Next Article in Journal
Alterations in Healthy Adult Canine Faecal Microbiome and Selected Metabolites as a Result of Feeding a Commercial Complete Synbiotic Diet with Enterococcus faecium NCIMB 10415
Next Article in Special Issue
Stress Evaluation of Mouse Husbandry Environments for Improving Laboratory Animal Welfare
Previous Article in Journal
miR-27a Targeting PIK3R3 Regulates the Proliferation and Apoptosis of Sheep Hair Follicle Stem Cells
Previous Article in Special Issue
Clicker Training Mice for Improved Compliance in the Catwalk Test
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

Aggression in Group-Housed Male Mice: A Systematic Review

1
Department of Animal Environment and Health, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, 532 23 Skara, Sweden
2
Department of Animal Environment and Health, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, 750 07 Uppsala, Sweden
3
Department of Animal Science and Technology, AstraZeneca, 431 83 Mölndal, Sweden
4
Swedish 3Rs Center, Swedish Board of Agriculture, 553 29 Jönköping, Sweden
5
Swedish National Committee for the Protection of Animals Used for Scientific Purposes, Swedish Board of Agriculture, 553 29 Jönköping, Sweden
6
Department of Animal Health and Antimicrobial Strategies, National Veterinary Institute (SVA), 751 89 Uppsala, Sweden
7
Institute of Environmental Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Animals 2023, 13(1), 143; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010143
Submission received: 14 November 2022 / Revised: 23 December 2022 / Accepted: 24 December 2022 / Published: 30 December 2022

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

When male mice are kept in groups at animal facilities, aggressive interactions between cage mates are not uncommon. Systematically reviewing previous studies that explored the cause of male mice aggression, we found that studies were disparate, using several different strains, a diverse set of environmental enrichments and different ways of grouping and housing mice, as well as different ways to observe aggression. Understanding the cause of male mice aggression is difficult when researchers use different methods and study designs. Nevertheless, our results suggest that home cage aggression is best studied in home cage environments and not by introducing unfamiliar mice to each other in a novel environment. In addition, while we were able to provide recommendations on how to minimize aggression, our assessment was that there is no universal solution that could be used by all animal facilities. Instead, it is important to realize that aggression is complex and that animal facilities might have to try different possible solutions to find what works best under their specific conditions.

Abstract

Aggression among group-housed male mice is a major animal welfare concern often observed at animal facilities. Studies designed to understand the causes of male mice aggression have used different methodological approaches and have been heterogeneous, using different strains, environmental enrichments, housing conditions, group formations and durations. By conducting a systematic literature review based on 198 observed conclusions from 90 articles, we showed that the methodological approach used to study aggression was relevant for the outcome and suggested that home cage observations were better when studying home cage aggression than tests provoking aggression outside the home cage. The study further revealed that aggression is a complex problem; one solution will not be appropriate for all animal facilities and all research projects. Recommendations were provided on promising tools to minimize aggression, based on the results, which included what type of environmental enrichments could be appropriate and which strains of male mice were less likely to be aggressive.

1. Introduction

In recent years, several publications have addressed the challenges of keeping male mice in groups (e.g., [1,2,3,4,5]). Mice are social animals and, according to current legislation and guidelines [6,7,8], mice should be group housed when used in research. However, aggression between male cage mates is one of the main problems in laboratory mouse husbandry, affecting both animal welfare and scientific quality [2].
Some general recommendations on how to prevent and minimize aggression have been listed [3,4,9,10]. These include keeping siblings together or grouping familiar mice before sexual maturity, housing male mice in small groups, transferring nesting material at cage cleaning, avoiding disturbances, handling mice with care and using strains with low level of aggression. However, despite being housed according to the general recommendations, mice sometimes fight when kept in groups, illustrating the complexity of the problem.
Aggression in group-housed male mice has been studied using several different methodological approaches [2]. One such approach is to observe undisturbed groups of mice in their home cages; these are usually referred to as home cage observations. There are also a number of different test protocols used to measure aggression and social dominance, such as the resident intruder test [11], the social defeat test [12], the novel arena social encounter test [13] and the tube test [14]. These types of tests mainly focus on territorial aggression and are designed to induce stress, emotional conflict and frustration, with aggression as a readout [2]. They are commonly used in studies where mice are used as animal models to study aggression, social defeat, deficits in social interaction or mood-related disorders (e.g., [12,15]). In these test situations, mice are removed from the social context in their home cage, placed in a novel cage or test arena, and often exposed to unfamiliar conspecifics in different staged encounters. Another way of measuring aggression is to score wounds on the body of the mice, continuously throughout the study and/or after the mice have been euthanized [16]. Sometimes a combination of home cage observations, different test protocols and wound scoring is used.
Aside from differences in methodology, there is a wide variation in different treatments used to study the effects on aggression, such as group composition, differences between strains, handling and cleaning routines, housing conditions and access to environmental enrichment. Environmental enrichment is used to enhance animal welfare by enabling animals to perform positive natural behaviors and increase their ability to gain certain control over their environment [17]. Transferring nesting material at cage cleaning has been shown to decrease aggression [9]. However, the effects of different structural enrichment items on aggression in male mice remain unclear [17]. For example, different studies using shelter as enrichment report both increased aggression [18,19] and no effect on aggression [20,21,22], while combinations of enrichment items with shelter included have been reported to decrease aggression [23,24].
In general, few experimental studies have addressed the problem of home cage aggression under normal husbandry conditions [5]. This might be another reason why results from experimental studies can be difficult to interpret and implement to daily practice in the lab.
To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review on aggression in group-housed male laboratory mice. In this dataset, data were systematically collected from articles investigating this subject. We also included articles that did not have aggression as the primary outcome, but rather as an additional finding. These articles were selected because they could contain valuable information and might not have been included in previous reviews with a focus on animal welfare. Our aim was to map how the literature in the field support, or do not support, available recommendations on how to prevent aggression in group-housed male mice, and to detect knowledge gaps that ought to be filled. We also wanted to address and describe how aggression has been measured in the literature, since this could influence the possibility of translating outcomes to normal husbandry conditions and contribute to useful recommendations.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Literature Search

We conducted a systematic literature search on the 12th of December 2018 using the online databases Medline, Embase and Web of Science. The search included words in titles, abstracts, author keywords and keywords of scientific publications (henceforth referred to as records) and was performed via the University Library at Karolinska Institutet, Sweden. We used the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses, PRISMA 2020, statement [25]. Details on the search strings are available in the Supplementary Information. Details of the systematic review protocol have also been registered in the International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols (INPLASY) with registration number INPLASY2022120078.
Additional articles from reference lists of three relevant and recent literature reviews [1,2,9] were also included. Duplicate records were removed so that each reference was only represented once.

2.2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The records were screened in a two-step procedure (Figure 1 and Figure S1). In the first step, titles and abstracts were screened to identify empirical studies on group- or single-housed male mice, investigating aggression, social dominance, wounds, stereotypies, stress, physiological parameters or details on husbandry, such as group size, cage cleaning procedures or enrichment. All records that matched these criteria were preliminarily included and the reports were assessed for eligibility in full text following set inclusion and exclusion criteria (Supplementary Figure S1). To be included the article had to: (i) be available as full text and written in English; (ii) investigate the effects of group housing of male mice (or male mice and castrated male/female mice); (iii) investigate aggression or social dominance. Studies that did not have a specific aim to measure aggression but still drew conclusions about aggression were included. Studies of wild mice or other rodents than mice were excluded.

2.3. Data Extraction

The methodology, treatment, outcome of experiments and additional relevant information were extracted from the included articles. Each experimental outcome will hereafter be referred to as an observation.
Sometimes, more than one observation was extracted from the same article, e.g., if the same study explored how enrichment affected aggression in two different strains, or if one study investigated the effect of both group size and kinship.
The 90 included articles were divided between two authors, who extracted information independently. The data were then verified by the author who had not performed the data extraction. The complete dataset, with all extracted information, is present in Supplementary Table S1.

2.4. Data Analysis

The author’s original conclusions were extracted from the articles and summarized and have in no way been reanalyzed.
The methods used to assess aggression were divided into four categories: home cage observations, test for aggression, wounding and general observation. General observation refers to studies where no specific method could be identified, but the author had made a comment about aggression in the specific experiment.
The treatments used to investigate the effects on aggression were categorized as follows: strain (different strains, substrains or genetically modified strains), enrichment (enriched/non-enriched conditions or different enrichment items used, as well as cage complexity and resource distribution), time spent in group (amount of time spent in a group), group formation (group size, sibling/non-sibling, communal nesting, weaning age, age at group formation, group composition and applying changes to the group composition), housing condition (cage size, density, cage setup, cleaning procedure, female mice close to cage, lightning paradigm, location, scent and identification method) and other (castration, castrated male or female partner, brain weight and general observations where no specific treatment had been applied).
The enrichment items used were first categorized into five major types, related to which behavioral needs they fulfilled: social (contact, non-contact), occupational (psychological, exercise), physical (cage, accessories), sensory (visual, auditory, other stimuli) and nutritional (delivery, type) [26]. To handle the large variation in enrichment items used and facilitate comparisons within these groups, the enrichments used were then further categorized into the following eight subgroups, according to the supposed purpose of the item: shelter, feed enrichment, nesting material, climbing structure, hiding device, locomotor enrichment, gnawing device and other (Supplementary Tables S3 for categorization and S4 for references). Shelter included items specifically designed to function as a shelter (e.g., nest box), whereas hiding device were structures that mice could hide behind, in or under, but not specifically designed for use as a shelter (e.g., tube).

3. Results

3.1. Search and Study Selection

The database literature search revealed 1420 potentially relevant articles. The reference lists of three recent literature reviews [1,2,9] added another 316, for a total of 1736 articles. After removal of duplicates, 1062 abstracts were screened and 605 articles were preliminarily included. In the full text screening, 90 articles were identified as suitable for inclusion in the systematic review (Figure 1).

3.2. Description of Data Set

In total, 198 observations were extracted from the 90 articles (Supplementary Table S1). Each observation corresponded to the outcome of an experiment with respect to the effect on aggression in group-housed male mice. In most cases, the observations referred to a specific strain and a specific treatment used to affect aggression. Thus, one article, and even one experiment, could include several observations (Table 1). Therefore, comparisons of the outcome on aggression had to be performed among the observations and not between articles.
Data extracted from the articles were divided into different categories based on different treatments used to study the effect on aggression (Table 1 and Table S2). The most common treatment used to study effects on aggression was enrichment, followed by time spent in group, strain and factors related to group formation such as group size and kinship of groups. Details of the housing conditions, such as cage size and cleaning procedure, were less commonly studied. Thirty-one articles contained observations from more than one treatment category.

3.3. Different Methodological Approaches Used to Study Aggression

The methodological approaches used to study aggression can be divided into three categories: home cage observations, test protocols to measure aggression and scoring of wounds. Home cage observations were most common, alone or in combination with other methodological approaches (Figure 2a, Supplementary Table S2). Nineteen articles used methodological approaches from more than one category e.g., home cage observations and wound scoring.
The use of different methodological approaches has changed over time (Figure 2b). Home cage observations, alone or in combination with other approaches, have increased in publications after 2001 as well as the practice of using more than one type of method to study aggression. Contrarily, the use of tests to measure aggression as a single methodological approach has decreased.
When using home cage observations as a single approach to study aggression, observations showed decreased aggression, increased aggression and no effect on aggression (Figure 2c). On the other hand, when using tests to measure aggression outside the home cage and wound scoring, as single approaches to study aggression, decreased aggression was seldom observed, while observations of increased aggression or no effect were more commonly reported (Figure 2c). In the sections describing the effects of different treatments below, the methodological approaches used are not described for each observation. Details can be found in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.

3.4. Details and Description of Housing and Husbandry

Details on cage interiors were included in 70% of the articles published before 2011 and in 80% of the articles published after 2011 (Figure 3). During the period 2001–2018, two-thirds of the articles included information on cleaning routines, compared with only one third of the articles published before 2001. On the contrary, information about kinship—whether groups were put together with male mice from different litters or with siblings—were included in a larger proportion of the publications before 2011, compared with articles published between 2011–2018 (from 75–78% before 2011 to 68% during 2011–2018; Figure 3).

3.5. The Effect of Enrichment on Aggression

In many of the articles included in this systematic review, mice were housed in cages with enrichment (Supplementary Table S1). However, the results presented in this section only include papers that specifically studied the effects of enrichment on aggression. In these studies, the standard cages may have had a baseline level of enrichment (not only bedding). Thus, the treatment was defined as additional items added to the enriched cages.
Enrichment was the most commonly used treatment to study effects on aggression (Table 1). More than 40 different enrichment items were used in the different articles (Supplementary Table S3) and different types of enrichment items were often studied together (Supplementary Table S4). The majority of all enrichment items used fell under the major type physical enrichment (38 items in total), including the subgroups nesting material (5 items, 18 observations), shelter (6, 18), hiding device (10, 23), climbing structure (9, 13), locomotor enrichment (1, 10) and other (7, 11). The remaining belonged to nutritional enrichments (4 items in total), including the subgroups feed enrichment (2, 9) and gnawing device (2, 2). Of the eight subgroups, hiding device was the enrichment category that most frequently resulted in decreased aggression (Figure 4). This category also had the highest number of observations, some with only one enrichment item and others where the hiding device was tested in combination with other items.
From all articles that studied enrichment, there were 13 observations of decreased aggression [21,23,24,27,28,29,30,31], 32 where the enrichment did not have any effect [20,21,22,23,29,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41] and 15 observations of increased aggression [18,19,22,32,34,37,42,43,44,45,46,47]. In addition, there were 4 observations with different outcomes depending on the method used to measure aggression [48,49,50,51] (Supplementary Table S4). More than half of the observations of decreased aggression were based on groups formed from non-sibling mice, or substrains of BALB/c mice. Non-sibling groups and groups with BALB/c mice were commonly used also in the observations reporting no effect on aggression. Conversely, sibling groups were more common among the observations of increased aggression and all observations except two were based on other strains than BALB/c. When looking at experiments that investigated the effect of enrichment in C57BL/6, no effect on aggression was most often observed. In one case there was a decrease in aggression, but increased aggression was never observed (Supplementary Table S4).
Cage complexity and resource distribution have also been studied [29,52]. Nadiah et al. (2014) used two connected cages and found that distributing a combination of enrichment items over both cages reduced aggression compared to putting all enrichment items in one cage [29]. Bergmann et al. (1995) studied an enriched setting with one or nine passages to the fodder rack. The enriched condition resulted in an increased proportion of wounded mice and the setting with only one passage to the fodder rack had the highest proportion of injuries [52].

3.6. Aggression Observed over Time

Changes in aggression over time have been studied in multiple articles (Table 1). However, the amount of time the mice were observed varied from a couple of days to weeks or months, and were therefore seldom comparable. With no exceptions, a within approach was used where aggression was measured multiple times and each mouse or group was compared with itself. In addition, in these studies, there were often interacting factors, such as enrichment items in the home cage, which sometimes had a direct influence on the results (Supplementary Table S1). For example, Haemisch et al. in two publications from 1994, reported increased aggression with time in enriched cages but did not see any effect in non-enriched cages [32,48]. Ambrose and Morton (2000), on the other hand, reported increased aggression with time in both enriched and non-enriched cages [24].
From all articles that studied the impact of time spent in a group, there were nine observations of decreased aggression over time [36,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61], ten with no effect [32,48,59,61,62,63,64,65] and 19 observations of increased aggression [13,18,20,24,32,33,48,57,66,67,68,69,70,71,72] (Supplementary Table S5). Aggression as an effect of time has been studied repeatedly in BALB/c [18,20,24,33,36,65,69,70], C57BL/6 [33,61,65] and CD-1 [13,20,67,71] mice. BALB/c and CD-1 were more often observed to become increasingly aggressive with time [13,20,24,33,67,69,70,71].

3.7. Strain Differences in Aggression

The most commonly used strains in this dataset of 90 articles were BALB/c (24 articles), C57BL/6 (17 articles) and CD-1 (14 articles) (Supplementary Table S2). Other strains were used in ≤7 articles.
Differences in aggression between strains, substrains or genetically modified lines were reported in 34 observations from 26 different articles (Supplementary Table S2). This included 22 observations of comparisons between two strains (Table 2). Similar to the complete dataset, BALB/c and C57BL/6 were the most commonly used strains. C57BL/6 was observed to be less aggressive than another strain on six occasions [23,73,74,75] and more aggressive only once [33]. BALB/c was observed to be less aggressive than another strain on five occasions [20,33,73,74,76] and more aggressive than another strain on three occasions [73,74,75]. Six direct comparisons between C57BL/6 and BALB/c were identified; three identified BALB/c as more aggressive [73,74,75], two did not see any difference [30,65] and one identified C57BL/6 as more aggressive [33]. C57BL/6 and BTBR have also been repeatedly compared but no differences in overall aggression were observed [61,77].
Differences in aggression between different substrains of C57BL/6 or BALB/c were studied on two occasions [21,41]. Gaskill et al. (2017) compared six different substrains of C57BL/6 and observed no difference in aggression [41] while Giles et al. (2018) saw more aggression after cage change in BALB/cJ as compared to BALB/cByJ [21].
Differences in aggression between genetically modified strains, compared to the corresponding wildtype, have also been observed [72,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87]. Out of ten observations, six reported increased aggression in the genetically modified strain [72,83,84,85,86,87]. Martinez-Cue et al. (2005) observed a decrease of aggression in a trisomic strain compared to its disomic control [80] while Trainor et al. (2007) saw no effect on aggression comparing a knockout strain with its wildtype [81]. Lewejohann et al. (2010) observed a decrease in aggression in a homozygous knockout strain but no difference in the heterozygous knockout strain [82]. In addition, Sorensen et al. (2005) saw decreased aggression in a transgenic C57BL/6 strain when housed in mixed groups with wildtype animals [72].

3.8. Size and Group Composition

An effect of group size on aggression was identified in twelve observations from ten articles (Table 3). However, the definition of small or large groups tended to vary between articles and, while small groups usually refer to three or sometimes four or five mice, large groups can refer to five mice all the way up to 10–12 or even 20 mice. Five observations saw the least aggression in small groups [54,59,70,88,89] while six observed the least aggression in large groups [42,90,91,92].
In this dataset, 65 articles presented information on kinship, whether groups were formed from sibling or mice from different litters (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S2). It was more than twice as common to form groups from non-siblings as compared to siblings: 41 and 16 articles respectively. Only six articles studied the effects of group formation on aggression [58,59,63,64,69,93]. In four articles, no effect on aggression was observed when comparing groups of siblings with groups of mice from different litters [59,63,64,69]. In the remaining two articles, one using wild type CD-1 and the other a transgenic line of CD-1, there was increased aggression in the non-sibling groups [58,93].
Bartolomucci et al. (2004) studied group formation from non-sibling CD-1 mice at weaning or adolescence and observed increased aggression in groups formed from juvenile CD-1 mice [93]. The effects of other early life events, such as weaning age [41,94] and nesting condition (standard or communal) [95], were also studied. The two articles studying weaning age showed contrary results; one observed increased aggression after early weaning [94] while the other observed increased aggression after late weaning [41]. When comparing standard or communal nesting, increased aggression was reported in communal nesting [95].
The effects of regrouping mice were studied in seven articles [41,89,94,96,97,98,99]. The group formation in the experiments varied widely and included single housing at delivery to be grouped later [96], rearranging group-housed mice into new groups [41,94,97,98] and removal of alpha males [99]. Only one of these treatments, breaking up groups of fighting mice into smaller groups with wounded mice only, resulted in decreased aggression [89].

3.9. Housing Conditions and Male Mouse Aggression

In this dataset, procedures and factors that related to housing conditions were the least studied category of treatments that could affect aggression (Table 1).
Aggression in relation to different cleaning procedures was studied in four articles [24,69,72,100]. Transferring the mice to a clean cage and moving nesting material to the new cage reduced aggression [69,100]. Placing clean sawdust in a soiled home cage and moving soiled sawdust or enrichment items to the clean cage increased aggression [24,69,100].
Another aspect that relates to housing conditions is the cage size, or density in the cage. Eleven observations from seven articles were related to this aspect: three defined as density [38,75] and eight as cage size [43,55,70,88,101]. All three observations of density, as well as three observations of cage size, showed no effect on aggression [38,43,75,101]. These six observations were made in six different strains. Increased aggression with increasing cage size was observed three times [70,76,88]. Two of these observations were based on BALB/c [70,88]. Contrarily, Poole and Morgan (1976) observed decreased aggression with increasing cage size in LACA/CFW mice, both comparing different groups and from within analysis of the same mice in different-sized cages [55].
Other practices relating to housing conditions have also been studied [36,41,102]. Disturbed lighting and having female mice close to the male mice cage increased aggression [36,102]. Gaskill et al. (2017) studied several parameters of the housing condition and observed increased aggression in mice identified by ear notch as compared to tail tattoo and increased aggression in lavender-scented cages, as well as a difference in aggression between racks. No differences were observed within the same rack [41].

4. Discussion

In the articles included in this systematic review, researchers investigated how aggression among group-housed male mice was affected by environmental enrichment, strain, age, housing conditions, group formation and time spent in groups. In all treatment categories, there were observations of decreased aggression, increased aggression, as well as no effect on aggression. When compiling the results, it was, in many cases, not possible to single out a clear effect of a specific treatment because of potential bias presented from other parameters. For example, effects of enrichment in the home cage could be affected by characteristics of the strain tested, the amount of time the mice were grouped together before the study or the length of the study itself. Altogether, this confirmed what has been pointed out in previous review articles on aggression in male mice [1,2]: that the problem is complex and that it is unlikely that one solution will fit every situation. We also found indications that the method used to assess aggression could influence the outcome, further complicating interpretation of results. Still, aggression was decreased when using certain environmental enrichment items and certain strains.

4.1. Methods Used for Assessment of Aggressive Behaviour

In the included articles, assessments of aggression among group-housed male mice were performed by observations in the home cage, using tests for aggression outside the home cage, by registration of wounds, or by a combination of these methods. Home cage observations of aggression were used in more than half of the articles and this approach has been more frequently used in recent years (Figure 2b). Studying aggressive behavior in the home cage is likely more relevant for understanding aggression under normal husbandry than using aggression tests outside the home cage, as the fighting observed in the home cage will be linked to social behavior in the group, whereas fighting in a novel environment or with an intruder captures aggressive behavior in relation to those factors.
Many conclusions of aggression in male mice have historically been drawn from assessment of aggressive behavior outside the home cage, using tests such as the resident intruder test and social defeat test (Figure 2b). This approach was used, alone or in combination, in about one third of the articles in the present systematic review (Supplementary Table S2). Not surprisingly, decreased aggression was rarely seen as an outcome in articles where the aggression was measured outside the home cage (Figure 2c). The large proportion of increased aggression seen when studying aggression outside the home cage could be a result of the method itself rather than a treatment effect, since the purpose of these tests is to provoke aggression. Aggression assessed in the home cage, on the other hand, resulted in decreased, increased or no difference in aggression, evenly distributed between the included articles. The importance of using reliable methods relevant for assessing aggression in the home cage when developing/evaluating preventative measures for group housing male mice was therefore elucidated in this systematic review. The observed effect of the treatments used to study the effects on aggression varied between articles regarding strain, environmental enrichment and group formation. In some cases, the results were contradictory even when using the same comparison/hypothesis. It is thus possible that the increased aggressive behavior was a result of the aggression test methods rather than true effects of the specific treatment.

4.2. Environmental Enrichment

Enrichment is commonly used as a means to improve animal welfare and for refinement of animal use in research [103,104]. However, general recommendations on how enrichment should be used to prevent aggression are not straightforward, as enrichment has been reported to have both positive results, negative results or no impact on aggression [2,17]. This was consistent with the results in this review; in almost half of the observations, enrichment had neither positive nor negative observed effects on aggression. Interestingly, enrichment increased aggression in only about 20% of the observations. It is worth mentioning that some research groups are reluctant to use enrichments because of a strong belief that enrichments will increase aggression. The presented results indicate that the type of enrichment used was important, e.g., hiding devices, feed enrichment and nesting materials, used alone or in combination with other enrichments, were associated with decreased aggression in male mouse groups (Figure 4). These enrichments resulted in increased aggression in only seven out of 50 observations (Supplementary Table S4). Enrichment items that mice can interact and work with, such as nesting material, often decrease aggression and are generally included in advice for reducing aggression among male mice [3,4,9,10]. In a study based on data from group-housed male mice in 40 animal facilities by Lidster et al. (2019), transfer of clean and dry nesting material at cage cleaning had a clear positive effect, with reduced aggression [3]. Transfer of nesting material was also perceived as the most effective approach to prevent fighting and aggression in male mouse groups when asking animal technicians and researchers at Swedish universities at workshops and in a survey [4]. The workshop and survey respondents also mentioned adding extra nesting material as efficient. In other species, such as non-human primates, visual barriers were recommended to minimize aggression when housing animals in social groups [105]. It could be that providing hiding devices to allow mice to remove themselves from sight of one another mimics the natural behavior response of fleeing when threatened by a conspecific [2,41], thus decreasing aggression in mice.
In the present review, locomotor enrichment (wheel) and shelter were associated with increased aggression in more than half of the observations. Interestingly, a combination of shelter and wheel were used in five of these nine observations. Mice can defend and monopolize valuable physical enrichment items, and only a limited number of items can fit in a conventional mouse cage. Lower levels of aggression when enrichment items were dispersed was found in both male [29] and female [106] mice. It might be that the shelter and wheel were only accessible for one or a few mice at a time, especially when the wheel was connected to the house, resulting in mice displaying resource aggression. If there were only one entrance, it might also be difficult to escape an aggressive encounter, with the risk of escalating aggression as a consequence. This could explain why increased aggressive behavior was found when these types of enrichments were used.
Interestingly, the results presented herein could indicate that environmental enrichment had a more pronounced positive effect when used in non-sibling male groups. There were no observations of decreased aggression in sibling groups when assessing effects of environmental enrichment. This could be due to the overall lower levels of aggression in groups of siblings. On the contrary, increased aggression in enriched environments was observed for both sibling and non-sibling groups (Supplementary Table S4).
Recommendations to prevent aggression that considered housing conditions for mice include transfer of nesting material at cage cleaning [3,4,9,10], avoiding disturbances and handling mice with care [9]. However, there are no clear recommendations for enrichment. This could be due to the wide variety of enrichment items and combinations of different items studied. In the reviewed studies, 42 different enrichment items were used in combination with different strains and group constellations, making it very difficult to interpret results and give clear recommendations. Many parameters of normal husbandry can affect aggression in group-housed laboratory mice. Some, such as enrichment, are often represented in empirical studies. Despite this, systematic evaluation of many enrichment items is lacking. Experienced personnel working with mice on a daily basis learn, through trial and error, what type of enrichments reduce or trigger aggression—but this type of information is rarely collected systematically by researchers. Information on basic husbandry and routines were missing in many articles in the present review. A complete methods section is a prerequisite to be able to repeat experiments, or to implement suggested practices in normal husbandry. The ARRIVE guidelines [107], originally published in 2010 [108], should ensure that studies are reported in enough detail. However, several important parameters were often missing from the reviewed studies.

4.3. Strain

A previously published recommendation advises the use of strains with low prevalence of aggression whenever possible [3]. In a study from 2019, Lidster et al. collected significant data on incidents of aggressive behavior in group-housed mice at animal facilities and determined strain to be one of the key factors influencing prevalence of aggression. They concluded that C57BL/6 and BALB/c were the two strains that showed lower prevalence of aggression [3]. Among the herein included articles, similar tendencies were found. For example, C57BL/6 mice were less aggressive than their counterparts in all but one study investigating the difference in aggression between strains. BALB/c mice also showed low levels of aggression when compared to other strains. In fact, BALB/c mice were more aggressive only when compared with C57BL/6, indicating that both these strains showed low levels of aggression. Contrary to these results, C57BL/6 has sometimes been reported as an aggressive strain when doing surveys and workshops at animal facilities [1,4]. Because C57BL/6 is a commonly used strain, it is possible that this conception among survey respondents reflected the large number of C57BL/6 housed at facilities, rather than it being a more aggressive strain. That being said, if animal technicians experience aggression among these strains, this should be taken seriously. It is possible that these strains are less aggressive overall and, under certain conditions, display low levels of aggression, but that aggression can be triggered in other circumstances. When choosing what strain to use for animal experiments, we still know very little about what triggers aggression in the various strains. Empirical studies focused on comparing aggression among strains have shown somewhat inconclusive results (Table 2 and Table S2). Considering that studies vary in a large number of factors other than strain—differences in number of animals per cage, age, cage size, enrichment items, etc.—it was difficult to interpret results and impossible to draw any major conclusions based on this dataset alone. Researchers must explore how aggressive behavior in various strains is influenced by environmental factors. For example, among the included articles herein, we noted a repeated negative relationship between cage size and aggression in BALB/c, something that could be further explored. Preferably, we would like to see systematic studies comparing multiple strains under varying environmental conditions to help us better understand variations in aggressive behavior and how it is influenced by other factors in group-housed male mice.

4.4. Group Formation

General recommendations exist, aimed at preventing and minimizing aggression in group-housed male mice. These are related to group formation, such as housing in small groups formed from siblings kept together or familiar mice grouped before sexual maturity [3,9,10]. Recent publications also suggested that male mice can be grouped with unfamiliar mice at weaning [4], shortly after weaning with one-week age difference between males [109], or before sexual maturity [110]. In the present review, there was only one article evaluating age-related aggression or grouping before and after sexual maturity. This article reported increased aggression in groups formed from unfamiliar mice at weaning, as compared to groups formed from littermates or groups formed from somewhat older unfamiliar mice [93]. However, time spent in groups was also studied in some articles, with a time frame varying from a few days up to 40 weeks (Supplementary Table S5). Scattered results were observed, including increased aggression, decreased aggression, and no observed change in aggression, indicating that the incidence of aggressive behavior was influenced by factors other than time spent together.
In the present review, there were inconclusive results on preferable group size for male mice. Decreased aggression was observed in smaller groups with 2–5 mice per group as well as in larger groups of 10–20 mice per group (Table 3). The literature on group size was divided; there were recommendations for 3 mice per group in some cases (e.g., [70]), while the incidence of aggression was significantly higher in groups of three, compared to groups of 4 and 5, in others [3].

4.5. Recommendations Based on Results from the Systematic Review

Based on this literature review, a few recommendations stand out as promising tools to minimize aggression among group-housed male mice and should be considered.
  • Enrichment: Use nesting material and other enrichment items that the mice can manipulate and work with. Use hiding devices where the mice can hide. Be careful when using enrichments that can be monopolized by one or a few individuals Note that environmental enrichment seem to be even more important in non-sibling groups.
  • Strain: Use less aggressive strains whenever possible. This literature review indicates that C57BL/6 and BALB/c show low prevalence of aggression, in line with recently published advice [3].
  • Solutions may vary: Diverging and varying opinions exist between research facilities concerning what works to prevent aggression. Try different solutions and adjust to specific needs in your lab. Be aware that the same solution might not work for all strains and research projects.
  • Methods used to study aggression: Be aware that the method used to study aggression could influence the relevance of the results for home cage aggression. It is therefore important to evaluate the method used to study aggression before drawing conclusions on whether or not the reported results should be implemented in daily practice. Recent studies using home cage observations could provide more reliable information.

4.6. Important Notes for Future Research

To facilitate the understanding of home cage aggression, the most relevant approach is to study aggression in groups of male mice using home cage observations, where the social group is kept intact (and not by aggression tests outside the home cage with unfamiliar mice).
It is important to include all data regarding housing and husbandry in publications so that results can be interpreted correctly, and comparisons can be made between studies. Follow the ARRIVE guidelines.
More systematic studies are needed, using various strains for the same treatments and housing conditions and the same methods to study aggression.

5. Conclusions

Aggression among group-housed male mice is a major welfare concern. Identifying factors that prevent aggression and thus enable mice to be held in stable social groups will not only minimize pain and suffering but also contribute to a reduction in the number of animals used in research.
The keys to success for male mouse group-housing are multifactorial. There is no one solution that fits all; solutions can and will vary and must be adjusted depending on animal facilities and research areas. Finding the factors that work at a given facility may take time and several factors should be evaluated before resorting to single housing. In this literature review, a number of recommendations have been identified as promising tools to minimize aggression among group-housed male mice. Taken together with other current information from the literature, this study could be an important complement when developing guidelines on how to prevent aggression when male mice are housed in groups.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13010143/s1, Supplementary information: Search strings; Figure S1: Outline of the screening strategy with inclusion and exclusion criteria; Table S1: Raw data - information extracted from all included articles; Table S2: Strains, treatments and methods used to study aggression in all included articles; Table S3: Enrichment items used in the articles that specifically studied enrichment; Table S4 Articles that study the effect of enrichment items on aggression; Table S5: Articles that study the effect of time spent in group.

Author Contributions

The study was designed by E.T. and J.Z. who planned the systematic search with support from the Karolinska Institutet. J.Z. and E.M.W. determined the inclusion and exclusion criteria with input from E.T. and B.E. Screening of records was conducted by E.S. and E.U. and screening of reports was conducted by K.A. and E.S., in both cases with support from E.M.W. and J.Z., K.A. and E.S. extracted data from the selected articles. The paper was written by E.M.W., J.Z., K.A., E.S. and E.T. and commented on by E.U. and B.E. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article (and its supplementary information files).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to librarians at the Karolinska Institutet University Library for professional help with the systematic search. Additionally, sincere thanks to Erika Roman for valuable comments on the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kappel, S.; Hawkins, P.; Mendl, M.T. To Group or Not to Group? Good Practice for Housing Male Laboratory Mice. Anim. Open Access J. MDPI 2017, 7, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Weber, E.M.; Dallaire, J.A.; Gaskill, B.N.; Pritchett-Corning, K.R.; Garner, J.P. Aggression in group-housed laboratory mice: Why can’t we solve the problem? Lab Anim. 2017, 46, 157–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Lidster, K.; Owen, K.; Browne, W.J.; Prescott, M.J. Cage aggression in group-housed laboratory male mice: An international data crowdsourcing project. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 15211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Zidar, J.; Weber, E.M.; Ewaldsson, B.; Tjäder, S.; Lilja, J.; Mount, J.; Svensson, C.; Svensk, E.; Udén, E.; Törnqvist, A.E. Group and Single Housing of Male Mice: Collected Experiences from Research Facilities in Sweden. Animals 2019, 9, 1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Theil, J.H.; Ahloy-Dallaire, J.; Weber, E.M.; Gaskill, B.N.; Pritchett-Corning, K.R.; Felt, S.A.; Garner, J.P. The epidemiology of fighting in group-housed laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 16649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. EN. Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 22 September 2010 on the Protection of Animals Used for Scientific Purposes. Off. J. Eur. Union 2010, 276, 33–76. [Google Scholar]
  7. National Research Council (US) Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th ed.; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2011. [CrossRef]
  8. UK Home Office Code of Practice for the Housing and Care of Animals Bred, Supplied or Used for Scientific Purposes Rodents and Rabbits. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/389807/44389_unact_animals_Rodents_and_Rabbits.pdf (accessed on 23 December 2022).
  9. Van Loo, P.L.P.; Van Zutphen, L.F.M.; Baumans, V. Male management: Coping with aggression problems in male laboratory mice. Lab. Anim. 2003, 37, 300–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Gaskill, B. Aggression in Laboratory Mice: Potential Influences and How to Manage It. Enrich. Rec. 2014, 18, 22–25. [Google Scholar]
  11. Koolhaas, J.M.; Coppens, C.M.; de Boer, S.F.; Buwalda, B.; Meerlo, P.; Timmermans, P.J. The resident-intruder paradigm: A standardized test for aggression, violence and social stress. J. Vis. Exp. JoVE 2013, e4367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Golden, S.A.; Covington, H.E.; Berton, O.; Russo, S.J. A standardized protocol for repeated social defeat stress in mice. Nat. Protoc. 2011, 6, 1183–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Terranova, M.L.; Laviola, G.; de Acetis, L.; Alleva, E. A description of the ontogeny of mouse agonistic behavior. J. Comp. Psychol. 1998, 112, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lindzey, G.; Winston, H.; Manosevitz, M. Social dominance in inbred mouse strains. Nature 1961, 191, 474–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kwiatkowski, C.C.; Akaeze, H.; Ndlebe, I.; Goodwin, N.; Eagle, A.L.; Moon, K.; Bender, A.R.; Golden, S.A.; Robison, A.J. Quantitative standardization of resident mouse behavior for studies of aggression and social defeat. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021, 46, 1584–1593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Gaskill, B.N.; Stottler, A.; Pritchett-Corning, K.R.; Wong, L.K.; Geronimo, J.; Garner, J.P. He’s getting under my skin! Comparing the sensitivity and specificity of dermal vs subcuticular lesions as a measure of aggression in mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 183, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Olsson, I.A.; Dahlborn, K. Improving housing conditions for laboratory mice: A review of “environmental enrichment”. Lab. Anim. 2002, 36, 243–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Van Loo, P.L.P.; Kruitwagen, C.; Koolhaas, J.M.; Van de Weerd, H.A.; Van Zutphen, L.F.M.; Baumans, V. Influence of cage enrichment on aggressive behaviour and physiological parameters in male mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 76, 65–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Howerton, C.L.; Garner, J.P.; Mench, J.A. Effects of a running wheel-igloo enrichment on aggression, hierarchy linearity, and stereotypy in group-housed male CD-1 (ICR) mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 115, 90–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Van Loo, P.L.P.; Van der Meer, E.; Kruitwagen, C.; Koolhaas, J.M.; Van Zutphen, L.F.M.; Baumans, V. Strain-specific aggressive behavior of male mice submitted to different husbandry procedures. Aggress. Behav. 2003, 29, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Giles, J.M.; Whitaker, J.W.; Moy, S.S.; Fletcher, C.A. Effect of environmental enrichment on aggression in BALB/cJ and BALB/cByJ mice monitored by using an automated system. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2018, 57, 236–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Swetter, B.J.; Karpiak, C.P.; Cannon, J.T. Separating the effects of shelter from additional cage enhancements for group-housed BALB/cJ mice. Neurosci. Lett. 2011, 495, 205–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Abramov, U.; Puussaar, T.; Raud, S.; Kurrikoff, K.; Vasar, E. Behavioural differences between C57BL/6 and 129S6/SvEv strains are reinforced by environmental enrichment. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 443, 223–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Ambrose, N.; Morton, D.B. The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2000, 3, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bloomsmith, M.A.; Brent, L.Y.; Schapiro, S.J. Guidelines for developing and managing an environmental enrichment program for nonhuman primates. Lab. Anim. Sci. 1991, 41, 372–377. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  27. Armstrong, K.R.; Clark, T.R.; Peterson, M.R. Use of Corn-Husk Nesting Material to Reduce Aggression in Caged Mice. Contemp. Top Lab. Anim. Sci. 1998, 37, 64–66. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lima, F.B.; Spinelli de Oliveira, E. What is the impact of low testosterone levels on the anatomical and behavioral repertoire of long-term enriched housing of male mice? Behav. Process. 2014, 108, 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Nadiah, M.Y.N.; Saadiah, M.B.H.; Nurdiana, S. Effects of Different Resource Distribution on Behaviour and Corticosterone Level of Male Mice (Mus musculus). APCBEE Procedia 2014, 8, 189–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Clipperton-Allen, A.E.; Ingrao, J.C.; Ruggiero, L.; Batista, L.; Ovari, J.; Hammermueller, J.; Armstrong, J.N.; Bienzle, D.; Choleris, E.; Turner, P.V. Long-Term Provision of Environmental Resources Alters Behavior but not Physiology or Neuroanatomy of Male and Female BALB/c and C57BL/6 Mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. JAALAS 2015, 54, 718–730. [Google Scholar]
  31. Nakamura, Y.; Ueno, A.; Nunomura, Y.; Nakagaki, K.; Takeda, S.; Suzuki, K. Effects of inducing exercise on growing mice by means of three-dimensional structure in rearing environment. Exp. Anim. 2016, 65, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Haemisch, A.; Gartner, K. The cage design affects intermale aggression in small groups of male laboratory mice: Strain specific consequences on social organization, and endocrine activations in two inbred strains (DBA/2J and CBA/J). J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 1994, 36, 101–116. [Google Scholar]
  33. Eskola, S.; Kaliste-Korhonen, E. Aspen wood-wool is preferred as a resting place, but does not affect intracage fighting of male BALB/c and C57BL/6J mice. Lab. Anim. 1999, 33, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Marashi, V.; Barnekow, A.; Ossendorf, E.; Sachser, N. Effects of different forms of environmental enrichment on behavioral, endocrinological, and immunological parameters in male mice. Horm. Behav. 2003, 43, 281–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Marashi, V.; Barnekow, A.; Sachser, N. Effects of environmental enrichment on males of a docile inbred strain of mice. Physiol. Behav. 2004, 82, 765–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Van der Meer, E.; Van Loo, P.L.P.; Baumans, V. Short-term effects of a disturbed light-dark cycle and environmental enrichment on aggression and stress-related parameters in male mice. Lab. Anim. 2004, 38, 376–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kaliste, E.K.; Mering, S.M.; Huuskonen, H.K. Environmental modification and agonistic behavior in NIH/S male mice: Nesting material enhances fighting but shelters prevent it. Comp. Med. 2006, 56, 202–208. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hunt, C.; Hambly, C. Faecal corticosterone concentrations indicate that separately housed male mice are not more stressed than group housed males. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 519–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Workman, J.L.; Fonken, L.K.; Gusfa, J.; Kassouf, K.M.; Nelson, R.J. Post-weaning environmental enrichment alters affective responses and interacts with behavioral testing to alter nNOS immunoreactivity. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2011, 100, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lockworth, C.R.; Kim, S.-J.; Liu, J.; Palla, S.L.; Craig, S.L. Effect of Enrichment Devices on Aggression in Manipulated Nude Mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. JAALAS 2015, 54, 731–736. [Google Scholar]
  41. Gaskill, B.N.; Stottler, A.M.; Garner, J.P.; Winnicker, C.W.; Mulder, G.B.; Pritchett-Corning, K.R. The effect of early life experience, environment, and genetic factors on spontaneous home-cage aggression-related wounding in male C57BL/6 mice. Lab Anim. 2017, 46, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Barnard, C.J.; Behnke, J.M.; Sewell, J. Environmental enrichment, immunocompetence, and resistance to Babesia microti in male mice. Physiol. Behav. 1996, 60, 1223–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. McGregor, P.K.; Ayling, S.J. Varied cages result in more aggression in male CFLP mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1990, 26, 277–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Haemisch, A.; Gartner, K. Effects of cage enrichment on territorial aggression and stress physiology in male laboratory mice. Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl. 1997, 640, 73–76. [Google Scholar]
  45. McQuaid, R.J.; Audet, M.C.; Anisman, H. Environmental enrichment in male CD-1 mice promotes aggressive behaviors and elevated corticosterone and brain norepinephrine activity in response to a mild stressor. Stress-Int. J. Biol. Stress 2012, 15, 354–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Mesa-Gresa, P.; Perez-Martinez, A.; Redolat, R. Environmental enrichment improves novel object recognition and enhances agonistic behavior in male mice. Aggress. Behav. 2013, 39, 269–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Leduc, R.Y.M.; Rauw, G.; Baker, G.B.; McDermid, H.E. What goes around can come around: An unexpected deleterious effect of using mouse running wheels for environmental enrichment. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2017, 56, 194–201. [Google Scholar]
  48. Haemisch, A.; Voss, T.; Gartner, K. Effects of environmental enrichment on aggressive behavior, dominance hierarchies, and endocrine states in male DBA/2J mice. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 1041–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. McQuaid, R.J.; Audet, M.-C.; Jacobson-Pick, S.; Anisman, H. Environmental enrichment influences brain cytokine variations elicited by social defeat in mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 987–996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. McQuaid, R.J.; Audet, M.-C.; Jacobson-Pick, S.; Anisman, H. The differential impact of social defeat on mice living in isolation or groups in an enriched environment: Plasma corticosterone and monoamine variations. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013, 16, 351–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. McQuaid, R.J.; Dunn, R.; Jacobson-Pick, S.; Anisman, H.; Audet, M.-C. Post-weaning Environmental Enrichment in Male CD-1 Mice: Impact on Social Behaviors, Corticosterone Levels and Prefrontal Cytokine Expression in Adulthood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Bergmann, P.; Militzer, K.; Buttner, D. Environmental enrichment and aggressive behaviour: Influence on body weight and body fat in male inbred HLG mice. J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 1995, 37, 69–78. [Google Scholar]
  53. Page, D.L.; Glenner, G.G. Social interaction and wounding in the genesis of “spontaneous” murine amyloidosis. Am. J. Pathol. 1972, 67, 555–567. [Google Scholar]
  54. Poole, T.B.; Morgan, H.D.R. Differences in aggressive behaviour between male mice (Mus musculus L.) in colonies of different sizes. Anim. Behav. 1973, 21, 788–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Poole, T.B.; Morgan, H.D.R. Social and territorial behaviour of laboratory mice (Mus musculus L.) in small complex areas. Anim. Behav. 1976, 24, 476–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Bishop, M.J.; Chevins, P.F.D. Urine odours and marking patterns in territorial laboratory mice (Mus musculus). Behav. Process. 1987, 15, 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Burright, R.G.; Freeman, M.J.; Donovick, P.J. Repeated tests of intermale aggression in mice (Mus musculus) are influenced by housing and test conditions. J. Comp. Psychol. 1988, 102, 303–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Hilakivi-Clarke, L.; Dickson, R.B. Stress influence on development of hepatocellular tumors in transgenic mice overexpressing TGF alpha. Acta Oncol. 1995, 34, 907–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Grimm, M.S.; Emerman, J.T.; Weinberg, J. Effects of social housing condition and behavior on growth of the Shionogi mouse mammary carcinoma. Physiol. Behav. 1996, 59, 633–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Vaughan, L.M.; Dawson, J.S.; Porter, P.R.; Whittaker, A.L. Castration promotes welfare in group-housed male Swiss outbred mice maintained in educational institutions. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. JAALAS 2014, 53, 38–43. [Google Scholar]
  61. Bove, M.; Ike, K.; Eldering, A.; Buwalda, B.; de Boer, S.F.; Morgese, M.G.; Schiavone, S.; Cuomo, V.; Trabace, L.; Kas, M.J.H. The Visible Burrow System: A behavioral paradigm to assess sociability and social withdrawal in BTBR and C57BL/6J mice strains. Behav. Brain Res. 2018, 344, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Svare, B.B.; Leshner, A.I. Behavioral correlates of intermale aggression and grouping in mice. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1973, 85, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. O’Donnell, V.; Blanchard, R.J.; Blanchard, D.C. Mouse aggression increases after 24 hours of isolation or housing with females. Behav. Neural Biol. 1981, 32, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Kareem, A.M.; Barnard, C.J. The importance of kinship and familiarity in social interactions between mice. Anim. Behav. 1982, 30, 594–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Fairless, A.H.; Katz, J.M.; Vijayvargiya, N.; Dow, H.C.; Kreibich, A.S.; Berrettini, W.H.; Abel, T.; Brodkin, E.S. Development of home cage social behaviors in BALB/cJ vs. C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2013, 237, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Zwirner, P.P.; Porsolt, R.D.; Loew, D.M. Inter-group aggression in mice: A new method for testing the effects of centrally active drugs. Psychopharmacologia 1975, 45, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Terranova, M.L.; Laviola, G.; Alleva, E. Ontogeny of amicable social behavior in the mouse: Gender differences and ongoing isolation outcomes. Dev. Psychobiol. 1993, 26, 467–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Benus, R.F.; Henkelmann, C. Litter composition influences the development of aggression and behavioural strategy in male Mus domesticus. Behaviour 1998, 135, 1229–1249. [Google Scholar]
  69. Van Loo, P.L.P.; Kruitwagen, C.; Van Zutphen, L.F.M.; Koolhaas, J.M.; Baumans, V. Modulation of aggression in male mice: Influence of cage cleaning regime and scent marks. Anim. Welf. 2000, 9, 281–295. [Google Scholar]
  70. Van Loo, P.L.; Mol, J.A.; Koolhaas, J.M.; Van Zutphen, B.F.; Baumans, V. Modulation of aggression in male mice: Influence of group size and cage size. Physiol. Behav. 2001, 72, 675–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Garris, D.R. Aggression-associated changes in murine olfactory tubercle bioamines. Brain Res. 2003, 963, 150–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Sorensen, D.B.; Johnsen, P.F.; Bibby, B.M.; Bottner, A.; Bornstein, S.R.; Eisenhofer, G.; Pacak, K.; Hansen, A.K. PNMT transgenic mice have an aggressive phenotype. Horm. Metab. Res. = Horm.-Und Stoffwechs. = Horm. Et Metab. 2005, 37, 159–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Bisazza, A. Social organization and territorial behaviour in three strains of mice. Boll. Zool. 1981, 48, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Mondragón, R.; Mayagoitia, L.; López-Luján, A.; Díaz, J.L. Social structure features in three inbred strains of mice, C57Bl/6J, Balb/cj, and NIH: A comparative study. Behav. Neural Biol. 1987, 47, 384–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Nicholson, A.; Malcolm, R.D.; Russ, P.L.; Cough, K.; Touma, C.; Palme, R.; Wiles, M.V. The response of C57BL/6J and BALB/cJ mice to increased housing density. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2009, 48, 740–753. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  76. Smith, A.L.; Mabus, S.L.; Muir, C.; Woo, Y. Effects of housing density and cage floor space on three strains of young adult inbred mice. Comp. Med. 2005, 55, 368–376. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  77. Babineau, B.A.; Yang, M.; Berman, R.F.; Crawley, J.N. Low home cage social behaviors in BTBR T+tf/J mice during juvenile development. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 114–115, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Fitchett, A.E.; Collins, S.A.; Mason, H.; Barnard, C.J.; Cassaday, H.J. Urinary corticosterone measures: Effects of strain and social rank in BKW and CD-1 mice. Behav. Process. 2005, 70, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Kempf, E.; Puglisi-Allegra, S.; Cabib, S.; Schleef, C.; Mandel, P. Serotonin levels and turnover in different brain areas of isolated aggressive or non-aggressive strains of mice. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 1984, 8, 365–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Martinez-Cue, C.; Rueda, N.; Garcia, E.; Davisson, M.T.; Schmidt, C.; Florez, J. Behavioral, cognitive and biochemical responses to different environmental conditions in male Ts65Dn mice, a model of Down syndrome. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 163, 174–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Trainor, B.C.; Workman, J.L.; Jessen, R.; Nelson, R.J. Impaired Nitric Oxide Synthase Signaling Dissociates Social Investigation and Aggression. Behav. Neurosci. 2007, 121, 362–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Lewejohann, L.; Kloke, V.; Heiming, R.S.; Jansen, F.; Kaiser, S.; Schmitt, A.; Lesch, K.P.; Sachser, N. Social status and day-to-day behaviour of male serotonin transporter knockout mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 211, 220–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Rodriguez-Arias, M.; Navarrete, F.; Daza-Losada, M.; Navarro, D.; Aguilar, M.A.; Berbel, P.; Minarro, J.; Manzanares, J. CB1 cannabinoid receptor-mediated aggressive behavior. Neuropharmacology 2013, 75, 172–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Rodriguez-Arias, M.; Navarrete, F.; Blanco-Gandia, M.C.; Arenas, M.C.; Aguilar, M.A.; Bartoll-Andres, A.; Valverde, O.; Minarro, J.; Manzanares, J. Role of CB2 receptors in social and aggressive behavior in male mice. Psychopharmacology 2015, 232, 3019–3031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. McNamara, G.I.; John, R.M.; Isles, A.R. Territorial Behavior and Social Stability in the Mouse Require Correct Expression of Imprinted Cdkn1c. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Tan, S.; Xiao, Y.; Yin, H.H.; Chen, A.I.; Soong, T.W.; Je, H.S. Postnatal TrkB ablation in corticolimbic interneurons induces social dominance in male mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, E9909–E9915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  87. Rodriguiz, R.M.; Chu, R.; Caron, M.G.; Wetsel, W.C. Aberrant responses in social interaction of dopamine transporter knockout mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 148, 185–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Bailoo, J.D.; Murphy, E.; Varholick, J.A.; Novak, J.; Palme, R.; Wurbel, H. Evaluation of the effects of space allowance on measures of animal welfare in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Blankenberger, W.B.; Weber, E.M.; Chu, D.K.; Geronimo, J.T.; Theil, J.; Gaskill, B.N.; Pritchett-Corning, K.; Albertelli, M.A.; Garner, J.P.; Ahloy-Dallaire, J. Breaking up is hard to do: Does splitting cages of mice reduce aggression? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 206, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Welch, B.L.; Welch, A.S. Graded effect of social stimulation upon d-amphetamine toxicity, aggressiveness and heart and adrenal weight. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1966, 151, 331–338. [Google Scholar]
  91. Barnard, C.J.; Behnke, J.M.; Sewell, J. Social behaviour and susceptibility to infection in house mice (Mus musculus): Effects of group size, aggressive behaviour and status-related hormonal responses prior to infection on resistance to Babesia microti. Parasitology 1994, 108 Pt 5, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Saldívar-González, J.A.; Posadas-Andrews, A.; Rojas, J.A.; Yoldi-Negrete, M.; Álvarez-Sekely, A.; Flores-Hernández, V.; Ortiz-León, S.; Foilloux-Morales, C.; Mayagoitia, L.; Mondragón-Ceballos, R. Effect of imipramine and electro convulsive stimulation in mice under social stress conditions. Curr. Top. Pharmacol. 2007, 11, 57–70. [Google Scholar]
  93. Bartolomucci, A.; Chirieleison, A.; Gioiosa, L.; Ceresini, G.; Parmigiani, S.; Palanza, P. Age at group formation alters behavior and physiology in male but not female CD-1 mice. Physiol. Behav. 2004, 82, 425–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Kikusui, T.; Takeuchi, Y.; Mori, Y. Early weaning induces anxiety and aggression in adult mice. Physiol. Behav. 2004, 81, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. D’Andrea, I.; Alleva, E.; Branchi, I. Communal nesting, an early social enrichment, affects social competences but not learning and memory abilities at adulthood. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 183, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Hilakivi-Clarke, L.A.; Lister, R.G. Are there preexisting behavioral characteristics that predict the dominant status of male NIH Swiss mice (Mus musculus)? J. Comp. Psychol. 1992, 106, 184–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Kaliste-Korhonen, E.; Eskola, S. Fighting in NIH/S male mice: Consequences for behaviour in resident-intruder tests and physiological parameters. Lab. Anim. 2000, 34, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Lee, W.; Khan, A.; Curley, J.P. Major urinary protein levels are associated with social status and context in mouse social hierarchies. Proceedings. Biol. Sci. 2017, 284, 20171570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Williamson, C.M.; Romeo, R.D.; Curley, J.P. Dynamic changes in social dominance and mPOA GnRH expression in male mice following social opportunity. Horm. Behav. 2017, 87, 80–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Gray, S.; Hurst, J.L. The effects of cage cleaning on aggression within groups of male laboratory mice. Anim. Behav. 1995, 49, 821–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Smith, A.L.; Corrow, D.J. Modifications to husbandry and housing conditions of laboratory rodents for improved well-being. ILAR J. 2005, 46, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Koyama, S.; Kamimura, S. Influence of social dominance and female odor on the sperm activity of male mice. Physiol. Behav. 2000, 71, 415–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Wurbel, H.; Garner, J.P. Refinement of rodent research through environmental enrichment and systematic randomization. Natl. Cent. Replace. Refinement Reduct. Anim. Res. 2007, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  104. Ratuski, A.S.; Weary, D.M. Environmental Enrichment for Rats and Mice Housed in Laboratories: A Metareview. Animals 2022, 12, 414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Olsson, I.A.S.; Westlund, K. More than numbers matter: The effect of social factors on behaviour and welfare of laboratory rodents and non-human primates. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 103, 229–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Akre, A.K.; Bakken, M.; Hovland, A.L.; Palme, R.; Mason, G. Clustered environmental enrichments induce more aggression and stereotypic behaviour than do dispersed enrichments in female mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 131, 145–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Percie du Sert, N.; Hurst, V.; Ahluwalia, A.; Alam, S.; Avey, M.T.; Baker, M.; Browne, W.J.; Clark, A.; Cuthill, I.C.; Dirnagl, U.; et al. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 2020, 18, e3000410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Kilkenny, C.; Browne, W.J.; Cuthill, I.C.; Emerson, M.; Altman, D.G. Improving Bioscience Research Reporting: The ARRIVE Guidelines for Reporting Animal Research. PLoS Biol. 2010, 8, e1000412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Azkona, G.; Caballero, J.M. Implementing strategies to reduce singly housed male mice. Lab Anim 2019, 53, 508–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Grífols, R.; Zamora, C.; Ortega-Saez, I.; Azkona, G. Postweaning Grouping as a Strategy to Reduce Singly Housed Male Mice. Animals 2020, 10, 2135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram. Potentially relevant records collected from a systematic literature search using three databases (Medline, Embase and Web of Science) and from scanning the reference lists of three recent literature reviews. Selection performed in two steps; 90 articles suitable for inclusion. Records refer to title, abstract, author keywords and keywords of scientific publications, while reports refer to the full text article.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram. Potentially relevant records collected from a systematic literature search using three databases (Medline, Embase and Web of Science) and from scanning the reference lists of three recent literature reviews. Selection performed in two steps; 90 articles suitable for inclusion. Records refer to title, abstract, author keywords and keywords of scientific publications, while reports refer to the full text article.
Animals 13 00143 g001
Figure 2. Different methodological approaches to study aggression in the data set. Number of articles (a) using the different methodological approaches to assess aggression, and proportion of articles (b) using the different methodological approaches before 2000, between 2001 and 2010 and after 2011. The color scheme for the different methodological approaches used in (a) also applies to (b). The proportion of increased, decreased and no effect on aggression varied between the use of home cage observation, test for aggression and wounding for assessment (c). In panel (c), observations using more than one methodological approach have been excluded.
Figure 2. Different methodological approaches to study aggression in the data set. Number of articles (a) using the different methodological approaches to assess aggression, and proportion of articles (b) using the different methodological approaches before 2000, between 2001 and 2010 and after 2011. The color scheme for the different methodological approaches used in (a) also applies to (b). The proportion of increased, decreased and no effect on aggression varied between the use of home cage observation, test for aggression and wounding for assessment (c). In panel (c), observations using more than one methodological approach have been excluded.
Animals 13 00143 g002
Figure 3. The availability of details on housing and husbandry has changed over time.
Figure 3. The availability of details on housing and husbandry has changed over time.
Animals 13 00143 g003
Figure 4. Effect on aggression using different types of enrichment. Note, this includes experiments where either one specific enrichment is used, or when several enrichments are used in combination. In the figure, the category other (11 observations) is excluded due to the variation of enrichments included in that category. Gnawing device is excluded due to a low number of observations (2). Four observations with different outcomes, dependent on the method used to measure aggression, are also excluded.
Figure 4. Effect on aggression using different types of enrichment. Note, this includes experiments where either one specific enrichment is used, or when several enrichments are used in combination. In the figure, the category other (11 observations) is excluded due to the variation of enrichments included in that category. Gnawing device is excluded due to a low number of observations (2). Four observations with different outcomes, dependent on the method used to measure aggression, are also excluded.
Animals 13 00143 g004
Table 1. Treatments used to study the effect on aggression. The different treatments are described in detail in the Methods section. One article can contain observations from more than one treatment category.
Table 1. Treatments used to study the effect on aggression. The different treatments are described in detail in the Methods section. One article can contain observations from more than one treatment category.
TreatmentsNumber of ArticlesNumber of Observations
Enrichment3364
Time in the group2738
Strain2635
Group formation2330
Housing condition1623
Other78
Table 2. Aggressive behavior, comparing two strains. BALB/c, C57BL/6, and CD-1 were most commonly used in the complete dataset and are therefore color-coded in this table; BALB/c in purple, C57BL/6 in pink and CD-1 in grey.
Table 2. Aggressive behavior, comparing two strains. BALB/c, C57BL/6, and CD-1 were most commonly used in the complete dataset and are therefore color-coded in this table; BALB/c in purple, C57BL/6 in pink and CD-1 in grey.
Methodological Approach Used to Study Aggression
Less AggressiveMore AggressiveHome Cage
Behavior
Test For
Aggression
WoundingGeneral
Observation
Reference
C57BL/6BALB/c x [75]
C57BL/6BALB/cxx [73]
C57BL/6BALB/cx [74]
C57BL/6129S a x [23]
C57BL/6Swissxx [73]
C57BL/6NIHx [74]
BALB/cCD-1 bx x [20]
BALB/cFVBx x[76]
BALB/cSwissxx [73]
BALB/cNIHx [74]
BALB/cC57BL/6xxx [33]
CBA/JDBA2 x [32]
CD-1BKWx [78]
NODFVBx x[76]
No DifferenceHome Cage
Behavior
Test for
Aggression
WoundingGeneral
Observation
Reference
C57BL/6129S c x [23]
C57BL/6BALB/cx [30]
C57BL/6BALB/cx [65]
C57BL/6BTBRx [77]
C57BL/6BTBRx [61]
C57BL/6 dBTBRx [77]
C57BL/6DBA/2 x [79]
BALB/cNODx x[76]
a In non-enriched cages only. b In severity of wounds. BALB/c had higher number of wounds. c In cages enriched with a combination of several types of enriched items. d More chasing observed in C57BL/6.
Table 3. Articles that studied the effects of group size on aggression. For each study, the group size with least aggression is marked in green. Yellow represents no difference between groups.
Table 3. Articles that studied the effects of group size on aggression. For each study, the group size with least aggression is marked in green. Yellow represents no difference between groups.
Group SizeMethodological Approach Used to
Study Aggression
Strain2345689101220Home Cage
Behavior
Test for
Aggression
WoundingGeneral
Observation
Reference
BALB/c x x x x x [70]
BALB/c x x x x[88]
Varying strainsxxxx x x [89]
Randomly bred xxx x x x [54]
DD/S strain x x x [59]
MF1 x x x [38]
BALB/c x x x [92]
CFLP x x x x [42]
CFLP x x x x [91]
DUB/ICR Swissx x x xx [90]
DUB/ICR Swissx x x xxx [90]
DUB/ICR Swissx x x xx [90]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Weber, E.M.; Zidar, J.; Ewaldsson, B.; Askevik, K.; Udén, E.; Svensk, E.; Törnqvist, E. Aggression in Group-Housed Male Mice: A Systematic Review. Animals 2023, 13, 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010143

AMA Style

Weber EM, Zidar J, Ewaldsson B, Askevik K, Udén E, Svensk E, Törnqvist E. Aggression in Group-Housed Male Mice: A Systematic Review. Animals. 2023; 13(1):143. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010143

Chicago/Turabian Style

Weber, Elin M., Josefina Zidar, Birgit Ewaldsson, Kaisa Askevik, Eva Udén, Emma Svensk, and Elin Törnqvist. 2023. "Aggression in Group-Housed Male Mice: A Systematic Review" Animals 13, no. 1: 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010143

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop