Next Article in Journal
Win and Loss Responses in the Monetary Incentive Delay Task Mediate the Link between Depression and Problem Drinking
Next Article in Special Issue
Attentional Bias for Sleep-Related Words as a Function of Severity of Insomnia Symptoms
Previous Article in Journal
Sex Differences in the Behavioural Aspects of the Cuprizone-Induced Demyelination Model in Mice
Previous Article in Special Issue
Assessment of Executive and Cognitive Functions in Children with Restless Sleep Disorder: A Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Facial Emotion Recognition Deficit in Children with Moderate/Severe Obstructive Sleep Apnea

1
Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Beijing Children’s Hospital, National Center for Children’s Health, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100045, China
2
National Center for International Research on Green Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
3
Lab of Light and Physio-Psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
4
Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Optical Information Materials and Technology & Institute of Electronic Paper Displays, South China Academy of Advanced Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
5
Beijing Key Laboratory of Pediatric Diseases of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Beijing Pediatric Research Institute, Beijing Children’s Hospital, National Center for Children’s Health, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100045, China
6
Division of Respiratory Medicine, Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Child Development and Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Child Health and Disorders, Chongqing 400014, China
7
International Science and Technology Cooperation Base of Child Development and Critical Disorders, Chongqing Key Laboratory of Pediatrics, Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing 400715, China
8
Department of Respiratory Medicine, Beijing Children’s Hospital, National Center for Children’s Health, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100045, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(12), 1688; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121688
Submission received: 31 October 2022 / Revised: 3 December 2022 / Accepted: 5 December 2022 / Published: 8 December 2022

Abstract

:
Although previous studies have reported a facial expression classification deficit among adults with SDB, we do not know whether these findings can be generalized to children. In our study, children with sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) were divided into three groups: primary snoring (n = 51), mild obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) (n = 39), and moderate/severe OSA (n = 26). All participants, including 20 healthy controls, underwent an overnight polysomnography recording and the Emotional Expression Recognition Task. Psychosocial problems were evaluated using the parent-reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). There was a borderline significant interaction between expression category and group on reaction times. Further analysis revealed that positive classification advantage (PCA) disappeared in the moderate/severe OSA group, whereas it persisted in the control, primary snoring, and mild OSA groups. Emotional symptoms were positively correlated with OAHI. In both the happy and sad conditions, RT was negatively related to age and body mass index (BMI) but was independent of the obstructive apnea-hypopnea index (OAHI), arterial oxygen (SaO2) and total sleep time. The accuracy of identifying a sad expression was negatively related to conduct problems. Children with moderate/severe OSA exhibited dysfunction in facial expression categorization, which could potentially affect social communication ability.

1. Introduction

Sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) is a common illness in childhood, with an incidence of 5.1% to 13.3% [1]. SDB is a chronic sleep disorder characterized by repetitive upper airway closures, oxygen desaturation and sleep fragmentation leading to cognitive dysfunction. In recent years, mounting studies have reported the prevalence of impaired social functioning in children with SDB [2,3], such as conflict resolution and social interaction [4].
The recognition of facial expressions is a higher-level social cognitive ability. Faces and facial expressions are of fundamental importance in social communication and behavioral regulation because they provide key signals for understanding the motivations, feelings, and intentions of others [5]. Positive facial expressions are typically detected more quickly than negative facial expressions, which refers to a positive classification advantage (PCA) [6]. This phenomenon is reported to be particularly pronounced for the recognition of happy (positive) and sad (negative) expressions [7,8,9,10]. The disappearance of this effect is considered to be suggestive of an impairment of social cognitive functioning (such as emotion recognition, emotional reactivity and emotion regulation) in certain mental disorders [11], such as schizophrenia [12], insomnia [13], learning disorder [14], attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [15] and major depressive disorder [16], which might have undesirable effects on family and peer relationships.
Deficits in facial emotional information processing have been observed in both experimental acute sleep deprivation [17,18,19,20] and chronic sleep disorders [6,10] in adults. Unlike specific deficits in neurocognitive performance in the adult SDB population, children with SDB have been found to show a generalized impact on neurocognitive functioning [21]. Inadequate sleep is associated with deficits in the processing of facial information during early adolescence, a key developmentally sensitive period in socio-emotional development [22]. Previous studies have reported a facial expression classification deficit among adults with SDB [9,23]. Thus, it is possible that this deficit also exists in the pediatric SDB population. Moreover, previous studies recruiting adult patients with OSA [9,23] have failed to consider the potential role of OSA severity (mild OSA vs. moderate/severe OSA), or the severity of SDB (primary snoring vs. mild OSA vs. moderate/severe OSA). A recent meta-analysis reported that cognitive impairment exists across SDB severity (PS, mild OSA and moderate/severe OSA) [21]. The primary objective of the present study was to directly test facial expression classification performance in children with SDB of varying severity (primary snoring, mild OSA, and moderate/severe OSA). A recent study using diffusion tensor imaging found white matter alterations in the brain in children with OSA [24]. These alternations might be related to emotion processing [25]—this suggests the existence of difficulty in recognizing emotion. Thus, we predicted that children with different severities of OSA or even children with primary snoring may have an emotional recognition deficit.
Besides the behavioral pattern of emotion recognition, some correlations were also identified in previous studies, which might be the potential targets for intervention studies to improve the ability to recognize emotion. The ability to recognize emotion followed increasing linear trends over the age range of 6–16 years for happy, fear, disgust and surprise, but not for sad and angry expressions [26]. Considering the close relationship between sleep and emotion [27], sleep characteristics might be related to the emotion recognition performance. For example, long total sleep duration was positively associated with improved ability to recognize facial expression [28], although the evidence was not totally consistent [29]. Though not conclusive, the association between BMI and emotion recognition has been repeatedly reported in the previous literature, suggesting that a high BMI might impair the ability to recognize emotion [30,31]. A recent systematic review found that facial emotion recognition problems were linked to ADHD and conduct problems [32]. Employing adults with OSA as participants, Guo et al. (2020) found that the behavioral performance of emotion recognition was closely related to AHI and not to SaO2 [9]. Similar studies in children with OSA are still lacking. Thus, the second objective of the current investigation was to explore the correlations of emotion recognition performance. According to previous research, we hypothesized that emotion recognition performance would be positively related to age and sleep duration, while negatively associated with BMI, conduct problems and AHI.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

We recruited 118 children with SDB and 20 age/gender-matched healthy controls without a history of habitual snoring. Patients were recruited from either the inpatient department or outpatient department at the Center of Sleep Medicine, Beijing Children’s Hospital in Beijing, China. Snoring (≥3 nights per week) was subjectively assessed by parent reports. Children who were referred to a sleep center for sleep monitoring due to habitual snoring were consecutively enrolled. Inclusion criteria were the age range of 6–14 years and the presence of habitual snoring. Children with a known severe chronic illness such as genetic syndromes, craniofacial anomalies and neuromuscular disorders or who had undergone adenotonsillectomy were excluded. None of the patients underwent any intervening treatment. All participants went to bed before 22:00 and got up after 06:00. A valid PSG recording required at least three hours of sleep time. All recruited children completed the emotional expression recognition task and the full-night standard PSG. Participants completed the behavioral tests each afternoon. Parents (predominantly mothers) provided demographic information, and questionnaires were collected on the day of the examination.
All participants were right-handed. Children were also excluded if they suffered from dyslipidemia, diabetes mellitus, neurodevelopmental delays, hypertension or congenital heart disease, or if they used psychostimulant medications.
Children provided verbal or written assent and a parent or legal guardian provided written informed consent. Research was in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the ethics committee of the Beijing Children’s Hospital.

2.2. Procedure

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a 25-item screening tool [33], which is widely used to assess different psychosocial and behavioral problems in children. [34,35,36,37] The SDQ contains five subscales containing five items each: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems and prosocial behavior. Higher scores indicate more significant degrees of behavioral and emotional problems, except for prosocial behavior, for which a higher score indicates fewer behavioral problems. In the current study, we used the Chinese parent version of the SDQ, which has been well validated in previous studies [38,39].
The emotional expression recognition task [9,13] was widely used to access the processing of emotional information in both healthy participants [40] and the participants with mental disorders [12,13,16], and thus, was employed in our study to explore emotion recognition ability and directly compare with the findings in adults with OSA [9]. In this task, the participants were instructed to recognize the expressions expressed by schematic faces, which has been established to be easily recognized by preschool-aged children [41] and effectively reflect the distinct brain activation pattern when comparing emotional with neutral schematic facial expressions [42]. In the current study, the schematic face was comprised with one circle as the face, two dots as eyes, one short vertical line as the nose and one short horizontal line as the mouth. The facial expression was manipulated by the shape of the mouth (see Figure 1). Eighteen different schematic face models were employed to avoid the low levels of processing and the boredom caused by the repetition of a single face stimulus. We obtained different face models by changing the face shape, particularly the mouths.
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a quiet and dimly lit room to complete the test. E-prime 2.0 was used to present the task and collect responses. Stimuli were presented at the center of a computer monitor (13-inch Apple Macbook Air laptop). When viewed at a distance of approximately 70 cm, the stimuli were displayed at a size of 6.89° × 6.30° of visual angle. Each face stimulus was presented twice, which led to 108 trials (18 face models × 3 expressions × 2 times) in one block in the formal experiment. Before the formal experiment, there were 12 practice trials to help the participants become familiar with the emotional expression recognition task. If necessary, additional practice trials were allowed to ensure that children understood the procedure.
Each trial of the task consisted of the following sequence of events (see Figure 1). Face stimuli were presented for 300 ms in a pseudo-randomized order, followed by a gray rectangle of the same size as the face stimuli. In the happy or sad condition, after the emotional expression stimulus disappeared, the gray rectangle remained on the screen until participants made a response by pressing one of the labeled keys on a standard computer keyboard. In the neutral expression condition, the gray rectangle was presented for 600 ms after the neutral expression disappeared. Participants were asked to classify the three schematic facial expressions (happy, sad or neutral) by pressing the corresponding key (happy: F; sad: J; neutral: no response) on the keyboard as quickly and accurately as possible. The assignment of response keys to different emotional expressions was balanced across participants. The participants completed two blocks with a short break between blocks.

2.3. Overnight Polysomnography (PSG)

All children underwent standard pediatric overnight clinical PSG monitoring (Compumedics E; Compumedics, Melbourne, Australia; or ALICE 5; Philips Respironics, Amsterdam, Netherlands) at Beijing Children’s Hospital. No consumption of coffee, tea or other stimulating substances was allowed 24 h before the study. All clinical assessments and diagnoses were made by trained and experienced clinicians before PSG. Nasal airflow, pulse oxygen saturation (SaO2), snoring, electroencephalography, electrooculogram, submental electromyogram, electrocardiogram, heart rate, body position and thoracic and abdominal movements were recorded. PSG data were manually scored by experienced sleep technicians according to the standard guidelines of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) [43]. Sleep data were calculated and evaluated by experienced pediatric PSG technicians. In the current study, SDB severity was categorized in accordance with the Chinese guidelines for the diagnosis and treatment of childhood obstructive sleep apnea (2020): primary snoring (OAHI < 1), mild OSA (1 < OAHI  ≤ 5), moderate/severe OSA (OAHI > 5) [44].

2.4. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were performed with SPSS 25.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Continuous data were expressed as mean ± SD or mean ± SE. Differences in continuous clinical data and PSG data were tested using ANOVA followed Turkey’s post hoc test. Categorical data were expressed as n(%). We used the chi-square test to compare categorical data. For the behavioral performance of the emotional expression recognition task, data exceeding ± 3SD were treated as outliers and were not included in the formal analysis. Repeated-measure ANOVAs were performed separately on reaction time (RT) for correct responses and accuracy rate (ACC) with group (control vs. primary snoring vs. mild OSA vs. moderate/severe OSA) as the between-participants factor and expression (happy vs. sad) as the within-participants factor. Post hoc analysis was conducted if the interaction was significant. Correlations among clinical, PSG, and behavioral data were analyzed using Pearson’s correlation coefficients for all participants. p values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. The strength of correlation according to the general guidelines [42] was also provided.

3. Results

3.1. Clinical Characteristics and PSG Characteristics

The data from two participants were excluded in the formal analysis due to the outliers (one PS patient on RTSad and one mild OSA patient on ACCSad). As a result, 116 children with SDB and 20 age/gender-matched healthy controls without a history of habitual snoring were included in the formal analyses. Table 1 shows participants’ clinical characteristics and PSG characteristics. We divided participants into four groups based on OAHI scores: controls (n = 20), primary snoring (n = 51), mild OSA (n = 39), and moderate/severe OSA (n = 26). The distribution of sample size for each group was similar to previous studies [45,46,47]. Age and gender did not differ between the four groups. However, the moderate/severe OSA group showed significantly higher body mass index (BMI) scores compared with the other three groups (p = 0.008). However, there was no difference in BMI z-score between the groups. Higher OAHI scores (p < 0.001), total arousal index (ArI) scores (p < 0.001) and lower SpO2 nadir (p < 0.001) were observed in the moderate/severe OSA group compared with the other three groups. No significant differences were found in time spent with SpO2 < 90% (p = 0.129) between the four groups.

3.2. Behavioral Performance

For each condition, error trials and RT below or above two standard deviations of the individual’s average RT were excluded. Behavioral results are shown in Table 2 and Figure 2. The main effect of group was not significant (F (1,132) = 0.318, p = 0.813, η2 = 0.007), indicating that RTs were similar between four groups (control: 524.45 ms, standard error [SE] = 44.74, PS: 567.73ms, SE = 28.02, mild OSA: 575.88 ms, SE = 32.04, MSOSA: 556.47 ms, SE = 39.24). A significant main effect was obtained for expression (F (1,132) = 35.165, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.210), indicating that RT was significantly faster for the recognition of happy stimuli (M = 534.46 ms, SE = 17.85) compared with that for the recognition of sad stimuli (M = 577.81 ms, SE = 19.42). The group × expression interaction was marginal (F (3,132) = 2.12, p = 0.100, η2 = 0.046). To further test the first hypothesis, a post hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction was applied and revealed that, in the normal control, PS and mild OSA group, the RT to happy face stimuli was faster than that for sad faces (p = 0.033; p < 0.001; p = 0.001, respectively), thus, reflecting obvious PCA. However, the difference on RT between the two expressions was not significant in the MSOSA group (p = 0.136). The first hypothesis was, thus, partially supported.
For ACC, there was no main effect of group (F (1,132) < 0.001, p  =  0.988, η2 < 0.001), and no significant interaction between groups and expression (F (3,132)  =  0.031, p  =  0.993, η2 = 0.001) and no main effect of expression (F (3,132)  =  0.785, p  =  0.504, η2 = 0.018).

3.3. Correlations Results

Emotional symptoms positively correlated with OAHI (r = 0.188 (small), p = 0.028). For RTs, happy and sad expressions were negatively related to age (r = −0.669 (large), p < 0.001 and r = −0.612 (large), p < 0.001) and BMI (r = −0.246 (small), p = 0.004 and r = −0.249 (small), p = 0.003). For ACC, the sad face categorization was positively correlated with conduct problems (r = −0.191 (small), p = 0.026). All non-significant results can be found in Supplementary Materials. The second hypothesis was, thus, partially supported.

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to use the schematic facial emotion recognition paradigm to explore whether deficits exist in face processing in children with SDB and the relationship between facial emotion recognition performance and psychosocial problems. Consistent with previous studies, PCA was observed in the healthy control group. In the RT analysis, although there were no between-group differences, we found a borderline interaction between expression and group, which was consistent with our hypothesis. Further analysis revealed that the PCA disappeared only in the moderate/severe OSA group, whereas it persisted in the PS and mild OSA groups. We found that emotional symptoms were positively correlated with OAHI. For both the happy and sad conditions, RT was negatively related to age and BMI. For the sad condition, ACC was negatively correlated with conduct problems.
With increasing concern regarding the relationship between social cognitive deficits and sleep disorder, facial emotion recognition has attracted substantial recent research attention in the field of sleep medicine [48]. Facial emotion recognition is commonly studied using emotional expression recognition tasks. Previous findings suggest that facial recognition may be impaired in children with disrupted sleep [22]. A recent behavioral study of emotion recognition in adult OSA reported that the phenomenon of PCA disappeared in the OSA group, but also that there were between-group differences in RT between an OSA group and a normal control group [9]. In the current study, when children with SDB were further divided into three groups (PS, mild OSA, and MSOSA), PCA disappeared only in the moderate/severe OSA group. Comparing behavioral data between the SDB group and the normal control group revealed a trend for worse performance in the SDB group, but the difference was not significant. Some previous studies of facial emotion recognition of OSA in adults employed different facial expressions. For example, Cronlein et al. used the Facial Expressed Emotion Labelling (FEEL) test, which included six facial expressions (sadness, happiness, anger, anxiety, surprise and disgust) [23]. The results revealed that emotion recognition was impaired only in two of the six facial expressions (happy and sad) conditions in adults with OSA. Previous studies have indicated that following poor or insufficient sleep, facial expression recognition deficits were particularly evident in the recognition of happy and sad expressions [10,13,23,49]. As in these previous studies, group differences were also found between happy and sad conditions in the current study. Previous studies reported that PCA occurs in both healthy adults and children [50,51,52], and this finding was replicated in the present study. Positive facial expressions may have an advantage in visual processing because of the importance of positive expressions in emotion regulation and social relationship formation, and because they occur at a higher frequency than negative facial expressions. Positive facial expressions can facilitate communication and trust, maximize reward, and foster alliances and collaboration [53]. It has long been suggested that the accurate identification of positive facial expressions is an evolutionary adaptation that facilitates functioning in the community. Aberrant emotional face processing is associated with social relationship dysfunction. The facial emotion recognition paradigm can be used as an objective and reliable behavioral marker in studies of emotional problems in children with SDB, but also to achieve a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying emotional and behavioral problems in this population.
We also found a positive association between emotional symptoms and OAHI, which is the gold standard for the evaluation of the severity of SDB in children. This is consistent with Horne et al. [54]. They found that the mild OSA and MSOSA group have more internalizing behavioral problems (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, somatic complaints) compared with the normal control group. More research with larger sample sizes is needed in the future.
The small but significant associations were also identified for BMI with emotion recognition performance, which replicated previous findings [30,31]. These findings suggested that interventions aim at losing weight might be beneficial to increase emotion recognition ability. The absence of association between TST and emotion recognition performance replicated previous findings [29], while contradicted most previous research [28]. Thus, this null result should be confirmed in future studies.
Previous studies reported that behavioral results in adults with OSA were positively related to the apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) and SaO2 [9,55]. AHI and SaO2 are both indicators of ventilation. We did not find any significant relationships between behavioral results and indicators of ventilation. Only two large negative correlations were found between age and RTHappy/RTSad, which was consistent with the increasing linear trend of emotion recognition ability over the age range of 6–16 years found in previous research [26]. Children’s facial recognition ability increased with age, which was consistent with a previous study [56]. It is well known that the pathological mechanisms of adult OSA and pediatric OSA are different [57]. The pathomechanisms may differ among children with SDB at different developmental stages, and follow-up studies should examine the effects of the developmental stage. The present study highlights the importance of assessing emotional symptoms in children with moderate-to-severe SDB.
In addition to the correlation between PSG parameters and emotional recognition performance, we also aimed to explore the relationships between social problems and emotional recognition performance. The negative association between conduct problems and ACCSad found in the current investigation was not consistent with the findings of a previous study [15]. In that study, the fear recognition deficit was found to be inversely correlated with hyperactivity in adolescent boys with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). These discrepancies might have been caused by differences in disorders (SDB vs. ADHD), age (children vs. adolescence) or gender (boys and girls vs. boys only).
The neural mechanisms underlying emotion recognition deficits in children with moderate/severe OSA might be related to white matter alterations in the brain (which are related to emotion processing [25]) identified in previous research [24]. However, the results only revealed brain white matter alterations in boys. Thus, this possibility requires further empirical investigation, and potential gender differences warrant further consideration.
The current study involved several limitations that should be considered. First, although an emotion recognition deficit was revealed for children with moderate/severe OSA, future studies will be needed to determine whether differences exist in emotion recognition performance between the moderate and severe OSA groups. Second, only happy and sad expression stimuli were employed in the current investigation. Future studies testing more categories of basic emotions (e.g., anger, fear and surprise) and complex emotions (e.g., shame, guilt and pride) may provide more comprehensive insights into the emotion recognition performance of children with SDB. The current investigation only used the static expressions, however, the processing of dynamic expressions might be different from that of static emotions, as previous research indicated [58]. It would be interesting to explore the recognition performance of dynamic emotions in the future. There are numerous tasks to assess emotion recognition performance, and a previous meta-analytic study found that task characteristics might exert influence on emotion recognition performance [59]. Thus, various task paradigms can be used in future studies to test the generalization of the current findings and the potential task-specific effects. It should be noted that some correlations were significant but small in the present investigation, thus, caution should be taken when making inferences from these correlation results; furthermore, studies with a large sample size and longitudinal design are needed to confirm the current findings and clarify the directionality of the associations.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our results indicate that children with moderate/severe OSA exhibit dysfunction of emotional expression categorization, which could potentially lead to a decline in social communication ability.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci12121688/s1, Table S1: Correlations between behavioral performance and demographics and PSG characteristics; Table S2: Correlations between PSG characteristics and psychosocial and behavioral problems.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Z.X. and X.N.; Methodology, F.-J.Z.; Software, F.-J.Z.; Validation, Y.W., X.X. and F.-J.Z.; Formal analysis, F.-J.Z.; Investigation, F.-J.Z.; Resources, Q.-W.C. and Y.W.; Data curation, Y.W.; Writing—original draft preparation, F.-J.Z., Q.-W.C. and Z.X.; Writing—review and editing, F.-J.Z., Q.-W.C. and Z.X.; Visualization, F.-J.Z. and Q.-W.C.; Supervision, Z.X. and X.N.; Project administration, Y.W.; Funding acquisition, Z.X. and X.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Beijing Natural Science Foundation (No. 7212033); Respiratory Research Project of National Clinical Research Center for Respiratory Diseases (No. HXZX-20210401).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the ethics committee of the Beijing Children’s Hospital (approval no: 2020-k-93).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Children provided verbal or written assent and a parent or legal guardian provided written informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

The data used to support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Abbreviations

AASMAmerican Academy of Sleep Medicine
ACCaccuracy rate
ADHDattention deficit and hyperactivity disorder
AHIapnea-hypopnea index
ArIarousal index
BMIbody mass index
MAOSAmoderate/severe obstructive sleep apnea
OAHIobstructive apnea-hypopnea index
OSAobstructive sleep apnea
PCApositive classification advantage
PSprimary snoring
PSGpolysomnography
RTreaction time
SaO2arterial oxygen
SDstandard deviation
SDBsleep-disordered breathing
SDQstrengths and difficulties questionnaire
SEstandard error
TSTtotal sleep time

References

  1. Guo, Y.; Pan, Z.; Gao, F.; Wang, Q.; Pan, S.; Xu, S.; Hui, Y.; Li, L.; Qian, J. Characteristics and risk factors of children with sleep-disordered breathing in Wuxi, China. BMC Pediatr. 2020, 20, 310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Operto, F.F.; Precenzano, F.; Bitetti, I.; Lanzara, V.; Fontana, M.L.; Pastorino, G.M.G.; Carotenuto, M.; Pisani, F.; Polito, A.N.; Smirni, D.; et al. Emotional Intelligence in Children with Severe Sleep-Related Breathing Disorders. Behav. Neurol. 2019, 2019, 6530539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Kheirandish-Gozal, L.; Yoder, K.; Kulkarni, R.; Gozal, D.; Decety, J. Preliminary functional MRI neural correlates of executive functioning and empathy in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 2014, 37, 587–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Pelayo, R.; Sivan, Y. Increased behavioral morbidity in school-aged children with sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics 2005, 116, 797–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dollion, N.; Grandgeorge, M.; Saint-Amour, D.; Hosein Poitras Loewen, A.; Francois, N.; Fontaine, N.M.G.; Champagne, N.; Plusquellec, P. Emotion Facial Processing in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Pilot Study of the Impact of Service Dogs. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 869452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kyle, S.D.; Beattie, L.; Spiegelhalder, K.; Rogers, Z.; Espie, C.A. Altered emotion perception in insomnia disorder. Sleep 2014, 37, 775–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Leppanen, J.M.; Hietanen, J.K. Positive facial expressions are recognized faster than negative facial expressions, but why? Psychol. Res. 2004, 69, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Liu, X.; Liao, Y.; Zhou, L.; Sun, G.; Li, M.; Zhao, L. Mapping the time course of the positive classification advantage: An ERP study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 13, 491–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Guo, J.; Ma, Y.; Liu, Z.; Wang, F.; Hou, X.; Chen, J.; Hong, Y.; Xu, S.; Liu, X. Performance of facial expression classification tasks in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2020, 16, 523–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lee, V.V.; Schembri, R.; Jordan, A.S.; Jackson, M.L. The independent effects of sleep deprivation and sleep fragmentation on processing of emotional information. Behav. Brain Res. 2022, 424, 113802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Collin, L.; Bindra, J.; Raju, M.; Gillberg, C.; Minnis, H. Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: A systematic review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 1505–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Liang, S.; Chen, S.; Zhao, L.; Miao, D. Categorization of emotional faces in schizophrenia patients: An ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 2019, 713, 134493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Xu, S.; Liu, X.; Zhao, L. Categorization of Emotional Faces in Insomnia Disorder. Front. Neurol. 2020, 11, 569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Operto, F.F.; Pastorino, G.M.G.; Stellato, M.; Morcaldi, L.; Vetri, L.; Carotenuto, M.; Viggiano, A.; Coppola, G. Facial Emotion Recognition in Children and Adolescents with Specific Learning Disorder. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Aspan, N.; Bozsik, C.; Gadoros, J.; Nagy, P.; Inantsy-Pap, J.; Vida, P.; Halasz, J. Emotion recognition pattern in adolescent boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. BioMed Res. Int. 2014, 2014, 761340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Xin, W.; Yu, R.; Zhao, L. Event-related-potential based evidence of cognitive dysfunction of processing emotional faces in major depressive disorder patients. Neurosci. Lett. 2021, 742, 135545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Tempesta, D.; Couyoumdjian, A.; Curcio, G.; Moroni, F.; Marzano, C.; de Gennaro, L.; Ferrara, M. Lack of sleep affects the evaluation of emotional stimuli. Brain Res. Bull. 2010, 82, 104–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Franzen, P.L.; Buysse, D.J.; Dahl, R.E.; Thompson, W.; Siegle, G.J. Sleep deprivation alters pupillary reactivity to emotional stimuli in healthy young adults. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 80, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Van der Helm, E.; Gujar, N.; Walker, M.P. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep 2010, 33, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Killgore, W.D.S.; Balkin, T.J.; Yarnell, A.M.; Capaldi, V.F., II. Sleep deprivation impairs recognition of specific emotions. Neurobiol. Sleep Circadian Rhythm. 2017, 3, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Menzies, B.; Teng, A.; Burns, M.; Lah, S. Neurocognitive outcomes of children with sleep disordered breathing: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2022, 63, 101629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Soffer-Dudek, N.; Sadeh, A.; Dahl, R.E.; Rosenblat-Stein, S. Poor sleep quality predicts deficient emotion information processing over time in early adolescence. Sleep 2011, 34, 1499–1508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Cronlein, T.; Langguth, B.; Eichhammer, P.; Busch, V. Impaired Recognition of Facially Expressed Emotions in Different Groups of Patients with Sleep Disorders. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0152754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Mei, L.; Li, X.; Zhou, G.; Ji, T.; Chen, J.; Xu, Z.; Peng, Y.; Liu, Y.; Li, H.; Zhang, J.; et al. Effects of obstructive sleep apnoea severity on neurocognitive and brain white matter alterations in children according to sex: A tract-based spatial statistics study. Sleep Med. 2021, 82, 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Kumar, R.; Macey, P.M.; Cross, R.L.; Woo, M.A.; Yan-Go, F.L.; Harper, R.M. Neural alterations associated with anxiety symptoms in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Depress. Anxiety 2009, 26, 480–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Lawrence, K.; Campbell, R.; Skuse, D. Age, gender, and puberty influence the development of facial emotion recognition. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Gregory, A.M.; Sadeh, A. Sleep, emotional and behavioral difficulties in children and adolescents. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 16, 129–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Walker, M.P.; van der Helm, E. Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 731–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Holding, B.C.; Laukka, P.; Fischer, H.; Banziger, T.; Axelsson, J.; Sundelin, T. Multimodal Emotion Recognition Is Resilient to Insufficient Sleep: Results From Cross-Sectional and Experimental Studies. Sleep 2017, 40, zsx145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Romero-Rebollar, C.; Garcia-Gomez, L.; Baez-Yanez, M.G.; Gutierrez-Aguilar, R.; Pacheco-Lopez, G. Adiposity affects emotional information processing. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 879065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Vaioli, G.; Scarpina, F. Facial Emotion Recognition in Obesity and in Fibromyalgia: A Systematic Review. NeuroSci 2021, 2, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Cooper, S.; Hobson, C.W.; van Goozen, S.H. Facial emotion recognition in children with externalising behaviours: A systematic review. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2020, 25, 1068–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Goodman, R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 581–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Paine, A.L.; van Goozen, S.H.M.; Burley, D.T.; Anthony, R.; Shelton, K.H. Facial emotion recognition in adopted children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Staff, A.I.; Luman, M.; van der Oord, S.; Bergwerff, C.E.; van den Hoofdakker, B.J.; Oosterlaan, J. Facial emotion recognition impairment predicts social and emotional problems in children with (subthreshold) ADHD. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2022, 31, 715–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hunnikin, L.M.; Wells, A.E.; Ash, D.P.; van Goozen, S.H.M. The nature and extent of emotion recognition and empathy impairments in children showing disruptive behaviour referred into a crime prevention programme. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 29, 363–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Burley, D.T.; Hobson, C.W.; Adegboye, D.; Shelton, K.H.; van Goozen, S.H.M. Negative parental emotional environment increases the association between childhood behavioral problems and impaired recognition of negative facial expressions. Dev. Psychopathol. 2022, 34, 936–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Du, Y.; Kou, J.; Coghill, D. The validity, reliability and normative scores of the parent, teacher and self report versions of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in China. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2008, 2, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Kou, J.; Du, Y.; Xia, L. Reliability and validity of “children strengths and difficulties questionnaire” in Shanghai norm. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2005, 17, 25–28. [Google Scholar]
  40. Xu, Q.; Yang, Y.; Wang, P.; Sun, G.; Zhao, L. Gender differences in preattentive processing of facial expressions: An ERP study. Brain Topogr. 2013, 26, 488–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. MacDonald, P.M.; Kirkpatrick, S.W.; Sullivan, L.A. Schematic drawings of facial expressions for emotion recognition and interpretation by preschool-aged children. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 1996, 122, 373–388. [Google Scholar]
  42. Wright, C.I.; Martis, B.; Shin, L.M.; Fischer, H.; Rauch, S.L. Enhanced amygdala responses to emotional versus neutral schematic facial expressions. Neuroreport 2002, 13, 785–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 3rd ed.; American Academy of Sleep Medicine: Darien, IL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  44. Working group of Chinese Guideline for the Diagnosis and Treatment of Childhood OSA; Subspecialty Group of Pediatrics, Society of Otorhinolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, Chinese Medical Association; Subspecialty Group of Respiratory Diseases, Society of Pediatrics, Chinese Medical Association; Society of Pediatric Surgery, Chinese Medical Association; Editorial Board of Chinese Journal of Otorhinolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery; Ni, X. Chinese guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of childhood obstructive sleep apnea (2020). Pediatr. Investig. 2021, 5, 167–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hunter, S.J.; Gozal, D.; Smith, D.L.; Philby, M.F.; Kaylegian, J.; Kheirandish-Gozal, L. Effect of Sleep-disordered Breathing Severity on Cognitive Performance Measures in a Large Community Cohort of Young School-aged Children. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2016, 194, 739–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Horne, R.S.; Yang, J.S.; Walter, L.M.; Richardson, H.L.; O’Driscoll, D.M.; Foster, A.M.; Wong, S.; Ng, M.L.; Bashir, F.; Patterson, R.; et al. Nocturnal dipping is preserved in children with sleep disordered breathing regardless of its severity. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2013, 48, 1127–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Yang, J.S.; Nicholas, C.L.; Nixon, G.M.; Davey, M.J.; Anderson, V.; Walker, A.M.; Trinder, J.A.; Horne, R.S. Determining sleep quality in children with sleep disordered breathing: EEG spectral analysis compared with conventional polysomnography. Sleep 2010, 33, 1165–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Beattie, L. How does sleep affect the perception of facial emotion? Sleep 2018, 41, zsy030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Chang, F.; Klumpp, H. Sleep quality and emotion recognition in individuals with and without internalizing psychopathologies. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2022, 75, 101719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Zsido, A.N.; Arato, N.; Ihasz, V.; Basler, J.; Budai, T.; Inhof, O.; Schacht, A.; Labadi, B.; Coelho, C.M. ‘Finding an emotional face’revisited: Differences in own-age bias and the happiness superiority effect in children and young adults. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Zhao, J.; Meng, Q.; An, L.; Wang, Y. An event-related potential comparison of facial expression processing between cartoon and real faces. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0198868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Song, J.; Liu, M.; Yao, S.; Yan, Y.; Ding, H.; Yan, T.; Zhao, L.; Xu, G. Classification of Emotional Expressions Is Affected by Inversion: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Calvo, M.G.; Nummenmaa, L. Detection of emotional faces: Salient physical features guide effective visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008, 137, 471–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Horne, R.S.C.; Ong, C.; Weichard, A.; Nixon, G.M.; Davey, M.J. Are there gender differences in the severity and consequences of sleep disordered in children? Sleep Med. 2020, 67, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Borges, J.G.; Ginani, G.E.; Hachul, H.; Cintra, F.D.; Tufik, S.; Pompeia, S. Executive functioning in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome patients without comorbidities: Focus on the fractionation of executive functions. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 1094–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Itier, R.J.; Taylor, M.J. Face recognition memory and configural processing: A developmental ERP study using upright, inverted, and contrast-reversed faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 487–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Alsubie, H.S.; BaHammam, A.S. Obstructive Sleep Apnoea: Children are not little Adults. Paediatr. Respir. Rev. 2017, 21, 72–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. De Almondes, K.M.; Junior, F.; Leonardo, M.E.M.; Alves, N.T. Facial Emotion Recognition and Executive Functions in Insomnia Disorder: An Exploratory Study. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hayes, G.S.; McLennan, S.N.; Henry, J.D.; Phillips, L.H.; Terrett, G.; Rendell, P.G.; Pelly, R.M.; Labuschagne, I. Task characteristics influence facial emotion recognition age-effects: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Aging 2020, 35, 295–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the experimental paradigm.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the experimental paradigm.
Brainsci 12 01688 g001
Figure 2. Reaction times of responses to different expression stimuli among groups. PS = primary snoring, MSOSA = moderate/severe OSA. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Reaction times of responses to different expression stimuli among groups. PS = primary snoring, MSOSA = moderate/severe OSA. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Brainsci 12 01688 g002
Table 1. Clinical and polysomnographic characteristics of participants.
Table 1. Clinical and polysomnographic characteristics of participants.
CharacteristicsControlPSMild OSAMSOSAp Value
n = 20n = 51n = 39n = 26
Gender (%)13M/7F34M/17F27M/12F17M/9F0.974
Age (years)8.5 ± 1.88.4 ± 1.88.3 ± 1.78.6 ± 1.90.964
BMI (kg/m2)17.5 ± 3.1 a17.5 ± 3.3 b17.8 ± 4.1 c20.8 ± 6.40.008
BMI z-score0.92 ± 1.950.69 ± 1.190.85 ± 1.521.43 ± 1.750.225
OAHI (events/hour)0.4 ± 0.3 a0.4 ± 0.2 b2.3 ± 1.0 c21.4 ± 36.1<0.001
TST436.7 ± 61.2430.9 ± 57.1419.7 ± 58.6 414.8 ± 63.20.508
ArI (events/hour)5.6 ± 1.6 a5.8 ± 1.9 b6.8 ± 2.4 c16.0 ± 13.4<0.001
SpO2 nadir (%)92.8 ± 1.8 d93.2 ± 2.7 e91.6 ± 2.8 f87.4 ± 8.9<0.001
Time with SpO2 < 90%0.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.01 ± 0.022.08 ± 9.130.129
SDQ
Emotional symptoms2.1 ± 2.02.4 ± 2.22.6 ± 2.42.8 ± 1.80.707
Conduct problems1.2 ± 1.01.5 ± 1.41.5 ± 1.31.4 ± 0.70.778
Hyperactivity2.8 ± 2.24.1 ± 2.54.0 ± 1.94.2 ± 1.90.106
Peer problems1.7 ± 1.22.4 ± 1.62.4 ± 1.72.4 ± 1.40.292
Prosocial behaviors8.4 ± 1.48.1 ± 2.07.9 ± 1.68.0 ± 1.60.796
Values are expressed as mean ± SD. PS = primary snoring; MSOSA = moderate/severe OSA; BMI = body mass index; OAHI = obstructive apnea-hypopnea index; TST = total sleep time; ArI = arousal index. Post hoc analysis revealed significant differences between groups. p < 0.05 was considered significant. a = Control < MSOSA, b = PS < MSOSA, c = Mild OSA < MSOSA, d = Control > MSOSA, e = PS >MSOSA, f = Mild OSA > MSOSA.
Table 2. Accuracy and reaction times of responses to different expression stimuli between the four groups (M ± SE).
Table 2. Accuracy and reaction times of responses to different expression stimuli between the four groups (M ± SE).
ControlPSMild OSAMSOSA
RTHappy (ms)505.23 ± 43.65533.30 ± 27.33554.59 ± 31.26544.70 ± 38.28
RTSad (ms)543.66 ± 47.52602.17 ± 29.76597.17 ± 34.03568.23 ± 41.68
ACCHappy (%)93.26 ± 1.1493.19 ± 0.7191.84 ± 0.8193.11 ± 1.00
ACCSad (%)93.13 ± 1.1093.19 ± 0.6591.95 ± 0.7593.00 ± 0.92
PS = primary snoring, MSOSA = moderate/severe OSA.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhao, F.-J.; Chen, Q.-W.; Wu, Y.; Xie, X.; Xu, Z.; Ni, X. Facial Emotion Recognition Deficit in Children with Moderate/Severe Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1688. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121688

AMA Style

Zhao F-J, Chen Q-W, Wu Y, Xie X, Xu Z, Ni X. Facial Emotion Recognition Deficit in Children with Moderate/Severe Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(12):1688. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121688

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhao, Fu-Jun, Qing-Wei Chen, Yunxiao Wu, Xiaohong Xie, Zhifei Xu, and Xin Ni. 2022. "Facial Emotion Recognition Deficit in Children with Moderate/Severe Obstructive Sleep Apnea" Brain Sciences 12, no. 12: 1688. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121688

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop