Next Article in Journal
Sleep in Normal Aging, Homeostatic and Circadian Regulation and Vulnerability to Sleep Deprivation
Next Article in Special Issue
Prognostic Factors in Neurorehabilitation of Stroke: A Comparison among Regression, Neural Network, and Cluster Analyses
Previous Article in Journal
Insomnia, Anxiety, and Depression Symptoms during the COVID-19 Pandemic May Depend on the Pre-Existent Health Status Rather than the Profession
Previous Article in Special Issue
Success of Hand Movement Imagination Depends on Personality Traits, Brain Asymmetry, and Degree of Handedness
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Efficacy of a Training on Executive Functions in Potentiating Rehabilitation Effects in Stroke Patients

1
Department of Psychology, Educational Science and Human Movement, University of Palermo, 90128 Palermo, Italy
2
IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, 30126 Venice, Italy
3
Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova, 35128 Padova, Italy
4
Department of Neuroscience & Padova Neuroscience Center, University of Padova, 35128 Padova, Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(8), 1002; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081002
Submission received: 13 May 2021 / Revised: 24 July 2021 / Accepted: 27 July 2021 / Published: 29 July 2021

Abstract

:
Cognitive impairment after a stroke has a direct impact on patients’ disability. In particular, impairment of Executive Functions (EFs) interferes with re-adaptation to daily life. The aim of this study was to explore whether adding a computer-based training on EFs to an ordinary rehabilitation program, regardless of the specific brain damage and clinical impairment (motor, language, or cognitive), could improve rehabilitation outcomes in patients with stroke. An EF training was designed to have minimal motor and expressive language demands and to be applied to a wide range of clinical conditions. A total of 37 stroke patients were randomly assigned to two groups: a training group, which performed the EF training in addition to the ordinary rehabilitation program (treatment as usual), and a control group, which performed the ordinary rehabilitation exclusively. Both groups were assessed before and after the rehabilitation program on neuropsychological tests covering multiple cognitive domains, and on functional scales (Barthel index, Functional Independence Measure). The results showed that only patients who received the training improved their scores on the Attentional Matrices and Phonemic Fluency tests after the rehabilitation program. Moreover, they showed a greater functional improvement in the Barthel scale as well. These results suggest that combining an EF training with an ordinary rehabilitation program potentiates beneficial effects of the latter, especially in promoting independence in activities of daily living.

1. Introduction

Stroke is a cerebrovascular accident, due to ischemia or hemorrhage, which causes loss of brain function. This disorder is characterized by a fast and focal development of neurologic symptoms along with motor, language, and other cognitive impairments. According to the World Health Organization, stroke is the second leading cause of death and the third leading cause of disability [1]. More than two-thirds of stroke survivors have limitations to live independently or have catastrophic impact in their daily life. Cognitive dysfunctions affect more than one third of these patients, may persist for years after stroke, and strongly contribute to disability. Prospective studies have shown that the cognitive status is a key factor in post-stroke recovery [2,3]. Patients with cognitive deficits at three months are 2.4 times more likely to have dependent living, irrespective of age and severity of physical impairment [4]. Given these prognostic implications, an adequate and timely cognitive assessment, together with an appropriate rehabilitative intervention, are of paramount importance.
Impairment of executive functions (EFs) represents one of the most common cognitive sequelae of stroke. Depending on the definition and instruments used for its assessment, the prevalence of executive dysfunction after stroke ranges from 19% to 75% [5]. In spite of this, some of them are ‘invisible’ to the patient’s awareness. Namely, while motor, language, and memory impairments are more apparent, deficits involving some EFs (such as working memory, sustained and shifting attention) are more insidious or manifest when patients fully return to the complex demands of daily life [6]. Therefore, even the treatment plans risk neglecting them.
EFs refer to high-level cognitive operations that direct lower-level processes toward goal-oriented actions [7]. This class of cognitive functions encompasses multiple abilities, including planning, problem solving, criterion setting, multitasking, working memory, cognitive control, inhibition, switching, and monitoring. All these functions are strongly interrelated but also separable and independent [8,9]. The complexity of EFs makes them very sensitive to brain changes resulting from stroke [10]. EF impairment is expected to generate cascade effects on other cognitive functions and to reduce the capacity to regain independence in activities of daily living [11]. Furthermore, it affects the ability to adapt to new or problematic situations, such as when alternative movement strategies are necessary to compensate for limb weakness or when planning in advance a sentence is required for rehabilitating verbal communication. Therefore, executive dysfunctions greatly affect the quality of a patient’s life and self-sufficiency, resulting in everyday difficulties across diverse neurological conditions [12,13,14].
Studies that investigated the role of executive functioning in recovery after stroke have found that this is a strong predictor for recovery of motor functions as well as dependence in daily living after discharge [15,16,17]. Previous systematic reviews on EF rehabilitation in stroke [5,18,19] concluded that high-quality evidence about the effect of EF rehabilitation is still limited but encouraging. In particular, computer-based rehabilitation programs, which are being increasingly used in clinical practice, are promising, although their effect on the improvement of cognitive functions after stroke and, more importantly, in activities of daily living, still needs further evidence [19]. Of note, some studies found benefits of using a combined approach to cognitive rehabilitation in brain injury, that is, to use computer-based rehabilitation programs in addition to conventional programs [20].
The goal of the present study was to evaluate the efficacy of a computerized training program aimed at potentiating EFs in patients with stroke. The peculiarity of the program is that it was aimed to be combined with ordinary rehabilitation programs (on speech, language, motor functions, etc.) and that its tasks demand minimal selective attention, expressive language, and motor resources. As such, it could be administered to a wide range of patients, regardless of the specific brain damage and deficits, even to those patients with severe impairments but preserved basic motor (movement of one hand) and language (comprehension of simple sentences) functions. The underlying hypothesis was that if EFs regulate more basic functions, from motor to more cognitive ones, then, strengthening EFs would support the recovery of these functions. Therefore, the combination of this EF training program with an ordinary rehabilitation, which could involve conventional as well as computerized interventions, both tailored to the patient (such as cognitive rehabilitation on specific attention, memory, and EFs), would improve final rehabilitation outcomes, especially daily life functions.
To this end, a training group and a well-matched control group were tested at two time points, before (T0) and after (T1) rehabilitation. The training group performed a computerized training to potentiate EF, in addition to the standard rehabilitation program (i.e., treatment as usual). The training was designed based on a brain-based EF model inspired by the ROBBIA model [9,21,22,23], and was adaptive. The control group underwent the standard rehabilitation program only. To evaluate the efficacy of the training program on the final rehabilitation outcomes, patients’ performance on a battery of standardized tests covering multiple cognitive domains, and scores on functional scales were measured at T0 and T1. We hypothesized a greater improvement in cognitive functions and in independence in daily life in the group that underwent the EF training compared to the control group.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Procedure

The study was conducted at the IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, Venice (Italy), a specialized rehabilitation hospital. The protocol was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki and received formal approval by the local Ethical Committee. All patients signed an informed consent prior to their participation. Exclusion criteria were: age below 18 years, premorbid ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke, comorbid neurological (e.g., epilepsy) or neurodegenerative (e.g., multiple sclerosis, dementia) disorder, premorbid or comorbid brain damage other than stroke (e.g., traumatic brain injury, tumor), psychiatric disorders, unstable medical conditions, sensory impairments that prevent processing and recognition of visual stimuli (e.g., agnosia or color blindness), dyslexia/alexia, severe vigilance or verbal comprehension deficits, impaired motor control of both hands. Patients were enrolled in the study during their hospitalization and were randomly assigned to one of the two groups. A single-blind randomized controlled design was adopted and a stratified randomization procedure guaranteed a balance of the two groups in terms of age, sex, education, time from event, and stroke severity, as assessed by the neurological examination. All participants received an inpatient rehabilitation program, based on their needs, in accordance with the routines at the clinic. The rehabilitation program could include: neuromotor rehabilitation (e.g., robotic gait training, virtual reality, and cycling), speech rehabilitation (e.g., conversational therapy, reading, and writing), occupational therapy (daily living activities, group activities, and garden therapy activities that engage patient in all stages of plant cultivation and care), and optokinetic stimulation, but also neuropsychological rehabilitation on specific cognitive abilities, such as visuo-spatial exploration, sustained attention, working memory, problem solving, planning, and performance monitoring. The typical rehabilitation program for stroke patients usually lasts from two to three months.
In addition to the rehabilitation program, the training group received the EF training as well. The training lasted 10 sessions, about one hour each, distributed over a mean of 15.7 days ± 2.3 SD. Both groups were assessed with a battery of neuropsychological tests at T0 (before training and rehabilitation or rehabilitation only), and at T1 (after training and rehabilitation or rehabilitation only). The time interval between T0 to T1 lasted about 40 days (45 days ± 19.5 for the training group, 40 days ± 16.3 for the control group, t(35) = 1.14, p = 0.439). Furthermore, measures on independency in activities of daily living were assessed at hospital admission and at hospital discharge (3.5 months ± 1.3 for the training group, 4.2 months ± 2 for the control group, t(35) = 1.22, p = 0.229), by means of functional scales.

2.2. Participants

Patients were consecutively selected according to their hospital admission, scrutinized according to the exclusion criteria, and then approached to ask for participation. A total of 43 patients, admitted from January 2018 through August 2020, were enrolled. Three patients were lost to follow-up assessment (T1), whereas three patients discontinued rehabilitation (see Figure S1 for a detailed patient flow). The final sample included 18 patients in the training group and 19 patients in the control group. Patients’ characteristics, namely age, sex, education, and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) standardized score [24]; TIB standardized score (Italian version of the National Adult Reading Test [25]), etiology (ischemic or hemorrhagic), time since event, symptoms reported at the hospital admission, and the rehabilitation programs completed during hospitalization are summarized in Table 1. Two participants were left-handed (one in the training group, one in the control group), and one was ambidextrous (in the training group). The a priori power calculation had estimated a sample of 16 patients per group in order to detect a small effect of training (partial η2 = 0.04), with a statistical power (1 − β) of 0.80, a significance level (α) of 0.05, and a repeated-measures correlation of 0.7 (G*Power 3 software [26]).
No significant differences at baseline were observed between groups with regard to age (t(35) = 0.068, p = 0.946), sex (χ2 = 0.218, p = 0.728), education (t(35) = 0.094, p = 0.925), MMSE (t(31) = 0.512, p = 0.613), TIB (t(31) = 1.342, p = 0.189), time since event (t(35) = 1.14, p = 0.259), etiology (χ2 = 0.013, p = 0.999), lesion side (χ2 = 0.09, p = 0.687), symptomatology (χ2 = 1.55, p = 0.671), and type of rehabilitation (χ2 = 0.992, p = 0.803).
Structural information of brain lesions was obtained from regular MRI (T1-weighted, T2-weighted and/or FLAIR scans) or computed tomography (CT). Figure 1 describes the overlap of patients’ lesions (two patients were excluded from this lesion overlap image due to poor data quality).

2.3. Training Program

The rationale of the program is partially founded on studies conducted in our laboratory and previously in the Don Stuss’ lab, which elaborated a brain-centered model of EFs based on two distinct domain-general EFs, namely, criterion setting and monitoring [9,21,22,23]. In line with this model, the program included four types of training tasks, targeting Working Memory (WM), Interference Control and Inhibition (ICI), Task-Switching (TS), and Monitoring (M). In each task, stimuli consisted of “cards”, displayed one at a time in the center of a laptop screen. The cards could contain one of the following stimuli: geometric symbols, words, faces, and objects. Since the training was aimed at potentiating domain-general processes, regardless of the specific materials, the type of stimuli varied within and across sessions. The order of the sessions/stimuli was counterbalanced across patients. The tasks were presented in a fixed order (WM, ICI, TS, and M), following a hierarchical logic. This logic relies on a hierarchical integrative model [29], based on developmental evidence, according to which working memory is the component that develops first, followed by inhibitory control and, finally, cognitive flexibility, which is built on both of them. The duration of the stimulus presentation was adapted to the single patient’s performance. Each task lasted approximately 10 min. All tasks were designed to involve minimal load on selective attention, expressive language, and motor abilities. Namely, the cards were displayed one at a time on the screen, and in all tasks the response required only a button press of the spacebar, with the index finger of the dominant hand (or the index finger of the non-hemiplegic hand), without selecting among alternative keys. Patients were unaware of all task manipulations. Stimulus presentation and data recording were controlled by the E-Prime 2 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh). Each patient was tested individually in a quiet and dedicated hospital room, with the continuous presence of a junior neuropsychologist (R.T. or E.R.), who set up the laptop and assisted the patient with the instructions.

2.3.1. Stimuli

Geometric symbols were adapted from a popular US game (www.setgame.com, accessed on 28 July 2021) and could differ in shape (ovals, diamonds, or flags), color (red, blue, or green), or number (from one to three). Faces could be female or male, Caucasian or not, neutral or emotional (adapted from [30]). Objects could be kitchen or garage tools, unimanual or bimanual, and made with metal or not (adapted from [31]). Words could refer to animals, fruits, or vegetables and begin with a specific letter. Examples of stimuli and trials are reported in Supplementary Materials.

2.3.2. Working Memory (WM) Task

The WM task was based on the 1-back paradigm [32]. Patients had to respond by pressing the spacebar whenever the card was identical to the one presented in the immediately preceding trial. A simple response time procedure was chosen, instead of the typical two-choice procedure, in order to unload the selection process.
The task comprised five blocks of 20 trials each (seven targets). The first two blocks were practice blocks and served to make patients familiarize with the task. During these blocks, feedback on each trial was provided to ensure instruction understanding. In the first block, the cards were displayed for 1500 ms and followed by a blank screen for 3000 ms (Inter-Trial-Interval, ITI). If the patient reached an accuracy >65%, then the stimulus duration and ITI were increased on the successive blocks to 1000 and 2500 ms, respectively. If the accuracy level was <65%, then the practice block was repeated until an accuracy level >65% was reached. If the patient obtained an accuracy level >85% in three consecutive sessions, a duration manipulation was applied, namely, the ITI duration was lengthened to load the process of stimuli retention in memory (to 3500, 4000, or 4500 ms).

2.3.3. Interference Control and Inhibition (ICI) Task

This task was designed to train both “cognitive” and “motor” inhibitory processes. Again, a series of cards was presented, one at a time, centered on the screen. Participants were required to respond whenever a card belonging to a specific category appeared (targets) and not to respond to the other ones. For example, in the case of geometric symbols, patients were instructed to respond to all red diamonds. In this case, the targets were cards containing one, two, or three red diamonds only. Cards with red ovals, red flags, green diamonds, and blue diamonds, represented distractor stimuli, since they share some but not all features with the target (i.e., color and shape, respectively). All other cards represented simple no-go stimuli. Alternatively, in the case of faces, participants could be required to respond to happy female faces. In this case, the targets were happy Caucasian and non-Caucasian female faces. Happy male faces and neutral female faces represented distractor stimuli, whereas neutral and sad male faces represented no-go stimuli. Withholding response to distractor trials required interference control, whereas withholding response to non-go trials required more general inhibition.
The ICI task comprised four blocks. The first two blocks were for practice and contained 18 trials each. According to performance levels (< or >65%) on practice blocks, the stimulus duration was set to 1500 or 1000 ms, and the ITI to 3000 or 2000 ms. The two successive task blocks had two different target types and contained 64 trials each (16 targets).

2.3.4. Task-Switching (TS)

Patients were asked to perform the same task as the ICI, namely to detect a target card. Yet, unlike the ICI task, shorter series of trials (9, 12, or 15) were presented. At the beginning of each series, the target card was displayed, which was one of the two target types used in the ICI task. For each series, the target card could be the same as the preceding series (“repeat series”) or could change (“switch series”). Compared to repeat series, in switch series participants were required to disengage from the previous/alternative target. Therefore, commission errors (i.e., responses to cards containing the alternative target) would reflect a failure of this process. The target cards varied across sessions.

2.3.5. Monitoring Task (M)

As in the TS task, short series of cards (9, 12, or 15) were presented one at a time, for 1500 ms, in the center of the screen. At the beginning of each series, a target card was shown. Patients had to detect as soon as possible this card among the series, which appeared towards the end (at the 8th, 9th, or 14th trial). In “predictable” series, the presented card followed a certain regularity (such as increasing number of symbols), which allowed the anticipation of the target occurrence. In “unpredictable” series, the cards presented in the series were randomly chosen. The M task trained the ability to monitor events in order to, implicitly, check rules (e.g., [33]). Two practice series were presented at the beginning, to familiarize with the instructions. Then, six predictable series and six unpredictable ones were administered. The patient was not aware of the predictability manipulation.

2.4. Outcome Measures

Two types of outcome measures were analyzed to quantify the effect of training: performance on neuropsychological tests and scores on functional scales. The neuropsychological test battery assessed short-term and working memory (Digit span forward and backward, Corsi block-tapping test [34]); attention and processing speed (Attentional matrices [35], Trail Making Test–A [36]); language (Naming test [37], Phonemic and Semantic fluency [38]); and executive functions (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, WCST [39,40], Five Point test [41,42], and Stroop test [43]). Some composite scores were considered: the error index in the Five Point test, which measures the percentage of perseverative or rule-breaking errors over the total number of designs [41], and the Stroop Inverse Efficiency Score (IES), which is derived by dividing mean Stroop interference response time by its corresponding accuracy. Additionally, the patients were tested with the Barthel Index [44] and the Functional Independence Measure (FIMTM [45,46]). These scales evaluate the level of disability in everyday contexts, namely, the dependency on a caregiver. The Barthel index allows the assessment of changes in the basic daily activities (such as self-care and locomotion) and consists of 10 items, scored on a five-point Likert scale, with total score ranging from 0 (totally dependent) to 100 (totally independent). The FIM assesses physical and cognitive changes in daily contexts (such as self-care and communication abilities) and consists of 18 items, scored on a seven-point Likert scale, with total score ranging from 18 (totally dependent) to 126 (totally independent). Disability severity did not differ between the two groups at T0, as indexed by the Barthel (training group: range 0–70, control group: range 0–65; t(35) = 0.156, p = 0.877) and the FIM score (training group: range 23–82, control group: range 24–83; t(35) = 0.533, p = 0.597) score.

2.5. Data Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 22.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics 2014). All dependent measures, namely, all tests and scales’ scores, were continuous variables. According to the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, some neuropsychological measures were normally distributed (TMT-A, Attentional matrices, Phonemic fluency, Semantic fluency, Five Point error index, and Barthel index), whereas some other measures were not (i.e., Digit span forward, Digit span backward, Corsi block-tapping test, Naming, WCST categories, WCST errors, and Stroop IES, FIM). In order to examine significant difference in scores between T0 and T1, paired t-tests, or Wilcoxon signed-rank test for the not normally distributed variables, were computed, separately for each group. To test group differences in Pre vs. Post changes, an ANCOVA model was applied to normally distributed variables, which included the score at T1 as a dependent variable, and the score at T0 as covariate (Post ~ Pre + Group + Pre × Group). This is considered the best analytical approach for examining data from two-group pre-post designs [47,48]. If, on the one hand, it adjusts for pre-treatment measures, it also takes into account that baseline individual scores affect cognitive training effects, namely, a negative relationship between a participant’s initial cognitive ability and the results of training [49,50]. For testing not normally distributed measures, the ANCOVA was conducted on rank-transformed scores [51,52]. The effect sizes for pre-test –post-test control group designs were estimated, based on the mean pre–post change in the treatment group minus the mean pre–post change in the control group, divided by the pooled pre-test standard deviation (dppc2 [53]). p-values < 0.05 for a two-sided test were considered statistically significant.

3. Results

Table 2 contains the mean scores (and standard deviations) for each test and group, at T0 and T1. The paired t-tests showed significant improvement at T1 relative to T0 for the Attentional matrices (t(17) = 4.23, p = 0.001) and for the Phonemic fluency (t(13) = 2.64, p = 0.020) scores, in the training group only. A tendency toward significance was also found in the Digit Span forward score (Z = 1.96, p = 0.05). Interestingly, a significantly higher proportion of patients in the training group shifted their performance to the normal range in the Attentional matrices test at T1 relative to T0 (McNemar test = 4.2, p = 0.031).
Scores on the functional scales revealed significant improvements from the hospital admission to the discharge for both groups (Barthel index: training group t(17) = 8.83, p < 0.001; control group t(18) = 9.93, p < 0.001; and FIM: training group t(17) = 9.8, p < 001, control group t(18) = 8, p < 0.001). All patients, except one patient in the training group and one patient in the control group, underwent an improvement in the Barthel index above the minimal detectable change (MDC) expected in test–retest assessment [54].
In Table 2, the group effect size on the post–pre comparison was reported. This pre-test–post-test control group size effect was larger than 0.4 in the Digit span, Phonemic fluency, errors on the WCST, Stroop interference (IES), and, more importantly, in the Barthel index. In all these measures, the ordinary rehabilitation combined with the training yielded a larger effect than the ordinary rehabilitation only. Namely, performance at T1 relative to T0 improved more in the training group relative to the control group in all tests. Only the Naming accuracy showed a larger positive change in the control group.
The ANCOVA model revealed that, when controlling for T0 scores, a significant Group effect emerged on the post-rehabilitation scores on the Barthel scale (F(1,33) = 4.2, p = 0.049, and partial η2 = 0.113). Namely, participants of the training group obtained higher scores at T1 compared to the control group. In order to explore the association between pre-rehabilitation Barthel scores and post–pre difference (gain) in Barthel scores, an ANCOVA with gain as dependent variable, pre-rehabilitation Barthel score as covariate, and Group as independent variable was performed. The results (see Figure 2) revealed a negative correlation between gain and pre-rehabilitation Barthel score (main effect of pre-rehabilitation Barthel score: F(1,33) = 21.32, p < 0.001, and partial η2 = 0.392), and confirmed an overall higher difference in the training group (F(1,33) = 4.2, p = 0.049, and partial η2 = 0.113). The interaction was not significant (F(1,33) = 2.12, p = 0.15), meaning that in both group pre-rehabilitation Barthel score and gain were correlated. As evident in Figure 2, patients with lower Barthel scores at T0 gained larger benefits than patients with higher Barthel scores at T0.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate whether a computerized EF training could boost the effects of stroke rehabilitation when added to an ordinary rehabilitation program, regardless the specific brain damage and cognitive/motor impairments. To this aim, two groups of patients with stroke were involved in the study, a group that received the training in addition to the routine rehabilitation, and a group that received the routine rehabilitation exclusively. A neuropsychological assessment before (T0) and after (T1) the rehabilitation was administered to both groups (it is noteworthy that the same amount of time from T0 to T1 passed for the two groups). Furthermore, the self-sufficiency in the daily activities was assessed by means of two functional scales.
The findings showed that only the patients’ group that received the training improved performance on the Attentional matrices and on the Phonemic fluency test, at T1 relative to T0. The effect sizes computed for pre-test–post-test control group designs revealed a moderate effect of training in Digit span, Phonemic fluency, errors on the WCST, and Stroop interference. Both groups significantly improved in their independency in activities of daily living after rehabilitation, as measured by the Barthel and FIM scales. Remarkably, the training group obtained significantly larger improvement on the Barthel scale compared to the control group. This result suggests that the training boosted the effects of routine rehabilitation in the level of independence in activities of daily living. Although the FIM scale was expected to be more sensitive than the Barthel scale, in fact our results are consistent with previous studies that compared the two scales and found that the Barthel scale has good responsiveness in detecting changes after rehabilitation in patients with stroke [55].
The results on neuropsychological outcomes revealed that the training could improve functions not directly targeted by the training, such as visual selective attention (Attentional matrices) and verbal fluency (Phonemic fluency). This transfer effect to “near” cognitive functions is in line with the study hypotheses, and confirms previous evidence on the near transfer effects of EF trainings in healthy individuals [56] as well as in mild cognitive impairment [57] and brain-injured patients [58]. Moreover, the effects of the training on abilities not directly targeted by the training, such as on activities of daily living, provides support to the idea that the training of EFs might support the improvement of other “far” abilities (e.g., [59]).
As in this study, a previous randomized controlled trial has examined the effect of adding a computer-based training of working memory to routine rehabilitation programs in patients with working memory deficit following brain damage (e.g., stroke or trauma), and has shown greater improvements in the training group compared to the control one, not only in cognitive tests but also in Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores [60]. Moreover, previous evidence documented the effectiveness of non-computer-based EF trainings (e.g., the Goal Management Training) in improving daily life activities when applied to patients with executive dysfunctions after various acquired brain injuries [61,62]. Unlike these investigations, the present work did not limit the training to patients with EF impairments and considered post-stroke patients only. We identified very few similar studies in the literature (summarized in Table S1), which combined a computerized attention and EF training with conventional rehabilitation in stroke patients, not specifically impaired on EF. Only two of them, out of five, found larger improvements in the trained compared to the control group on non-cognitive aspects, that is, higher patients’ satisfaction with the results of treatment [63] and lower anxiety and depression symptoms [64]. None of them, however, could observe greater improvement in daily living activities.
The improvements observed in this study might be attributed to some methodological strengths and novelties of the training. Namely, (i) it relied on minimal motor, language and attention requirements, therefore, all patients, even with multiple and/or severe impairments, could benefit from it; (ii) it was inspired by a brain-based model [9,21]; and (iii) it followed a hierarchical structure. Given the close correlation between EF changes and executive control fronto–parietal brain networks [65,66], we might speculate that the training had acted by reinforcing these networks (see [67]), although further replication with neuroimaging extension is desirable to confirm this hypothesis.
In order to test the robustness of the present findings and the maintenance of its effect over time, there is a need for further investigations adopting larger samples, a quantitative severity index, a more symmetric distribution of lesion’s side across patients, and a long-term follow-up. Furthermore, in order to maximize the likelihood of meaningful and stable gain and to obtain a larger effect size, the effect of additional training sessions should be assessed [68].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, although these results are preliminary given the relatively low sample sizes, they suggest that adaptive computerized trainings on EFs added to conventional rehabilitation treatment, regardless of the specific stroke lesions and impairments, might be effective in potentiating/promoting recovery not only of related cognitive processes but also of broader functional abilities. We summarize the main contribution of this work in the finding that boosting EFs in an adaptive and theory-grounded manner might optimize functional recovery in post-stroke patients, even in cases where EF disorders are not specifically present. This training approach could potentially be applied to a wide range of clinical conditions, although the patients’ characteristics that would benefit the most from it should be investigated in future studies.

Supplementary Materials

The following resources are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci11081002/s1, Figure S1: Consort flow diagram; Figure S2: Working Memory (WM) task, Figure S3: Interference Control and Inhibition (ICI) task, Figure S4: Task-Switching (TS), and Figure S5: Monitoring task (M); Table S1.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: V.T., A.V., and F.B.; methodology: V.T., and A.V.; software: V.T., and L.W.; validation: V.T., F.B., E.R., R.T., and A.V.; formal analysis: V.T.; investigation: R.T., E.R., and L.W.; resources: A.V., F.B., and F.M.; data curation: R.T., E.R., V.T., and F.B.; writing original draft preparation: V.T., and A.V.; writing review and editing: V.T., F.B., R.T., E.R., F.M., L.W., and A.V.; visualization: V.T., and L.W.; supervision: A.V., and F.M.; project administration: A.V., F.B., and F.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially funded by the FP7 European Research Council Starting Grant LEX-MEA GA n. 313692 to A.V., and by the Ricerca Finalizzata 2018—Young Researchers Grant of the Italian Ministry of Health to F.B. and A.V. (GR-2018-12367927).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of San Camillo Hospital IRCSS, Venice, Italy (protocol code 2017.08-FEP, date of approval 13/07/2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Johnson, C.O.; Nguyen, M.; Roth, G.A.; Nichols, E.; Alam, T.; Abate, D.; Abd-Allah, F.; Abdelalim, A.; Abraha, H.N.; Abu-Rmeileh, N.M.; et al. Global, regional, and national burden of stroke, 1990–2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Winstein, C.J.; Stein, J.; Arena, R.; Bates, B.; Cherney, L.R.; Cramer, S.C.; Deruyter, F.; Eng, J.J.; Fisher, B.; Harvey, R.L.; et al. Guidelines for adult stroke rehabilitation and recovery. Stroke 2016, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Nys, G.M.S.; Van Zandvoort, M.J.E.; De Kort, P.L.M.; Van Der Worp, H.B.; Jansen, B.P.W.; Algra, A.; De Haan, E.H.F.; Kappelle, L.J. The prognostic value of domain-specific cognitive abilities in acute first-ever stroke. Neurology 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Tatemichi, T.K.; Desmond, D.W.; Stern, Y.; Paik, M.; Sano, M.; Bagiella, E. Cognitive impairment after stroke: Frequency, patterns, and relationship to functional abilities. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1994, 57, 202–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Poulin, V.; Korner-Bitensky, N.; Dawson, D.R.; Bherer, L. Efficacy of executive function interventions after stroke: A systematic review. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 2012, 19, 158–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Boyd, L.A.; Hayward, K.S.; Ward, N.S.; Stinear, C.M.; Rosso, C.; Fisher, R.J.; Carter, A.R.; Leff, A.P.; Copland, D.A.; Carey, L.M.; et al. Biomarkers of stroke recovery: Consensus-based core recommendations from the stroke recovery and rehabilitation roundtable*. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Stuss, D.T.; Alexander, M.P.; Shallice, T.; Picton, T.W.; Binns, M.A.; Macdonald, R.; Borowiec, A.; Katz, D.I. Multiple frontal systems controlling response speed. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 396–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 2017, 86, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Ambrosini, E.; Arbula, S.; Rossato, C.; Pacella, V.; Vallesi, A. Neuro-cognitive architecture of executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Cortex 2019, 119, 441–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Levine, B.; Schweizer, T.A.; O’Connor, C.; Turner, G.; Gillingham, S.; Stuss, D.T.; Manly, T.; Robertson, I.H. Rehabilitation of executive functioning in patients with frontal lobe brain damage with goal management training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Stablum, F.; Umiltà, C.; Mogentale, C.; Carlan, M.; Guerrini, C. Rehabilitation of executive deficits in closed head injury an anterior communicating artery aneurysm patients. Psychol. Res. 2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Marshall, G.A.; Rentz, D.M.; Frey, M.T.; Locascio, J.J.; Johnson, K.A.; Sperling, R.A. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011, 7, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Royall, D.R.; Palmer, R.; Chiodo, L.K.; Polk, M.J. Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: The Freedom House Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. McDowd, J.M.; Filion, D.L.; Pohl, P.S.; Richards, L.G.; Stiers, W. Attentional abilities and functional outcomes following stroke. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2003, 58, P45–P53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Robertson, I.H.; Ridgeway, V.; Greenfield, E.; Parr, A. Motor recovery after stroke depends on intact sustained attention: A 2-year follow-up study. Neuropsychology 1997, 11, 290–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Hanks, R.A.; Rapport, L.J.; Millis, S.R.; Deshpande, S.A. Measures of executive functioning as predictors of functional ability and social integration in a rehabilitation sample. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Watson, P.A.; Gignac, G.E.; Weinborn, M.; Green, S.; Pestell, C. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological predictors of outcome following stroke and other non-traumatic acquired brain injuries in adults. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020, 30, 194–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chung, C.S.Y.; Pollock, A.; Campbell, T.; Durward, B.R.; Hagen, S. Cognitive rehabilitation for executive dysfunction in adults with stroke or other adult non-progressive acquired brain damage. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2013, 4, CD008391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. van de Ven, R.M.; Murre, J.M.J.; Veltman, D.J.; Schmand, B.A. Computer-based cognitive training for executive functions after stroke: A systematic review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Cicerone, K.D.; Langenbahn, D.M.; Braden, C.; Malec, J.F.; Kalmar, K.; Fraas, M.; Felicetti, T.; Laatsch, L.; Harley, J.P.; Bergquist, T.; et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2011, 92, 519–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Vallesi, A. The quest for hemispheric asymmetries supporting and predicting executive functioning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Vallesi, A. Organisation of executive functions: Hemispheric asymmetries. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2012, 24, 367–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Stuss, D.T.; Alexander, M.P. Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2007, 362, 901–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Magni, E.; Binetti, G.; Bianchetti, A.; Rozzini, R.; Trabucchi, M. Mini-mental state examination: A normative study in Italian elderly population. Eur. J. Neurol. 1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Sartori, G.; Colombo, L.; Vallar, G.; Rusconi, M.L.; Pinarello, A. TIB: Test di Intelligenza Breve per la valutazione del quoziente intellettivo attuale e premorboso. Prof. Psicol. 1997, 1, 2–24. [Google Scholar]
  26. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rorden, C.; Bonilha, L.; Fridriksson, J.; Bender, B.; Karnath, H.-O. Age-specific CT and MRI templates for spatial normalization. Neuroimage 2012, 61, 957–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Nachev, P.; Coulthard, E.; Jäger, H.R.; Kennard, C.; Husain, M. Enantiomorphic normalization of focally lesioned brains. Neuroimage 2008, 39, 1215–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Garon, N.; Bryson, S.E.; Smith, I.M. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Tottenham, N.; Tanaka, J.W.; Leon, A.C.; McCarry, T.; Nurse, M.; Hare, T.A.; Marcus, D.J.; Westerlund, A.; Casey, B.; Nelson, C. The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 168, 242–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Tarantino, V.; Mazzonetto, I.; Formica, S.; Causin, F.; Vallesi, A. The neural bases of event monitoring across domains: A simultaneous ERP-fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Smith, E.E.; Jonides, J. Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cogn. Psychol. 1997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Vallesi, A.; Crescentini, C. Right fronto-parietal involvement in monitoring spatial trajectories. Neuroimage 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Monaco, M.; Costa, A.; Caltagirone, C.; Carlesimo, G.A. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: Standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 34, 749–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Della Sala, S.; Laiacona, M.; Spinnler, H.; Ubezio, C. A cancellation test: Its reliability in assessing attentional deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Med. 1992, 22, 885–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Giovagnoli, A.R.; Del Pesce, M.; Mascheroni, S.; Simoncelli, M.; Laiacona, M.; Capitani, E. Trail making test: Normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 1996, 17, 305–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Sartori, G.; Job, R. The oyster with four legs: A neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Novelli, G.; Papagno, C.; Capitani, E.; Laiacona, M.; Vallar, G.; Cappa, S.F. Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su soggetti normali. Arch. Psicol. Neurol. Psichiatr. 1986, 47, 477–506. [Google Scholar]
  39. Caffarra, P.; Vezzadini, G.; Dieci, F.; Zonato, F.; Venneri, A. Modified card sorting test: Normative data. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2004, 26, 246–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kopp, B.; Lange, F.; Steinke, A. The reliability of the Wisconsin card sorting test in clinical practice. Assessment 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cattelani, R.; Dal Sasso, F.; Corsini, D.; Posteraro, L. The Modified Five-Point Test: Normative data for a sample of Italian healthy adults aged 16–60. Neurol. Sci. 2011, 32, 595–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tucha, L.; Aschenbrenner, S.; Koerts, J.; Lange, K.W. The Five-Point Test: Reliability, validity and normative data for children and adults. PLoS ONE 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Caffarra, P.; Vezzadini, G.; Dieci, F.; Zonato, F.; Venneri, A. Una versione abbreviata del test di Stroop: Dati normativi nella popolazione Italiana. Nuova Riv. Neurol. 2002, 12, 111–115. [Google Scholar]
  44. Galeoto, G.; Lauta, A.; Palumbo, A.; Castiglia, S.F.; Mollica, R.; Santilli, V.; Sacchetti, M.L. The Barthel Index: Italian translation, adaptation and validation. Int. J. Neurol. Neurother. 2015, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tesio, L.; Granger, C.V.; Perucca, L.; Franchignoni, F.P.; Battaglia, M.A.; Russell, C.F. The FIMTM instrument in the United States and Italy: A comparative study. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Slade, A.; Penta, M.; Tripolski, M.; Biering-Sørensen, F.; Carter, J.; Marincek, C.; Phillips, S.; Simone, A.; Tennant, A.; Lundgren-Nilsson, Å.; et al. Cross-cultural validity of functional independence measure items in stroke: A study using Rasch analysis. J. Rehabil. Med. 2005, 37, 23–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Nunes, E.V.; Pavlicova, M.; Hu, M.C.; Campbell, A.N.; Miele, G.; Hien, D.; Klein, D.F. Baseline matters: The importance of covariation for baseline severity in the analysis of clinical trials. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Zhang, S.; Paul, J.; Nantha-Aree, M.; Buckley, N.; Shahzad, U.; Cheng, J.; DeBeer, J.; Winemaker, M.; Wismer, D.; Punthakee, D.; et al. Empirical comparison of four baseline covariate adjustment methods in analysis of continuous outcomes in randomized controlled trials. Clin. Epidemiol. 2014, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Lövdén, M.; Brehmer, Y.; Li, S.C.; Lindenberger, U. Training-induced compensation versus magnification of individual differences in memory performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Clifton, L.; Clifton, D.A. The correlation between baseline score and post-intervention score, and its implications for statistical analysis. Trials 2019, 20, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Vickers, A.J. Parametric versus non-parametric statistics in the analysis of randomized trials with non-normally distributed data. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Feys, J. New nonparametric rank tests for interactions in factorial designs with repeated measures. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Methods 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Morris, S.B. Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organ. Res. Methods 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yang, C.M.; Wang, Y.-C.; Lee, C.-H.; Chen, M.-H.; Hsieh, C.-L. A comparison of test–retest reliability and random measurement error of the Barthel Index and modified Barthel Index in patients with chronic stroke. Disabil. Rehabil. 2020, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Van Der Putten, J.J.M.F.; Hobart, J.C.; Freeman, J.A.; Thompson, A.J. Measuring change in disability after inpatient rehabilitation: Comparison of the responsiveness of the Barthel Index and the Functional Independence Measure. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Karbach, J.; Kray, J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Burgio, F.; Delazer, M.; Meneghello, F.; Pertl, M.-T.; Semenza, C.; Zamarian, L. Cognitive training improves ratio processing and decision making in patients with mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimer’s. Dis. 2018, 64, 1213–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Weicker, J.; Villringer, A.; Thöne-Otto, A. Can impaired working memory functioning be improved by training? A meta-analysis with a special focus on brain injured patients. Neuropsychology 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Nikravesh, M.; Aghajanzadeh, M.; Maroufizadeh, S.; Saffarian, A.; Jafari, Z. Working memory training in post-stroke aphasia: Near and far transfer effects. J. Commun. Disord. 2021, 89, 106077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Åkerlund, E.; Esbjörnsson, E.; Sunnerhagen, K.S.; Björkdahl, A. Can computerized working memory training improve impaired working memory, cognition and psychological health? Brain Inj. 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Stamenova, V.; Levine, B. Effectiveness of goal management training® in improving executive functions: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Tornås, S.; Løvstad, M.; Solbakk, A.-K.; Evans, J.; Endestad, T.; Hol, P.K.; Schanke, A.-K.; Stubberud, J. Rehabilitation of executive functions in patients with chronic acquired brain injury with goal management training, external cuing, and emotional regulation: A randomized controlled trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2016, 22, 436–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Prokopenko, S.V.; Mozheyko, E.Y.; Petrova, M.M.; Koryagina, T.D.; Kaskaeva, D.S.; Chernykh, T.V.; Shvetzova, I.N.; Bezdenezhnih, A.F. Correction of post-stroke cognitive impairments using computer programs. J. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 325, 148–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. De Luca, R.; Leonardi, S.; Spadaro, L.; Russo, M.; Aragona, B.; Torrisi, M.; Maggio, M.G.; Bramanti, A.; Naro, A.; De Cola, M.C.; et al. Improving cognitive function in patients with stroke: Can computerized training be the future? J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2018, 27, 1055–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lin, Z.; Tao, J.; Gao, Y.; Yin, D.; Chen, A.; Chen, L. Analysis of central mechanism of cognitive training on cognitive impairment after stroke: Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Int. Med. Res. 2014, 42, 659–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Brown, C.A.; Schmitt, F.A.; Smith, C.D.; Gold, B.T. Distinct patterns of default mode and executive control network circuitry contribute to present and future executive function in older adults. Neuroimage 2019, 195, 320–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cao, W.; Cao, X.; Hou, C.; Li, T.; Cheng, Y.; Jiang, L.; Luo, C.; Li, C.; Yao, D. Effects of cognitive training on resting-state functional connectivity of default mode, salience, and central executive networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. McDonald, M.W.; Black, S.E.; Copland, D.A.; Corbett, D.; Dijkhuizen, R.M.; Farr, T.D.; Jeffers, M.S.; Kalaria, R.N.; Karayanidis, F.; Leff, A.P.; et al. Cognition in stroke rehabilitation and recovery research: Consensus-based core recommendations from the second stroke recovery and rehabilitation roundtable. Int. J. Stroke 2019, 14, 774–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Lesion coverage proportion maps of training and control groups. Lesions were manually segmented and overlaid onto the reference standard brain template by means of the clinical toolbox [27,28]. The percentage values represent the lesion overlap within (A,B) and between (C) groups.
Figure 1. Lesion coverage proportion maps of training and control groups. Lesions were manually segmented and overlaid onto the reference standard brain template by means of the clinical toolbox [27,28]. The percentage values represent the lesion overlap within (A,B) and between (C) groups.
Brainsci 11 01002 g001
Figure 2. Relation between scores on the Barthel Index before rehabilitation (Pre) and the difference scores between Post- and Pre-rehabilitation, in the training and control groups.
Figure 2. Relation between scores on the Barthel Index before rehabilitation (Pre) and the difference scores between Post- and Pre-rehabilitation, in the training and control groups.
Brainsci 11 01002 g002
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the two patients’ groups. Mean (and standard deviation) or frequencies are reported in cells. MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; TIB: Italian version of the National Adult Reading Test. P-values were obtained from independent sample t-tests (Age, Education, MMSE, TIB, and Time since event) or chi-square (M/F, Etiology, Symptomatology, and Rehabilitation program) statistics.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the two patients’ groups. Mean (and standard deviation) or frequencies are reported in cells. MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; TIB: Italian version of the National Adult Reading Test. P-values were obtained from independent sample t-tests (Age, Education, MMSE, TIB, and Time since event) or chi-square (M/F, Etiology, Symptomatology, and Rehabilitation program) statistics.
Training Group
(n = 18)
Control Group
(n = 19)
p
Age (years)64.6 (12.7)64.9 (12.7)0.946
M/F12/614/50.728
Education (years)9.4 (4)9.3 (4.2)0.925
MMSE25.1 (2.5)24.6 (3.3)0.557
TIB106.1 (9.4)101.1 (11.7)0.189
Time since event (months)3.1 (2.4)4.2 (3.4)0.259
Etiology
Ischemia13120.999
Hemorrhage66
Lesion side
Left hemisphere770.956
Right hemisphere911
Bilateral11
Symptomatology
Aphasia, dysartria6100.671
Neglect78
Sensory-motor impairments
(e.g., hemiplegia/
hemiparesis)
1819
Cortical visual impairments
(e.g., hemianopsia)
42
Rehabilitation program
Speech therapy590.803
Motor therapy1818
Occupational therapy88
Neuropsychological rehab1217
Table 2. Means and standard deviations (in parentheses) for Pre- (T0) and Post-rehabilitation (T1) sessions. P-values derived from Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for not normally distributed variables (Digit span forward, Digit span backward, Corsi block-tapping test, Naming, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test WCST categories and errors, Stroop Inverse Efficiency Score, and Functional Independence Measure), and from paired t-tests for normally distributed variables (Trail Making Test-A, Attentional matrices, Phonemic fluency, Semantic fluency, Five Point error index, and Barthel index). The pre-test–post-test control group effect sizes were computed by the Morris’s formula [53]. In bold p-values < 0.050 and effect sizes > 0.4. N varies across tests because some participants could not perform all tests due to aphasia or neglect.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations (in parentheses) for Pre- (T0) and Post-rehabilitation (T1) sessions. P-values derived from Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for not normally distributed variables (Digit span forward, Digit span backward, Corsi block-tapping test, Naming, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test WCST categories and errors, Stroop Inverse Efficiency Score, and Functional Independence Measure), and from paired t-tests for normally distributed variables (Trail Making Test-A, Attentional matrices, Phonemic fluency, Semantic fluency, Five Point error index, and Barthel index). The pre-test–post-test control group effect sizes were computed by the Morris’s formula [53]. In bold p-values < 0.050 and effect sizes > 0.4. N varies across tests because some participants could not perform all tests due to aphasia or neglect.
Training GroupControl Group
T0T1npT0T1npd
Memory
Digit span forward4.6 (1)5.2 (0.9)160.0504.7 (1)4.9 (0.9)160.3310.56
Digit span backward3.5 (0.8)3.2 (1.1)160.4623.1 (1.3)2.7 (1)160.3530.12
Corsi block-tapping3.9 (0.9)4.2 (1)180.193.8 (1.1)4 (0.8)180.5890.13
Attention and processing speed
Attentional matrices36.7 (13.5)42.6 (12.7)180.00134.3 (10.6)37.1 (10.2)190.1150.32
TMT-A (s)87.1 (46.3)82.3 (55.3)170.54377.9 (37.9)68.9 (28.2)160.0770.16
Language
Naming 14 (1.4)14.1 (1.6)170.27613.8 (1.5)14.3 (1)170.069−0.37
Phonemic fluency24.1 (11.8)29.1 (11.2)140.02021.6 (11.3)22.6 (11.7)160.5690.41
Semantic fluency33.8 (13.3)34.2 (10.7)90.87225.7 (11.3)26.6 (8)150.554−0.05
Executive functions
WCST cat4 (2)4.2 (1.9)160.5213.2 (2)3.2 (1.9)160.8070.13
WCST err6.6 (4.4)5.4 (6.3)160.5047 (4.7)7.2 (5.1)160.5740.41
Five Point error index24.3 (22.1)22.5 (24.3)180.80224.4 (20.3)18.6 (12.8)180.1580.25
Stroop IES 45.2 (45.1)37.5 (15.6)150.53247.7 (26.4)44.6 (28.2)130.1580.49
Functional scales
Barthel index25.5 (23.6)75 (15)18<0.00124.5 (18.5)66.8 (20)19<0.0010.42
FIM56.3 (16.7)92.8 (13.5)18<0.00153.4 (16.4)87.4 (21.1)19<0.0010.19
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tarantino, V.; Burgio, F.; Toffano, R.; Rigon, E.; Meneghello, F.; Weis, L.; Vallesi, A. Efficacy of a Training on Executive Functions in Potentiating Rehabilitation Effects in Stroke Patients. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081002

AMA Style

Tarantino V, Burgio F, Toffano R, Rigon E, Meneghello F, Weis L, Vallesi A. Efficacy of a Training on Executive Functions in Potentiating Rehabilitation Effects in Stroke Patients. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(8):1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081002

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tarantino, Vincenza, Francesca Burgio, Roberta Toffano, Elena Rigon, Francesca Meneghello, Luca Weis, and Antonino Vallesi. 2021. "Efficacy of a Training on Executive Functions in Potentiating Rehabilitation Effects in Stroke Patients" Brain Sciences 11, no. 8: 1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081002

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop