Next Article in Journal
An Overview of Systemic Health Factors Related to Rapid Oral Health Deterioration among Older People
Next Article in Special Issue
Central Sensitization in Cancer Survivors and Its Clinical Implications: State of the Art
Previous Article in Journal
Verification of Safety and Efficacy of Sleeve Gastrectomy Based on National Registry by Japanese Society for Treatment of Obesity
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Biology of Chronic Pain and Its Implications for Pain Neuroscience Education: State of the Art
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Habituation to Pain in Patients with Chronic Pain: Clinical Implications and Future Directions

by
Maite M. van der Miesen
1,*,
Catherine J. Vossen
1,2 and
Elbert A. Joosten
1,2
1
Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Management, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNS), Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences (FHML), Maastricht University, 6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Maastricht University Medical Centre, 6229 HX Maastricht, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12(13), 4305; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12134305
Submission received: 17 May 2023 / Revised: 18 June 2023 / Accepted: 20 June 2023 / Published: 27 June 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Biology of Chronic Pain: Applications in Clinical Practice)

Abstract

:
In this review, the latest insights into habituation to pain in chronic pain are summarized. Using a systematic search, results of studies on the evidence of habituation to (experimental) pain in migraine, chronic low back pain, fibromyalgia, and a variety of chronic pain indications are presented. In migraine, reduced habituation based on self-report and the EEG-based N1 and N2–P2 amplitude is reported, but the presence of contradictory results demands further replication in larger, well-designed studies. Habituation to pain in chronic low back pain seems not to differ from controls, with the exception of EEG measures. In fibromyalgia patients, there is some evidence for reduced habituation of the N2–P2 amplitude. Our analysis shows that the variability between outcomes of studies on habituation to pain is high. As the mechanisms underlying habituation to pain are still not fully understood and likely involve several pathways, it is now too early to conclude that habituation to pain is related to clinical outcomes and can be used as a diagnostic marker. The review ends with a discussion on future directions for research including the use of standard outcome measures to improve comparisons of habituation to pain in patients and controls, as well as a focus on individual differences.

1. Introduction

Habituation is a simple non-associative form of learning that is defined as a response decrement resulting from repeated stimulation, which does not involve sensory adaptation or motor fatigue [1]. Habituation has been reported for numerous stimuli such as auditory, visual, and sensory and has been measured in humans using reflexes, ratings, and physiological measures such as skin conductance, electroencephalography (EEG), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [2]. In addition to habituation, sensitization to repetitive stimulation might occur, which is defined as an increase in response [1,3]. In the field of pain, habituation is usually studied using external stimuli such as heat or electrical current [4].
Reduced habituation has been suggested to occur in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders such as autism [2]. For chronic pain, however, this is not yet well established. Although deficits in habituation may occur in relation to chronic pain, most research in the field has focused on sensitization processes, especially central sensitization. Central sensitization is defined by the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) as an increased responsiveness of the central nervous system to normal or subthreshold input [5]. Central sensitization is thought to be implicated in several chronic pain disorders such as fibromyalgia (see, e.g., [6]) and is characterized by hyperalgesia (increase in sensitivity) and allodynia (pain due to a stimulus that would normally not cause pain). Interestingly, central sensitization is often studied based on the cellular level. Recently, researchers argued that the behavioral approach, such as measuring pain ratings as an outcome, should be more emphasized in research on sensitization to pain [7]. Notably, in this review, when discussing sensitization, we refer to an increase in pain on the behavioral level.
For both healthy individuals and chronic pain patients, the mechanism of habituation and sensitization to pain is not fully understood. Several theories have been proposed such as the dual-process theory [8]. This theory states that habituation and sensitization processes may interact to produce the behavioral outcome [8]. One recent proposed mechanism is stimulus-dependent feedback inhibition or inhibitory potentiation, which decreases incoming stimuli [9]. Prior experience, thus, affects the firing of neurons. This mechanism can be seen as a form of predictive coding [9].
Reduced habituation to pain in chronic pain patients has been mainly reported in the indications migraine, chronic low back pain (CLBP), and fibromyalgia [4]. Although numerous studies have been published, no review of the literature is, to our knowledge, available. The main aim of this review is, therefore, to summarize the literature on habituation to pain based on effects of repeated painful stimulation in chronic pain patients (with a focus on migraine, CLBP, and fibromyalgia) as well as its potential treatment targets and clinical implications. We hope that this review may serve as a knowledge basis to design new innovative studies on habituation to pain in chronic pain.
In this review, results of patients versus controls are discussed. When comparing two groups with respect to habituation, several terms are used such as altered, decreased, or impaired habituation. McDiarmid and colleagues (2017) formulated recommendations to interpret the responses of repeated stimulation, i.e., habituation curves or trajectories [2]. In the literature included in the review, however, these recommendations have not been fully implemented, and a quantifiable measure of habituation is not available. For the current review, therefore, we used the term reduced habituation to pain if patients showed less habituation following repeated painful stimulation as compared to controls (i.e., the decrease in VAS was less than in controls). Furthermore, reduced habituation in patients may also include and be indicative for sensitization to pain. If available, the direction of the effect, i.e., whether patients and/or controls showed habituation, no change, or sensitization to pain, is discussed and presented in tables.

2. Materials and Methods

The search was preregistered at the Open Science Framework (osf.io/nypbw). Articles were selected using a systematic search of PubMed, PsycINFO, and Web of Science databases (up until January 2023). For extensive search criteria and the selection procedure of included articles, see the Supplementary Materials (Figure S1). The articles included from our systematic search were split into those investigating healthy individuals (revised manuscript submitted) and chronic pain patients, which are the focus of this review.

3. Results

The systematic database search of PubMed, PsycINFO, and Web of Science resulted in the inclusion of n = 40 articles. The results of this search showed that most studies were performed in patients with headache disorders (mostly migraine, n = 17), CLBP (n = 7), and fibromyalgia (n = 7) (see Figure 1A). Therefore, we subdivided these sections accordingly. Sample sizes varied greatly between nine and 199 included participants (Figure 1B). Most research included self-report ratings of pain (n = 25), whereas EEG was the most used method (n = 30) followed by solely self-report ratings (n = 8) (Figure 1C). Heat stimuli using a thermode and heat stimuli using a laser were most frequently used for repeated painful stimulation (Figure 1D). Only two studies investigated long-term habituation to pain [10,11], whereas all other studies investigated short-term habituation to pain. For this, a wide range of stimulus repetitions were used, with a median of 30 (Figure 1E). The majority of these studies performed individual calibration to decide on the stimulus intensity, whereas 42.5% of the studies used a fixed intensity level (Figure 1F).

3.1. Headache Disorders

Headache disorders are among the most common chronic central nervous system disorders, with migraine being the indication most studied [12]. In migraine research, several research lines have focused on habituation to sensory stimuli (e.g., visual, auditory, and painful responses) [13]. It has been hypothesized that reduced habituation to pain in migraine patients may be caused by increased cortical excitability, decreased inhibition, or decreased pre-activation levels [14]. Data on habituation to pain using self-report and EEG, with a focus on migraine patients are presented in Figure 2 and Table 1.
For self-report using electrical stimuli, reduced habituation was reported at the trigeminal area but not at the tibial region [15]. Other studies that included different stimulation sites showed similar habituation for self-report at different stimulation sites [16,17]. Studies using heat stimuli showed mixed effects: habituation in both patients and in controls [16], reduced habituation in patients with migraine with aura for predicted pain [18], or reduced habituation in patients without aura [19]. Interestingly, a large study using laser stimuli showed no self-reported habituation differences between migraine patients and controls [20], which contrasts with earlier findings [17,21].
Results from evoked potentials with heat stimuli showed similar habituation [16] or reduced habituation in patients versus controls [18,19]. Several studies focused on laser-evoked potentials (LEPs) and reported a reduced N2–P2 amplitude habituation in migraine patients [17,21,22,23,24]. Another study reporting evidence for reduced habituation of the N2–P2 amplitude showed that this effect over time was accompanied by increased connectivity between the thalamus and somatosensory areas in migraine patients, but not in controls [14]. Contradictorily, an observer-blinded longitudinal study with large sample sizes (n = 30–49) using advanced statistical models did not report any group differences and noted similar habituation of the N2–P2 amplitude for migraine patients and controls [20]. The authors of the latter study provided a detailed comparison of studies using LEP’s in migraine patients and argued that the evidence for reduced habituation in migraine patients is low [20]. Results for N1 amplitude habituation are less frequently reported and also mixed, with two studies reporting differences [23,25], although the latter did not compare groups directly, and one study reported no group differences [20].
Furthermore, it was shown that LEP amplitudes did not differ between chronic tension-type headache (CTTH) patients and healthy controls, although migraine patients showed reduced habituation compared to CTTH patients [23]. Additionally, the LEP amplitudes were not influenced by migraine phase [17,20], visually induced analgesia [26], or the presence or absence of aura [20]. Menstrual phase affected the amount of habituation in both migraine patients and healthy controls [24].
In summary, taking into account the large variability in study design, parameters, and outcome measures, it is tentatively concluded that there is only limited evidence that migraine patients show reduced habituation for both self-report and the N1 and N2–P2 amplitude. This conclusion takes into consideration that several studies reported contradictory effects (see Figure 2), and one blinded study with a large sample size did not report differences [20]. More conclusive evidence is needed, and this should be based on large-scale randomized study designs.
Table 1. Overview of habituation to pain in chronic headache disorders. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. C, control; CHEP, contact heat-evoked potential; CTTH, chronic tension-type headache; EEG, electroencephalography; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; M, migraine; MOA, migraine without aura; MOH, medication-overuse headache; MWA, migraine with aura; NRS, numeric rating scale; PREP, pain-related evoked potential; sLORETA, standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Table 1. Overview of habituation to pain in chronic headache disorders. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. C, control; CHEP, contact heat-evoked potential; CTTH, chronic tension-type headache; EEG, electroencephalography; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; M, migraine; MOA, migraine without aura; MOH, medication-overuse headache; MWA, migraine with aura; NRS, numeric rating scale; PREP, pain-related evoked potential; sLORETA, standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Sample SizeTimescaleType of StimuliSiteNr of Stimuli for Habituation AnalysisDurationISIHabituation MeasurementHabituationAnalysisMain Habituation Result
Bassez et al., 2020 [14]M = 23
C = 20
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Right forehead15Intensity (and, thus, duration) varied per participant, 15–45 msSelf-paced, ± 10 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitude
Dynamic causal modelling
% change between first and third block
Connectivity changes over blocks
M < C
Increased thalamo-somatosensory connectivity in migraine patients
Beese et al., 2015 [16]M = 22 (12 with aura)
C = 22
Short-termHeat (thermode)Volar forearm, left and right cheek20-15–18 sNRS: single trial
CHEP: N2–P2 amplitude
Average of first 5 trials vs. last 5 trials
Average of first 5 trials vs. last 5 trials
NRS for each site and block: M ↘ C ↘
N2–P2 for each site and block: M ↘ C ↘
de Tommaso et al., 2005 [17]M = 14 (without aura)
C = 10
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Right and left hand and supraorbital zone6020 ms10 sNRS: single trial
EEG: N2–P2 amplitude
Trend over block Trend over blockNRS both hand and face: M ↗ < C ↘
N2-P2 both hand and face: M → < C ↘
No differences between migraine phase
de Tommaso et al., 2005 [21]M = 14 (without aura)
C = 10
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Right supraorbital zone6320 ms10 sVAS: single trial
EEG: N2–P2 amplitude
Trend over block
Trend over block
VAS: M → C ↘
N2-P2: M → C ↘
de Tommaso et al., 2009 [24]M = 9 (without aura)
C = 10
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Right dorsum of the hand and supraorbital zone6025 ms10–15 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudeRatio of amplitude between block 1 and 3Both hand and face: M < C
Menstrual cycle affects habituation of N2–P2
de Tommaso et al., 2015 [22]M = 31 (without aura)
C = 19
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand3030 ms10 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudeStimuli were divided in three blocks, % change relative to first blockM < C
De Tommaso et al., 2016 [27]M = 20
C = 20
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand, right supraorbital zone and the skin over the right trapezius3030 ms7 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudeStimuli were divided in three blocks, % change relative to first blockBaseline N2–P2 hand and face: M < C
Shoulder: M = C
After onabotulintoxin A treatment, N2–P2 habituation at face increased, no effect on hand and shoulder habituation
De Tommaso et al., 2021 [28]M = 17Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand and left and right supraorbital zone3030 ms10 sEEG: N1, N2, P2 amplitudeRatio between average of last and first 10 stimuliNo effect of erenumab on N1 and P2 at both sides, increased habituation after erenumab at N2 of left forehead
Di Clemente et al., 2013 [25]M = 13 (without aura)
C = 15
Short-termHeat (YAP laser)Dorsum of the right hand and right supraorbital zone45-10 sEEG: N1 and N2–P2 amplitude% change between first and third blockN1 hand and face: M < C
N2–P2 hand and face:
M = C
Topiramate reduces N1 habituation deficit/affects habituation
Di Lorenzo et al., 2019 [29]M = 18 (without aura)
C = 18
Short-termElectricalRight supraorbital notch10Train of 3 0.1 ms pulses with 5 ms interal (total 10.3 ms)30–35 sPREP: N–P amplitudeSlope between first and second blockBefore treatment: M < C
After treatment: no group comparison
Ketogenic diet increased habituation in migraine
Ferraro et al., 2012 [30]MOH = 14 (group I treatment effective = 8; group II treatment not effective = 6)
C = 14
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand and perioral region9010 ms8–12 sVAS: after each block
EEG: N1 and N2–P2 amplitude
Percentage of the first block
Percentage of the first block
VAS: Before treatment for hand and face: MOH group I and II < C
After treatment hand: MOH group II < C and MOH group I
After treatment face: MOH group I and II = C EEG: N1 hand before treatment: MOH group I and II < C
N1 hand after treatment: MOH group I and II = C
N1 face before treatment: MOH group II < C
N1 face after treatment: MOH group I and II < C
N2–P2 hand and face before treatment: MOH group I and II < C
N2–P2 hand and face after treatment: MOH group II < C and MOH group I
Treatment affected habituation in clinically improved patients
Gierse-Plogmeier et al., 2009 [15]M = 20 (10 with aura)
C = 20
Short-termElectricalTrigeminal (masseter region), peripheral (tibial region)201 ms2 sVAS: last stimulus of trainGroup comparison of difference score between trainsTrigeminal:
M < C
Peripheral:
M = C
Lev et al., 2010 [19]M = 21 (with aura)
C = 22
Short-termHeat (thermode)Left volar forearm60-NRS: single trial
CHEP: N2–P2 amplitude and sLORETA
Group comparison of inter-train changeGroup comparison of inter-train changeNRS: M ↗ < C →
N2-P2: M ↗ < C ↘ >
M < C activity in contralateral orbitofrontal cortex
M > C in contralateral primary somatosensory cortex, insula, parahippocampal cortex, and bilateral posterior cingulate cortex
Lev et al., 2013 [18]MWA = 20MOA = 19
C = 22
Short-termHeat (thermode)Left volar forearm50-10 sNRS: single trial
CHEP: N2–P2 amplitude and sLORETA
Group comparison of inter-train change Group comparison of inter-train changeNRS: Predicted pain: MWA ↗
< C → and MOA → Unpredicted pain: MOA ↗ = MWA ↗ = C →
N2-P2: Predicted pain: MOA → and MWA ↗ < C ↘
Unpredicted pain: MOA ↗ and MWA ↗ < C →
Predicted pain: MWA < C activity in right inferior frontal gyrus and supplementary motor area
MWA > C activity in primary and secondary somatosensory cortex, motor cortex, and bilateral posterior cingulate cortex
MOA > C activity in right insula
Unpredicted pain: MWA > C activity in bilateral medial frontal cortex, right anterior cingulate cortexMOA > C activity in right motor cortex, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex, left orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal cortex, and insula
Sava et al., 2018 [26]M = 14 (without aura)
C = 11
Short-termHeat (thermode)Right volar wrist or forehead20707 ms10–22 sCHEP: P1–P2 slopeSlope over average of 5 blocksMirror did not influence habituation in controls or migraine patients
Sebastianelli et al., 2023 [31]M = 15Short-termElectricalSupraorbital nerve at the forehead18Three 0.1 ms pulses with 5 ms interval40 sEEG: N–P amplitudeSlope of the amplitude between the first and third blockNo effect of onabotulintoxin A on habituation
Uglem et al., 2017 [20]M = 49 (27 without aura, 4 with, 18 both)
C = 30
Short-termHeat (YAP laser)Dorsum of the right hand426 ms6–10 sNRS: single trial
EEG: N1 and N2–P2 amplitude
Multilevel modelsNRS: M → = C →
N1: M → = C →
N2-P2: M ↘ = C ↘
Habituation was mainly similar between migraine phases
Valeriani et al., 2003 [23]M = 24 (without aura)
CTTH = 19
C = 28
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Left and right dorsum of the hand and face45 for face, 90 for hand10 ms8–12 sEEG: N1–P1 and N2–P2 amplitude% decrease over blocksN1–P1 hand: M → C ↘
N1–P1 face: M → C →
N2–P2 hand: M ↘ < C ↘,
CTTH ↘ = C ↘, M ↘ < CTTH ↘
N2–P2 face: M → < C ↘, CTTH ↘ = C ↘
Figure 2. Effect of habituation to pain in migraine patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; NRS, numeric rating scale; VAS, visual analogue scale [14,15,17,18,19,20,22,23,24,25,29].
Figure 2. Effect of habituation to pain in migraine patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; NRS, numeric rating scale; VAS, visual analogue scale [14,15,17,18,19,20,22,23,24,25,29].
Jcm 12 04305 g002

3.2. Chronic Low Back Pain

Chronic low back pain (CLBP) is known for its high prevalence and large global impact on health and society [32]. The majority of CLBP patients have pain without a specific patho-anatomical cause, and this pain is, therefore, described as “nonspecific” [33].
The available literature for habituation to pain in CLBP patients is relatively limited, yet still diverse (see Figure 3 and Table 2). Early short-term studies using pressure or electrical stimuli did not demonstrate any self-report differences between CLBP patients and controls, independent of the presence [34] or absence [35] of habituation to pain. CLBP patients (diverse pain population and short disease duration) and controls reported similar (long-term) habituation to pain over time, as well as within-session sensitization to pain with use of an 8 day heat paradigm [10,11]. No changes in brain activity related to differences in habituation were shown between CLBP patients and controls, both over days or within sessions [11]. In patients with painful radiculopathy, LEP habituation was reduced, although this effect was not apparent for pain ratings [36]. Two studies adopted newer analysis methods for the study of habituation. Vossen et al. (2015) explored the EEG-amplitude signal at a very detailed scale, partitioning the post-stimulus epoch in 20 ms areas under the curve (event-related fixed-interval areas; ERFIAs) in combination with multilevel modeling [37]. Reduced habituation to pain was reported in CLBP patients at 340 to 460 ms post stimulus after painful stimuli [37]. The applicability of high temporal resolution analysis of LEP signals and habituation in radiculopathy patients was shown to be limited as a result of the data quality [38].
Both short- and long-term habituation to pain in CLBP patients did not seem to differ from that noted in controls according to self-report and long-term fMRI studies (Figure 3). Temporarily restricted effects, as measured with EEG, however, were shown to effect habituation to pain in CLBP patients. The latter needs further replication to investigate the robustness and reproducibility of this effect.

3.3. Fibromyalgia

Fibromyalgia is characterized by chronic widespread pain and potential comorbidities such as disturbed sleep and psychological problems [39].
Hollins et al. (2011) investigated habituation to pain as a function of the time course of pain ratings in patients with fibromyalgia and controls using heat pain stimuli [40]. Both fibromyalgia patients and controls displayed first an adaptation phase followed by a sensitization phase within each run. In addition, they showed habituation to pain over the runs. The magnitude of the initial adaptation phase increased over the runs. For both the habituation (within and over runs) and the sensitization to pain, no group effects were found [40]. Conversely, de Tommaso et al. (2011) did report differences in fibromyalgia patients compared tocontrols with respect to habituation of pain ratings [41] (see Figure 4). It should be taken into account that differences in sample size, stimulus type, and the way of measuring habituation make it difficult to compare the results from de Tommaso et al. (2011) with those reported by Hollins and colleagues (2011) (see Table 3). Analyses using EEG showed reduced habituation for the N2, P2, and N2–P2 amplitude in fibromyalgia patients, but not for the N1 amplitude [41]. Follow-up EEG studies from the same laboratory reported again reduced habituation to pain of the N2–P2 amplitude in fibromyalgia patients as compared to controls [42,43]. Interestingly, in a comparative study, a more pronounced reduction in habituation to pain was shown in patients with comorbid migraine or sensory deficits as compared with fibromyalgia patients without comorbidities [42]. Habituation to pain on the thigh (for the N2 component) and foot (for the P2 component) did not result in differences between fibromyalgia patients and controls [44].
On the basis of the EEG studies in fibromyalgia patients, there is some evidence for reduced habituation to pain of the N2–P2 amplitude (see Figure 4). These effects demand further replication in order to infer clinical significance. Only two studies investigated self-report with contradictory findings, which needs further investigation.

3.4. Other Chronic Pain Indications

In this subsection, the studies on habituation to pain related to a variety of chronic pain indications (burning mouth syndrome, temporomandibular disorder, cardia syndrome X, chronic pancreatitis, spinal cord injury-related neuropathic pain, and complex regional pain syndrome) are summarized. Temporomandibular disorder is characterized by chronic pain located in the jaw and temporomandibular joint and is a subgroup of primary orofacial pain [47,48]. The same applies to burning mouth syndrome, which may cause a chronic burning sensation in and around the mouth [49].
Using fMRI, patients with burning mouth syndrome (BMS) showed reduced brain activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), bilateral ventral midcingulate cortex (MCC), left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and cerebellum over the course of four thermal stimuli [50] (see Table 4). This habituation effect of brain activity was not noted in controls, who only showed increased brain activity in the PCC over time [50]. Patients with temporomandibular disorder (TMD) did not show different habituation from both controls and fibromyalgia patients [40].
In patients with cardiac syndrome X, reduced habituation was shown after laser stimulation, which was more apparent at the chest than at the hand for self-report ratings and the N2–P2 amplitude [51]. Olesen et al. (2013) investigated contact-heat evoked potentials (CHEPs) in chronic pancreatitis patients [52]. Reduced habituation for both pain ratings and the N2–P2 amplitude over time was shown and this was more pronounced for stimulation at the chest (pancreatic area) as compared to the forearm [52]. Studies based on analysis of patients with and without spinal cord injury (SCI)-related central neuropathic pain reported mixed results (see Table 4). One study demonstrated reduced habituation to pain in patients with neuropathic pain for pain ratings and CHEPs as compared to both healthy controls and to SCI patients without central neuropathic pain [53]. Conversely, absence of any difference in habituation of CHEPs or habituation of pain ratings between SCI with and without neuropathic pain were also reported [54,55]. It should be noted that SCI is characterized by its heterogeneity based on lesion size, location, and type of injury, and that this may significantly affect the development of chronic neuropathic pain in these patients. Moreover, although all three studies tested above the level of injury, Kumru et al. (2012) stimulated at the shoulder, while Albu et al. (2015) and Lütolf et al. (2022) stimulated at the hand and forearm, respectively. This variability on top of the heterogeneity of patients described above may underlie the differences in the literature on effect of habituation to pain in patients with this indication.
In patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) both heat pain ratings and pinprick ratings did not result in (reduced) habituation or differences between patients and healthy individuals [56]. A study using EEG including chronic pain patients based on a variety of indications reported no differences in pain ratings over a series of electrical stimuli [57]. Nevertheless, both the presence of chronic pain and the hypervigilance independently affected habituation of the EEG signal at several time latencies [57].
In summary, a trend can be noted toward reduced habituation to pain in a variety of chronic pain indications. Nonetheless, the available evidence is often based on one study for a specific pain indication and with small sample sizes. These constraints do not allow making conclusive statements about differences in habituation effects specifically related to the individual indications or to chronic pain in general.
Table 4. Overview of habituation to pain in other indications. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; BMS, burning mouth syndrome; C, control; CAD, coronary artery disease; CSX, cardiac syndrome X; CHEP, contact heat evoked potential; CP, chronic pancreatitis; CRPS, complex regional pain syndrome; EEG, electroencephalography; F, fibromyalgia; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; NRS, numeric rating scale; SCI-NP, spinal cord injury with neuropathic pain; SCI-noNP, spinal cord injury without neuropathic pain; TMD, temporomandibular disorder; VAS, visual analogue scale; VRS, verbal rating scale.
Table 4. Overview of habituation to pain in other indications. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; BMS, burning mouth syndrome; C, control; CAD, coronary artery disease; CSX, cardiac syndrome X; CHEP, contact heat evoked potential; CP, chronic pancreatitis; CRPS, complex regional pain syndrome; EEG, electroencephalography; F, fibromyalgia; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; NRS, numeric rating scale; SCI-NP, spinal cord injury with neuropathic pain; SCI-noNP, spinal cord injury without neuropathic pain; TMD, temporomandibular disorder; VAS, visual analogue scale; VRS, verbal rating scale.
Sample SizeTimescaleType of StimuliSiteNr of Stimuli for Habituation AnalysisDurationISIHabituation MeasurementHabituationAnalysisMain Habituation Result
Albu et al., 2015 [54]SCI-noNP = 10
SCI-NP = 10 C = 10
Short-termHeat (thermode)Thenar eminence of dominant hand10-20 sNRS: single trial
CHEP: N2–P2 amplitude
Percentage last stimulus with respect to first stimulusNRS and CHEP:
SCI-noNP = SCI-NP = C
Hollins et al., 2011 [40]F = 17
TMD = 29
C = 29
Short-termHeat (thermode)Base of the thumb333 s12 sVAS: single trialDecrease over blocksF = TMD = C
Kumru et al., 2012 [53]SCI-noNP = 22
SCI-NP = 32
C = 16
Short-termHeat (thermode)Shoulder14-30 sNRS: single trial
CHEP: N2–P2 amplitude
% change of last compared to first stimulusNRS and CHEP:
SCI-NP < SCI-noNP
SCI-NP < C
Lütolf et al., 2022 [55]SCI-noNP = 13, SCI-NP = 17, C = 14Short-termHeat (thermode)Right volar forearm10-15–19 sNRS: single trialPercentage decreaseSCI-noNP = SCI-NP = C
Olesen et al., 2013 [52]Chronic pancreatitis = 15
C = 15
Short-termHeat (thermode)Right forearm and upper abdominal area93-8–12 sVAS: first and last stimulus of block
CHEP: N1 and N2–P2 amplitude
Change over blocksAbdominal: CP → < C ↘
Forearm: CP → < C ↘
Abdominal:
N1: CP = C
N2–P2: CP → < C ↘
Forearm:
N1: CP = C
N2–P2: CP → = C ↘
Scheuren et al., 2023 [56]CRPS = 20, C = 16Short-termHeat (thermode), pinprickAffected and control area15-13–17sNRS: single trial% change in third compared to first block and trend over blocksHeat: CRPS → = C →
Pinprick: CRPS → = C →
Shinozaki et al., 2016 [50]BMS = 16
C = 15
Short-termHeat (thermode)Right palm and right lower lip432 s104 sNRS: single trial
fMRI: BOLD
Stimulus 1 compared to 4NRS: Lip: BMS → C ↘ Palm: BMS → C →
fMRI: Lip: Reduced activity in BMS patients over time in the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, bilateral ventral midcingulate cortex, left posterior cingulate cortex, right angular gyrus, and left cerebellum. Increased activity in controls over time in the left posterior cingulate cortex.
Valeriani et al., 2005 [51]Cardiac syndrome X = 16
Coronary artery disease = 10
C = 13
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand and chest9010 ms8–12 sVAS: per block
EEG: N1–P1 and N2–P2 amplitude
Trend over blocksChest:
VAS and N2–P2:
Cardiac SX → < C ↘ and CAD ↘
N1–P1: Cardiac SX = CAD = C
Hand:
VAS: Cardiac SX ↗ < C ↘ and CAD ↘
N1–P1: Cardiac SX = CAD = C
N2–P2: Cardiac SX = CAD = C
Vossen et al., 2018 [57]Chronic pain (various) = 33
C = 33
Short-termElectricalLeft middle finger2510 ms9–11 sVRS: single trial
EEG: amplitude
Multilevel model with event-related fixed-interval areasVRS: Chronic pain ↘ = C ↘
No influence of hypervigilance on pain ratings
Chronic pain status and hypervigilance independently influenced the EEG-amplitude

4. Treatments and Clinical Implications

In this section, the clinical implications for habituation (or sensitization) to pain in chronic pain patients and potential treatments targeting (reduced) habituation are discussed.

4.1. Habituation to Pain and Clinical Outcomes

Our search revealed that most studies on habituation to pain and clinical outcomes were related to fibromyalgia or migraine patients (see Table 1 and Table 3).
In fibromyalgia, habituation was shown to be correlated with pain at tender points [42], and patients with reduced habituation showed greater widespread pain [43]. De Tommaso et al. (2011) furthermore reported a correlation between reduced habituation of the (EEG-based) N2 amplitude and self-reported depressive symptoms in fibromyalgia patients [41], although this was not replicated in a larger scale study [42]. Furthermore, an association of habituation to pain with self-reported daily activity was reported [45]. Two studies investigated the relation between EEG signal intensity and intra-epidermal nerve fiber density (IENFD) in fibromyalgia patients. Reduced habituation of the N2–P2 component [46] or P2 component [44] was reported to be related to reduced IENFD. Subgroup analysis of those fibromyalgia patients with a reduced distal IENFD revealed that the P2 component increased over time [44].
In migraine patients, reduced habituation at the trigeminal area was correlated with migraine attack frequency [15]. Changes in brain activity in the somatosensory cortex and parietal cortex were shown to be correlated with attacks per month, whereas orbitofrontal activity correlated with disease duration [18]. Disease duration was further correlated with reduced habituation between migraine phases based on the EEG-signal (i.e., N2–P2 amplitude) [20]. Habituation to pain did not correlate with number of days until the next attack in the migraine patients [20].
In conclusion, some evidence exists that cortical habituation might be linked to the severity and frequency of pain complaints in fibromyalgia or migraine patients, as well as to IENFD in fibromyalgia patients.

4.2. Treatments Targeting Habituation to Pain

Currently, the literature on the treatments and effects on habituation to pain is limited to headache patients only. In medication-overuse headache patients, habituation of the N2–P2 amplitude was partially restored after 6 weeks in those that had clinically improved after an acute medication withdrawal treatment [30]. These findings suggest that medication overuse aggravates symptoms by central sensitization. In another study, preventive application of topiramate, an antiepileptic drug targeting among others GABA (more inhibition) and glutamate (less excitation), normalized the habituation pattern to nociceptive stimulation in migraine patients for the N1 amplitude, but at the same time did not result in effects on habituation of the N2–P2 amplitude [25]. The authors reasoned that topiramate has an effect on the sensory-discriminative component involved in habituation to pain, i.e., the secondary somatosensory cortex. Moreover, treatment with a ketogenic diet improved habituation of electrical evoked potentials, although a comparison to controls without the diet was not available [29]. A ketogenic diet has several mechanisms of action, including enhancing GABA transmission, and increasing BDNF expression and attenuation of inflammation [58]. With respect to the N2–P2 amplitude, one study reported that onabotulintoxin A (affecting neurotransmitter release) was effective for reduced habituation to pain, but only in the trigeminal area [27,59]. Furthermore, this treatment was shown to be more effective in migraine patients with severe reduced habituation [27]. This effect was, however, not shown in a similar study using electrical stimuli [31]. Furthermore, a recent pilot study reported that Erenumab (an antibody against receptors of the nociceptive neurotransmitter calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP)) affected the initially reduced habituation of the N2 amplitude in migraineurs [28]. On the basis of the findings of this pilot study, further confirmation is needed based on large-scale (randomized) studies. Numerous other pharmacological options are available for chronic pain treatment, such as nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs), opioids, pregabalin, and selective serotonin and noradrenalin reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) [60]. It would be interesting to investigate the effects of these treatments on habituation to pain.

4.3. Discussion and Future Directions

Possible treatments for habituation to pain may target different mechanisms as described above. These underlying mechanisms are complex and include various extra- and intracellular pathways. For example, topiramate has been reported to act via multiple mechanisms of action, such as the blockage of voltage-gated sodium channels, the enhancement of GABA-A receptors, the inhibition of L-type voltage-gated calcium channels, and/or the blockage of AMPA receptors [61]. These mechanisms are known to be involved in the development and maintenance of chronic pain and can be used as targets. GABA-neurotransmission is often linked to habituation of cellular processes in the CNS as its release was shown to be increased as a result of short-term habituation to an olfactory stimulus in Drosophila [9]. The mechanism of action of a ketogenic diet appears to include an anti-inflammatory and glycolytic metabolism pathway and with that appears to be an anticonvulsant. Similar to seizures, chronic pain is postulated to be related to increased excitability of neurons [62]; therefore, it is reasonable to study effects of this ketogenic diet on habituation to pain. An fMRI study in healthy participants reported evidence for a role of dopamine in habituation to pain, based on use of the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist haloperidol [63].
A recent review focusing on the genetic and molecular changes involved in habituation in general illustrated the complexity of the mechanism of action and molecules involved in habituation. In this review, various cellular pathways were highlighted, and the identification of 258 genes were reported as possible targets for drugs [64]. From this perspective, future research could investigate the effects of many more candidate drugs and their effect on habituation (and sensitization) to pain.
In conclusion, the mechanisms underlying habituation to pain are poorly understood and likely to be related to a complex set of pathways including those related to inflammation, immune responses, neurotrophins, and/or neurotransmission. Research should focus on which pathways and molecules are most dominant in order to target them specifically, and this then may result in major impact on habituation to pain. In addition, further research may include other chronic pain indications and pharmacological options targeting habituation to pain.
For now, no diagnostic markers are available for the prediction of habituation to pain. In addition, it is unclear when reduced habituation is of clinical relevance. For example, is a decrease of 0.5 point versus a 1.0 point decrease on the VAS after repeated stimulation an indication of reduced habituation to pain? Overall, it is too early to state that habituation trajectories (the response pattern resulting from repeated painful stimulation) are linked to clinical outcomes and could be used as a diagnostic marker for the prediction of chronic pain. Specifically for migraine patients, Brighina and colleagues stated that lack of habituation to pain probably represents a more general marker of neural dysfunction, with overlap of migraine with other pathologies such as chronic pain and Parkinson’s disease [65].

5. Challenges in the Field

There are several challenges in the field of habituation to pain in chronic pain conditions. Importantly, chronic pain indications are very heterogeneous. In addition, even within each individual chronic pain condition, there might be age and sex differences and differences in medication use (e.g., [11,38,43]). Furthermore, the experimental pain paradigms used are very diverse, including different modalities (e.g., heat and electric), stimulation sites, and the measure of habituation. Moreover, the link to clinical outcomes and experience of (chronic) pain in the studies is limited. The baseline pain levels of patients could potentially affect habituation, but this has not yet been investigated, with the exception of one study reporting reduced habituation in patients with greater widespread pain [43]. Furthermore, the experimental pain paradigms used in the studies included in this systematic review are not necessarily clinical pain-provoking. The latter would be of interest for the field. However, in addition to these differences, it is still of interest to investigate whether the antinociceptive system(s) differ in patients with chronic pain as compared to pain-free subjects. It is hypothesized that several neuroplasticity changes have already occurred (e.g., central sensitization) in chronic pain patients, and these changes may contribute to reduced habituation to pain [6,66,67]. In conclusion, although it is a challenge to standardize experimental pain paradigms in relation to specific pain indications it should be given much more attention in future studies. This is needed to better understand general effects on habituation to pain in chronic pain.

6. Future Directions for Research

Future research in chronic pain patients may inform us on the robustness of differences in habituation to pain in chronic pain patients as compared to (for instance) healthy controls and its underlying mechanisms. Neural measures such as EEG and fMRI could be analyzed in more detail using, for example, multilevel models for increased understanding of habituation to pain in chronic pain patients. Currently, evidence linking the self-report (behavioral) and EEG or fMRI (neural) measures is limited.
A second point which can be concluded from our review is that most studies were based on small sample sizes (median = 19.5) and did not always include a control group. Hence, there is a need for larger, blinded studies (i.e., the assessor is blinded for the group), including control groups and randomized controlled trials for potential treatment effects. In addition, direct group comparisons are necessary to obtain more conclusive results. Our review showed that one group sometimes showed significant habituation, while the other group did not; hence, it was concluded that there was no difference between patients and controls. However, without directly comparing groups, this conclusion cannot be made [68].
In general, chronic pain indications are very heterogeneous, and this makes generalization of conclusions often very difficult. An alternative might be to focus on individual differences in habituation to pain. Studying individual characteristics and differences may result into a better understanding of the heterogeneity in both patients and controls, and these effects may then be linked to clinical outcomes. Current studies in chronic pain patients did not focus on individual differences or age- and sex-related differences. This, however, would be an interesting topic for further research as studies in healthy individuals pointed out large individual differences (e.g., [69]), but conflicting evidence for age and sex (e.g., [38,70,71,72]. Ideally, individual differences in habituation to pain could also be used in prediction models for chronic pain or treatment effects. Longitudinal designs might then help to unravel the role of habituation in (the transition to) chronic pain. Investigation of a surgical population as they may develop postoperative (chronic) pain is recommended [73].
An important issue in the correct analysis of studies on habituation to pain is the use and selection of statistical tests. In order to test and improve comparison of effects in studies on habituation to pain, we are in need of clear standardized measures to compare across studies and between patients and controls. Currently, there are several outcome measures for habituation to pain such as direct comparison of trials, linear effects (e.g., tested with a repeated-measures ANOVA), percentage change over averaged trials, habituation quotient (i.e., ratio between the average response in the first and last block), or fitting a (linear or quadratic) slope. With this variability in outcome measures, a standardized systematic comparison (such as in a meta-analysis) is not possible. Recently, recommendations for interpreting different habituation (to pain) patterns have been proposed [2]. With this, effects on habituation to pain might possibly be linked to phenotypes. On some occasions, it could be that patients show similar reduced habituation, but that the control group shows a different effect (see Figure 5), which is not captured in statistical tests.
Therefore, we propose the fitting of a slope as indicator of the trajectory of the habituation (linear, quadratic, etc.). The use of these slopes and trajectory of habituation to pain has several advantages over current measures; it does not require any calculation of the dependent variable (such as averaging), it is easy interpretable and indicates the direction of effects, i.e., habituation or sensitization, and, when tested against zero, it can also indicate the significance of changes compared to zero (see Figure 6). However, it would require a fixed number of trials to be comparable across studies, and it can be influenced by other factors such as interstimulus interval and type of stimulation. This is in general of influence for habituation to pain, which is why the field will greatly benefit from standard setups and measures. If the field progresses to standard protocols and outcome measures as they are currently used in quantitative sensory testing (QST), it will be possible to increase the understanding of habituation to pain and its potential role and link to chronic pain [74,75]. In addition, recently developed statistical analysis methods, such as the high-temporal-resolution EEG analysis method and the event-related fixed-interval area method, are promising improvements in the detailed investigation of habituation of pain [38,76].
Furthermore, recommendations such as the use of standard terminology, comparison of similar outcome measures (i.e., not comparing EEG effects with rating effects), taking into account the use of different timescales when analyzing and interpreting the data, and taking into account individual differences will improve future study design and analyses.

Limitations

In this review, the effects of habituation to non-painful stimuli, the pain threshold, physiological measures such as skin conductance, and stimulation paradigms where the intensity was adjusted were not included (e.g., [77,78]). Thus, reduced habituation to pain in chronic pain patients may exist according to the use of different measures, and future research is needed to explore these measures. Furthermore, this review focused mainly on habituation to pain but not on sensitization to pain in chronic pain patients. These closely related processes should preferably be described and studied together, but most studies only deal with either habituation or sensitization to pain.

7. Conclusions

This review systematically summarized the available evidence on habituation to pain in different chronic pain indications. Although several studies reported reduced habituation to pain in migraine for self-report and the EEG-based N1 and N2–P2 amplitude, further evidence and confirmation based on larger, well-designed studies is needed. In CLBP patients, the evidence argues against any general differences, except for EEG measures. In fibromyalgia, there is evidence for reduced habituation to pain of the N2–P2 amplitude. Currently, the evidence of a diagnostic marker or linking habituation to pain to clinical outcomes is limited. Future studies should include standard outcome measures to improve the comparison of habituation to pain in chronic pain patients and controls. The mechanisms underlying habituation to pain are poorly understood and likely to be related to a complex set of pathways. Recent use of genetic and molecular analysis techniques allows for better understanding and selection of new pharmacological treatment options which then may help to reduce pain in chronic pain patients.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm12134305/s1: Material and Methods; Figure S1. Flow diagram of selection and inclusion; Table S1: Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Iris Coppieters for helpful comments on the manuscript. Our dear colleague Amanda Kaas significantly contributed to the topic and preregistration of the review but sadly passed away before the manuscript was finished.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rankin, C.H.; Abrams, T.; Barry, R.J.; Bhatnagar, S.; Clayton, D.F.; Colombo, J.; Coppola, G.; Geyer, M.A.; Glanzman, D.L.; Marsland, S. Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009, 92, 135–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. McDiarmid, T.A.; Bernardos, A.C.; Rankin, C.H. Habituation is altered in neuropsychiatric disorders-A comprehensive review with recommendations for experimental design and analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 80, 286–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Thompson, R.F.; Spencer, W.A. Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol. Rev. 1966, 73, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. De Paepe, A.L.; Williams, A.C.C.; Crombez, G. Habituation to pain: A motivational-ethological perspective. Pain 2019, 160, 1693–1697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. IASP. International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP). Available online: https://www.iasp-pain.org/resources/terminology/. (accessed on 11 April 2023).
  6. Harte, S.E.; Harris, R.E.; Clauw, D.J. The neurobiology of central sensitization. J. Appl. Biobehav. Res. 2018, 23, e12137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. van den Broeke, E.; Crombez, G.; Vlaeyen, J.W. Re-conceptualizing sensitization in pain: A case for a behavioural approach. PsychArchives 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Groves, P.M.; Thompson, R.F. Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psychol. Rev. 1970, 77, 419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ramaswami, M. Network plasticity in adaptive filtering and behavioral habituation. Neuron 2014, 82, 1216–1229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. May, A.; Rodriguez-Raecke, R.; Schulte, A.; Ihle, K.; Breimhorst, M.; Birklein, F.; Jürgens, T.P. Within-session sensitization and between-session habituation: A robust physiological response to repetitive painful heat stimulation. Eur. J. Pain 2012, 16, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Rodriguez-Raecke, R.; Ihle, K.; Ritter, C.; Muhtz, C.; Otte, C.; May, A. Neuronal differences between chronic low back pain and depression regarding long-term habituation to pain. Eur. J. Pain 2014, 18, 701–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Leonardi, M.; Steiner, T.J.; Scher, A.T.; Lipton, R.B. The global burden of migraine: Measuring disability in headache disorders with WHO’s Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). J. Headache Pain 2005, 6, 429–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Casillo, F.; Sebastianelli, G.; Di Renzo, A.; Cioffi, E.; Parisi, V.; Di Lorenzo, C.; Serrao, M.; Coppola, G. The monoclonal CGRP-receptor blocking antibody erenumab has different effects on brainstem and cortical sensory-evoked responses. Cephalalgia 2022, 42, 1236–1245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bassez, I.; Van de Steen, F.; Ricci, K.; Vecchio, E.; Gentile, E.; Marinazzo, D.; de Tommaso, M. Dynamic Causal Modelling of the Reduced Habituation to Painful Stimuli in Migraine: An EEG Study. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Gierse-Plogmeier, B.; Colak-Ekici, R.; Wolowski, A.; Gralow, I.; Marziniak, M.; Evers, S. Differences in trigeminal and peripheral electrical pain perception in women with and without migraine. J. Headache Pain 2009, 10, 249–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Beese, L.C.; Putzer, D.; Osada, N.; Evers, S.; Marziniak, M. Contact heat evoked potentials and habituation measured interictally in migraineurs. J. Headache Pain 2015, 16, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. de Tommaso, M.; Lo Sito, L.; Di Fruscolo, O.; Sardaro, M.; Pia Prudenzano, M.; Lamberti, P.; Livrea, P. Lack of habituation of nociceptive evoked responses and pain sensitivity during migraine attack. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2005, 116, 1254–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Lev, R.; Granovsky, Y.; Yarnitsky, D. Enhanced pain expectation in migraine: EEG-based evidence for impaired prefrontal function. Headache 2013, 53, 1054–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Lev, R.; Granovsky, Y.; Yarnitsky, D. Orbitofrontal disinhibition of pain in migraine with aura: An interictal EEG-mapping study. Cephalalgia 2010, 30, 910–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Uglem, M.; Omland, P.M.; Stjern, M.; Gravdahl, G.B.; Sand, T. Habituation of laser-evoked potentials by migraine phase: A blinded longitudinal study. J. Headache Pain 2017, 18, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. de Tommaso, M.; Libro, G.; Guido, M.; Losito, L.; Lamberti, P.; Livrea, P. Habituation of single CO2 laser-evoked responses during interictal phase of migraine. J. Headache Pain 2005, 6, 195–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. de Tommaso, M.; Trotta, G.; Vecchio, E.; Ricci, K.; Van de Steen, F.; Montemurno, A.; Lorenzo, M.; Marinazzo, D.; Bellotti, R.; Stramaglia, S. Functional Connectivity of EEG Signals Under Laser Stimulation in Migraine. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Valeriani, M.; de Tommaso, M.; Restuccia, D.; Le Pera, D.; Guido, M.; Iannetti, G.D.; Libro, G.; Truini, A.; Di Trapani, G.; Puca, F.; et al. Reduced habituation to experimental pain in migraine patients: A CO2 laser evoked potential study. Pain 2003, 105, 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. de Tommaso, M.; Valeriani, M.; Sardaro, M.; Serpino, C.; Fruscolo, O.D.; Vecchio, E.; Cerbo, R.; Livrea, P. Pain perception and laser evoked potentials during menstrual cycle in migraine. J. Headache Pain 2009, 10, 423–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Di Clemente, L.; Puledda, F.; Biasiotta, A.; Viganò, A.; Vicenzini, E.; Truini, A.; Cruccu, G.; Di Piero, V. Topiramate modulates habituation in migraine: Evidences from nociceptive responses elicited by laser evoked potentials. J. Headache Pain 2013, 14, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Sava, S.L.; de Pasqua, V.; de Noordhout, A.M.; Schoenen, J. Visually induced analgesia during face or limb stimulation in healthy and migraine subjects. J. Pain Res. 2018, 11, 1821–1828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. De Tommaso, M.; Delussi, M.; Ricci, K.; Montemurno, A.; Carbone, I.; Vecchio, E. Effects of onabotulintoxinA on habituation of laser evoked responses in chronic migraine. Toxins 2016, 8, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. de Tommaso, M.; Delussi, M.; Gentile, E.; Ricci, K.; Quitadamo, S.G.; Libro, G. Effect of single dose Erenumab on cortical responses evoked by cutaneous a-delta fibers: A pilot study in migraine patients. Cephalalgia 2021, 41, 1004–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Di Lorenzo, C.; Coppola, G.; Bracaglia, M.; Di Lenola, D.; Sirianni, G.; Rossi, P.; Di Lorenzo, G.; Parisi, V.; Serrao, M.; Cervenka, M.C.; et al. A ketogenic diet normalizes interictal cortical but not subcortical responsivity in migraineurs. BMC Neurol 2019, 19, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Ferraro, D.; Vollono, C.; Miliucci, R.; Virdis, D.; De Armas, L.; Pazzaglia, C.; Le Pera, D.; Tarantino, S.; Balestri, M.; Di Trapani, G.; et al. Habituation to pain in “medication overuse headache”: A CO2 laser-evoked potential study. Headache 2012, 52, 792–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Sebastianelli, G.; Casillo, F.; Di Renzo, A.; Abagnale, C.; Cioffi, E.; Parisi, V.; Di Lorenzo, C.; Serrao, M.; Pierelli, F.; Schoenen, J.; et al. Effects of Botulinum Toxin Type A on the Nociceptive and Lemniscal Somatosensory Systems in Chronic Migraine: An Electrophysiological Study. Toxins 2023, 15, 76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hoy, D.; Bain, C.; Williams, G.; March, L.; Brooks, P.; Blyth, F.; Woolf, A.; Vos, T.; Buchbinder, R. A systematic review of the global prevalence of low back pain. Arthritis Rheum. 2012, 64, 2028–2037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Maher, C.; Underwood, M.; Buchbinder, R. Non-specific low back pain. Lancet 2017, 389, 736–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Arntz, A.; Merckelbach, H.; Peters, M.; Schmidt, A.J. Chronic low back pain, response specificity and habituation to painful stimuli. J. Psychophysiol. 1991, 5, 177–188. [Google Scholar]
  35. Peters, M.L.; Schmidt, A.J.; Van den Hout, M.A. Chronic low back pain and the reaction to repeated acute pain stimulation. Pain 1989, 39, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hüllemann, P.; von der Brelie, C.; Manthey, G.; Düsterhöft, J.; Helmers, A.K.; Synowitz, M.; Baron, R. Reduced laser-evoked potential habituation detects abnormal central pain processing in painful radiculopathy patients. Eur. J. Pain 2017, 21, 918–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Vossen, C.J.; Vossen, H.G.; Joosten, E.A.; van Os, J.; Lousberg, R. Does habituation differ in chronic low back pain subjects compared to pain-free controls? A cross-sectional pain rating ERP study reanalyzed with the ERFIA multilevel method. Medicine 2015, 94, e865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Kersebaum, D.; Fabig, S.C.; Sendel, M.; Muntean, A.C.; Baron, R.; Hullemann, P. Revealing the time course of laser-evoked potential habituation by high temporal resolution analysis. Eur. J. Pain 2021, 25, 2112–2128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Häuser, W.; Ablin, J.; Fitzcharles, M.-A.; Littlejohn, G.; Luciano, J.V.; Usui, C.; Walitt, B. Fibromyalgia. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2015, 1, 15022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hollins, M.; Harper, D.; Maixner, W. Changes in pain from a repetitive thermal stimulus: The roles of adaptation and sensitization. Pain 2011, 152, 1583–1590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. de Tommaso, M.; Federici, A.; Santostasi, R.; Calabrese, R.; Vecchio, E.; Lapadula, G.; Iannone, F.; Lamberti, P.; Livrea, P. Laser-evoked potentials habituation in fibromyalgia. J. Pain 2011, 12, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. de Tommaso, M.; Nolano, M.; Iannone, F.; Vecchio, E.; Ricci, K.; Lorenzo, M.; Delussi, M.; Girolamo, F.; Lavolpe, V.; Provitera, V.; et al. Update on laser-evoked potential findings in fibromyalgia patients in light of clinical and skin biopsy features. J. Neurol. 2014, 261, 461–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. de Tommaso, M.; Ricci, K.; Libro, G.; Vecchio, E.; Delussi, M.; Montemurno, A.; Lopalco, G.; Iannone, F. Pain Processing and Vegetative Dysfunction in Fibromyalgia: A Study by Sympathetic Skin Response and Laser Evoked Potentials. Pain Res. Treat. 2017, 2017, 9747148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Vecchio, E.; Quitadamo, S.G.; Ricci, K.; Libro, G.; Delussi, M.; Lombardi, R.; Lauria, G.; de Tommaso, M. Laser evoked potentials in fibromyalgia with peripheral small fiber involvement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2022, 135, 96–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. McLoughlin, M.J.; Stegner, A.J.; Cook, D.B. The relationship between physical activity and brain responses to pain in fibromyalgia. J. Pain 2011, 12, 640–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Vecchio, E.; Lombardi, R.; Paolini, M.; Libro, G.; Delussi, M.; Ricci, K.; Quitadamo, S.G.; Gentile, E.; Girolamo, F.; Iannone, F.; et al. Peripheral and central nervous system correlates in fibromyalgia. Eur. J. Pain 2020, 24, 1537–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Gauer, R.; Semidey, M.J. Diagnosis and treatment of temporomandibular disorders. Am. Fam. Physician 2015, 91, 378–386. [Google Scholar]
  48. Nicholas, M.; Vlaeyen, J.W.S.; Rief, W.; Barke, A.; Aziz, Q.; Benoliel, R.; Cohen, M.; Evers, S.; Giamberardino, M.A.; Goebel, A.; et al. The IASP classification of chronic pain for ICD-11: Chronic primary pain. Pain 2019, 160, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Jääskeläinen, S.K.; Woda, A. Burning mouth syndrome. Cephalalgia 2017, 37, 627–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Shinozaki, T.; Imamura, Y.; Kohashi, R.; Dezawa, K.; Nakaya, Y.; Sato, Y.; Watanabe, K.; Morimoto, Y.; Shizukuishi, T.; Abe, O.; et al. Spatial and Temporal Brain Responses to Noxious Heat Thermal Stimuli in Burning Mouth Syndrome. J. Dent. Res. 2016, 95, 1138–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Valeriani, M.; Sestito, A.; Le Pera, D.; De Armas, L.; Infusino, F.; Maiese, T.; Sgueglia, G.A.; Tonali, P.A.; Crea, F.; Restuccia, D.; et al. Abnormal cortical pain processing in patients with cardiac syndrome X. Eur. Heart J. 2005, 26, 975–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Olesen, S.S.; Hansen, T.M.; Graversen, C.; Valeriani, M.; Drewes, A.M. Cerebral excitability is abnormal in patients with painful chronic pancreatitis. Eur. J. Pain 2013, 17, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kumru, H.; Soler, D.; Vidal, J.; Tormos, J.M.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Valls-Sole, J. Evoked potentials and quantitative thermal testing in spinal cord injury patients with chronic neuropathic pain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012, 123, 598–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Albu, S.; Gómez-Soriano, J.; Avila-Martin, G.; Taylor, J. Deficient conditioned pain modulation after spinal cord injury correlates with clinical spontaneous pain measures. Pain 2015, 156, 260–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lütolf, R.; Rosner, J.; Curt, A.; Hubli, M. Indicators of central sensitization in chronic neuropathic pain after spinal cord injury. Eur. J. Pain 2022, 26, 2162–2175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Scheuren, P.S.; De Schoenmacker, I.; Rosner, J.; Brunner, F.; Curt, A.; Hubli, M. Pain-autonomic measures reveal nociceptive sensitization in complex regional pain syndrome. Eur. J. Pain 2023, 27, 72–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Vossen, C.J.; Luijcks, R.; van Os, J.; Joosten, E.A.; Lousberg, R. Does pain hypervigilance further impact the lack of habituation to pain in individuals with chronic pain? A cross-sectional pain ERP study. J. Pain Res. 2018, 11, 395–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Masino, S.A.; Rho, J.M. Mechanisms of Ketogenic Diet Action. In Jasper’s Basic Mechanisms of the Epilepsies; Noebels, J.L., Avoli, M., Rogawski, M.A., Olsen, R.W., Delgado-Escueta, A.V., Eds.; National Center for Biotechnology Information (US): Bethesda, MD, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  59. Burstein, R.; Blumenfeld, A.M.; Silberstein, S.D.; Manack Adams, A.; Brin, M.F. Mechanism of Action of OnabotulinumtoxinA in Chronic Migraine: A Narrative Review. Headache J. Head Face Pain 2020, 60, 1259–1272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Marcianò, G.; Vocca, C.; Evangelista, M.; Palleria, C.; Muraca, L.; Galati, C.; Monea, F.; Sportiello, L.; De Sarro, G.; Capuano, A. The Pharmacological Treatment of Chronic Pain: From Guidelines to Daily Clinical Practice. Pharmaceutics 2023, 15, 1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Chong, M.; Libretto, S.E. The rationale and use of topiramate for treating neuropathic pain. Clin. J. Pain 2003, 19, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Masino, S.A.; Ruskin, D.N. Ketogenic diets and pain. J. Child Neurol. 2013, 28, 993–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Bauch, E.M.; Andreou, C.; Rausch, V.H.; Bunzeck, N. Neural Habituation to Painful Stimuli Is Modulated by Dopamine: Evidence from a Pharmacological fMRI Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Blok, L.E.R.; Boon, M.; van Reijmersdal, B.; Höffler, K.D.; Fenckova, M.; Schenck, A. Genetics, molecular control and clinical relevance of habituation learning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 143, 104883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Brighina, F.; Cosentino, G.; Fierro, B. Is lack of habituation a biomarker of migraine? A critical perspective. J. Headache Pain 2015, 16, A13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Rennefeld, C.; Wiech, K.; Schoell, E.D.; Lorenz, J.; Bingel, U. Habituation to pain: Further support for a central component. Pain 2010, 148, 503–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ji, R.R.; Kohno, T.; Moore, K.A.; Woolf, C.J. Central sensitization and LTP: Do pain and memory share similar mechanisms? Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26, 696–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Nieuwenhuis, S.; Forstmann, B.U.; Wagenmakers, E.-J. Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: A problem of significance. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 1105–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Slepian, P.M.; France, C.R.; Rhudy, J.L.; Himawan, L.K.; Güereca, Y.M.; Kuhn, B.L.; Palit, S. Behavioral Inhibition and Behavioral Activation are Related to Habituation of Nociceptive Flexion Reflex, but Not Pain Ratings. J. Pain 2017, 18, 349–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Edwards, R.R.; Fillingim, R.B. Effects of age on temporal summation and habituation of thermal pain: Clinical relevance in healthy older and younger adults. J. Pain 2001, 2, 307–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Greffrath, W.; Baumgärtner, U.; Treede, R.D. Peripheral and central components of habituation of heat pain perception and evoked potentials in humans. Pain 2007, 132, 301–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Jepma, M.; Jones, M.; Wager, T.D. The dynamics of pain: Evidence for simultaneous site-specific habituation and site-nonspecific sensitization in thermal pain. J. Pain 2014, 15, 734–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Hoofwijk, D.; Fiddelers, A.A.; Peters, M.L.; Stessel, B.; Kessels, A.G.; Joosten, E.A.; Gramke, H.-F.; Marcus, M.A. Prevalence and predictive factors of chronic postsurgical pain and poor global recovery 1 year after outpatient surgery. Clin. J. Pain 2015, 31, 1017–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Rolke, R.; Baron, R.; Maier, C.A.; Tölle, T.; Treede, R.-D.; Beyer, A.; Binder, A.; Birbaumer, N.; Birklein, F.; Bötefür, I. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): Standardized protocol and reference values. Pain 2006, 123, 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Rolke, R.; Magerl, W.; Campbell, K.A.; Schalber, C.; Caspari, S.; Birklein, F.; Treede, R.D. Quantitative sensory testing: A comprehensive protocol for clinical trials. Eur. J. Pain 2006, 10, 77–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Vossen, C.J.; Vossen, H.G.; Marcus, M.A.; van Os, J.; Lousberg, R. Introducing the event related fixed interval area (ERFIA) multilevel technique: A method to analyze the complete epoch of event-related potentials at single trial level. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Coppola, G.; Pierelli, F.; Schoenen, J. Habituation and migraine. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009, 92, 249–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Flor, H.; Diers, M.; Birbaumer, N. Peripheral and electrocortical responses to painful and non-painful stimulation in chronic pain patients, tension headache patients and healthy controls. Neurosci. Lett. 2004, 361, 147–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Overview of included studies. Gray diamonds indicate the median. (A) Number of studies per chronic pain indication. (B) Sample sizes for chronic and healthy populations. (C) Number of studies per modality and whether they included self-report. (D) Type of stimulation used. (E) Number of stimuli to measure habituation. (F) Use of individual calibration in studies. BMS, burning mouth syndrome; CLBP, chronic low back pain; CRPS, complex regional pain syndrome; CTTH, chronic tension-type headache; EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; MOH, medication-overuse headache; SCI-NP, spinal cord injury with neuropathic pain; SCI-noNP, spinal cord injury without neuropathic pain; TMD, temporomandibular disorder.
Figure 1. Overview of included studies. Gray diamonds indicate the median. (A) Number of studies per chronic pain indication. (B) Sample sizes for chronic and healthy populations. (C) Number of studies per modality and whether they included self-report. (D) Type of stimulation used. (E) Number of stimuli to measure habituation. (F) Use of individual calibration in studies. BMS, burning mouth syndrome; CLBP, chronic low back pain; CRPS, complex regional pain syndrome; CTTH, chronic tension-type headache; EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; MOH, medication-overuse headache; SCI-NP, spinal cord injury with neuropathic pain; SCI-noNP, spinal cord injury without neuropathic pain; TMD, temporomandibular disorder.
Jcm 12 04305 g001
Figure 3. Effect of habituation to pain in CLBP patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; VAS, visual analogue scale [10,11,34,35,36,37].
Figure 3. Effect of habituation to pain in CLBP patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; VAS, visual analogue scale [10,11,34,35,36,37].
Jcm 12 04305 g003
Figure 4. Effect of habituation to pain in fibromyalgia patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; VAS, visual analogue scale [40,41,42,43,44].
Figure 4. Effect of habituation to pain in fibromyalgia patients versus controls. Note: Only those studies using direct group comparisons are included. EEG, electroencephalography; VAS, visual analogue scale [40,41,42,43,44].
Jcm 12 04305 g004
Figure 5. Example of patient groups that show similar habituation, whereas control groups differ in their response. With conventional analyses, panel one will result in no group differences whereas panel two will result in group differences, complicating the conclusion of patient vs. control effects.
Figure 5. Example of patient groups that show similar habituation, whereas control groups differ in their response. With conventional analyses, panel one will result in no group differences whereas panel two will result in group differences, complicating the conclusion of patient vs. control effects.
Jcm 12 04305 g005
Figure 6. Example of data with a fitted (linear) slope that indicates the direction of the effect (after tested against zero), i.e., habituation, no change, or sensitization.
Figure 6. Example of data with a fitted (linear) slope that indicates the direction of the effect (after tested against zero), i.e., habituation, no change, or sensitization.
Jcm 12 04305 g006
Table 2. Overview of habituation to pain in CLBP. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; C, control; CLBP; chronic low back pain; EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; ISI, inter-stimulus interval, NRS, numeric rating scale; P, painful radiculopathy; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Table 2. Overview of habituation to pain in CLBP. ↘: decrease over time (habituation). →: no significant change. ↗: increase over time (sensitization). <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; C, control; CLBP; chronic low back pain; EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; ISI, inter-stimulus interval, NRS, numeric rating scale; P, painful radiculopathy; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Sample SizeTimescaleType of StimuliSiteNr of Stimuli for Habituation AnalysisDurationISIHabituation MeasurementHabituationAnalysisMain Habituation Result
Arntz et al., 1991 [34]CLBP = 22
C = 21
Short-termElectricalThumb of left hand201 s15–45 sVAS: pretest, last trial of blocks, post-testTrend over timeCLBP ↘ = C ↘
Hüllemann et al., 2017 [36]Painful radiculopathy = 27
C = 20
Short-termHeat (YAP laser)Middle ventral thigh1005 ms8–12 sNRS: single trail EEG: N2–P2 amplitudeTrend over blocksP ↘ = C ↘
P ↘ < C ↘
Kersebaum et al., 2021 [38]Painful radiculopathy n = 14 for twelve blocks and
n = 18 for six blocks, controls n = 10 for twelve blocks and
n = 14 for six blocks
Short-termHeat (YAP laser)Middle ventral thigh1005 ms8–12 sNRS: single trial
EEG: N2–P2 amplitude
High-temporal-resolution analysisNRS: over 12 blocks P ↘ C NA, over six blocks P → C →
N2–P2: over 12 blocks P ↘ C NA, over six blocks P ↘ C →
May et al., 2012 [10]CLBP = 21
C = 66
Long-term (8 days)Heat (thermode)Left volar forearm4806 sVAS: average rating of last 6 stimuliTrend over timeWithin-session: CLBP ↗ = C ↗
Between-session: CLBP ↘ = C ↘
Peters et al., 1989 [35]CLBP = 20
C = 20
Short-termPressureIndex finger of non-dominant hand670% of pain tolerance time4 minVAS: single trialsTrend over timeCLBP → = C →
Rodriguez-Raecke et al., 2014 [11]CLBP = 19
C = 21
Long-term (8 days)Heat (thermode)Left volar forearm4806 sVAS: average rating of last 6 stimulifMRI: BOLDTrend over time
Whole-brain contrast
Within-session: CLBP ↗ = C ↗
Between-session: CLBP ↘ = C ↘
CLBP = C
Vossen et al., 2015 [37]CLBP = 65
C = 76
Short-termElectricalLeft middle finger15010 ms9–11 sEEG: amplitudeMultilevel model with event-related fixed-interval areasCLBP ↘ < C ↘
Table 3. Overview of habituation to pain in fibromyalgia patients. <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. C, control; EEG, electroencephalography; F = fibromyalgia; FMD; fibromyalgia with proximal and distal denervation; FMN, fibromyalgia with normal skin biopsy; FMP, fibromyalgia with proximal denervation; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; M = migraine; TMD, temporomandibular disorder; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Table 3. Overview of habituation to pain in fibromyalgia patients. <: reduced habituation in the chronic pain patient compared to the control group. =: no significant difference in habituation between the groups. C, control; EEG, electroencephalography; F = fibromyalgia; FMD; fibromyalgia with proximal and distal denervation; FMN, fibromyalgia with normal skin biopsy; FMP, fibromyalgia with proximal denervation; ISI, inter-stimulus interval; M = migraine; TMD, temporomandibular disorder; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Sample SizeTimescaleType of StimuliSiteNr of Stimuli for Habituation AnalysisDurationISIHabituation MeasurementHabituationAnalysisMain Habituation Result
de Tommaso et al., 2011 [41]F = 14
C = 13
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand, right supraorbital zone and knee2025 ms10 sVAS: average per block EEG: N1, N2, P2, and N2–P2 amplitudeQuotient between third and first blockVAS: F < C
N1: F = C
N2, P2, and N2–P2: F < C
No differences between sites
Self-reported depressive symptoms correlate with N2 habituation
de Tommaso et al., 2014 [42]F combined = 199
F = 94
F with M = 70F with sensory deficits = 35
C = 109
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand, chest and knee1030 ms10 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudeQuotient between third and first blockAll sites: F combined < C
F < C
F with M < C
F with sensory deficits < C
F with M < F
F with M < F with sensory deficits
No correlation between habituation and self-reported depressive symptoms
de Tommaso et al., 2017 [43]F = 50
C = 30
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand and foot3030 ms10 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudePercent amplitude change between third and first group of responsesHand and foot: F < C
Hollins et al., 2011 [40]F = 17
TMD = 29
C = 29
Short-termHeat (thermode)Base of the thumb333 s12 sVAS: single trialDecrease over blocksF = TMD = C
McLoughlin et al., 2011 [45]F = 16
C = 18
Short-termHeat (thermode)Left hand palm520 s20 sVAS: single trialDifference scoreSelf-reported activity correlated negatively with pain and unpleasantness difference scores in patients
Vecchio et al., 2020 [46]F = 81Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Dorsum of the right hand, in subgroups also thorax and dorsum of the foot3030 ms10 sEEG: N2–P2 amplitudeRatio between third and first blockThigh: habituation index of N2–P2 predicted intra-epidermal nerve fiber density
Vecchio et al., 2022 [44]F = 41 (F with normal skin biopsy FMN = 18, F with proximal denervation FMP = 22, F with proximal and distal denervation FMD = 7)
C = 15
Short-termHeat (CO2 laser)Thigh and dorsum of the foot3030 ms10 sEEG: N2 and P2 amplitudeChange over time between third and first blockThigh N2: F = C
Thigh P2: F < C (all groups, FMN > FMP, FMD)
Foot P2: F = C
Patients with reduced intra-epidermal nerve fiber density showed less habituation of the P2 component
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

van der Miesen, M.M.; Vossen, C.J.; Joosten, E.A. Habituation to Pain in Patients with Chronic Pain: Clinical Implications and Future Directions. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 4305. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12134305

AMA Style

van der Miesen MM, Vossen CJ, Joosten EA. Habituation to Pain in Patients with Chronic Pain: Clinical Implications and Future Directions. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2023; 12(13):4305. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12134305

Chicago/Turabian Style

van der Miesen, Maite M., Catherine J. Vossen, and Elbert A. Joosten. 2023. "Habituation to Pain in Patients with Chronic Pain: Clinical Implications and Future Directions" Journal of Clinical Medicine 12, no. 13: 4305. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12134305

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop