Next Article in Journal
Correction: Iannuzzi et al. Might Fibroblasts from Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease Reflect the Brain Pathology? A Focus on the Increased Phosphorylation of Amyloid Precursor Protein Tyr682 Residue. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 103
Previous Article in Journal
Movement Sonification Techniques to Improve Balance in Parkinson’s Disease: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial
Previous Article in Special Issue
Language Nativeness Modulates Physiological Responses to Moral vs. Immoral Concepts in Chinese–English Bilinguals: Evidence from Event-Related Potential and Psychophysiological Measures
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Multilingual Language Diversity Protects Native Language Production under Different Control Demands

1
Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
2
Department of Psychology, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
3
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA 16801, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(11), 1587; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13111587
Submission received: 5 October 2023 / Revised: 6 November 2023 / Accepted: 10 November 2023 / Published: 13 November 2023

Abstract

:
The use of multiple languages has been found to influence individuals’ cognitive abilities. Although some studies have also investigated the effect of multilingualism on non-native language proficiency, fewer studies have focused on how multilingual experience affects native language production. This study investigated the effect of multilingualism on native language production, specifically examining control demands through a semantic Go/No-Go picture naming task. The multilingual experience was quantified using language entropy, which measures the uncertainty and diversity of language use. Control demands were achieved by manipulating the proportion of Go (i.e., naming) trials in different conditions. Results showed that as control demands increased, multilingual individuals exhibited poorer behavioral performance and greater brain activation throughout the brain. Moreover, more diverse language use was associated with higher accuracy in naming and more interconnected brain networks with greater involvement of domain-general neural resources and less domain-specific neural resources. Notably, the varied and balanced use of multiple languages enabled multilingual individuals to respond more efficiently to increased task demands during native language production.

1. Introduction

The ability to speak multiple languages is a common occurrence in today’s world, as globalization and multiculturalism continue to shape our societies. Many individuals are exposed to and use multiple languages in their daily lives, whether through immigration, education, or cultural immersion. Multilingualism has been extensively studied in psycholinguistic research as researchers seek to understand the cognitive processes underlying second language acquisition, bilingual language control, and the cognitive consequences of speaking multiple languages [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Yet, few studies have explored how the experience of using multiple languages, particularly the diversity (i.e., number of languages used) and uncertainty (i.e., probability of using a particular language in a specific language context), affects individuals’ ability to produce their native language. Moreover, although multilingualism has been shown to influence cognitive control [7,8], less is known about how the use of multiple languages interacts with these cognitive consequences during language production. The current study, therefore, investigates how multilingual experience modulates native language production in the context of manipulating control demands.
Language production often engages a left-lateralized frontal-temporal-parietal network [9,10]. Fluent production often involves cognitive control, as demonstrated in both behavioral and neural studies [11,12,13,14,15]. In contrast to monolinguals, multilinguals are challenged with processing information across all the languages they are proficient in and switching between languages to maintain effective communication in various contexts. Several studies have suggested that bilinguals’ language production places greater demands on competition and cognitive control than monolingual individuals [16,17,18]. The constant need to control two languages has led to research focusing on the cognitive consequences of lifelong bilingualism. Many studies have reported that bilinguals have cognitive and neural benefits compared with monolinguals [7,8], especially under higher task demands [19,20,21]. A common distributed network including the bilateral inferior frontal and temporal cortices and the anterior cingulate gyrus was found to be more efficiently activated in bilinguals across different cognitive control tasks [22,23,24]. Moreover, how multiple languages are used in daily communicative contexts is expected to modulate the effect of multilingualism on cognitive and brain functions [25,26].
While there is growing evidence of non-language cognitive benefits associated with using multiple languages [8,27,28], fewer studies have asked how this experience affects language processing. In addition to non-language cognitive benefits, some studies have also reported that multilingual experience has certain benefits for general language ability (e.g., metalinguistic awareness, effective selection of the language to be spoken, and linguistic reasoning) [29,30,31]. Yet, other studies have disclosed certain disadvantages in language processing for multilinguals compared with monolinguals. For instance, the Frequency Lag Hypothesis suggests that bilingual performance in both languages is worse than that of monolingual speakers due to their less frequent exposure to each language [32]. In addition, the Dynamic Model of Multilingualism [33] pointed out that individuals who need to manage two or more language systems may experience a heavier language load, potentially leading to disadvantages in language ability. These hypotheses have been supported by various findings showing that, compared with their monolingual peers, bilinguals have smaller vocabularies [34], lower scores on verbal fluency [35], and more tip of the tongue experiences [36].
One of the starkest consequences reflecting the potential downsides of multilinguals’ language ability is known as first language (L1) attrition, which refers to the gradual loss of proficiency or fluency in an individual’s first language resulting from exposure to other languages [37,38]. L1 attrition has often been found in the immigration population because of the change in the language environment. The loss of L1 is also related to the age of onset of bilingualism, the influence of L2 exposure and proficiency, as well as insufficient activation of L1 [39,40,41,42,43]. All these factors highlight the necessity of considering language usage and exposure comprehensively [44]. Yet, it is uncertain if multilingual individuals who still regularly use their native languages while also speaking other languages are susceptible to the same language attrition as those who undergo a change in the language environment. Alternatively, could their balanced and diverse language experiences potentially enhance their native language abilities? Some previous studies have found that multilinguals who live in their L1 environments still encounter difficulties in sentence parsing, processing of morphosyntactic variation, and comprehending or expressing complex ideas in their first language [45,46,47]. Regarding L1 production, Parker Jones and Green [48] found that bilinguals need to recruit more brain resources to achieve the same level of performance as monolinguals. On the other hand, Rossi and Newman [49] found that, with greater brain activation, bilinguals had higher accuracy in picture naming than monolinguals. Furthermore, while studies have revealed that highly proficient bilingual individuals tend to engage language-related and domain-general cognitive areas more extensively than monolinguals during language production [48,49,50,51], the functional neuroanatomic underpinnings of native language processing in multilingual individuals with more diverse language experiences remain unidentified.
As touched upon briefly, research on the effects of multilingualism should emphasize a comprehensive understanding of the multilingual experience, regardless of whether the effect is on cognitive control or language processing. In recent years, research on multilingualism has moved away from treating bilingualism or multilingualism as a categorical variable. Instead, researchers have begun to recognize the heterogeneity of language use by taking a more context-specific approach [25,52,53,54,55,56,57]. This more nuanced approach acknowledges the way in which languages are used, the frequency of use, and the level of proficiency can vary widely among multilingual individuals, and each dimension can significantly influence cognitive and language abilities [58,59,60,61].
To estimate individual- and contextual-level differences in language engagement, a measurement called language entropy has been developed [53]. Specifically, language entropy measures the degree of uncertainty and diversity when using multiple languages in different contexts, with high entropy indicating equal engagement and less predictable use of different languages and low entropy indicating a predictable use of one dominant language. This measurement can facilitate our understanding of the extent of multilingual usage and its impact on cognitive control and language processing. Using language entropy, some studies have reported that higher entropy values correlate with enhanced subjective and objective assessments of second-language proficiency [52], along with increased proactive control engagement [62,63]. From a functional connectivity standpoint, individuals with higher language entropy were found to exhibit increased brain network specialization and segregation, primarily in the default mode and executive control networks, as well as stronger connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and bilateral putamen [62,64]. Yet, some studies have reported that higher entropy is associated with smaller vocabulary in the second language [65]. Despite these findings, few studies have asked how language entropy would modulate native language ability.
In summary, despite numerous studies investigating the relationship between multilingualism and cognitive control, few have examined the impact of multilingual experience on native language production. Therefore, in the current study, we investigated how the experience of using multiple languages affects both behavioral and neural mechanisms of native language production, focusing on not only functional activation but also task-based functional connectivity. Recently developed measures, such as language entropy, have enabled researchers to capture the diversity in multiple language use more accurately. If higher language entropy is associated with worse production performance and less efficient neural processing, it would provide evidence suggesting that the diverse multilingual experience impairs L1 performance. Conversely, if higher language entropy is associated with enhanced L1 production and more efficient neural processing, it would suggest that the extensive use of multiple languages benefits L1 processing. Additionally, previous studies have suggested that control demands affect language production performance and result in greater activation in frontal-parietal-temporal circuits [66,67,68]. Furthermore, the effect of multilingualism on language and control processes might be modulated by task difficulty during production [19,20,21]. Therefore, we manipulated the control demands (i.e., task difficulty) involved in language production using a Go/No-Go picture naming paradigm, which has been validated in previous studies [69,70]. If the mixed and frequent use of multiple languages is beneficial, particularly in dealing with more challenging tasks, we would expect to observe a larger difference between high- and low-entropy individuals when the control demands are higher. Last but not the least, the participants included in the study were all multilinguals, speaking at least three languages, and had extensive experience using multiple languages from childhood. Therefore, the current study highlights a uniquely rich multilingual context where many different languages are being used.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Forty healthy participants were originally recruited, and four were excluded from the analysis: one for having excessive head movement (greater than 1/2 voxel), one for misunderstanding the instructions, and two others whose accuracy for the fMRI task was lower than 65%. The remaining 36 participants (27 females; aged 18–26 years, mean age = 21.1 years, SD = 1.8 years) were all native Cantonese multilinguals who were also highly proficient in Mandarin Chinese and had experience of learning and speaking another language, such as English, Portuguese, or Japanese. To understand language proficiency and overall multilingual language diversity, all participants were asked to complete a Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3) [71]. The overall proficiency of each language, ranging from 0 to 1, was computed based on the average score of the participant’s self-rated proficiency on different components (i.e., speaking, listening, reading, and writing) of each of their languages. Paired-sample t-tests showed that all participants were significantly more proficient in Cantonese compared with Mandarin Chinese (Cantonese: mean = 0.85, SD = 0.13; Mandarin Chinese: mean = 0.79, SD = 0.15; t = 2.77, p < 0.01), and both Cantonese and Mandarin Chinese proficiency were significantly higher than other languages (other languages: mean = 0.58, SD = 0.10; Cantonese vs. other languages: t = 10.28, p < 0.001; Mandarin Chinese vs. other languages: t = 7.27, p < 0.001).
All participants were right-handed, reported no history of psychological or neurological disorders, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Before the MRI session, all participants completed a behavioral neuropsychological testing session to gauge their language and cognitive abilities. The neuropsychological tests utilized in this study and the assessment scores are reported in Table 1. Participants all signed consent prior to the beginning of the study. The study protocols, procedures, and consent forms were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Macau.

2.2. Stimuli and Procedure

A semantic Go/No-Go picture naming task was performed by all participants in the MRI scanner using their native language, Cantonese. Photographs were displayed to participants, who were instructed to promptly and accurately name the photograph. Control demand during production was manipulated by varying the proportion of Go trials (i.e., naming trials) and No-Go trials (i.e., trials that inhibited responses were needed) across three conditions: All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias (see Figure 1 for the task illustration). Specifically, in the All Go condition, participants were required to name all photographs (100% Go trials), and this was always administered first to minimize the possible influence of subsequent Go/No-Go task demands. In the Go Bias and No-Go Bias conditions, participants were instructed to name the photograph only if the photograph indicates a nonliving object (e.g., basketball) but inhibit their response if the photograph indicates a living object (e.g., fox). In the Go Bias condition, 74% of trials were Go trials, while 26% were No-Go trials. In the No-Go Bias condition, 26% of trials were Go trials, while 74% were No-Go trials. Control demands involved in production increased as the proportion of Go trials decreased from All Go to Go Bias and No-Go Bias. Furthermore, since the Go/No-Go decision was unnecessary in the All Go condition, the specific task demand escalated from the All Go condition to the Bias condition. In the No-Go Bias condition that contains a majority of No-Go trials (i.e., 74%), a prepotent inhibition tendency needs to be overcome when responding to Go trials. Therefore, though the task demands within two Bias conditions are consistent, the naming demand increased from the Go Bias condition to the No-Go Bias condition as the number of Go trials decreased. Prior to the formal scanning, a practice run similar to the real task but with different photographs was conducted in a simulator to ensure participants’ adaptation to the environment and the experimental task. In the scanner, participants always performed the All Go runs first before being informed regarding the Go/No-Go conditions.
Photographs were selected from two picture databases [84,85] and open-access online resources, including a broad range of common objects (e.g., people, animals, food, tools, nature, music instruments, clothes, transportations, and household items). We recruited 17 healthy, native Cantonese speakers who were not involved in the MRI experiment to provide Cantonese names for the selected photos. Only items with naming consistency higher than 65% were included in the final stimulus set with 318 colored photographs in total. All the selected stimuli were assigned to three conditions, with 106 unique items per condition (All Go, 78 nonliving and 28 living; Go Bias, 78 nonliving and 28 living; and No-Go Bias, 28 nonliving and 78 living). Target word frequency was obtained from the Cantonese-based Sketch Engine database [86]. Living and nonliving stimuli across three conditions were comparable in word length, word frequency, and name agreement (ps > 0.2).
Colored stimuli photographs with white backgrounds, lasting 1.5 s each, were displayed on a monitor with a variable inter-stimulus interval (ISI) ranging from 1 to 11 s (mean ISI = 4.08 s) to optimize the hemodynamic response using the Optseq2 method [87]. For each photograph, participants were instructed to speak out its name or withhold their response based on the requirements for each condition. The responses were encouraged to be a single word without any additional comments. In the MRI scanner, participants completed a total of 6 runs, with 2 runs in each condition. During the scan, overt verbal responses were recorded and filtered using a dual-channel, MR-compatible, fiber optic microphone system provided by Optoacoustics Ltd. in Or-Yehuda, Israel. After the scan, participants were asked to rename all the photographs displayed earlier in a silent room to confirm their knowledge of the object names in situations where they refrained from responding (i.e., during No-Go trials).

2.3. Acquisition of MRI Data

We used a 3T Siemens Prisma MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil to collect MRI data. T1-weighted images with anterior and posterior commissures identified were collected using a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence as anatomical references (Repetition Time [TR] = 2300 ms; Echo Time [TE] = 2.28 ms; Inversion Time [TI] = 900 ms; flip angle = 8°; echo spacing = 7 ms; acceleration factor = 2; field of view [FOV] = 256 mm2; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm; 160 contiguous slices).
Functional images relying on the BOLD response were collected using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 2500 ms; TE = 25 ms; flip angle = 90°; echo spacing = 0.49 ms; FOV = 240 mm2; voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm; 41 contiguous axial slices, parallel to the AC–PC line, interleaved acquisition). Two additional volumes obtained as the first two volumes of each run were deleted for steady-state equilibrium, resulting in 128 volumes (320 s) in total for each of the six function runs.

2.4. Behavioral Data Analyses

Language entropy is a newly developed method for assessing the diversity of language usage in multilinguals [52,53,62,63]. The calculation of language entropy was based on Shannon entropy, which is a classical measure of information diversity and uncertainty [88]. In the current study, language entropy was calculated based on three questions from the LHQ. These questions assessed language use experience in 17 different communicative contexts via the time spent in each language (including social media, reading, writing, and speaking with different people) and the frequency of language activities on a 7-point scale (including different types of self-engaged activities). Language entropy in each context was first calculated with the following equation: H = i = 1 n P i l o g 2 ( P i ) using the LanguageEntropy package [89] in the R environment [90]. In this formula, n represents the total possible languages within a context, and Pi is the proportion that languagei is used within the context. The mean entropy was then calculated across all contexts for each participant. Lower language entropy values indicate an unbalanced and less diverse use of different languages, whereas higher entropy values indicate a more balanced and mixed use of each language, leading to higher uncertainty for which language will be used.
For the in-scanner picture naming task, responses were coded based on the recordings from the scan. Since we were mainly interested in language production, behavioral and fMRI analyses were only conducted on the Go trials, which represent word retrieval and production processes. To give readers some information regarding the No-Go trials, we added Supplementary Figure S1 (on the osf site) to demonstrate the commission error rate (failure to inhibit responses) difference between the two Bias conditions. In all conditions, responses were coded as correct if they were consistent with the target names (e.g., lemon for lemon) or acceptable alternatives that matched the photographs and were from the same category as the target word (e.g., lime for lemon). Responses that did not match the photographs (e.g., ice for tea) or trials without response were marked as incorrect.
It is essential to note that previous studies have demonstrated significant differences in behavioral performance and neural responses for naming living and nonliving things [91,92,93,94]. Therefore, to avoid any potential confounding effects of different categories (i.e., living vs. nonliving) and to minimize the discrepancy in numbers of Go trials, between the All Go and Bias conditions, we focused exclusively on the Go trials of nonliving things in our study (i.e., a subset of Go trials in the All Go condition, and all Go trials in the Bias conditions). This action only affected the number of trials in the All Go condition included in the analysis. Yet, the analysis and results, including all trials in the All Go condition, are comparable with the subset analysis (see Supplementary Figure S2 on the osf site).
The incorrect response rate of the Go trials was calculated for each condition by dividing the number of incorrect responses by the total number of nonliving Go trials in the corresponding condition. To effectively demonstrate how production performance changed as control demand increased, the categorical conditions for control demand were systematically coded on a numeric scale (i.e., All Go is 1, Go Bias is 2, and No-Go Bias is 3). Since the incorrect response rates were calculated based on each task condition, a generalized linear model was conducted on the incorrect response rate to analyze the effects of control demand and language diversity and their interaction, employing the glm function with a Gaussian family from the stats package in the R environment [95].
To calculate the reaction times (RTs) for the Go trials, the response onsets of the recordings provided in the scanner were extracted using customized Praat scripts [96], which could detect response onsets based on the pitch deviations of the filtered auditory signal. All these onsets were double-checked based on both the audio and visual speech streams. The RTs of Go trials were calculated by subtracting the photograph onsets (from E-Prime output) from the response onsets. Incorrect trials and those whose reaction times exceeded 2.5 standard deviations were excluded. Categorical task conditions were also coded on a numeric scale. To investigate the main effects of task conditions and language entropy and their interaction, a mixed-effects regression was conducted on trial-level RTs employing the lmer function in the lme4 package [97]. We also included random intercepts and slopes attributable to different participants, as well as random intercepts related to the target words in the model, as suggested by Barr and Levy [98]. The p values for regression coefficients were obtained using the lmerTest package [99].

2.5. fMRI Data Analyses

Prior to data processing, the data quality was assessed using the fBIRN QA tool [100], measuring the mean signal fluctuation to noise ratio (SFNR), the number of potentially clipped voxels, and per-slice variation. Additionally, the functional and anatomical images were visually inspected for artifacts and signal drop-out. Brain structure was extracted using the Optimized Brain Extraction for Pathological Brains script (optiBET) [101], with the skull and other non-brain tissues separated and stripped out. We used FSL (Version 6.0.5) with FEAT (fMRI expert analysis tool) Version 6.0 [102,103] for further processing and analyses. Procedures, including motion correction (FSL MCFLIRT), B0 unwarping with field mapping, slice timing correction, spatial smoothing (with FWHM equal to 5 mm) high-pass filtering, coregistration, and normalization, were also conducted. When modeling the BOLD signal for each event, we employed a double-gamma hemodynamic response function, and only correct trials were included in the analyses. The first-level analyses were conducted on each participant’s individual runs, in which standard motion parameters were included as confounding EVs (Explanatory Variables) to eliminate any motion effects that may persist after motion correction. Then group-level analyses using FMRIB’s local analysis of mixed effects were conducted across runs and participants (FLAME 1+2) [103,104]. Specifically, we identified regions responsive to nonliving Go trials across different conditions, respectively (i.e., Go trials in All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias conditions), compared with the implicit baseline of each participant. To investigate the effect of control demands, we then compared parametric functional activation across task conditions for Go trials (All Go < Go Bias, All Go < No-Go Bias, Go Bias < No-Go Bias). Conditional differences were all masked by the condition with higher control demands. Furthermore, to assess the main effect of language entropy, we collapsed nonliving Go trials from all conditions for each participant and correlated their activation with the corresponding language entropy deviation of each participant. The activation maps for the language entropy correlation were masked by the contrast reflecting overall Go trial activation. Significant activations were identified through a two-step process. Initially, Z-statistical images (Gaussianized T/F) were voxel-level thresholded at p < 0.01. To account for multiple comparisons, clusters of voxels were corrected following Gaussian random field theory [105] at a corrected threshold of p = 0.05. The estimated significance level of each cluster was compared with the cluster probability threshold, and only clusters surpassing this threshold were included in the results [106].
Additional analyses were conducted to estimate how language entropy modulated the effect of control demand (i.e., the interaction between language entropy and control demand). Specific to Go trials, we correlated participants’ language entropy with changes in brain activation from the All Go condition to the Go Bias condition (Go Bias > All Go), the All Go condition to the No-Go Bias condition (No-Go Bias > All Go), and the Go Bias to the No-Go Bias condition (No-Go Bias > Go Bias). Positive correlations (i.e., higher language entropy associated with larger increases in activation) would indicate that participants with greater language diversity tend to be more affected by the increased control demand. On the other hand, negative correlations (i.e., higher language entropy associated with smaller increases in activation) would suggest that participants with greater language diversity are less affected by the increased control demand. These negative correlations would then reflect that multilingual experience can help multilinguals better cope with increased control demands. To ensure that only meaningful differences were included in the analyses, the interaction between language entropy and control demand was masked with activation triggered by changes in control demand (e.g., the effect of language entropy on changes in brain activation from the All Go to the Go Bias condition was masked with the activation map comparing the All Go with the Go Bias condition). All results were overlayed on a representative brain in MNI space and reported using MNI coordinates. The Harvard-Oxford Structural Atlas [107] was used to identify the regions of the whole brain.

2.6. Functional Connectivity Analyses

To investigate the network connectivity status modulated by language entropy at different task demands, we analyzed task-based language network segregation. We started by creating 264 sphere nodes (5 mm radius) across the whole brain based on the coordinates identified by Power and Cohen [108]. Then we divided these ROIs into 14 networks, including language-specific networks (Left language network and Right language homologous network) and networks related to other general functions (Cingulo-opercular/Fronto-parietal control, Ventral/Dorsal attention, Hand/Mouth somatomotor, Visual, Salience, Auditory, Subcortical, Cerebellar, and Default), using similar methods as in the previous study [109]. The language networks selected were based on Fedorenko and Hsieh [110], and other networks were from Power and Cohen [108]. Thirty-three nodes that cannot fit with any network were excluded from the analysis. The final set was compromised with 231 nodes across 14 networks (see Supplementary Table S1 on the osf site).
Functional connectivity data were analyzed using the CONN functional connectivity toolbox (Version 18.a) under MATLAB environment [111]. For each condition of each participant, the fully processed time-series data of each node was extracted and correlated with the time-series of every other node. A Fisher-Z transformation was conducted on all correlations. Negative correlations were excluded from further analysis due to the uncertainty of their meaning. Within-network connectivity was obtained by averaging the node-to-node correlation of each node pair in the target network (i.e., diagonal blocks). Between-network connectivity was the average correlation between each node in a network and all other nodes outside of that network (i.e., off-diagonal blocks). Finally, left-language network segregation was calculated as the difference between the within-network connectivity and between-network connectivity of the language network divided by the within-network connectivity [112,113].
Consistent with the analysis for fMRI activation, we also investigated the effects of task demand and language entropy, as well as their interaction, on language network segregation separately. It should be noted that since network connectivity reflects the brain states throughout the whole run and the Go Bias and No-Go Bias conditions reflect similar task demands, we combined the two Bias conditions in the analyses. Mixed-effect regressions were conducted using the lmer function in the lme4 package [97] in the R environment [95].

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Results

3.1.1. Error Rates

To investigate the effect of control demand and language entropy on language production, a generalized linear regression was conducted on the incorrect response rates of nonliving Go trials for each participant across the three conditions (All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias; Table 2 and Figure 2a). Results showed a main effect of control demand (β = 5.99, SE = 0.12, p < 0.001), manifesting significantly higher incorrect response rates as task difficulty increased (i.e., All Go < Go Bias < No Go Bias; All Go vs. Go Bias: β = −1.25, SE = 0.17, p < 0.001; All Go vs. No-Go Bias: β = −14.81, SE = 0.24, p < 0.001; Go Bias vs. No-Go Bias: β = −13.57, SE = 0.24, p < 0.001). There was also a significant main effect of language entropy, such that the incorrect response rates for participants with lower language entropy were significantly higher than those with higher language entropy (β = −0.96, SE = 0.23, p < 0.001). Yet, there was no significant interaction between language entropy and control demand (β = 0.07, SE = 0.12, p = 0.53). Therefore, results from incorrect response rates suggested that higher language entropy was beneficial for native language production across different control demands.

3.1.2. Reaction Times (RTs)

A linear mixed-effect model was conducted on RTs of nonliving Go trials to explore the effect of control demand and language entropy on the speed of language production (see Table 2 and Figure 2b). Results revealed a significant main effect of control demand (β = 56.77, SE = 17.57, p < 0.01), such that RTs were different among the three task conditions (i.e., All Go < Go Bias < No-Go Bias; All Go vs. Go Bias: β = −50.58, SE = 26.17, p = 0.05; All Go vs. No-Go Bias: β = −116.28, SE = 36.77, p < 0.01; Go Bias vs. No-Go Bias: β = −65.70, SE = 35.04, p = 0.06). There was no significant main effect of language entropy on RTs (β = −9.14, SE = 24.84, p = 0.72). Also, the interaction between language entropy and condition was not significant (β = 11.05, SE = 9.38, p = 0.25). These results suggested that language production performance declined as the task became more challenging, although language entropy did not have a significant effect on reaction times.

3.2. fMRI Activation Results

3.2.1. Basic Pattern of Activation

The Go trials in all conditions (i.e., All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias) elicited similar activation patterns in regions that were known to be involved in language and cognitive processing, including bilateral frontal pole, superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri, frontal orbital cortex, pre- and post-central gyri, superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri, angular gyri, precuneus cortex, supramarginal gyri, lateral occipital cortex, occipital pole, as well as bilateral cingulate cortex and insular gyri (see Supplementary Figure S3 and Supplementary Table S2 on the osf site).

3.2.2. Main Effects of Control Demand

To reveal the neural response sensitive to increased control demand, we compared the functional activation of nonliving Go trials across three conditions as a function of increased demands (i.e., All Go < Go Bias, Go Bias < No-Go Bias, All Go < No-Go Bias). Participants showed greater activation during the Go Bias condition compared with the All Go condition in the right frontal pole, extending to the bilateral middle and inferior frontal gyri and the left precentral gyri (Table 3 and Figure 3a). The No-Go Bias condition elicited greater activation compared with the Go Bias in bilateral orbitofrontal cortex, bilateral operculum cortex, bilateral inferior frontal gyri, right hemisphere regions including superior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, superior and middle temporal gyri, supramarginal gyrus, and angular gyrus, as well as bilateral cingulate cortex and insular cortex (Table 3 and Figure 3b). Similar activation patterns were also found when comparing the Go trials in the No-Go Bias condition and the All Go condition. Compared with the All Go condition, the No-Go Bias condition showed increased activation in bilateral orbitofrontal and operculum cortex, bilateral inferior frontal gyri extending to the right superior and middle frontal gyrus, right superior and middle temporal gyri, right precuneus cortex, right lingual gyrus, as well as bilateral cingulate cortex and insular cortex (Table 3 and Figure 3c). In general, as control demand increased, participants elicited greater activation in regions related to language processing as well as domain-general control.

3.2.3. Main Effect of Language Entropy

To investigate the effect of language entropy, we correlated the functional activation across all nonliving Go trials for each participant with their demeaned language entropy score. Positive correlations were found in the right hemisphere, including the inferior frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal cortex, lingual gyrus, and occipital cortex (Table 4 and Figure 4a). Negative correlations were found between language entropy and brain activation in the left superior and middle frontal gyri, bilateral anterior cingulate and paracingulate gyri, left precentral gyri and right postcentral gyri, left superior parietal lobule, left superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri, left supramarginal gyrus, bilateral angular gyri, and bilateral occipital cortex (Table 4 and Figure 4b).

3.2.4. Interaction between Language Entropy and Control Demand

To investigate how language diversity modulated the functional response to increased control demand (i.e., language entropy × control demand interaction), we correlated participants’ language entropy with changes in brain activation as a function of control demand (i.e., All Go < Go Bias < No-Go Bias). Higher language entropy was found to be associated with smaller increases (i.e., negative correlation) in activation from the All Go to the Bias conditions in the left precuneus cortex and left pre- and postcentral gyri (see Table 5 and Figure 5). No significant relationship with language entropy was found when comparing the Go Bias and No-Go Bias conditions.

3.3. Functional Connectivity Results

3.3.1. Main Effect of Task Demand

Mixed-effect regression with random intercepts and slopes for different participants showed that there was no significant main effect of task demand on language network segregation (Figure 6, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, p = 0.14). This result indicated that language network segregation during language production was relatively consistent despite the differences in task demand.

3.3.2. Main Effect of Language Entropy

Regression analyses on language network segregation showed a significant main effect of language entropy (Figure 6, β = −0.04, SE = 0.013, p < 0.01), such that higher language entropy was associated with lower language network segregation. This relationship suggested that individuals with more diverse language use experience showed a broader involvement of functional networks during native language production.

3.3.3. Interaction between Language Entropy and Task Demand

Regression results showed that there was no significant interaction between language entropy and task demand on language network segregation (Figure 6, β = −0.02, SE = 0.01, p = 0.09). This, combined with the main effect of entropy, suggested that individuals’ multilingual experience independently influenced functional connectivity patterns during tasks which were not necessarily modulated by task demands.

4. Discussion

In the present study, we investigated how the experience of using multiple languages affected native language production in the context of manipulating control demands, proving evidence from behavioral aspects as well as fMRI activation and functional connectivity. There are a few highlights in the current study. First, we treated multilingual language experience as a continuous variable in a dynamic and integrated way rather than using the conventional categorical classification. Second, while the majority of previous studies focused on bilinguals, we tested multilinguals who spoke at least three languages, further supplementing and enhancing the previous literature. We found that a diverse and balanced multilingual experience enhanced multilinguals’ native language production and was associated with greater involvement of other networks. Additionally, multilinguals with higher language entropy were better able to cope with increased task demands in language production. That is, our results suggest that the experience of using multiple languages extensively is beneficial for maintaining the native language. Below, we discuss these results in detail.
First, increased control demand significantly affected native language production both behaviorally and neurally. Specifically, as control demand increased (All Go < Go Bias < No-Go Bias), participants showed increased incorrect response rates, longer reaction times, and increased brain activation in bilateral cognitive control and language-related regions (see Table 3 and Figure 3 for more details). These results are consistent with previous studies that manipulated control demands in language production [13,66,68,69,114,115]. In response to increased control demands, multilinguals recruited more neural resources from the bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyri, the left superior and middle temporal gyri, and the bilateral insular cortex. These regions are part of the well-established left frontal-parietal-temporal language-related circuits and homologous regions in the right hemisphere [116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123]. Additionally, regions related to domain-general cognition, such as the bilateral cingulate and prefrontal cortex, were more activated in response to the increased control demand in the current study [124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131]. All these results suggest that multilinguals’ native language production under higher control demands involves additional domain-specific and domain-general neural resources.
Furthermore, the main focus of the current paper is to investigate the effect of multiple language use experience measured by language entropy. Behaviorally, higher language entropy was associated with better L1 production, reflected by lower incorrect response rates. Yet, there was no significant effect of language entropy on reaction times, which is likely due to a speed-accuracy trade-off where multilinguals with higher language entropy tended to sacrifice naming speed for response accuracy. Neurally, individuals with higher language entropy showed less activation across many regions that are typically involved in language processing (e.g., left temporal cortex, left precentral and right postcentral gyri, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, bilateral angular gyri). Along with the enhanced behavioral performance in multilinguals with higher language entropy, these results suggest that the diverse and balanced use of multiple languages benefits multilinguals’ native language production and its neural efficiency (i.e., using fewer neural resources to achieve enhanced behavioral performance). The current findings offer a new perspective on the traditional view regarding L1 attrition and suggest that the use of multiple languages could be beneficial for the maintenance of one’s native language. Yet, unlike bilinguals who lost contact with their native language, multilinguals in the present study still maintained continuous exposure to their native language. This raises the possibility that multilingual experience may have different implications for native language production across different multilingual groups. Moreover, the differences between the observed facilitation and L1 attrition further demonstrate that exposure to both the native language and other languages is important for the maintenance of native language ability [42,43,132].
In addition to the negative correlations between language entropy and brain activation, higher language entropy was also associated with greater activation in regions related to cognitive control (e.g., the right inferior frontal gyrus). Consistently, functional connectivity results showed that individuals with higher language entropy had lower language network segregation, indicating greater involvement of other brain networks during language production. These entropy-brain relationships might reflect a strategy shift such that multilinguals with more extensive use of multiple languages employ more domain-general (i.e., more activation in frontal regions) and less domain-specific (i.e., less activation in language regions) neural resources during native language production.
Furthermore, individuals with varying degrees of multilingualism exhibited differences in their ability to regulate task demands during their native language production. While there was no statistically significant interaction observed on behavioral performance, multilinguals with higher language entropy were less affected by increased control demand during production, as evidenced by their neural efforts. Specifically, when comparing Bias conditions with the All Go condition, higher language entropy was steadily associated with smaller increases in activation in the left postcentral gyrus. As the site where the primary sensory cortex is located, the left postcentral gyri has been found to be related to successful overt naming [69,70]. Moreover, it has also been shown to be critical in modulating attentional processing demands [133,134]. It is worthwhile to note that the modulation effect of language entropy on the effect of control demand during production was only present when comparing the Bias conditions with the All Go condition. As suggested in previous studies, the switch from the All Go to the Bias conditions may reflect increased task demands (i.e., additional Go/No-Go tasks in the Bias conditions), whereas the difference between the Go and No-Go Bias conditions might reflect a change in the domain of naming demands (i.e., more cognitive resources were taken to overcome the prepotent inhibition tendency and name the target photos in the No-Go Bias condition) [69]. Therefore, the results suggest that the modulation effect of multiple language use experiences might be strongest when there is an increase in general task demands but not when there is an increase in naming demands. Recall that, in general, higher language entropy was associated with less activation in language-related brain regions. Together with the interaction effects, all these results suggest that the multiple language experience is beneficial for native language production in general, especially in the context of higher task demands.
Although our results shed light on the influence of control demands, multilingual experience, and their interaction on native language production, there are a few limitations. First, though we used language entropy to quantify the usage of multiple languages in a more comprehensive and context-specific way, the calculation of language entropy currently relies on subjective reports. Future research could seek more objective measures to reflect multilinguals’ language experience. Second, the gender distribution of participants was unbalanced. Though previous studies have suggested that gender plays a trivial role in brain function, cognition, and behavior (d = 0.20 or d ≤ 0.10) [135,136], future studies should target a more balanced sample of different genders when possible.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our findings demonstrate that multilinguals are sensitive to the increased control demands during native language production, showing poorer behavioral performance and more extensive brain activation. Importantly, a more balanced and diverse language use experience facilitated native language production both behaviorally and neurally. With more extensive use of multiple languages, multilinguals might undergo a strategy shift such that they tend to employ more domain-general and less domain-specific neural resources during native language production. Additionally, the diverse and balanced use of multiple languages was beneficial for multilinguals in dealing with increased task demands in native language production.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci13111587/s1, Figure S1: Behavioral results for commission errors of No-Go trials; Figure S2: Behavioral results for all of the Go trials; Figure S3: Basic activation patterns across three conditions; Table S1: Network information for functional connectivity analyses; Table S2: Basic activation patterns across three conditions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.K. and H.Z.; Formal analysis, K.K., Y.X. and H.Y.; Funding acquisition, H.Z.; Investigation, K.K., Y.X., H.Y. and H.Z.; Methodology, K.K., M.T.D. and H.Z.; Project administration, H.Z.; Supervision, H.Z.; Visualization, K.K.; Writing—original draft, K.K.; Writing—review and editing, M.T.D. and H.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [32200845], the Science and Technology Development Fund, Macao S.A.R [FDCT, 0153/2022/A], and the Start-up Research Grant [SRG2022-00003-ICI] and Multi-Year Research Grant [MYRG2022-00148-ICI] from the University of Macau to Haoyun Zhang.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Macau (protocol code BSERE22-APP001-ICI, approved on 17 March 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to the beginning of the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article and Supplementary Materials. The data presented in this study are openly available on OSF Platform at https://osf.io/wavnz/?view_only=e609e584222540b581b09b0316ade6f5 (accessed on 9 November 2023).

Acknowledgments

We thank Stella Lam Zi Kwan, Jing Zhong, and other members from the Language Aging and Bilingualism Lab for their help in data collection. We thank the staff at the Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences (CCBS) at the University of Macau, where the study was conducted, for their support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Abutalebi, J.; Green, D.W. Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2008, 23, 557–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I. Cognitive and Linguistic Processing in the Bilingual Mind. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 19, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kroll, J.F.; Dussias, P.E.; Bogulski, C.A.; Kroff, J.R.V. Chapter 7 Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2012; Volume 56, pp. 229–262. [Google Scholar]
  4. Morales, J.; Yudes, C.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia 2015, 66, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Quinteros Baumgart, C.; Billick, S.B. Positive Cognitive Effects of Bilingualism and Multilingualism on Cerebral Function: A Review. Psychiatr. Q. 2018, 89, 273–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Morgan-Short, K. Insights into the neural mechanisms of becoming bilingual: A brief synthesis of second language research with artificial linguistic systems. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 23, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bialystok, E. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 233–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I. How does bilingualism modify cognitive function? Attention to the mechanism. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2022, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Price, C.J. The anatomy of language: A review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1191, 62–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Geranmayeh, F.; Brownsett, S.L.; Leech, R.; Beckmann, C.F.; Woodhead, Z.; Wise, R.J. The contribution of the inferior parietal cortex to spoken language production. Brain Lang. 2012, 121, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Shao, Z.; Meyer, A.S.; Roelofs, A. Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Mem. Cogn. 2013, 41, 1200–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fedorenko, E.; Thompson-Schill, S.L. Reworking the language network. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Shao, Z.; Roelofs, A.; Acheson, D.J.; Meyer, A.S. Electrophysiological evidence that inhibition supports lexical selection in picture naming. Brain Res. 2014, 1586, 130–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Freund, M.; Gordon, B.; Nozari, N. Conflict-based regulation of control in language production. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 10–13 August 2016. [Google Scholar]
  15. Nozari, N.; Novick, J. Monitoring and control in language production. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 26, 403–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Green, D.W. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1998, 1, 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Calabria, M.; Costa, A.; Green, D.W.; Abutalebi, J. Neural basis of bilingual language control. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1426, 221–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Zirnstein, M.; van Hell, J.G.; Kroll, J.F. Cognitive control ability mediates prediction costs in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognition 2018, 176, 87–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Qu, L.; Low, J.J.W.; Zhang, T.; Li, H.; Zelazo, P.D. Bilingual advantage in executive control when task demands are considered. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 19, 277–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Jiao, L.; Liu, C.; Wang, R.; Chen, B. Working memory demand of a task modulates bilingual advantage in executive functions. Int. J. Biling. 2019, 23, 102–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Massa, E.; Köpke, B.; El Yagoubi, R. Age-related effect on language control and executive control in bilingual and mon-olingual speakers: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia 2020, 138, 107336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Garbin, G.; Sanjuan, A.; Forn, C.; Bustamante, J.; Rodriguez-Pujadas, A.; Belloch, V.; Hernandez, M.; Costa, A.; Ávila, C. Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage 2010, 53, 1272–1278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Luk, G.; Anderson, J.A.; Craik, F.I.; Grady, C.; Bialystok, E. Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain Cogn. 2010, 74, 347–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Mohades, S.G.; Struys, E.; Van Schuerbeek, P.; Baeken, C.; Van De Craen, P.; Luypaert, R. Age of second language acquisition affects nonverbal conflict processing in children: An fMRI study. Brain Behav. 2014, 4, 626–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Green, D.; Abutalebi, J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 515–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Woumans, E.; Ceuleers, E.; Van der Linden, L.; Szmalec, A.; Duyck, W. Verbal and nonverbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2015, 41, 1579–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Antón, E.; Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A. The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0206770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lehtonen, M.; Fyndanis, V.; Jylkkä, J. The relationship between bilingual language use and executive functions. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2023, 2, 360–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kroll, J.F.; Bogulski, C.A.; McClain, R. Psycholinguistic perspectives on second language learning and bilingualism: The course and consequence of cross-language competition. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2012, 2, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Thompson, A.S. The interface of language aptitude and multilingualism: Reconsidering the bilingual/multilingual di-chotomy. Mod. Lang. J. 2013, 97, 685–701. [Google Scholar]
  31. Huang, K.-J. On bilinguals’ development of metalinguistic awareness and its transfer to L3 learning: The role of language characteristics. Int. J. Biling. 2018, 22, 330–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gollan, T.H.; Slattery, T.J.; Goldenberg, D.; Van Assche, E.; Duyck, W.; Rayner, K. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency-lag hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2011, 140, 186–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Herdina, P.; Jessner, U. A Dynamic Model of Multilingualism: Perspectives of Change in Psycholinguistics; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2002; Volume 121. [Google Scholar]
  34. Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.; Luk, G. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 859–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Rosselli, M.; Ardila, A.; Araujo, K.; Weekes, V.A.; Caracciolo, V.; Padilla, M.; Ostrosky-Solí, F. Verbal Fluency and Repetition Skills in Healthy Older Spanish-English Bilinguals. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2000, 7, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Gollan, T.H.; Acenas, L.-A.R. What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and tagalog-English bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2004, 30, 246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Köpke, B.; Keijzer, M. Introduction to psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic approaches to language attrition. In The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gallo, F.; Bermudez-Margaretto, B.; Shtyrov, Y.; Abutalebi, J.; Kreiner, H.; Chitaya, T.; Petrova, A.; Myachykov, A. First Language Attrition: What It Is, What It Isn’t, and What It Can Be. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 686388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Flores, C. The effect of age on language attrition: Evidence from bilingual returnees. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2010, 13, 533–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Flores, C. Differential effects of language attrition in the domains of verb placement and object expression. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 550–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Flores, C. Problematizing the scope of language attrition from the perspective of bilingual returnees. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2017, 7, 691–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kasparian, K.; Steinhauer, K. When the second language takes the lead: Neurocognitive processing changes in the first language of adult attriters. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kasparian, K.; Vespignani, F.; Steinhauer, K. First Language Attrition Induces Changes in Online Morphosyntactic Processing and Re-Analysis: An ERP Study of Number Agreement in Complex Italian Sentences. Cogn. Sci. 2017, 41, 1760–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Herdina, P.; Jessner, U. The implications of language attrition for dynamic systems theory: Next steps and consequences. Int. J. Biling. 2013, 17, 752–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Dussias, P.E.; Sagarra, N. The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish–English bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2007, 10, 101–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Linck, J.A.; Kroll, J.F.; Sunderman, G. Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 1507–1515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Requena, P.E.; Berry, G.M. Cross-linguistic influence in L1 processing of morphosyntactic variation: Evidence from L2 learners. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2021, 42, 153–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Jones, O.P.; Green, D.W.; Grogan, A.; Pliatsikas, C.; Filippopolitis, K.; Ali, N.; Lee, H.L.; Ramsden, S.; Gazarian, K.; Prejawa, S.; et al. Where, When and Why Brain Activation Differs for Bilinguals and Monolinguals during Picture Naming and Reading Aloud. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 22, 892–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Rossi, E.; Newman, S.; Kroll, J.F.; Diaz, M.T. Neural signatures of inhibitory control in bilingual spoken production. Cortex 2018, 108, 50–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Rodriguez-Fornells, A.; Rotte, M.; Heinze, H.-J.; Nösselt, T.; Münte, T.F. Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature 2002, 415, 1026–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kovelman, I.; Baker, S.A.; Petitto, L.-A. Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 153–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gullifer, J.W.; Kousaie, S.; Gilbert, A.C.; Grant, A.; Giroud, N.; Coulter, K.; Klein, D.; Baum, S.; Phillips, N.; Titone, D. Bilingual language experience as a multidimensional spectrum: Associations with objective and subjective language proficiency. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2020, 42, 245–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Gullifer, J.W.; Titone, D. Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2020, 23, 283–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Jiao, L.; Grundy, J.G.; Liu, C.; Chen, B. Language context modulates executive control in bilinguals: Evidence from language production. Neuropsychologia 2020, 142, 107441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Jiao, L.; Liu, C.; de Bruin, A.; Chen, B. Effects of language context on executive control in unbalanced bilinguals: An ERPs study. Psychophysiology 2020, 57, e13653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Li, P.; Dong, Y. Language experiences and cognitive control: A dynamic perspective. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; pp. 27–52. [Google Scholar]
  57. Titone, D.A.; Tiv, M. Rethinking multilingual experience through a Systems Framework of Bilingualism. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2022, 26, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Coderre, E.L.; Van Heuven, W.J.; Conklin, K. The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in mon-olinguals and bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2013, 16, 420–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Brito, N.H.; Sebastián-Gallés, N.; Barr, R. Differences in Language Exposure and its Effects on Memory Flexibility in Monolingual, Bilingual, and Trilingual Infants. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 670–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Yow, W.Q.; Li, X. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Ooi, S.H.; Goh, W.D.; Sorace, A.; Bak, T.H. From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 867–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Gullifer, J.W.; Chai, X.J.; Whitford, V.; Pivneva, I.; Baum, S.; Klein, D.; Titone, D. Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia 2018, 117, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Gullifer, J.W.; Titone, D. Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2021, 150, 414–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Li, X.; Ng, K.K.; Wong, J.J.Y.; Lee, J.W.; Zhou, J.H.; Yow, W.Q. Bilingual language entropy influences executive functions through functional connectivity and signal variability. Brain Lang. 2021, 222, 105026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kałamała, P.; Chuderski, A.; Szewczyk, J.; Senderecka, M.; Wodniecka, Z. Bilingualism caught in a net: A new approach to understanding the complexity of bilingual experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2023, 152, 157–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kan, I.P.; Thompson-Schill, S.L. Effect of name agreement on prefrontal activity during overt and covert picture naming. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004, 4, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Piai, V.; Roelofs, A.; Acheson, D.J.; Takashima, A. Attention for speaking: Domain-general control from the anterior cingulate cortex in spoken word production. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Britt, A.E.; Ferrara, C.; Mirman, D. Distinct Effects of Lexical and Semantic Competition during Picture Naming in Younger Adults, Older Adults, and People with Aphasia. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Zhang, H.; Eppes, A.; Beatty-Martínez, A.; Navarro-Torres, C.; Diaz, M.T. Task difficulty modulates brain-behavior correlations in language production and cognitive control: Be-havioral and fMRI evidence from a phonological go/no-go picture-naming paradigm. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 18, 964–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Zhang, H.; Eppes, A.; Diaz, M.T. Task difficulty modulates age-related differences in the behavioral and neural bases of language production. Neuropsychologia 2019, 124, 254–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Li, P.; Zhang, F.; Yu, A.; Zhao, X. Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 23, 938–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Nasreddine, Z.S.; Phillips, N.A.; Bédirian, V.; Charbonneau, S.; Whitehead, V.; Collin, I.; Cummings, J.L.; Chertkow, H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool for Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 695–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Wong, A.; Xiong, Y.Y.; Kwan, P.W.; Chan, A.Y.; Lam, W.W.; Wang, K.; Chu, W.C.; Nyenhuis, D.L.; Nasreddine, Z.; Wong, L.K.; et al. The validity, reliability and clinical utility of the Hong Kong Montreal Cognitive Assessment (HK-MoCA) in patients with cerebral small vessel disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2009, 28, 81–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Ferraro, F.R.; Chelminski, I. Preliminary normative data on the Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Form (GDS-SF) in a young adult sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 1996, 52, 443–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. de Craen, A.J.M.; Heeren, T.J.; Gussekloo, J. Accuracy of the 15-item geriatric depression scale (GDS-15) in a community sample of the oldest old. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2003, 18, 63–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  77. Gong, Y. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised in China Version; Hunan Medical College: Changsha, China, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  78. Acheson, D.J.; Wells, J.B.; MacDonald, M.C. New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 278–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Patterson, J. Controlled Oral Word Association Test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Kreutzer, J.S., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 703–706. [Google Scholar]
  80. Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Keys, B.A.; Carter, C.S.; Cohen, J.D.; Kaye, J.A.; Janowsky, J.S.; Taylor, S.F.; Yesavage, J.A.; Mumenthaler, M.S.; et al. Context processing in older adults: Evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Braver, T.S.; Gray, J.R.; Burgess, G.C. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. Var. Work. Mem. 2007, 75, 106. [Google Scholar]
  82. Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. MacLeod, C.M. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 163–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Moreno-Martínez, F.J.; Montoro, P.R. An ecological alternative to Snodgrass & Vanderwart: 360 high quality colour images with norms for seven psycholinguistic variables. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37527. [Google Scholar]
  85. Brodeur, M.B.; Guérard, K.; Bouras, M. Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) Phase II: 930 New Normative Photos. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e106953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kilgarriff, A.; Reddy, S.; Pomikálek, J.; Avinesh, P.V.S. A corpus factory for many languages. In Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’10), Valletta, Malta, 19–21 May 2010. [Google Scholar]
  87. Dale, A.M. Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999, 8, 109–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Shannon, C.E. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 1948, 27, 379–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Gullifer, J.W.; Titone, D. Compute Language Entropy with {Language Entropy}; McGill University: Montreal, QC, Canada, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  90. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio, PBC: Boston, MA, USA, 2020; Available online: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  91. Gaffan, D.; Heywood, C.A. A spurious category-specific visual agnosia for living things in normal human and nonhuman primates. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1993, 5, 118–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Capitani, E.; Laiacona, M.; Barbarotto, R.; Trivelli, C. Living and non-living categories. Is there a “normal” asymmetry? Neuropsychology 1994, 32, 1453–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Farah, M.J.; Rabinowitz, C. Genetic and environmental influences on the organisation of semantic memory in the brain: Is “living things” an innate category? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2003, 20, 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Giussani, C.; Riva, M.; Gallucci, M.; Boukhatem, L.; Sganzerla, E.P.; Demonet, J.-F.; Roux, F.-E. Anatomical correlates for category-specific naming of living and non-living things. NeuroImage 2011, 56, 323–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022; Available online: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  96. Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer. Version 6.3.08. Available online: http://www.praat.org (accessed on 16 October 2019).
  97. Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv 2014, arXiv:1406.5823. [Google Scholar]
  98. Barr, D.J.; Levy, R.; Scheepers, C.; Tily, H.J. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 2013, 68, 255–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Zheng, X.; Roelofs, A.; Erkan, H.; Lemhöfer, K. Dynamics of inhibitory control during bilingual speech production: An electrophysiological study. Neuropsychologia 2020, 140, 107387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Glover, G.H.; Mueller, B.A.; Turner, J.A.; Van Erp, T.G.; Liu, T.T.; Greve, D.N.; Voyvodic, J.T.; Rasmussen, J.; Brown, G.G.; Keator, D.B.; et al. Function biomedical informatics research network recommendations for prospective multicenter func-tional MRI studies. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2012, 36, 39–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Lutkenhoff, E.S.; Rosenberg, M.; Chiang, J.; Zhang, K.; Pickard, J.D.; Owen, A.M.; Monti, M.M. Optimized Brain Extraction for Pathological Brains (optiBET). PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e115551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Smith, S.M.; Jenkinson, M.; Woolrich, M.W.; Beckmann, C.F.; Behrens, T.E.; Johansen-Berg, H.; Bannister, P.R.; De Luca, M.; Drobnjak, I.; Flitney, D.E.; et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 2004, 23 (Suppl. S1), S208–S219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Woolrich, M.W.; Behrens, T.E.; Beckmann, C.F.; Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S.M. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. NeuroImage 2004, 21, 1732–1747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Beckmann, C.F.; Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S.M. General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. NeuroImage 2003, 20, 1052–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Worsley, K.J. Statistical analysis of activation images. Funct. MRI Introd. Methods 2001, 14, 251–270. [Google Scholar]
  106. Hayasaka, S.; Nichols, T.E. Validating cluster size inference: Random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage 2003, 20, 2343–2356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Desikan, R.S.; Ségonne, F.; Fischl, B.; Quinn, B.T.; Dickerson, B.C.; Blacker, D.; Buckner, R.L.; Dale, A.M.; Maguire, R.P.; Hyman, B.T.; et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 2006, 31, 968–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Power, J.D.; Cohen, A.L.; Nelson, S.M.; Wig, G.S.; Barnes, K.A.; Church, J.A.; Vogel, A.C.; Laumann, T.O.; Miezin, F.M.; Schlaggar, B.L.; et al. Functional Network Organization of the Human Brain. Neuron 2011, 72, 665–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Zhang, H.; Diaz, M.T. Resting State Network Segregation Modulates Age-Related Differences in Language Production. Neurobiol. Lang 2023, 4, 382–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Fedorenko, E.; Hsieh, P.-J.; Nieto-Castañón, A.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Kanwisher, N. New Method for fMRI Investigations of Language: Defining ROIs Functionally in Individual Subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 2010, 104, 1177–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Nieto-Castanon, A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012, 2, 125–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Chan, M.Y.; Park, D.C.; Savalia, N.K.; Petersen, S.E.; Wig, G.S. Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, E4997–E5006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Cassady, K.E.; Adams, J.N.; Chen, X.; Maass, A.; Harrison, T.M.; Landau, S.; Baker, S.; Jagust, W. Alzheimer’s Pathology Is Associated with Dedifferentiation of Intrinsic Functional Memory Networks in Aging. Cereb. Cortex 2021, 31, 4781–4793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Geranmayeh, F.; Wise, R.J.; Mehta, A.; Leech, R. Overlapping Networks Engaged during Spoken Language Production and Its Cognitive Control. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 8728–8740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Kuzmina, E.; Weekes, B.S. Role of cognitive control in language deficits in different types of aphasia. Aphasiology 2016, 31, 765–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Blank, S.C.; Scott, S.K.; Murphy, K.; Warburton, E.; Wise, R.J.S. Speech production: Wernicke, Broca and beyond. Brain 2002, 125, 1829–1838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Aron, A.R.; Fletcher, P.C.; Bullmore, E.T.; Sahakian, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 115–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Indefrey, P.; Levelt, W.J. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 2004, 92, 101–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Chikazoe, J.; Konishi, S.; Asari, T.; Jimura, K.; Miyashita, Y. Activation of Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus during Response Inhibition across Response Modalities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Abutalebi, J.; Green, D.W. Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2016, 19, 689–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Sierpowska, J.; Fernandez-Coello, A.; Gomez-Andres, A.; Camins, C.; Castañer, S.; Juncadella, M.; Gabarrós, A.; Rodríguez-Fornells, A. Involvement of the middle frontal gyrus in language switching as revealed by electrical stimulation mapping and functional magnetic resonance imaging in bilingual brain tumor patients. Cortex 2018, 99, 78–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Hazem, S.R.; Awan, M.; Lavrador, J.P.; Patel, S.; Wren, H.M.; Lucena, O.; Semedo, C.; Irzan, H.; Melbourne, A.; Ourselin, S.; et al. Middle Frontal Gyrus and Area 55b: Perioperative Mapping and Language Outcomes. Front. Neurol. 2021, 12, 646075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Sugimoto, H.; Abe, M.S.; Otake-Matsuura, M. Word-producing brain: Contribution of the left anterior middle temporal gyrus to word production patterns in spoken language. Brain Lang. 2023, 238, 105233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. Emotion, Decision Making and the Orbitofrontal Cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 295–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Mayer, J.S.; Roebroeck, A.; Maurer, K.; Linden, D.E. Specialization in the default mode: Task-induced brain deactivations dissociate between visual working memory and attention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 31, 126–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Levy, B.J.; Wagner, A.D. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: Reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011, 1224, 40–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Seghier, M.L. The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscience 2013, 19, 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. McLeod, K.R.; Langevin, L.M.; Goodyear, B.G.; Dewey, D. Functional connectivity of neural motor networks is disrupted in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 2014, 4, 566–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Marek, S.; Hwang, K.; Foran, W.; Hallquist, M.N.; Luna, B. The Contribution of Network Organization and Integration to the Development of Cognitive Control. PLoS Biol. 2015, 13, e1002328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Rosselli, M.; Ardila, A.; Bernal, B. Angular gyrus connectivity model for language: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Rev. De Neurol. 2015, 60, 495–503. [Google Scholar]
  131. Friedman, N.P.; Robbins, T.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 2022, 47, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Schmid, M.S.; Dusseldorp, E. Quantitative analyses in a multivariate study of language attrition: The impact of extralinguistic factors. Second Lang. Res. 2010, 26, 125–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Pardo, J.V.; Pardo, P.J.; Janer, K.W.; Raichle, M.E. The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1990, 87, 256–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Kennedy, K.M.; Rodrigue, K.M.; Bischof, G.N.; Hebrank, A.C.; Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.; Park, D.C. Age trajectories of functional activation under conditions of low and high processing demands: An adult lifespan fMRI study of the aging brain. NeuroImage 2014, 104, 21–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Hyde, J.S. Gender Similarities and Differences. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014, 65, 373–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Hirnstein, M.; Hugdahl, K.; Hausmann, M. Cognitive sex differences and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review of 40 years of research. Laterality 2019, 24, 204–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Task design. Overview of the categorical semantic Go/No-Go picture naming task. Examples of Go and No-Go trials for each of the three conditions: All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias. Correct names for the two No-Go trials (withhold naming, in green) are fox and clown, respectively. Control demand for Go trials increases from the All Go to Go Bias to No-Go Bias condition. Task demand increases from All Go to Bias conditions. Naming demand increases from Go Bias to No-Go Bias conditions.
Figure 1. Task design. Overview of the categorical semantic Go/No-Go picture naming task. Examples of Go and No-Go trials for each of the three conditions: All Go, Go Bias, and No-Go Bias. Correct names for the two No-Go trials (withhold naming, in green) are fox and clown, respectively. Control demand for Go trials increases from the All Go to Go Bias to No-Go Bias condition. Task demand increases from All Go to Bias conditions. Naming demand increases from Go Bias to No-Go Bias conditions.
Brainsci 13 01587 g001
Figure 2. Behavioral results for the nonliving Go trials in Go/No-Go picture naming task. (a) Incorrect response rates for nonliving Go trials across conditions. Higher language entropy was associated with lower incorrect response rates. Incorrect response rates increased as control demand increased. (b) Reaction times (RTs) for nonliving Go trials across conditions. RTs were significantly longer as control demand increased. Yet, the main effect of language entropy and its interaction with control demand were not significant.
Figure 2. Behavioral results for the nonliving Go trials in Go/No-Go picture naming task. (a) Incorrect response rates for nonliving Go trials across conditions. Higher language entropy was associated with lower incorrect response rates. Incorrect response rates increased as control demand increased. (b) Reaction times (RTs) for nonliving Go trials across conditions. RTs were significantly longer as control demand increased. Yet, the main effect of language entropy and its interaction with control demand were not significant.
Brainsci 13 01587 g002
Figure 3. fMRI activation as a function of control demand. Overview of the regions in which there were significant increases in activation of (a) Go Bias condition > All Go condition, (b) No-Go Bias condition > Go Bias condition, and (c) No-Go Bias condition > All Go condition. Slices are depicted in increments of 10 mm, starting at z = −8 and ending at z = 62. Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Figure 3. fMRI activation as a function of control demand. Overview of the regions in which there were significant increases in activation of (a) Go Bias condition > All Go condition, (b) No-Go Bias condition > Go Bias condition, and (c) No-Go Bias condition > All Go condition. Slices are depicted in increments of 10 mm, starting at z = −8 and ending at z = 62. Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Brainsci 13 01587 g003
Figure 4. Correlations between language entropy and fMRI activation during production. (a) Regions in which positive correlations were found, i.e., regions in which higher language entropy values were associated with more activation. (b) Regions in which negative correlations were found, i.e., regions in which higher language entropy was associated with less activation. Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Figure 4. Correlations between language entropy and fMRI activation during production. (a) Regions in which positive correlations were found, i.e., regions in which higher language entropy values were associated with more activation. (b) Regions in which negative correlations were found, i.e., regions in which higher language entropy was associated with less activation. Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Brainsci 13 01587 g004
Figure 5. Negative correlations between language entropy and increased activation across conditions: (a) Negative correlation between language entropy and increases in activation between the All Go and Go Bias conditions (All Go < Go Bias). (b) Negative correlation between language entropy and increases in activation between the All Go and No-Go Bias conditions (All Go < No-Go Bias). Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Figure 5. Negative correlations between language entropy and increased activation across conditions: (a) Negative correlation between language entropy and increases in activation between the All Go and Go Bias conditions (All Go < Go Bias). (b) Negative correlation between language entropy and increases in activation between the All Go and No-Go Bias conditions (All Go < No-Go Bias). Activations were reported for clusters that had a corrected p-value of p < 0.05 at the cluster level.
Brainsci 13 01587 g005
Figure 6. The relationship between language segregation, language entropy, and task demand. Higher language entropy was associated with lower language network segregation. Yet, the main effect of task demand and its interaction with language entropy were not significant.
Figure 6. The relationship between language segregation, language entropy, and task demand. Higher language entropy was associated with lower language network segregation. Yet, the main effect of task demand and its interaction with language entropy were not significant.
Brainsci 13 01587 g006
Table 1. Participants’ demographic characteristics and neuropsychological testing scores.
Table 1. Participants’ demographic characteristics and neuropsychological testing scores.
MeanSD
Demographic Characteristics
   N36
   Age (years)21.11.8
   Education (years)15.11.7
   Gender (F/M)27/9
Neuropsychological testing 1
   MoCA (Score out of 30)28.781.16
   GDS-152.441.98
   Color Vision (Score out of 25)24.031.42
   Forward Digit Span (Score out of 8)7.310.74
   Backward Digit Span (Score out of 7)5.001.41
   Vocabulary (WAIS-RC) (Score out of 40)27.785.87
   Reading Habits (Score out of 35)20.614.17
   VF_Cantonese (Animal + Vegetable, Correct token)31.725.88
   VF_Mandarin (Fruit, Correct token)13.143.25
   VF_English (Occupation, Correct token)8.813.53
   Simple Speed (ms)259.4138.56
   Complex Speed (ms)292.8660.05
   AX-CPT: AY RT (ms)378.7051.70
   AX-CPT: AY ER (out of 1)0.190.17
   Stroop Effect (ms) (Incongruent-Congruent)22.6870.82
1 Neuropsychological testing included a Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) to assess general cognitive states [72,73]; a 15 items geriatric depression scale (GDS-15, a shortened version of the Geriatric Depression Scale) which has been verified as a reliable measure of depression in younger and older populations [74,75]; a Color Vision test; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) digit span tasks with forward and reverse sets to assess working memory [76]; a vocabulary test from the WAIS revised by China (WAIS-RC) to gauge vocabulary knowledge [77]; a reading habits questionnaire [78]; a categorical verbal fluency task (VF) using different semantic categories as another assessment of vocabulary in each language [79]; a simple and choice processing speed task to assess speed; an AX continuous performance task (AX-CPT) [80,81], and a Stroop task to assess executive functions [82,83]. All these tasks were administered and performed in Mandarin Chinese. SD represents standard deviation.
Table 2. Behavioral results on nonliving Go trials (incorrect response rate and reaction times).
Table 2. Behavioral results on nonliving Go trials (incorrect response rate and reaction times).
Incorrect Response Rate (%)Reaction Times (ms)
All Go18.5 (5.7)1127.57 (279.77)
Go Bias19.8 (6.5)1182.52 (288.13)
No-Go Bias33.3 (9.0)1229.99 (313.96)
The incorrect response rate of the Go trials was calculated for each condition by dividing the number of incorrect responses by the total number of nonliving Go trials in the corresponding condition. Values provided are means, with standard deviations in the parentheses.
Table 3. fMRI activation as a function of control demand.
Table 3. fMRI activation as a function of control demand.
HemisphereVoxelsCoordinates (mm)Z Value
xyz
Go Bias > All Go
Frontal poleRight3274636323.27
Middle frontal gyrusRight 5032324.06
Inferior frontal gyrusRight 5022303.43
Middle frontal gyrusLeft250−4616344.19
Inferior frontal gyrusLeft −4212344.00
Precentral gyrusLeft −4010323.37
NoGo Bias > Go Bias
Frontal orbital cortexLeft1477−3418−83.47
Frontal operculum cortexLeft −3420104.36
Inferior frontal gyrusLeft −4018103.15
Central opercular cortexLeft −366124.97
Insular cortexLeft −34884.25
Frontal orbital cortexRight10813022−84.12
Frontal operculum cortexRight 382064.17
Inferior frontal gyrusRight 541464.07
Central opercular cortexRight 386104.58
Precentral gyrusRight 52663.59
Insular cortexRight 36684.79
Anterior cingulate gyrusLeft2660−220345.04
Anterior cingulate gyrusRight 416365.45
Paracingulate gyrusLeft −1018423.60
Paracingulate gyrusRight 818424.01
Superior frontal gyrusRight3091016604.57
Superior temporal gyrusRight26552−14−83.54
Middle temporal gyrusRight 56−18−64.69
Angular gyrusRight53960−46244.55
Supramarginal gyrusRight 64−44243.48
NoGo Bias > All Go
Frontal orbital cortexLeft894−382224.48
Inferior frontal gyrusLeft −461823.48
Frontal operculum cortexLeft −3416105.09
Central opercular cortexLeft −44823.75
Insular cortexLeft −344104.86
Frontal orbital cortexRight352382423.49
Frontal operculum cortexRight 441224.60
Central opercular cortexRight 368103.94
Insular cortexRight 381044.88
Middle frontal gyrusRight1725224283.94
Inferior frontal gyrusRight 5016244.13
Anterior cingulate gyrusLeft1089−424324.81
Anterior cingulate gyrusRight 420324.80
Paracingulate gyrusLeft −1018404.65
Paracingulate gyrusRight 426304.69
Superior frontal gyrusRight238124664.53
Superior temporal gyrusRight19254−14−43.64
Middle temporal gyrusRight 52−26−43.54
Posterior cingulate gyrusRight2136−30264.28
Precuneus cortexRight20420−5443.31
Lingual gyrusRight 20−5843.61
Table 4. Correlations between language entropy and fMRI activation during production.
Table 4. Correlations between language entropy and fMRI activation during production.
HemisphereVoxelsCoordinates (mm)Z Value
xyz
Positive correlation
Frontal orbital cortexRight682232−244.72
Inferior frontal gyrusRight1985032105.64
Lingual gyrusRight45122−52−125.08
Temporal occipital fusiform cortexRight 26−52−164.83
Occipital fusiform gyrusRight 24−68−45.10
Negative correlation
Anterior cingulate gyrusLeft1795−84263.30
Anterior cingulate gyrusRight 840105.51
Paracingulate gyrusLeft −442204.14
Paracingulate gyrusRight 436104.20
Superior frontal gyrusLeft42−1026623.66
Middle frontal gyrusLeft408−368383.49
Precentral gyrusLeft −460385.05
Superior temporal gyrusLeft41−54−2−144.19
Middle temporal gyrusLeft398−48−28−83.52
Temporal fusiform cortexLeft −38−30−143.49
Temporal fusiform cortexRight13130−26−223.19
Inferior temporal gyrusLeft71−56−32−203.71
Postcentral gyrusRight4912−40523.11
Posterior supramarginal gyrusLeft808−36−54583.48
Superior parietal lobeLeft −36−56443.63
Angular gyrusLeft −40−58464.47
Angular gyrusRight9746−62203.77
Lateral occipital cortexRight 48−64204.87
Lateral occipital cortexLeft56−46−70264.36
Table 5. Correlations between language entropy and increased activation across conditions.
Table 5. Correlations between language entropy and increased activation across conditions.
HemisphereVoxelsCoordinates (mm)Z Value
xyz
GoBias > AllGo: Negative correlation
Precuneus cortexLeft15−12−48563.50
Postcentral gyrusLeft −12−48543.31
NoGoBias > AllGo: Negative correlation
Postcentral gyrusLeft29−16−46583.80
Occipital poleRight654−90143.49
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kang, K.; Xiao, Y.; Yu, H.; Diaz, M.T.; Zhang, H. Multilingual Language Diversity Protects Native Language Production under Different Control Demands. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1587. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13111587

AMA Style

Kang K, Xiao Y, Yu H, Diaz MT, Zhang H. Multilingual Language Diversity Protects Native Language Production under Different Control Demands. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(11):1587. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13111587

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kang, Keyi, Yumeng Xiao, Hanxiang Yu, Michele T. Diaz, and Haoyun Zhang. 2023. "Multilingual Language Diversity Protects Native Language Production under Different Control Demands" Brain Sciences 13, no. 11: 1587. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13111587

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop