsivity: A Systematic Literature Review Examining the Theory of Planned Behavior. Pharmacy 2022, 10, 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/pharmacy10040085

AMA Style

Hohmann LA, Garza KB. The Moderating Power of Impulsivity: A Systematic Literature Review Examining the Theory of Planned Behavior. Pharmacy. 2022; 10(4):85. https://doi.org/10.3390/pharmacy10040085

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hohmann, Lindsey A., and Kimberly B. Garza. 2022. "The Moderating Power of Impulsivity: A Systematic Literature Review Examining the Theory of Planned Behavior" Pharmacy 10, no. 4: 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/pharmacy10040085

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop