Next Article in Journal
Effect of Terahertz Antenna Radiation in Hypersonic Plasma Sheaths with Different Vehicle Shapes
Next Article in Special Issue
Identification of Geriatric Depression and Anxiety Using Activity Tracking Data and Minimal Geriatric Assessment Scales
Previous Article in Journal
Modular Combined DC-DC Autotransformer for Offshore Wind Power Integration with DC Collection
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Low-Frequency Oscillations and Force Control Capabilities as a Function of Force Level in Older Women

Division of Sport Science, Neuromechanical Rehabilitation Research Laboratory, Health Promotion Center, and Sport Science Institute, Incheon National University, Incheon 22012, Korea
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(4), 1812; https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041812
Submission received: 19 December 2021 / Revised: 31 January 2022 / Accepted: 9 February 2022 / Published: 10 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Biotechnology and Sports Engineering II)

Abstract

:
Force variability is potentially related to altered low-frequency oscillations in motor outputs. This study examines the contributions of low-frequency oscillations in force to altered force control performances from lower to higher targeted force levels in older women. Fourteen older women executed unilateral hand-grip force control tasks at 10% and 40% of maximum voluntary contraction (MVC). Force control performances were estimated by calculating force accuracy (root-mean-square-error), force variability (standard deviation), and force regularity (approximate entropy). We additionally quantified low-frequency oscillations in force using absolute powers across four different frequency bands: (a) 0–0.5 Hz, (b) 0.5–1.0 Hz, (c) 1.0–1.5 Hz, and (d) 1.5–2.0 Hz. The findings reveal that from lower to higher targeted force level older women show greater force error, force variability, and force regularity with increased values of absolute power in force across the four frequency bands. The multiple regression models identified a significant relationship between greater force frequency power below 0.5 Hz and more impairments in force control performances. These findings suggest that force frequency oscillation below 0.5 Hz is a key predictor indicating altered stability of task performances across different targeted force levels in older women.

1. Introduction

Force control capabilities are essential for successfully conducting various activities of daily living [1,2]. Moreover, the isometric force control paradigm may be a viable option to estimate sensorimotor integration functions in the motor system [3,4] because a performer can produce and maintain isometric forces around the targeted force level via continuous online motor corrections by processing visual feedback. During the isometric force control task, quantifying inconsistency of motor outputs (i.e., force variability) provides information regarding how the motor system controls and regulates motor-related neural oscillations for successful task performance [5,6,7].
A recent literature review raised the possibility that force variability properties in the motor system may be associated with changes in the low-frequency oscillations of motor outputs [8]. For healthy young adults, when the concurrent visual information was removed, force oscillations below 0.5 Hz increased with greater force variability during the index finger’s abduction force control tasks [9]. Moreover, older adults and patients with stroke revealed more low force oscillations with higher motor variability than those in the control groups [9,10]. While processing a greater amount of visual information (i.e., higher visual gain conditions), elderly people especially elevated force frequency properties below 0.5 Hz that predicted more force variability across force control tasks using upper and lower limbs [9,11]. Interestingly, these greater force oscillations below 0.5 Hz were additionally associated with increased oscillation of muscle activity below 0.5 Hz (i.e., EMG burst power) for both younger and older adult groups [11,12]. Taken together, these findings support an idea that force frequency power below 0.5 Hz may be a crucial predictor indicating altered neuromuscular variability in the motor system.
A recent literature review proposed several possible mechanisms underlying the relationship between the greater powers in the low-frequency band and increased force variability [8]. Potentially, the central nervous system (CNS) may select a control strategy for increasing the efficiency in controlling and generating more accurate force outputs by grouping the activations in the motor neuron pools into a low-frequency unit. Next, the low-frequency oscillations in forces may be considered as neuronal noise that presumably originated from higher physiological stress in the sympathetic nervous system (e.g., breathing and heart rate) [13]. Finally, the altered voluntary drive from the brain when more difficult task requirements were provided [9,12,14] may be responsible for the low force frequency oscillations.
Importantly, force control performances can be affected by various neuromuscular factors such as different targeted force levels. For example, force variability (i.e., standard deviation of force outputs) typically increased at higher targeted force levels [15,16]. Given that this scaling relation between force variability and targeted force level presumably reflects neuronal changes in the central and peripheral nervous systems [17,18,19], increased force variability at higher targeted force levels would be influenced by greater force oscillations below 0.5 Hz. In fact, a prior study reported that increased force variability from 10% to 30% of maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) in healthy young adults was significantly associated with greater force frequency power below 0.5 Hz [12]. Previously, older adults revealed higher force variability than young adult controls across various submaximal targeted force levels (e.g., 10–50% of MVC) because of age-related deficits in the nervous systems [16,20]. Force control at higher targeted levels is necessary to facilitate greater neural involvements in the motor system (e.g., hyperactivation across sensorimotor cortex and greater low-frequency oscillations in motor neuron discharge rates) [21,22]. However, given that elderly people showed a tendency to increase the neural activations to compensate for age-related impairments in the motor system [23], controlling degrees of freedom of neural resources may be challenging for older adults, resulting in more erroneous and variable forces [24]. Taken together, these findings suggest that elderly people may additionally show greater force oscillations below 0.5 Hz from lower to higher targeted force levels that potentially lead to more impairments in force control capabilities.
Thus, the purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of different force levels on changes in force control performances and low-frequency oscillations in force for older adults. In particular, we focused on older women since females in the older group demonstrated more impairments in hand-grip force control performances than males [25] because of the interactive effects of aging and loss of sexual hormones facilitating more dysfunctions in the motor system [26]. Participants executed unilateral hand-grip force control tasks with their dominant hand at 10% and 40% of MVC. Beyond the previous findings that focused on force variability changes [8,9,10,11], we additionally examined the potential contribution of low-frequency oscillations in force to various force control performances, including force accuracy, variability, and regularity. Based on a significant relationship between greater powers in the low force frequency band (<0.5 Hz) and increased force variability [8], we hypothesized that force control performances (i.e., greater task error, variability, and regularity) would be impaired from lower to higher targeted force levels, and furthermore the increased force oscillations below 0.5 Hz would be associated with more force control impairments.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Fourteen healthy older right-handed women who experienced menopause (time since menopause > six years) participated in this study. Before the testing, we confirmed that all participants had no musculoskeletal deficits in their upper extremities and cognitive dysfunctions (mini-mental state examination scores ≥ 26) [27]. Specific demographic details are shown in Table 1. All participants read and signed the study protocols and an informed consent form approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Incheon National University prior to beginning the experiments.

2.2. Experimental Apparatus and Procedures

Hand-grip force control tasks for the dominant hand were administered because grip strength is progressively reduced in the aging population [28,29], and further decreased grip strength predicts various age-related diseases [30]. Participants sat in front of a 54.6 cm LED monitor (1920 × 1080 pixels and 60 Hz of a refresh rate) located 80 cm away at eye level and placed both arms on the table with comfortable positions (15–20° of shoulder flexion and 25–40° of elbow flexion). To perform grip force control tasks, participants used their dominant hands and maintained their non-dominant hands on the pad (Figure 1A). To avoid unintentional force generations caused by other joint actions, we instructed them to fix both arms on the table during the task execution. We used a customized isometric force control device (SEED TECH Co., Ltd., Bucheon, South Korea), including handles (a diameter = 30 mm) embedded force transducers (Micro Load Cell-CZL635-3135, range = 220 lbs., Phidgets Inc., Calgary, Canada). A 16-bit analog-to-digital converter (A/D; ADS1148 16-Bit 2Ksps and a minimum detectable force = 0.0192 N) collected all force signals (a sampling rate = 200 Hz) amplified by an INA122 with an excitation voltage of 5 V (Texas Instruments Inc., Dallas, TX, USA). All experimental procedures were administered using a custom Microsoft Visual C++ program (Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA, USA), and a custom Matlab Program (Math WorksTM Inc., Natick, MA, USA) was used for additional offline analysis.

2.2.1. MVC task

To set individualized targeted force levels, participants conduced two consecutive MVC trials. We instructed participants to produce their maximal forces for 5 s and provided 60 s of rest between MVC trials. We selected a peak value of force outputs for each trial, and calculated participant’s MVC by averaging the two peak values. Then, two levels of targeted force levels (i.e., 10% and 40% of MVC) for each participant were calculated.

2.2.2. Submaximal Force Control Task

We administered three consecutive submaximal force control trials for each targeted force level (i.e., 10% and 40% of MVC). The order of the two targeted force levels was randomly assigned. During a submaximal force control trial, participants produced and maintained isometric force outputs around the targeted force level for 20 s. Two types of visual information were provided on the screen: (a) isometric force production = a red line, and (b) targeted force level = a white line (Figure 1B). To minimize potential fatigue effects, we set 30 s of rest between trials and 60 s of rest between targeted force levels.

2.3. Data Analysis

To prevent potential initial isometric force control adjustment and early termination effects, the first 5 s and final 1 s of force data were removed. Thus, the middle 14 s of force data was analyzed for each trial. Using a bidirectional fourth-order Butterworth filter (a cut-off frequency = 30 Hz), all force data were additionally filtered. Force control performances were estimated by (a) force accuracy: root-mean-square error (RMSE), (b) force variability: standard deviation (SD), and (c) force regularity: approximate entropy (ApEn). Force regularity is a temporal structure of force variability so that ApEn values denote regularity of future force signals in a time series in comparison to the prior force signals. The values of ApEn close to 2 represent less regularity of force data indicating more adaptive force productions, whereas the values of ApEn close to 0 mean more regularity of force data. Using the Pincus algorithm (Formula 1), we calculated the ApEn value for each trial [31,32].
ApEn   X ,   m ,   r = ln C m r C m + 1 r
where Cm (r) indicates number of repetitive patterns of length m of vector X within r criterion of similarity. Cm + 1 (r) indicates number of repetitive patterns of length m + 1. Consistent with previous studies [33,34], we set m as 2 and r as 0.2 × SD.
Low-frequency oscillations in force signals were calculated by performing power spectrum analysis with a fast Fourier transform using a custom Matlab Program (Math WorksTM Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Consistent with prior studies [11,12], we quantified absolute power (N2) for the four frequency bands: (a) 0–0.5 Hz, (b) 0.5–1.0 Hz, (c) 1.0–1.5 Hz, and (d) 1.5–2.0 Hz.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

To determine altered force control performances across different targeted force levels, we conducted paired t-tests on RMSE, SD, and ApEn. Furthermore, two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on each factor (Force Level × Frequency Band; 2 × 4) were used for the low-frequency oscillations in force signals. When assumptions of sphericity were violated, we report Greenhouse-Geisser’s degrees of freedom adjustment. Bonferroni pairwise comparisons were used for the post hoc test. To determine the relationship between altered low-frequency oscillations in force and force control performances between two targeted force levels, we performed three multiple linear regression analyses on changes in absolute powers for each frequency band (four explanatory variables at 40% of MVC—those at 10% of MVC) and changes in force control performance (a dependent variable at 40% of MVC—those at 10% of MVC) using the stepwise method. Both explanatory and dependent variables were the changes in variables from lower to higher targeted force levels. All statistical analyses were conducted using the IBM SPSS Statistics 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and the alpha level was set at 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Force Control Performance and Low-Frequency Oscillations in Force

From 10% to 40% of MVC, paired t-tests revealed impairments in force control performances: (a) increased values in RMSE (t13 = −10.215; p < 0.001; Figure 2A), (b) greater values in SD (t13 = −8.239 and p < 0.001; Figure 2B), and (c) less values in ApEn (t13 = 12.247 and p < 0.001; Figure 2C). Moreover, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA on the absolute power showed a significant force level × frequency band interaction [F(1.882, 24.468) = 25.675; p < 0.001; partial η2 = 0.664; Figure 2D]. Post hoc analyses showed that the absolute power for four frequency bands significantly increased from 10% to 40% of MVC. At 10% of MVC, the absolute power in 0.5–1.0 Hz was significantly greater than those in 1.5–2.0 Hz and the absolute power in 1.0–1.5 Hz was significantly greater than those in 1.5–2.0 Hz. At 40% of MVC, the absolute powers were significantly different among all pairs of frequency bands.

3.2. Relationship between Changes in Low-Frequency Oscillations in Force and Force Control Performances across Different Targeted Force Levels

A multiple linear regression analysis using a stepwise method identified a significant relationship between increased values of absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz and higher RMSE values (Y = 0.920 + 0.047 X; R2 = 0.533; p = 0.003; Figure 3A). For SD values, the analysis found a significant relationship between increased values of absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz and 0.5–1.0 Hz and higher SD values (Y = 0.307 + 0.037 X0–0.5 Hz + 0.018 X0.5–1.0 Hz; R2 = 0.978; p < 0.001; Figure 3B). Finally, a multiple linear regression analysis using a stepwise method reported a significant relationship between increased values of absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz and less ApEn values (Y = −0.141–0.008 X; R2 = 0.890; p < 0.001; Figure 3C).

4. Discussion

The current study examines altered low-frequency oscillations in force and force control performances across different targeted force levels in older women. From 10% to 40% of MVC, older women increased force error, force variability, and force regularity during unilateral hand-grip force control tasks, and further revealed greater force oscillations in four different frequency bands below 2 Hz. Interestingly, the multiple linear regression analyses using a stepwise method identified that greater amounts of force oscillations below 0.5 Hz significantly predicted higher levels of force error, force variability, and force regularity from lower to higher targeted force levels, respectively.

4.1. Changes in Force Control Capabilities and Low-Frequency Oscillations at Different Targeted Levels

Not surprisingly, older women revealed force control deficits such as increased force error and absolute force variability at 40% of MVC as compared with those at 10% of MVC consistent with most prior findings [12,15,16,35]. Presumably, simultaneously maintaining and correcting force outputs via the processing of visual information at higher targeted levels is challenging so that these procedures required more neural involvements in the motor system, such as greater brain activation across sensorimotor areas and low-frequency oscillations in motor neuron discharge rates [21,22]. Importantly, the neural involvements for the elderly people tended to increase to compensate for age-related impairments in the motor system [23]. These greater neural resources during the task may increase difficulty in controlling the degrees of freedom of motor actions for the older adults, resulting in more erroneous and variable forces [24]. Moreover, higher force regularity at 40% of MVC than at 10% of MVC is in line with previous studies [16,36,37]. The prior findings indicated that the motor system may decrease motor adaptations (e.g., increased motor optimality) rather than identify various motor options (e.g., increased motor flexibility) while performing more difficult tasks (e.g., tasks that required stronger force productions and tasks without visual feedback) [37,38].
Beyond the prior findings that showed the contribution of low-frequency oscillations in force signals to force variability [8], we found that these relationship patterns were observed in both force accuracy and regularity across different targeted force level. A recent study reported that increased relative force power below 0.25 Hz was significantly associated with higher task error (R2 = 0.36) and force variability (R2 = 0.33) when healthy young participants executed more challenging dynamic force control tasks (i.e., tracking 1 Hz of the sinusoidal target line than 0.5 Hz of the sinusoidal target line) [14]. Common findings from two studies that focused on healthy young adults suggested that task constraints (e.g., more difficult task requirements such as higher targeted force level and more oscillatory targeted force level) influenced changes in the low force frequency band (<0.5 Hz) resulting in impaired force control performances [12,14]. Taken together, we confirmed that the potential effects of task constraint on low-frequency oscillation in force signals additionally appeared in the older women group in addition to the young adults [12].
Interestingly, the current findings expanded prior findings by showing a significant relationship between force frequency power below 0.5 Hz and force regularity across different targeted force levels. Christou and colleagues posited that greater force oscillations below 0.5 Hz may be related to the CNS’s motor control strategies [8,14]. When higher force outputs are generated, a greater number of spinal motor neurons are recruited [39]. For example, producing and controlling isometric force outputs at a higher targeted force level (>30% of MVC) may require the recruitment of large motor neurons that innervate fast-twitch, fatigable muscle fibers, in addition to the early recruitment of small motor neurons that innervate slow-twitch muscle fibers [39]. Thus, the CNS is necessary to control numerous spinal motor neurons in an efficient way (i.e., grouping spinal motor neurons into low-frequency units) that may increase force frequency power below 0.5 Hz instead of individually modulating spinal motor neurons. In fact, a prior simulation study found that motor unit synchronization patterns, defined as a tendency for comparable temporal intervals of discharge rate between motor units, increased with more efforts toward producing forces leading to deterioration of motor actions (e.g., greater force variability and low force frequency oscillations) [40]. Dideriksen and colleagues additionally reported simulated results that more regular force generation patterns (i.e., lower values of ApEn) appeared with a greater number of motor neurons involvements [41]. Importantly, the inter-spike intervals in aging muscles tended to be less complex (i.e., higher regularity), and furthermore elderly people hardly continued task stability along with more flexibility [42]. Overall, this motor control strategy may be responsible for producing more stereotyped motor outputs (i.e., increased force regularity) rather than generating more flexible force control.

4.2. Potential Mechanisms underlying the Contribution of Low-Frequency Oscillations to Force Control Capabilities

Potentially, the effects of changes in low force frequency oscillations on force control performances may originate from neural involvements of the central and peripheral nervous systems. Given that producing stronger muscle forces increased corticomuscular coherences across various frequency bands (e.g., beta and gamma bands) [43], greater low-frequency oscillations at higher targeted force level may be influenced by voluntary drive from supraspinal level to activations in the spinal motor neurons [8]. Moreover, despite no muscle activation findings in this study, several studies reported a potential relationship between whole muscle activity oscillations in the gamma band and low-frequency burst (<1 Hz) in muscle activity contributing to a force frequency structure below 0.5 Hz [11,12,14]. Intriguingly, several studies have focused on low-frequency oscillations of brain activity (i.e., electroencephalogram: EEG frequency power below 0.5–4 Hz) that potentially appeared in specific pathologies (e.g., epileptic seizure, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and stroke) [44,45,46]. Indeed, a recent study that examined postural variability during a forward lean task reported greater low-frequency of center of pressure (COP) oscillations below 0.5 Hz in the older adult group than in the young adult group, and furthermore found these changes were associated with greater COP variability collapsed across the two groups [47]. Presumably, low-frequency oscillations of various biological signals may be related to motor variability in human movements. Thus, future studies should determine whether low-frequency oscillations in force signals are the results of intentional motor control strategies from the brain or inherent noise components of biological signals in the motor system.

4.3. Study Limitations

Although we identified the relationship between low-frequency oscillations and force control capabilities in older women, these findings should be carefully interpreted. First, given that only older women participated in this study, whether changes in force oscillation below 5 Hz from 10% to 40% of MVC are different between younger and older adult groups is still inconclusive. Previously, older adults revealed greater force oscillations below 5 Hz with higher motor variability than those in younger adults while processing a greater amount of visual feedback [9], and furthermore a lower mean firing rate of motor units appeared in older adults during submaximal force control tasks (10–50% of MVC) [48]. Thus, comparing altered low-frequency oscillations between younger and older groups across different targeted force levels may provide further information regarding age-related motor control deficits. Moreover, some studies that used conventional surface EMG devices evidenced that low-frequency oscillations in muscle activity were associated with force frequency power below 0.5 Hz [12,14]. Beyond these findings, to determine potential relationship between altered motor neuron pools and force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz caused by aging, future studies should examine low-frequency properties of multiple motor units activations and force control capabilities between younger and older adults according to various task constraints.

5. Conclusions

In summary: the current study identifies that increased force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz in older women are significantly associated with more impairments in force control performances from lower to higher targeted force levels. Our findings expand prior results shown in healthy young individuals [12] by demonstrating the effects of increased force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz on impaired force control capabilities estimated by both conventional (higher force error and variability) and non-linear (greater force regularity) measurements. These findings suggest that impaired force control capabilities from lower to higher targeted force levels may originate from changes in low force frequency properties regardless of the aging progress. Moreover, in addition to recent findings that show greater force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz with higher visual gain condition in older adults [11], we suggest that low-frequency oscillation in force is a key predictor indicating altered stability of task performances in elderly people affected by various task requirements.

Funding

This research was funded by a National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean government (MSIT), Grant Number NRF-2018R1C1B5084455.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Incheon National University (approval #7007971-201810-002A and the study protocol was approved on 16 December 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not Available.

Acknowledgments

Not Available.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wang, D.X.M.; Yao, J.; Zirek, Y.; Reijnierse, E.M.; Maier, A.B. Muscle mass, strength, and physical performance predicting activities of daily living: A meta-analysis. J. Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle 2020, 11, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Kang, N.; Cauraugh, J.H. Force control in chronic stroke. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 52, 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Slifkin, A.B.; Vaillancourt, D.E.; Newell, K.M. Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. J. Neurophysiol. 2000, 84, 1708–1718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Vieluf, S.; Sleimen-Malkoun, R.; Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Jirsa, V.; Reuter, E.-M.; Godde, B.; Temprado, J.-J.; Huys, R. Dynamical signatures of isometric force control as a function of age, expertise, and task constraints. J. Neurophysiol. 2017, 118, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Vaillancourt, D.E.; Thulborn, K.R.; Corcos, D.M. Neural basis for the processes that underlie visually guided and internally guided force control in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 2003, 90, 3330–3340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Archer, D.B.; Kang, N.; Misra, G.; Marble, S.; Patten, C.; Coombes, S.A. Visual feedback alters force control and functional activity in the visuomotor network after stroke. NeuroImage Clin. 2017, 17, 505–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Harris, C.M.; Wolpert, D.M. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 1998, 394, 780–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lodha, N.; Christou, E.A. Low-frequency oscillations and control of the motor output. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Fox, E.J.; Baweja, H.S.; Kim, C.; Kennedy, D.M.; Vaillancourt, D.E.; Christou, E.A. Modulation of force below 1 Hz: Age-associated differences and the effect of magnified visual feedback. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e55970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Lodha, N.; Misra, G.; Coombes, S.A.; Christou, E.A.; Cauraugh, J.H. Increased force variability in chronic stroke: Contributions of force modulation below 1 Hz. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e83468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Park, S.H.; Kwon, M.; Christou, E.A. Motor output oscillations with magnification of visual feedback in older adults. Neurosci. Lett. 2017, 647, 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Moon, H.; Kim, C.; Kwon, M.; Chen, Y.T.; Onushko, T.; Lodha, N.; Christou, E.A. Force control is related to low-frequency oscillations in force and surface EMG. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e109202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Christou, E.A.; Jakobi, J.M.; Critchlow, A.; Fleshner, M.; Enoka, R.M. The 1- to 2-Hz oscillations in muscle force are exacerbated by stress, especially in older adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 2004, 97, 225–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Park, S.H.; Kim, C.; Yacoubi, B.; Christou, E.A. Control of oscillatory force tasks: Low-frequency oscillations in force and muscle activity. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2019, 64, 89–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Slifkin, A.B.; Newell, K.M. Noise, information transmission, and force variability. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 837–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Vieluf, S.; Temprado, J.-J.; Berton, E.; Jirsa, V.K.; Sleimen-Malkoun, R. Effects of task and age on the magnitude and structure of force fluctuations: Insights into underlying neuro-behavioral processes. BMC Neurosci. 2015, 16, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Jones, K.E.; Hamilton, A.F.D.C.; Wolpert, D.M. Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. J. Neurophysiol. 2002, 88, 1533–1544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ward, N.S.; Swayne, O.B.C.; Newton, J.M. Age-dependent changes in the neural correlates of force modulation: An fMRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 2008, 29, 1434–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Mayhew, S.D.; Porcaro, C.; Tecchio, F.; Bagshaw, A.P. fMRI characterisation of widespread brain networks relevant for behavioural variability in fine hand motor control with and without visual feedback. NeuroImage 2017, 148, 330–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sorond, F.A.; Cruz-Almeida, Y.; Clark, D.J.; Viswanathan, A.; Scherzer, C.R.; De Jager, P.; Csiszar, A.; Laurienti, P.J.; Hausdorff, J.M.; Chen, W.G.; et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility in older adults: Neural mechanisms of mobility impairment. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Boil. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015, 70, 1526–1532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Cramer, S.C.; Weisskoff, R.M.; Schaechter, J.D.; Nelles, G.; Foley, M.; Finklestein, S.P.; Rosen, B.R. Motor cortex activation is related to force of squeezing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002, 16, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Dideriksen, J.L.; Negro, F.; Enoka, R.M.; Farina, D. Motor unit recruitment strategies and muscle properties determine the influence of synaptic noise on force steadiness. J. Neurophysiol. 2012, 107, 3357–3369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Noble, J.W.; Eng, J.J.; Kokotilo, K.J.; Boyd, L.A. Aging effects on the control of grip force magnitude: An fMRI study. Exp. Gerontol. 2011, 46, 453–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Christou, E.A. Aging and variability of voluntary contractions. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2011, 39, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Choi, K.-H.; Kim, D.-M.; Lee, S.-Y.; Lee, J.-H.; Kong, Y.-K. Evaluation of the controlled grip force exertion tasks associated with age, gender, handedness and target force level. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 2017, 24, 507–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Messier, V.; Rabasa-Lhoret, R.; Barbat-Artigas, S.; Elisha, B.; Karelis, A.D.; Aubertin-Leheudre, M. Menopause and sarcopenia: A potential role for sex hormones. Maturitas 2011, 68, 331–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mehmet, H.; Yang, A.W.H.; Robinson, S.R. Measurement of hand grip strength in the elderly: A scoping review with recommendations. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 2020, 24, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kurina, L.M.; Gulati, M.; Everson-Rose, S.A.; Chung, P.J.; Karavolos, K.; Cohen, N.J.; Kandula, N.; Lukezic, R.; Dugan, S.A.; Sowers, M.; et al. The Effect of menopause on grip and pinch strength: Results from the Chicago, Illinois, site of the study of women’s health across the nation. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2004, 160, 484–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Li, Y.-Z.; Zhuang, H.-F.; Cai, S.-Q.; Lin, C.-K.; Wang, P.-W.; Yan, L.-S.; Lin, J.-K.; Yu, H.-M. Low grip strength is a strong risk factor of osteoporosis in postmenopausal women. Orthop. Surg. 2018, 10, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Pincus, S.M. Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1991, 88, 2297–2301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Pincus, S.M.; Goldberger, A.L. Physiological time-series analysis: What does regularity quantify? Am. J. Physiol. 1994, 266, H1643–H1656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Vaillancourt, D.E.; Slifkin, A.B.; Newell, K.M. Regularity of force tremor in Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001, 112, 1594–1603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cauraugh, J.H.; Lodha, N.; Naik, S.K.; Summers, J.J. Bilateral movement training and stroke motor recovery progress: A structured review and meta-analysis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2010, 29, 853–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Strote, C.; Gölz, C.; Stroehlein, J.K.; Haase, F.K.; Koester, D.; Reinsberger, C.; Vieluf, S. Effects of force level and task difficulty on force control performance in elderly people. Exp. Brain Res. 2020, 238, 2179–2188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Mosconi, M.W.; Mohanty, S.; Greene, R.K.; Cook, E.H.; Vaillancourt, D.E.; Sweeney, J.A. Feedforward and feedback motor control abnormalities implicate cerebellar dysfunctions in autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 2015–2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Hu, X.; Loncharich, M.; Newell, K.M. Visual information interacts with neuromuscular factors in the coordination of bimanual isometric force. Exp. Brain Res. 2011, 209, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Gorniak, S.L. The relationship between task difficulty and motor performance complexity. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2019, 81, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Conwit, R.A.; Stashuk, D.; Tracy, B.; McHugh, M.; Brown, W.F.; Metter, E.J. The relationship of motor unit size, firing rate and force. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999, 110, 1270–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Yao, W.; Fuglevand, R.J.; Enoka, R.M. Motor-unit synchronization increases EMG amplitude and decreases force steadiness of simulated contractions. J. Neurophysiol. 2000, 83, 441–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Dideriksen, J.; Elias, L.A.; Zambalde, E.P.; Germer, C.M.; Molinari, R.G.; Negro, F. Influence of central and peripheral motor unit properties on isometric muscle force entropy: A computer simulation study. J. Biomech. 2021, 110866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Chen, Y.-C.; Lin, L.L.; Hwang, I.-S. Novel behavioral and neural evidences for age-related changes in force complexity. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 2018, 73, 997–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Liu, J.; Sheng, Y.; Liu, H. Corticomuscular coherence and its applications: A review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Demanuele, C.; James, C.J.; Sonuga-Barke, E.J. Distinguishing low frequency oscillations within the 1/f spectral behaviour of electromagnetic brain signals. Behav. Brain Funct. 2007, 3, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Ramanathan, D.S.; Guo, L.; Gulati, T.; Davidson, G.; Hishinuma, A.K.; Won, S.-J.; Knight, R.T.; Chang, E.F.; Swanson, R.A.; Ganguly, K. Low-frequency cortical activity is a neuromodulatory target that tracks recovery after stroke. Nat. Med. 2018, 24, 1257–1267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Cassidy, J.M.; Wodeyar, A.; Wu, J.; Kaur, K.; Masuda, A.K.; Srinivasan, R.; Cramer, S.C. Low-frequency oscillations are a biomarker of injury and recovery after stroke. Stroke 2020, 51, 1442–1450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Delmas, S.; Watanabe, T.; Yacoubi, B.; Christou, E.A. Age-associated increase in postural variability relate to greater low-frequency center of pressure oscillations. Gait Posture 2021, 85, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Connelly, D.M.; Rice, C.L.; Roos, M.R.; Vandervoort, A.A. Motor unit firing rates and contractile properties in tibialis anterior of young and old men. J. Appl. Physiol. 1999, 87, 843–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Experimental setup. (A) hand-grip isometric force control, and (B) visual information during force control task.
Figure 1. Experimental setup. (A) hand-grip isometric force control, and (B) visual information during force control task.
Applsci 12 01812 g001
Figure 2. Force control performances and low-frequency oscillations in force across two targeted force levels. (A) Force accuracy (RMSE), (B) force variability (SD), (C) force regularity (ApEn), and (D) low-frequency oscillations in force (absolute power) for four frequency bans as a function of different targeted force level. Data are mean ± standard error. Asterisk (*) denotes a significant difference between two targeted force levels. Number sign (#) indicates a significant difference in comparison to 0–0.5 Hz. Percent sign (%) indicates a significant difference in comparison to 0.5–1.0 Hz. Ampersand (&) means a significant difference in comparison to 1.0–1.5 Hz.
Figure 2. Force control performances and low-frequency oscillations in force across two targeted force levels. (A) Force accuracy (RMSE), (B) force variability (SD), (C) force regularity (ApEn), and (D) low-frequency oscillations in force (absolute power) for four frequency bans as a function of different targeted force level. Data are mean ± standard error. Asterisk (*) denotes a significant difference between two targeted force levels. Number sign (#) indicates a significant difference in comparison to 0–0.5 Hz. Percent sign (%) indicates a significant difference in comparison to 0.5–1.0 Hz. Ampersand (&) means a significant difference in comparison to 1.0–1.5 Hz.
Applsci 12 01812 g002
Figure 3. Regression models showing relationship between change of low-frequency oscillations in force and force control performances across two targeted force levels. (A) force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz (absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz) vs. force accuracy (RMSE), (B) force frequency oscillations below 1.0 Hz (absolute powers in 0–0.5 Hz and 0.5–1.0 Hz) vs. force variability (SD), and (C) force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz (absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz) vs. force regularity (ApEn).
Figure 3. Regression models showing relationship between change of low-frequency oscillations in force and force control performances across two targeted force levels. (A) force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz (absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz) vs. force accuracy (RMSE), (B) force frequency oscillations below 1.0 Hz (absolute powers in 0–0.5 Hz and 0.5–1.0 Hz) vs. force variability (SD), and (C) force frequency oscillations below 0.5 Hz (absolute power in 0–0.5 Hz) vs. force regularity (ApEn).
Applsci 12 01812 g003
Table 1. Demographic information.
Table 1. Demographic information.
CharacteristicsOlder Women
Sample Size (N)14
Age (years)63.5 ± 2.4
Time since menopause (year)13.2 ± 5.3
Handedness14 right handed
Weight (kg)58.9 ± 6.2
Skeletal Muscle Mass (kg)20.1 ± 1.7
Body Fat Mass (kg)21.4 ± 4.5
BMI (%)24.1 ± 2.3
Mini-mental state exam (score)28.1 ± 1.7
Data were stated as mean ± standard deviation. BMI: body mass index.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kang, N. Low-Frequency Oscillations and Force Control Capabilities as a Function of Force Level in Older Women. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 1812. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041812

AMA Style

Kang N. Low-Frequency Oscillations and Force Control Capabilities as a Function of Force Level in Older Women. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(4):1812. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041812

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kang, Nyeonju. 2022. "Low-Frequency Oscillations and Force Control Capabilities as a Function of Force Level in Older Women" Applied Sciences 12, no. 4: 1812. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041812

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop