Next Article in Journal
“It’s Like Living with a Sassy Teenager!”: A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Owners’ Comments about Dogs between the Ages of 12 Weeks and 2 Years
Next Article in Special Issue
Effects of Two Environmental Enrichment Methods on Cognitive Ability and Growth Performance of Juvenile Black Rockfish Sebastes schlegelii
Previous Article in Journal
Dead Laying Hens Detection Using TIR-NIR-Depth Images and Deep Learning on a Commercial Farm
Previous Article in Special Issue
Differences in the Natural Swimming Behavior of Schizothorax prenanti Individual and Schooling in Spatially Heterogeneous Turbulent Flows
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Environmental Enrichment on the Behavior of Octopus vulgaris in a Recirculating Aquaculture System

1
Department of Veterinary Medical Sciences—DIMEVET, University of Bologna, Via Tolara di Sopra 50, Ozzano dell’Emilia, 40064 Bologna, Italy
2
School of Biosciences and Veterinary Medicine, University of Camerino, Via Circonvallazione 93/95, 62024 Matelica, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(11), 1862; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13111862
Submission received: 20 April 2023 / Revised: 15 May 2023 / Accepted: 1 June 2023 / Published: 2 June 2023

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Octopuses, like other cephalopods, have specific behaviors that correspond to a sequence of body patterns. Each pattern is the result of various components that, when performed simultaneously, have different outcomes. The vast repertoire in pattern production is associated with the complexity and variety of environmental enrichment. The greater the complexity of the environment, the greater the possibility of observing a wide variety of patterns. In this study, we evaluated how different environmental conditions affect subjects of Octopus vulgaris maintained in an aquaculture system through the observation of major body patterns. The results showed that octopuses kept in an enriched environment showed significantly more body patterns and gained significantly more weight than the subjects kept in a basic environment. The body patterns manifested by the octopuses maintained in a basic environment were similar to those exhibited under situations of hostility and inter/intra-specific conflict. They did not interact much with the surrounding habitat, the conspecifics, or the operator. Therefore, environmental enrichment is recommended for the individuals of this species that are kept in a recirculating aquaculture system (RAS).

Abstract

Octopus vulgaris is a commercially valuable species. It is overexploited in the natural environment and is considered to be an innovative species for aquaculture. However, large-scale farming is generally designed only based on economic requirements, disregarding any form of enrichment that induces the natural behavior of aquatic species. Although many studies have shown the influence of environmental enrichment on terrestrial vertebrates, fish, and cephalopod mollusks, information on the effect of environmental enrichment on the body patterns of O. vulgaris is limited. Therefore, in this study, we assessed how different environmental conditions (Basic vs. Enriched) affect sub-adults of O. vulgaris kept in recirculation systems, through qualitative–quantitative studies of the main body patterns and their potential application in the commercial production of this species. The results indicated that octopuses kept in the enriched environment showed several body patterns and gained a significantly higher weight than those kept in the basic environment. The body patterns displayed by the individuals kept in the basic environment were similar to those exhibited under situations of hostility and inter/intra-specific conflict. Hence, the environment of octopuses needs to be enriched, especially for the large-scale production of this species.

1. Introduction

The common octopus, Octopus vulgaris (Cuvier, 1797), is a commercially valuable species around the world [1]. Its demand in recent years has greatly increased as it is used in various culinary preparations [2]. Individuals are captured via industrial and artisanal fishing [3] and have been overexploited in recent decades. Therefore, some countries in the European Union [4,5] have introduced strict regulations, and many researchers have investigated aquaculture techniques as an alternative source of supply [1]. In recent decades, O. vulgaris has been considered an innovative species for aquaculture due to its short life cycle, high growth and fertility rate, favorable food conversion index, easy adaptation to captivity, and acceptance of food of low commercial value [6,7,8]. Today, octopuses are reared in sea cages and semi-open systems on an industrial scale in Galicia, and a few years ago the rearing cycle was successfully closed thanks to a protocol patented by the Spanish Institute of Oceanography [9,10].
Aquaculture-related production is continuously expanding due to an increase in the global demand for protein sources for human consumption [11,12]. However, the large-scale production of fish requires constantly monitored and controlled environments that, if neglected and poorly maintained, may compromise animal welfare and their survival [13,14]. Under farming conditions, the environments in which fish are generally maintained are bare and/or depleted, designed only based on economic requirements. Under such rearing conditions, the development of any favorable natural behavior of fish might be restricted [11,12,15].
Efforts to improve animal welfare and reduce the adverse effects on aquaculture are reflected in the European Directive 2010/63/EU, which also includes “live cephalopods” (nautiloids, cuttlefish, squid, and octopus) [16,17]. Improving the welfare of farmed fish (e.g., reducing stress) by applying effective management protocols can increase the productivity of aquaculture [12].
Environmental enrichment (EE) strongly affects the productivity of aquaculture. Enrichment improves the welfare of captive animals by providing access to important stimuli or by promoting activities and behavioral variety [18]. The concept of enrichment is broad and involves any technique that facilitates the biological functioning of a captive animal by modifying its environment, including the encouragement of natural behaviors [19]. Environmental enrichment can be divided into different categories, depending on the objectives to be achieved [20]. Specifically, it involves (a) physical enrichment, which includes modifications or additions to tanks, i.e., structural complexity; (b) cognitive enrichment, which involves the stimulation of sensory organs and the brain; (c) nutritional enrichment, which includes varying the type and administration method of food; (d) social enrichment, which involves contact and interactions with conspecifics; (e) occupational enrichment, which involves the reduction of physical and psychological monotony by introducing environmental variations and providing opportunities for exercising and performing the preferred behaviors [17,21,22]. Before providing enrichment, it is necessary to determine whether the animal for which it is designed requires it. Thus, understanding the physiological needs of the species, its behavioral repertoire, and sensory capabilities is necessary [20]. Additionally, the changes and/or improvements resulting from the enrichment [23] also need to be evaluated. Fish and cephalopods have dissimilar behaviors, and thus, they might need different quantities and types of enrichment. Therefore, studies on EE are constantly being updated [11,17].
Although many studies have shown the influence of EE on terrestrial vertebrates, for example, rats [24,25], chimpanzees [26], and black bears [27], and on fish, such as African catfish [28], black rockfish [29], zebrafish [30,31], chinook salmon [32], rainbow trout [33], and seabream [34] that are used in research and aquaculture, there are only a few studies that have evaluated the effects of different types of environmental enrichment on cephalopod mollusks. Among these studies, the species mainly investigated include Sepia officinalis [35,36], Sepia pharaonis [37,38], Octopus bimaculoides [39,40], Callistoctopus aspilosomatis [41], Enteropus octdofleini [42], and Octopus maya [43]; however, similar studies on the behavior of O. vulgaris are lacking. The focus of papers on this species has been more on individual learning [44,45], social behavior [46,47], feeding behavior [48,49], problem solving [50], and play and puzzle solving [51,52,53,54]. Octopuses, like other cephalopods, show specific behaviors that correspond to a sequence of body patterns. Each of these patterns results from various components (postural, locomotor, textural, and chromatic). When these patterns and behaviors occur simultaneously, they are used to camouflage themselves in the environment, communicate with conspecifics, intimidate potential predators, and procure food. The vast repertoire of produced patterns is related to the complexity and variety of environmental enrichment [55,56]. Thus, high environmental complexity corresponds to a greater number of behaviors [57,58]. Octopuses exhibit vision-dependent intelligent behaviors, such as spatial learning, associative learning, and observational learning [41,45,55]. These characteristics suggest that octopuses are visually and tactically influenced by their surroundings, and thus, EE can strongly influence cephalopods [41]. Specifically, EE induces adult neurogenesis in the learning and multisensory integration centers, increasing cell proliferation and synaptogenesis in O. vulgaris [59]. Hence, behavioral observations are an effective, non-invasive indicator of welfare and an early warning system in aquaculture [60,61].
In this study, we assessed how different conditions of environmental enrichment affect the behavior of O. vulgaris subjects maintained in RAS and their application in the commercial production of this species.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals

Wild sub-adults of O. vulgaris (≥700 g body weight—BW), suitable for human consumption, were caught at the end of winter 2022 by professional fishermen, using a traditional non-invasive capture system (“polpara”), in the Ionian Sea (Gallipoli, Italy). Each animal was placed in a PVC cylinder, which was netted to avoid aggression, and transported in an insulated tank (300 L) to the laboratory in Cesenatico, where they were classified by weight and sex. The weight of each animal was recorded using an electronic scale (model WLC 20/A2, ±0.1 g, RADWAG, Radom, Poland) and then the average weight was calculated for males and females. The males were confirmed by inspecting the hectocotylus.
In total, 12 subjects were selected: 6 M, 752.7 ± 39.5 g BW, and 6 F, 723.7 ± 275.4 g BW. The animals were divided by sex into two tanks (700 L) connected to a water recirculation system. The individuals were acclimatized for three days. In this system, the initial seawater temperature (15 ± 0.5 °C, salinity 35 psu) and photoperiod (10 h light:14 h dark) matched the octopus catch conditions. After acclimatization, six couples were formed and placed in the RAS (three couples/tank).

2.2. Recirculating Aquaculture System (RAS)

The RAS consisted of two identical rectangular tanks (3 × 0.62 × 0.50 m; total volume 2 m3), a protein skimmer (0.05 m3), a biological filter (0.21 m3), and a circulation pump (maximum flow rate: 16,000 L/h). The system was also equipped with a thermal control system, a UV-sterilizing lamp, an ozonator, and an aerator.
Each tank was modified to create two types of environments and adapted to house the individuals under study. Compartments were created in both tanks with a removable grid to separate the broodstock. This grid had an opening of 20 mm, which allowed interaction between individuals while safeguarding the territorial instincts and safety of the animals. Each animal had a space of 216 cm3 (72 × 50 × 60) [62]. The tanks were equipped with transparent glass covers to maintain natural light conditions and prevent the animals from escaping.

2.3. Experimental Design

To observe the effects of environmental enrichment, two different environments were set up:
-
Basic (BAS), consisting of a blue, factory-like environment, with only social enrichment (contact with conspecifics and operator) and food (ad libitum feeding and live food);
-
Enriched (ENR) environment, with the presence of physical enrichment: substrate (sand), wall color (the walls were “naturalized” with beige-colored polypropylene panels); cognitive enrichment: the presence of seashells, stones, and plastic toys; social enrichment (contact with conspecifics and the operator) and food (ad libitum feeding and live food).
In total, six individuals (3 M and 3 F) were placed in the two different environments for a total of six replicas in each condition. Each replica was placed in the tank and had its own artificial den.
For behavioral observations, we followed the body patterns and components reviewed by Borrelli et al. [63]. Observations were made for 4 h/day (9–11 a.m. and 3–5 p.m.), 5 days per week, and were performed by previously trained staff. For data collection, a special form was developed where the staff member could make daily records including the subject, the number of occurrences of behaviors, and add any additional comments. All daily observations were then transferred to the computer and processed as follows:
-
the total number of observations in the two environments;
-
the total number of observations in both sexes (regardless of the environment);
-
the number of weekly observations in the two environments;
-
the percentage of observations of various behaviors in each environment.
All octopuses underwent the conditioning program, which consisted of an increase in temperature of 1 °C/week until 20 °C and an increase in a photoperiod of 1 h/month up to 15 h light and 9 h dark [64]. The animals were fed ad libitum once a day with a mix of frozen (20%) and fresh (80%) fish and crustacean (40% Squilla mantis, 40% Carcinus sp., and 20% Boops boops) [64].

2.4. Growth Performance

All animals were weighed at the beginning (Wi, initial weight in g), every two weeks, and at the end of the experiment (Wf, final weight in g). The following indices were calculated:
Specific Growth Rate (SGR) (% BW/day) =
ln Wf ln Wi ÷ Days × 100
Absolute Growth Rate (AGR) (g/day) =
Wf Wi ÷ Days

2.5. Statistical Analyses

To test the behavioral differences between individuals in the two environments, PERMANOVA was conducted based on the environmental factor (two levels: Basic and Enriched) and the sex factor (two levels: male and female). To find differences between individuals from the two environments, the normality of the data was first checked by performing Shapiro–Wilk tests. Since the data did not follow a normal distribution, non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon tests) were conducted for each behavior, considering the environmental factor. A two-way ANOVA for all the individuals was conducted considering the factors (time and environment) and the variable weight. Another two-way ANOVA was performed on the variable number of behaviors considering the environmental condition and temporal factors. First, normality was determined by the Shapiro–Wilk test, and the homogeneity of variances was determined by Cochran’s test (p > 0.05). Tukey’s post hoc test was performed after the ANOVA.
Finally, the variables SGR and AGR were also tested by performing ANOVA, considering the environment as a factor. For both variables, preliminary tests were conducted to determine the normality and homogeneity of variances.
All ANOVA and Wilcoxon tests were performed using R (R Development Core Team; packages “GAD”, “ggplot”, “fmsb” 2018). The PERMANOVA test was performed using Past 4.10.

2.6. Ethics

All octopuses were handled following the regulations of the European Union concerning the protection of experimental animals (Dir. 2010/63/EU) and the regulations of the Ethics Committee of Bologna University (prot. ID 4459-17 February 2023).

3. Results

The study was conducted for six weeks, during which, 14 body patterns and 5 components were identified in the ENR tank, whereas 9 body patterns and 3 components were identified in the BAS tank (Table 1). Specifically, the environmental factor significantly (p < 0.001) influenced the total number of behavioral observations (body patterns and components) (311.6 ± 0.6 ENR vs. 210.4 ± 0.7 BAS) (Figure 1). The expression of body patterns was also significantly influenced by the sex factor (p < 0.05) (Figure 1). However, these variables were not influenced by the interaction between the sex factor and the environmental factor (p = 0.1967). The results of ANOVA showed that both factors significantly affected the number of observations (p < 0.001), but the interaction between the factors was not significant (Figure 2). The octopuses in the ENR tank exhibited a higher number of behaviors in the first week than the octopuses in the BAS tank, a trend that remained unchanged throughout the trial. In contrast, the octopuses in the BAS tank showed a steady decrease in the number of behaviors from the first week to the fifth week and then increased the number of behaviors in the last week of observations. From the results of Tukey’s test, among the time levels, the comparison between T1 and T5 and T3 and T4 was not significant. When comparing the two environments across time levels, all time intervals except T1 and T3 were significant. Within ENR, the comparison between T3 and T2 was significant (p < 0.05) (Figure 2).
Regarding the influence of the environment on each body pattern and manifested component, the Wilcoxon test showed significance for all recorded behaviors/components with p < 0.05 except for Uniform Brownish-Red (UBR), Uniform Light Gray (ULG), Longitudinal Stripes (LS), and Withdrawal maneuver (wm) (Figure 3).
In the BAS environment, the most prevalent patterns included ULG (19.7 ± 0.9), UBR (18.7 ± 1.2), URB (18.7 ± 1.1), CM (17 ± 0.8), and D (32 ± 1.9). Among the components, e (25.3 ± 1.4) and fes (29.7 ± 1.6) were the most prevalent (Figure 2).
In the ENR environment, the most prevalent patterns included GR (32.3 ± 1.1), GDB (24 ± 1), GLGB (22 ± 1.2), AR (19 ± 0.8), and FAR (38 ± 1.6). The most prevalent components included e (58 ± 0.4), cm (6 ± 0.6), and sw (16.3 ± 0.8) (Figure 2).
When the weight of the animals was analyzed without differentiation based on sex, the two-way ANOVA showed that the factors of environment and time significantly influenced (p < 0.001) the weight variable (Figure 2). The octopuses in the ENR environment gained significantly more weight at the end of the test than those in the BAS environment (1373.9 ± 62.3 g vs. 903.8 ± 42.5 g) (Table 2). The post hoc test for the BAS environment factor showed that all comparisons for the different time level intervals were significant except for those between T3 and T1 and T3 and T2. In the ENR environment, all comparisons across time levels intervals were highly significant (p < 0.01). In the comparison between the two levels of the environment in the various time levels, comparisons between T1 and T0 between ENR and BAS were not significant (Figure 4).
The results regarding SGR and AGR showed that for both indices, the environment had a statistically significant influence (p < 0.01). Significantly higher values (SGR = 51 ± 0.1% and AGR = 15.4 ± 1.2 g) were found in the ENR environment than in the BAS environment (0.5 ± 0.1% and 3.7 ± 0.6 g) (Table 2).

4. Discussion

Enrichment in aquaculture can be defined as the addition of new environmental stimuli to meet the physiological and behavioral needs of the farmed species. Tonkins et al. [35] and Arechavala-Lopez et al. [11] showed that several aquatic species, when maintained in enriched environments, can improve their behavior and, consequently, their welfare.
In this study, we found that in the common octopus kept in captivity, different environmental conditions can influence its behavior during the growing phase. We also found that enrichment conditions (ENR) positively affected its behavior and welfare. Under ENR conditions, the octopuses showed increased activity and interaction, both intra-specifically and inter-specifically and the most frequent behaviors were those that indicated a calm condition. Specifically, the welfare manifestation of ENR octopuses was reinforced by the high number of observations of body patterns related to ‘camouflage’ (GR, GDB, ULG, and GLGB) that, according to Hanlon and Messenger [55], Borrelli et al. [63], and Cowdry [65], is a key manifestation of octopus well-being.
Although the main body patterns associated with intraspecific and interspecific conflicts and/or threats (CM, EU, UBR, and URB) were recorded in both environments, there were substantial differences in the quantity and time spent performing these patterns. Specifically, in the ENR environment, they were exhibited for a short period, generally at the beginning of the trial, while in the BAS environment, they were exhibited almost constantly throughout the trial. In the BAS environment, the presence of social and dietary enrichment alone was not sufficient for ensuring a good fit of octopuses, although they are known to have a beneficial effect on the behavior of other aquatic species, especially those used in farming [11]. Some patterns had different values in two different environments; for example, D had a positive valence in the octopuses maintained in the ENR, and it was recorded only during resting, denning, and food consumption, while in the BAS, it was frequently exhibited to avoid contact with the surrounding environment. The greater difficulty in adapting to the BAS environment was also indicated by the locomotor component, which was abandoned by octopuses in the BAS by mid-trial. These individuals exhibited hostile behaviors toward conspecifics and external sources (operators), and their ability to change patterns during the entire trial was limited. The result regarding dynamic responses was unexpected. This is a disturbance behavior that occurs when the animal is in danger, unprepared, and outside its den [56]. This behavior was exhibited in a few individuals and to a limited extent (1.5%) only in ENR octopuses and in an incomplete form (Incomplete Dymantic, ID), i.e., when the animal was in the den. This behavior might be related to the “personality” of the animal, i.e., variations in behaviors between individuals determined by genetic and environmental factors [69,70]. A study on Octopus rubescens suggested that octopuses might have personalities like other animals [69]. Given that the ID pattern was infrequent, we hypothesized that this might be a fear reaction related to the initial mistrust of some individuals toward operators, which was gradually overcome. However, Yasumuro and Ikeda [41] studied C. aspilosomatis and found that this pattern was exhibited by octopuses in the bare tank with significantly greater frequency than in their counterparts who were maintained in the enriched environment.
The color of the tank associated with the depth, source of light, and clarity of the water affects the degree to which light is absorbed, reflected, diffused, and attenuated in the farming environment. Thus, in the case of O. vulgaris, a species that has excellent vision and continuously searches for camouflage in the environment [71], the color of the tank is an essential aspect of the aquaculture sector.
In this study, the blue background of the BAS tank may have been the cause of different behaviors among individuals. The important role of color in cephalopods was also highlighted by Okamoto et al. [72], who observed that adults of O. vulgaris and O. aegina preferred black, red, and orange tanks when offered a choice among seven different colors. McLean [73] also found that tank and background color of commercial breeding tanks, which are mainly produced in blue, black, or green, can affect the physiological and behavioral processes of teleosts, elasmobranchs, amphibians, and marine invertebrates, including cephalopods. Batzina et al. [74,75] and Batzina and Karakatsouli [76] showed that individuals of Sparus aurata (giltheads) maintained in a controlled environment preferred a blue substrate and showed less aggressive behavior. In contrast, O. niloticus and P. trituberculatus showed higher growth and better growth indices in blue-colored tanks [77,78]. The color of the tank has also been shown to influence reproduction rates; in fact, Volpato et al. [79] found a higher reproductive rate associated with increased excavation and construction of the nest in Nile tilapia maintained in tanks with a blue substrate.
Both physical enrichment (sand) and cognitive enrichment (objects of various kinds) improved the lives of the octopuses in the tank, considering that they exhibited the behaviors that are normally performed by this species in a natural environment using visual and tactile cues. Many studies have shown that the presence of a substrate, such as sand, can improve the welfare of aquatic species, especially those that interact with the bottom or live close to it [11,21].
In a study, S. officinalis showed better behavior when kept in tanks containing sand than in those without it [35]. Additionally, since cuttlefish perform sand digging for long durations until they attain sexual maturity [80], environmental limitations might affect their development and cognitive learning [55]. Flatfish also benefit from the presence of substrate in rearing tanks and exhibit increased resting behavior, fewer skin lesions, and increased growth [81,82].
Not only the presence of a sandy bottom but also the presence of objects, such as seashells, stones, and plastic toys, in the ENR tank played a positive role as cephalopods possess high cognitive abilities and need a continuous motor and cognitive stimulation [17,83]. This ability to interact with objects has been found in Octopus maya under laboratory conditions [43] and also in Octopus vulgaris in the wild [51]. Cuttlefish farmed in environments with objects, such as artificial algae and rocks, showed normal development of learning and memory, unlike those in unenriched environments [36]. Beigel and Boal [40] and Yasumuro and Ikeda [41] found that individuals of O. bimaculoides and C. aspilsomatis farmed in tanks with some objects/toys were more active and stimulated than those kept in tanks that lacked them. This correlation, important in aquaculture, has also been observed in other fish species. Zhang et al. [29] studied black rockfish, Ojelade et al. [28] studied African Catfish, Batzina and Karakatsouli [84] studied gilthead seabream, and Rosengren et al. [85] studied Atlantic Salmon and found a greater weight gain in fish exposed to physical enrichment conditions than those reared under sterile conditions.
Regarding zootechnical performance, ENR octopuses showed a greater weight gain than BAS ones. Additionally, the AGR and SGR values in the ENR tank showed three times higher values than the BAS tank, and these results correspond to those of other studies in which a suitable environment and a predominantly crustacean diet were found to promote octopus growth [64,86,87]. In contrast, octopuses kept in BAS tanks showed inappetence and rejection of food, which led to a decrease in their weight. These results are also supported by the different behaviors expressed by the octopuses., e.g., certain patterns such as the FAR (behavior performed during a prey attack), which was exhibited more in the ENR environment and was directed exclusively towards the administered food (live crustaceans), while in the BAS tank, it was used by the octopuses mainly as a hostile behavior towards conspecifics, in particular by males towards females, followed by the locomotor defensive component (removal maneuver). Low growth is a consequence of fatigue caused by living in a basic environment. Chronic difficulties have inhibitory effects on growth and energy metabolism in farm animals [88]. In a study conducted on S. pharaonis [38], individuals maintained in a bare environment lacking any enrichment showed lower growth and reduced behavioral abilities than those maintained in an enriched environment.
The limited number of body patterns associated with the onset of weight loss recorded in octopuses maintained in the BAS environment like an increase in denning (D), led to the decision to suspend the trial to ensure the welfare of the animals.
From the perspective of commercially farming this species, this study highlighted that O. vulgaris maintained in captivity needs adequate environmental enrichment. Breeding in a bare, noisy, and chromatically unsuitable environment, such as many recirculation systems (RAS), not only reduces the zootechnical performance of this species but also might endanger its survival. However, the issues related to the increased cost of environmental enrichment [22] and the increased sanitation issues that might occur in a closed-loop system due to the presence of extraneous elements and sandy substrates in the tank should not be ignored.

5. Conclusions

The results obtained in this work highlighted how octopuses, kept in RAS, require an enrichment as close as possible to the natural one (ENR). In the ENR environment, the octopuses exhibited significantly more patterns/components and behavioral observations demonstrating how an enriched environment improves social interactions and promotes greater weight gain than BAS subjects, an aspect not to be underestimated especially in view of the commercial breeding of the species.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C. and O.M.; methodology, A.C., A.E., P.E., L.G. (Laura Gentile), and R.B.; animal husbandry: A.C., R.B. and P.E.; formal analysis, A.C., L.G. (Laura Gentile) and A.E.; investigation, A.C., L.G. (Laura Gentile) and P.E.; resources, A.R., A.P. and O.M.; data curation, A.C., P.E., L.G. (Laura Gentile) and O.M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.C.; writing—review and editing, all authors; supervision, A.R., L.G. (Livio Galosi), A.P. and O.M.; funding acquisition, O.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by RFO 2020, J54I20000110005—Mordenti Oliviero.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All sampling and manipulation procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Bologna (ID 4459-17 February 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by “Economia del Mare” (Cesenatico, Italy). The authors are particularly grateful to Roberto Casali, for providing food for octopus feeding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Iglesias, J.; Fuentes, L.; Villanueva, R. Octopus vulgaris. Paralarval Culture. In Cephalopod Culture; Iglesias, J., Fuentes, L., Villanueva, R., Eds.; Springer Dordrecht: New York, NY, USA; Berlin, Germany, 2014; pp. 427–450. [Google Scholar]
  2. FAO. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture 2022—Towards Blue Transformation; FAO: Rome, Italy, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  3. Sauer, W.H.H.; Gleadall, I.G.; Downey-Breedt, N.; Doubleday, Z.; Gillespie, G.; Haimovici, M.; Ibáñez, C.M.; Katugin, O.N.; Leporati, S.; Lipinski, M.R.; et al. World octopus fisheries. Rev. Fish. Sci. Aquac. 2021, 29, 279–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lishchenko, F.; Perales-Raya, C.; Barret, C.; Oesterwind, D.; Power, A.M.; Larivain, A.; Laptikhovsky, V.; Karatza, A.; Badouvas, N.; Lishchenko, A.; et al. A review of recent studies on the life history and ecology of European cephalopods with emphasis on species with the greatest commercial fishery and culture potential. Fish. Res. 2021, 236, 105847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jereb, P.; Allcock, A.L.; Lefkaditou, E.; Piatkowski, U.; Hastie, L.C.; Pierce, G.J. Cephalopod biology and fisheries in Europe: II. Species Accounts. ICES Coop. Res. Rep. 2015, 325, 375. [Google Scholar]
  6. Vaz-Pires, P.; Seixas, P.; Barbosa, A. Aquaculture potential of the common octopus (Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797): A review. Aquaculture 2004, 238, 221–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Mangold, K. Food, feeding and growth in Cephalopods. Mem. Natl. Mus. Vict. 1983, 44, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Iglesias, J.; Sánchez, F.; Otero, J.; Moxica, C. Culture of octopus (Octopus vulgaris): Present knowledge, problems and perspectives. Cah. Opt. Méditerr. 2000, 47, 313–321. [Google Scholar]
  9. García, J.G.; Luaces, M.; Veiga, C.; Rey-Méndez, M. Farming costs and benefits, marketing details, investment risks: The case of Octopus vulgaris in Spain. In Cephalopod Culture; Iglesias, J., Fuentes, L., Villanueva, R., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands; New York, NY, USA; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 149–161. [Google Scholar]
  10. Vizcaíno, R.; Guardiola, F.A.; Prado-Alvarez, M.; Machado, M.; Costas, B.; Gestal, C. Functional and molecular immune responses in Octopus vulgaris skin mucus and haemolymph under stressful conditions. Aquac. Rep. 2023, 29, 101484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Arechavala-Lopez, P.; Cabrera-Álvarez, M.J.; Maia, C.M.; Saraiva, J.L. Environmental enrichment in fish aquaculture: A review of fundamental and practical aspects. Rev. Aquac. 2022, 14, 704–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Martos-Sitcha, J.A.; Mancera, J.M.; Prunet, P.; Magnoni, L.J. Editorial: Welfare and Stressors in Fish: Challenges Facing Aquaculture. Front. Physiol. 2020, 11, 162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Segner, H.; Reiser, S.; Ruane, N.; Rösch, R.; Steinhagen, D.; Vehanen, T. Welfare of Fishes in Aquaculture 2019; FAO: Budapest, Hungary, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ashley, P.J. Fish welfare: Current issues in aquaculture. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 104, 199–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Jones, N.A.R.; Webster, M.M.; Salvanes, A.G.V. Physical enrichment research for captive fish: Time to focus on the DETAILS. J. Fish Biol. 2021, 99, 704–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Sykes, A.V.; Galligioni, V.; Estefanell, J.; Hetherington, S.; Brocca, M.; Correia, J.; Ferreira, A.; Pieroni, E.M.; Fiorito, G. FELASA Working Group Report Capture and Transport of live cephalopods: Recommendations for scientific purposes. Lab. Anim. 2022. under review. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cooke, G.M.; Tonkins, B.M.; Mather, J.A. Care and Enrichment for Captive Cephalopods. In The Welfare of Invertebrate Animals; Carere, C., Mather, J., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 179–208. [Google Scholar]
  18. Wells, D.L. Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive animals: A review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 118, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Newberry, R.C. Environmental enrichment: Increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1195, 44, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Young, R.J. Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2003; p. 240. [Google Scholar]
  21. Näslund, J.; Johnsson, J.I. Environmental enrichment for fish in captive environments: Effects of physical structures and substrates. Fish Fish. 2014, 17, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gerber, B.; Stamer, A.; Stadtlander, T. Environmental enrichment and its effect on fish welfare: Review. Excellence Sustain. 2015, 74. [Google Scholar]
  23. Alligood, C.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Leighty, N.R. Applying behavior-analytic methodology to the science and practice of environmental enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Woodcock, E.A.; Richardson, R. Effects of environmental enrichment on rate of contextual processing and discriminative ability in adult rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2000, 73, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Leggio, M.G.; Mandolesi, L.; Federico, F.; Spirito, F.; Ricci, B.; Gelfo, F.; Petrosini, L. Environmental enrichment promotes improved spatial abilities and enhanced dendritic growth in the rat. Behav. Brain. Res. 2005, 163, 78–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Celli, M.L.; Tomonaga, M.; Udono, T.; Teramoto, M.; Nagano, K. Tool use task as environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzees. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 81, 171–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Deguchi, Y.; Tokunaga, M.; Yamamoto, A.; Takahashi, S.; Ono, Y.; Maruyama, M.; Kimura, K.; Tsujimoto, T.; Iwase, T. Seasonal variation of effects of environmental enrichments for captive Asiatic black bears (Ursus thibetanus). Anim. Behav. Manag. 2008, 44, 159–165. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ojelade, O.C.; Durosaro, S.O.; Akinde, A.O.; Abdulraheem, I.; Oladepo, M.B.; Sopein, C.A.; Bhadmus, A.S.; Olateju, M. Environmental enrichment improves the growth rate, behavioral and physiological response of juveniles of Clarias gariepinus under laboratory conditions. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 980364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Zhang, Z.; Fu, Y.; Zhao, H.; Zhang, X. Social enrichment affects fish growth and aggression depending on fish species: Applications for aquaculture. Front. Mar. Sci. 2022, 9, 1011780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. De Pasquale, C.; Kemerer, N.; White, N.; Yost, M.; Wolfkill, J.; Sturgill, J.; Li, X. The influence of an enriched environment in enhancing recognition memory in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 749746. [Google Scholar]
  31. Lee, C.J.; Paull, G.C.; Tyler, C.R. Effects of environmental enrichment on survivorship, growth, sex ratio and behaviour in laboratory maintained zebrafish Danio rerio. J. Fish. Biol. 2019, 94, 86–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Rosburg, A.J.; Fletcher, B.L.; Barnes, M.E.; Treft, C.E.; Bursell, B.R. Vertically suspended environmental enrichement structures improve the growth of juvenile landlocked fall chinook Salmon. Int. J. Innov. Stud. Aquat. Biol. Fish. 2019, 5, 17–24. [Google Scholar]
  33. Brunet, V.; Kleiber, A.; Patinote, A.; Sudan, P.L. Positive welfare effects of physical enrichments from the nature, functions and feeling-based approaches in farmed rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Aquaculture 2022, 550, 737825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Arechavala-Lopez, P.; Diaz-Gil, C.; Saraiva, J.L.; Moranta, D.; Castanheira, M.F.; Nuñez-Velázqueza, S.; Ledesma-Corvi, S.; Mora-Ruiz, M.R.; Grau, A. Effects of structural environmental enrichment on welfare of juvenile seabream (Sparus aurata). Aquacult. Rep. 2019, 15, 100224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tonkins, B.M.; Tyers, A.M.; Cooke, G.M. Cuttlefish in captivity: An investigation into housing and husbandry for improving welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 168, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Dickel, L.; Boal, J.G.; Budelmann, B.U. The effect of early experience on learning and memory in cuttlefish. Dev. Psychobiol. 2000, 36, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Yasumuro, H.; Ikeda, Y. Environmental enrichment affects the ontogeny of learning, memory, and depth perception of the pharaoh cuttlefish Sepia pharaonis. Zool. 2018, 128, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Yasumuro, H.; Ikeda, Y. Environmental enrichment accelerates the ontogeny of cryptic behavior in pharaoh cuttlefish (Sepia pharaonis). Zool. Sci. 2016, 33, 255–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Boal, J.G. Behavioural research methods for octopuses and cuttlefishes. Vie Milieu 2011, 61, 203–210. [Google Scholar]
  40. Beigel, M.; Boal, J.G. The effect of habitat enrichment on the mudflat octopus. Shape Enrich. 2006, 15, 3–6. [Google Scholar]
  41. Yasumuro, H.; Ikeda, Y. Effects of environmental enrichment on the behavior of the tropical octopus Callistoctopus aspilosomatis. Mar. Freshwater. Behav. Physiol. 2011, 44, 143–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Anderson, R.C.; Wood, J.B. Enrichment for giant Pacific octopuses: Happy as a clam? J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2001, 4, 57–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Vergara-Ovalle, F.; Ayala-Guerrero, F.; Rosas, C.; Sanchez-Castillo, H. Novel object recognition in Octopus maya. Anim. Cognit. 2023, 26, 1065–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Borrelli, L.; Fiorito, G. Behavioral analysis of learning and memory in cephalopods. In Learning Theory and Behaviour; Byrne, J.H., Ed.; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 605–628. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fiorito, G.; Scotto, P. Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science 1992, 256, 545–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Tricarico, E.; Borrelli, L.; Gherardi, F.; Fiorito, G. I know my neighbour: Individual recognition in Octopus vulgaris. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Borrelli, L. Testing the Contribution of Relative Brain Size and Learning Capabilities on the Evolution of Octopus vulgaris and Other Cephalopods. Ph.D. Thesis, Open University, London, UK.
  48. Fiorito, G.; Gherardi, F. Prey-handling behaviour of Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca, Cephalopoda) on bivalve preys. Behav. Process. 1999, 46, 75–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Nixon, M.; Maconnachie, E. Drilling by Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) in the Mediterranean. J. Zool. Lond. 1988, 216, 687–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Fiorito, G.; von Planta, C.; Scotto, P. Problem solving ability in Octopus vulgaris Lam. (Mollusca, Cephalopoda). Behav. Neural Biol. 1990, 53, 217–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. O’brien, C.E.; Di Miccoli, V.; Fiorito, G. A preliminary investigation of the response of Octopus vulgaris to experimental stimuli in the wild. J. Molluscan Stud. 2021, 87, eyab032. [Google Scholar]
  52. Richter, J.N.; Hochner, B.; Kuba, M.J. Pull or push? Octopuses solve a puzzle problem. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0152048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Kuba, M.J.; Meisel, D.V.; Byrne, R.A.; Griebel, U.; Mather, J.A. Looking at play in Octopus vulgaris. Berliner Palaontol. Abh. 2003, 19, 426–438. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kuba, M.J.; Byrne, R.A.; Meisel, D.V.; Mather, J.A. When do Octopus play? Effects of repeated testing, object type, age and food deprivation on object play in Octopus vulgaris. J. Comp. Psychol. 2006, 120, 184–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Hanlon, R.T.; Messenger, J.B. Cephalopod Behaviour, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2018; p. 376. [Google Scholar]
  56. Packard, A.; Sanders, G.D. Body patterns of Octopus vulgaris and maturation of the response to disturbance. Anim. Behav. 1971, 19, 780–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ikeda, Y. Color Change in Cephalopods. In Pigments, Pigment Cells and Pigment Patterns; Hashimoto, H., Goda, M., Futahashi, R., Kelsh, R., Akiyama, T., Eds.; Springer: Singapore, 2021; pp. 425–449. [Google Scholar]
  58. Boyle, P.; Rodhouse, P. Physiological Ecology. In Cephalopods: Ecology and fisheries; Boyle, P., Rodhouse, P., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: Ames, IA, USA, 2005; p. 452. [Google Scholar]
  59. Bertapelle, C.; Polese, G.; Di Cosmo, A. Enriched environment increases PCNA and PARP1 levels in Octopus vulgaris central nervous system: First evidence of adult neurogenesis in lophotrochozoan. J. Exp. Zool. 2017, 328, 347–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Martins, C.I.; Galhardo, L.; Noble, C.; Damsgård, B.; Spedicato, M.T.; Zupa, W.; Beauchaud, M.; Kulczykowska, E.; Massabuau, J.C.; Carter, T.; et al. Behavioural indicators of welfare in farmed fish. Fish Physiol. Biochem. 2012, 38, 17–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Dawkins, M.S. Behaviour as a tool in the assessment of animal welfare. Zool 2003, 106, 383–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fiorito, G.; Affuso, A.; Basil, J.; Cole, A.; De Girolamo, P.; D’Angelo, L.; Dickel, L.; Gestal, C.; Grasso, F.; Kuba, M.; et al. Guidelines for the Care and Welfare of Cephalopods in Research—A consensus based on an initiative by CephRes, FELASA and the Boyd Group. Lab. Anim. 2015, 49, 1–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Borrelli, L.; Gherardi, F.; Fiorito, G. A Catalogue of Body Patterning in Cephalopoda; Firenze Univerity Press, Stazione Zoologica A. Dohrn: Firenze, Italy, 2006; p. 622. [Google Scholar]
  64. Casalini, A.; Roncarati, A.; Emmanuele, P.; Guercilena, N.; Bonaldo, A.; Parma, L.; Mordenti, O. Evaluation of reproductive performances of the common octopus (Octopus vulgaris) reared in water recirculation systems and fed different diets. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 15261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Cowdry, E.V. The Colour Changes of Octopus Vulgaris Lmk; University Library, The Librarian: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1911; p. 53. [Google Scholar]
  66. Packard, A.; Sanders, G.D. What the octopus shows to the world. Endeavour 1969, 28, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  67. Messenger, J.B. Cephalopod chromatophores: Neurobiology and natural history. Biol. Rev. 2001, 76, 473–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Packard, A. The behaviour of Octopus vulgaris. Bull. Inst. Oceanogr. 1963, 1, 35–49. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mather, J.A.; Anderson, R.C. Personalities of octopuses (Octopus rubescens). J. Comp. Psychol. 1993, 107, 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Carere, C.; Grignani, G.; Bonanni, R.; Della Gala, M.; Carlini, A.; Angeletti, D.; Mather, J.A. Consistent individual differences in the behavioural responsiveness of adult male cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 167, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. McLean, E. Fish tank color: An overview. Aquaculture 2021, 530, 735–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Okamoto, M.; Anraku, K.; Kawamura, G.; Tanaka, Y. Selectivity of color of shelter by Octopus vulgaris and O. aegina under different background colors. Nippon Suisan Gakkaishi 2001, 67, 672–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. McLean, E. Background color and cultured invertebrates–A review. Aquaculture 2021, 537, 736523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Batzina, A.; Kalogiannis, D.; Dalla, C.; Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z.; Chadio, S.; Karakat-souli, N. Blue substrate modifies the time course of stress response ingilthead seabream Sparus aurata. Aquaculture 2014, 420, 247–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Batzina, A.; Dalla, C.; Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z.; Karakatsouli, N. Effects of environmental enrichment on growth, aggressive behaviour and brain monoamines of gilthead seabream Sparus aurata reared under different social conditions. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 2014, 169, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Batzina, A.; Karakatsouli, N. Is it the blue gravel substrate or only its blue color that improves growth and reduces aggressive behavior of gilthead seabream Sparus aurata? Aquac. Eng. 2014, 62, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Opiyo, M.A.; Ngugi, C.C.; Rasowo, J. Combined effects of stocking density and background colour on growth performance and survival of Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus, L.) fry reared in aquaria. J. Fish. Sci. 2014, 8, 228–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wang, J.; Peng, K.; Lu, H.; Li, R.; Song, W.; Liu, L.; Wang, H.; Wang, C.; Shi, C. The effect of tank colour on growth performance, stress response and carapace colour of juvenile swimming crab Portunus trituberculatus. Aquac. Res. 2019, 50, 2735–2742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Volpato, G.L.; Duarte, C.R.A.; Luchiari, A.C. Environmental color affects Nile tilapia reproduction. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 2004, 37, 479–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Mather, J.A. Sand digging in Sepia officinalis: Assessment of a Cephalopod mollusc’s “fixed” behavior pattern. J. Comp. Psychol. 1986, 100, 315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Kang, D.Y.; Kim, H.C.; Myeong, J.I.; Min, B.H. Effect of infiltrating substrate on growth and double-sided pigmentation of flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus) fry. KOSFAS 2014, 47, 406–412. [Google Scholar]
  82. Ottesen, O.H.; Noga, E.J.; Sandaa, W. Effect of substrate on progression and healing of skin erosions and epidermal papillomas of Atlantic halibut, Hippoglossus hippoglossus (L.). J. Fish Dis. 2007, 30, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Mather, J.A.; Dickel, L. Cephalopod complex cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017, 16, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Batzina, A.; Karakatsouli, N. The presence of substrate as a means of environmental enrichment in intensively reared gilthead seabream Sparus aurata: Growth and behavioral effects. Aquaculture 2012, 370, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Rosengren, M.; Kvingedal, E.; Näslund, J.; Johnsson, J.I.; Sundell, K. Born to be wild: Effects of rearing density and environmental enrichment on stress, welfare, and smolt migration in hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 2016, 74, 396–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Sánchez, F.J.; Valverde, C.J.; Garcìa, B. Octopus vulgaris: Ongrowing. In Cephalopod Culture; Iglesias, J., Fuentes, L., Villanueva, R., Eds.; Springer Dordrecht: New York, NY, USA; Berlin, Germany, 2014; pp. 451–466. [Google Scholar]
  87. Rodriguez-González, T.; Valverde, J.C.; Sykes, A.V.; García, B.J. Performance of raw material thermal treatment on formulated feeds for common octopus (Octopus vulgaris) ongrowing. Aquaculture 2015, 442, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Etim, N.N.; Williams, M.E.; Evans, E.I.; Offiong, E.E. Physiological and behavioural responses of farm animals to stress: Implications to animal productivity. Am. J. Adv. Agric. Res. 2013, 1, 53–61. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. The effect of environment and sex on the total number of observations. The data are shown as mean ± standard deviation. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences between the environments (Basic vs. Enriched) and sex (male vs. female) (PERMANOVA; *** p < 0.01 and * p < 0.05).
Figure 1. The effect of environment and sex on the total number of observations. The data are shown as mean ± standard deviation. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences between the environments (Basic vs. Enriched) and sex (male vs. female) (PERMANOVA; *** p < 0.01 and * p < 0.05).
Animals 13 01862 g001
Figure 2. Boxplot showing the trend in the total number of behaviors in the Enriched (green) and Basic (blue) environments at various time intervals. Significant differences between the environmental conditions at each experimental time are marked by dissimilar letters (a, b, c, d, e, f, and g) (p < 0.05).
Figure 2. Boxplot showing the trend in the total number of behaviors in the Enriched (green) and Basic (blue) environments at various time intervals. Significant differences between the environmental conditions at each experimental time are marked by dissimilar letters (a, b, c, d, e, f, and g) (p < 0.05).
Animals 13 01862 g002
Figure 3. A radar chart representing total body patterns and components exhibited in the two environments (Basic and Enriched); p < 0.05 for all behaviors/components exhibited, except for UBR, ULG, LS, and wm.
Figure 3. A radar chart representing total body patterns and components exhibited in the two environments (Basic and Enriched); p < 0.05 for all behaviors/components exhibited, except for UBR, ULG, LS, and wm.
Animals 13 01862 g003
Figure 4. Boxplot showing the weight gain of octopuses in the Enriched (green) and Basic (blue) environments at various time intervals. Significant differences among environmental conditions at each experimental time point are shown using dissimilar letters (a, b, c, d, and e) (p < 0.05).
Figure 4. Boxplot showing the weight gain of octopuses in the Enriched (green) and Basic (blue) environments at various time intervals. Significant differences among environmental conditions at each experimental time point are shown using dissimilar letters (a, b, c, d, and e) (p < 0.05).
Animals 13 01862 g004
Table 1. Body patterns and components identified in different environments (Basic and Enriched). The frequency of observation is reported as a percentage. The means presence, means absence.
Table 1. Body patterns and components identified in different environments (Basic and Enriched). The frequency of observation is reported as a percentage. The means presence, means absence.
NBody PatternMeaningBASENRBAS%ENR%References
1Conflict Mottle—CMDisturbance11.33.4[56,65]
2Unilateral Effect—UEDisturbance11.53.4[56]
3Uniform Brownish- Red—UBRIntraspecific encounters, disturbance12.48.2[65]
4Uniform Reddish- Brown—URBSocial interactions, disturbance12.43.9[65]
5Full Attack Response—FARFeeding behavior7.916.9[63]
6Denning—DRest, feeding, disturbance21.27.1[55]
7Uniform Light Gray—ULGCamouflage13.07.4[65]
8Ground Light Grayish- brown—GLGBCamouflage, rest6.09.8[65]
9Longitudinal Stripes—LSIntraspecific interactions, disturbance4.3-[56,65]
10Fighting—FIntraspecific interactions-1.2[63]
11Incomplete Dymantic—IDDisturbance-1.5[56]
12Broad Conflict Mottle—BCMDisturbance-3.6[66]
13Acute Resemblance—ARCamouflage-8.5[67]
14Ground Dark Brown—GDBCamouflage, rest-10.7[65]
15General Resemblance—GRCamouflage-14.4[63,67]
NComponentsMeaningBASENRBAS%ENR%References
1Envelope—ePrey capture42.766.4[63]
2Withdrawal Manoeuvre—wmDefensive posture7.32.3[56]
3Funnel Directed Toward External Stimulus—fesReaction against the disturbance50-[56]
4Cleaning Manoeuvre—cmRapid twirling of the arms-6.9[56]
5Arms Raised—arPostural component-5.7[68]
6Swimming—swLocomotor component-18.7[66]
Table 2. The growth trend of O. vulgaris subjects in the Enriched and Basic tanks. Shown below are the initial weight (Wi, g), final weight (Wf, g), Specific Growth Rate (SGR, %), and Absolute Growth Rate (AGR, g/d). The data are presented as the mean ± S.D. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences between the environments (** p < 0.01).
Table 2. The growth trend of O. vulgaris subjects in the Enriched and Basic tanks. Shown below are the initial weight (Wi, g), final weight (Wf, g), Specific Growth Rate (SGR, %), and Absolute Growth Rate (AGR, g/d). The data are presented as the mean ± S.D. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences between the environments (** p < 0.01).
AnimalWi (g)Wf (g)SGR (%)AGR (g/d)
ENREF1731.71354.81.4714.8
EF2689.31270.81.4613.8
EF3764.11371.41.3914.5
Mean F728.4 ± 37.51332. 3 ± 53.91.44 ± 0.0414.4 ± 0.5
EM1705.41374.11.5915.9
EM2758.81418.41.4915.7
EM3725.71454.21.6517.3
Mean M730 ± 271415.6 ± 40.11.58 ± 0.116.3 ± 0.9
MEAN ENR729.2 ± 29.21373.9 ± 62.3 **1.51 ± 0.1 **15.4 ± 1.2 **
BASPF1728.7911.40.534.4
PF2675.6862.70.584.5
PF3752.8907.10.443.7
Mean F719 ± 39.5893.7 ± 270.52 ± 0.14.2 ± 0.4
PM1778.3921.40.403.4
PM2814.2968.90.413.7
PM3733.6851.40.352.8
Mean M775.4 ± 40.4913.9 ± 59.10.39 ± 0.033.3 ± 0.4
MEAN BAS747.2 ± 47.2903.8 ± 42.50.5 ± 0.13.7 ± 0.6
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Casalini, A.; Gentile, L.; Emmanuele, P.; Brusa, R.; Elmi, A.; Parmeggiani, A.; Galosi, L.; Roncarati, A.; Mordenti, O. Effects of Environmental Enrichment on the Behavior of Octopus vulgaris in a Recirculating Aquaculture System. Animals 2023, 13, 1862. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13111862

AMA Style

Casalini A, Gentile L, Emmanuele P, Brusa R, Elmi A, Parmeggiani A, Galosi L, Roncarati A, Mordenti O. Effects of Environmental Enrichment on the Behavior of Octopus vulgaris in a Recirculating Aquaculture System. Animals. 2023; 13(11):1862. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13111862

Chicago/Turabian Style

Casalini, Antonio, Laura Gentile, Pietro Emmanuele, Riccardo Brusa, Alberto Elmi, Albamaria Parmeggiani, Livio Galosi, Alessandra Roncarati, and Oliviero Mordenti. 2023. "Effects of Environmental Enrichment on the Behavior of Octopus vulgaris in a Recirculating Aquaculture System" Animals 13, no. 11: 1862. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13111862

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop