Next Article in Journal
Efficient Genome Editing in Bacillus licheniformis Mediated by a Conditional CRISPR/Cas9 System
Next Article in Special Issue
The Role of the Microbiota–Gut–Brain Axis and Antibiotics in ALS and Neurodegenerative Diseases
Previous Article in Journal
Microbial Safety of Milk Production and Fermented Dairy Products in Africa
Previous Article in Special Issue
Reduction in the Choking Phenomenon in Elite Diving Athletes Through Changes in Gut Microbiota Induced by Yogurt Containing Bifidobacterium animalis subsp. lactis BB-12: A Quasi Experimental Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Gut Microbiota and Infant’s Temperament in the First Year of Life in a Chinese Birth Cohort

1
School of Health Sciences, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430071, China
2
Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally.
Microorganisms 2020, 8(5), 753; https://doi.org/10.3390/microorganisms8050753
Submission received: 16 April 2020 / Revised: 14 May 2020 / Accepted: 15 May 2020 / Published: 17 May 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Microbiota-Gut-Brain Axis)

Abstract

:
Infant temperament characteristics play a critical role in children’s developmental pathways and can predict adulthood psychopathology. The diversity and composition of the gut microbiota are associated with human temperament in both adults and young children. However, the relationship between the gut microbiota and temperament in 12-month-old infants is rarely studied; this developmental period is when temperament reaches a relatively stable stage. We used high-throughput sequencing methods to explore whether temperament characteristics were associated with gut microbiota diversity and composition. Infants’ fecal samples were collected at 12 months of age for the gut microbiota analysis. Based on the primary caregivers’ reports, infants’ temperaments were measured using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire-revised (IBQ-R). This study included 51 infants, including 20 boys and 31 girls, with a mean age of 12.25 months. Results showed that soothability was positively correlated with maternal education level (β = 0.29, p = 0.043, adjust p = 0.025) and the abundance of Bifidobacterium genera (β = 0.62, p = 0.004, adjust p = 0.002). Conversely, cuddliness was negatively correlated with the abundance of Hungatella genera. There was no significant difference in temperament based on gender. This study demonstrated that gut microbiota composition was associated with temperament in 12-month-old infants. These results point to the importance of gut microbiota balance. Future studies on the mechanisms behind the gut microbiota affecting temperament are warranted.

1. Introduction

Studying temperament is an effective approach to study the differences in individual behavioral characteristics [1]. Based on the widely used Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ) [1] and Infant Behavior Questionnaire-revised (IBQ-R) [2], cultural and sociodemographic modulatory factors could shape the development of temperament in early life. These factors include maternal race, maternal education level, caregivers’ psychosocial characteristics, family economic status, numbers of siblings, and feeding type [2,3,4,5]. However, infant temperament differences are hypothesized to be biologically based [6]. In studying infants’ temperaments, previous studies were restricted by infants’ age, sex, and birth order, and there was less focus on the exploration of the natural, biological mechanisms associated with temperament such as gut microbiota, immune factors, and inflammatory cytokines [2,7,8].
The bidirectional microbiota–gut–brain axis (MGB) connects the gut microbiota with the human brain, and this synergy in turn influences human behaviors [9,10]. The MGB axis [11] includes the central nervous system, the enteric nervous system, the autonomic nervous system, the neuro-endocrine (e.g., serotonin) and neuro-immune pathways (e.g., pro-inflammatory cytokines), and the gut microbiome [11,12]. The MGB pathway is proven to affect both the central nervous system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA); additionally, the MGB is proven to modulate multiple pathways such as neurotransmitters (e.g., serotonin), dopamine, and cytokines (e.g., interferon-γ and interleukin-6) [13]. In the growing field of MGB research, an increasing number of studies have found that emotional disturbances and social behaviors are associated with the dysbiosis of the gut microbiota. A randomized controlled human trial has shown that multispecies, probiotic consumption can reduce sadness and aggressive thoughts [14]; e.g., a treatment of Lactobacillus helvecticus R0052 and Bifidobacterium longum R0175 in healthy people can significantly reduce anxiety and depression [15]. Similarly, treatment with Bacteroides fragilis of autism-like behaviors in mice improved behavioral defects including anxiety, compulsive marble burying, and ultrasonic vocalizations [16]. The gut microbiota is intricately associated with specific brain regions that are linked to emotional stimuli and cognition development [17]. Gao and colleagues [18] studied 12-month-old infants using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and found that gut microbiota diversity was associated with functional connectivity between the amygdala and thalamus; gut microbiota diversity was also associated with connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula. These studies suggest a significant connection between gut microbiota and human mental states and emotions.
Evidence supports associations between the gut microbiota and temperament in both children and adults. In adult populations, Kim and Park initially proved that different temperament traits are associated with various enterotypes [19]. Additionally, in a study of 77 children aged 18–27 months old, Christian et al. found that Rikenellaceae genera were significantly associated with temperament in both boys and girls [20]. Coincidentally, Kim and Park have also suggested that temperament differences induced by the gut microbiota may occur in early life [19]. Temperament can be detected as early as 3 months of age and dynamically changes during the first year of life; however, a 12-month-old infant’s temperament will usually remain stable until early childhood [1,2]. Putnam and colleagues [21] found that considerable homotypic continuity existed in human temperament from infancy to 7 years of age. Infant gut microbiota developed rapidly due to the transition to an adult diet after the age of 12 months [22]. Thus, 12 months of age is an intriguing time point for us to study in order to explore the relationship between temperament and gut microbiota. However, whether gut microbiota diversity and composition are associated with temperament in infancy at the age of 12 months old or younger is rarely studied.
In this study, we hypothesized that (1) infants’ temperament characteristics are associated with the gut microbiota diversity and composition at age 12 months; and (2) the temperament differences with respect to the gut microbiota are independent of the infants’ gender, maternal education status, delivery mode, feeding type, and probiotic consumption. The purpose of this study was to examine 51 infants’ gut microbiota (using high-throughput sequencing) and temperament ratings (based on caregivers’ report) at 12 months old and to explore the relationships between these two variables.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design, Setting, and Participants

This study enrolled 62 mother–infant dyads. Mothers during late pregnancy were recruited from an obstetric outpatient clinic in a Grade III A Hospital of Wuhan University in Wuhan, Hubei Province in China. All women were healthy. Infants were followed for 1 year after birth from November 2017 to February 2018. Because 11 of the families could not be reached by telephone or email at 12 months postpartum, 51 healthy infants were enrolled in the study. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Boards of Medical School of Wuhan University (Approval code: JKHL2017-03-03; Approval date: 03/03/2017). Informed consents were obtained from all the participants.

2.2. Data Collection

Every maternal participant completed three questionnaires face-to-face, including demographic characteristics (e.g., maternal age, education status, child’s sex), infants’ early life events (feeding type, probiotic consumption at age 6 months), and an assessment of infants’ temperament at an age of 12 months.
Our previous study [23] revealed that early life Clostridium butyricum consumption was associated with infants’ gut microbiota during later life. We included the probiotic consumption data for analysis in this study. The newborn consumption of probiotics containing Clostridium butyricum (C. butyricum, at least 75 × 10,000,000 CFU) was to facilitate defecation and prevent jaundice in early neonates (0–1 months after birth). In this study, 42 newborns were prophylactically given probiotics.
The Infants Behavior Questionnaire-Revised (IBQ-R) Chinese version was used to assess infants’ temperament at 12 months [2]. The IBQ-R was developed for infants aged 3–12 months old. This measure includes 14 dimensions of temperament that are subdivided into three composite scales: extraversion, negative affectivity, and orienting/regulation. This measure has good reliability and validity [2].
Infants’ fecal samples were collected at participants’ homes by a trained researcher; samples were taken from infants’ diapers according to the Human Microbiome Project (HMP) protocol [24]. Temperament questionnaires were completed at the time of sample collection. After collection, fecal samples were transported in a cooler (+4 °C, 1.5 h on average) to the laboratory and were frozen at −80 °C prior to further analysis. In total, 51 fecal samples were collected for high quality Illumina sequencing. Please refer to our previous study [23] for the processing of DNA extraction, polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplification, sequencing, and data analysis.

2.3. Statistical Methods

We compared infant temperaments between boys and girls using the Mann–Whitney U test. The multivariate analysis of the general liner model (GLM) was used to analyze the significant factors of infant temperament. The Spearman correlation (denoted by r) was used to investigate the association between infant temperament and infant microbiota. We subsequently chose the relevant gut microbiota and prior predictors (e.g., infants’ sex and maternal education status) to identify potential associations in temperament using multiple linear regressions, controlling for delivery mode, feeding type, and probiotic consumption.
For the gut microbiota data analysis, the distribution of microbial community was visualized by Circos at the genus level. Principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) was conducted using the OTU information from each sample according to the Bray–Curtis distance matrix (gender: male vs. female, delivery mode: cesarean vs. vaginal, probiotic consumption: yes vs. no, and feeding type: breast vs. mixed). A permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) was used to analyze the explaining degree of different grouping factors to the sample difference based on OTU level using Bray–Curtis distance matrix. A genus relative abundance ≥ 1% was selected for the abundance analysis.
The data for genera relative abundance were the centered log ratio (CLR) transformed before all analyses. We set the significance level at α = 0.05. Multiple hypothesis tests were adjusted using the Benjamini–Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR), and associations were considered significant below an FDR threshold of 0.05. These analyses were performed using SPSS version 21 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA) and R [25].

3. Results

3.1. Participant Characteristics

This study included 51 infants (20 boys and 31 girls). These infants had a mean age of 12.3 (mean ± SD: 12.3 ± 0.25) months. In this study, all the mothers were of Han nationality; the mean maternal age at the time of delivery was 32.3 (Mean ± SD: 32.3 ± 4.51) years. All mothers were married (Table 1).

3.2. Infant Temperament at 12 Months Old

Mean (SD) scores of three IBQ-R dimensions were 5.11 (0.55) for extraversion, 3.86 (0.74) for negative affectivity, and 4.69 (0.66) for orienting/regulation. In addition, gender presented no differences in temperament ratings according to the Mann–Whitney U test (p = 0.18, p = 0.26, p = 0.62). Maternal education level (β = 0.13, CI = (0.07, 0.19), p = 0.043, adjust p = 0.025) was positively related to soothability.

3.3. Infant Gut Microbiota at 12 Months Old

In total, 2,541,440 usable sequences and 308 OTUs were obtained from these infants using Illumina MiSeq. Pan analysis indicated that only a small number of new shared phylotypes would be expected with additional sequencing. The value of Good’s coverage for all children was 99.6%.
The bacterial distribution was characterized in terms of the relative taxonomic abundances. A total of 10 phyla, 18 classes, 25 orders, 46 families, and 128 genera were obtained from 51 infants’ samples at 12 months of age. Figure 1 shows the taxonomic compositions of the dominant bacteria at the genus level. In terms of the relative abundance, 15 genera were above 1%, and the five most abundant genera were Veillonella (22.6%), Bifidobacterium (17.04%), Bacteroides (14.75%), Escherichia-Shigella (7.87%), and the Ruminococcus gnavus group (5.14%).
Gender was not separated well by principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) based on the Bray–Curtis distance matrix at the OTU level (ANOSIM: R = −0.03, adjust p = 0.56, Figure 2); gender was also not separated by probiotic consumption (ANOSIM: R = −0.08, adjust p = 0.80), feeding type (ANOSIM: R = 0.05, adjust p = 0.15) or delivery mode (ANOSIM: R = 0.05, adjust p = 0.06).

3.4. Association between Infant Temperament and the Gut Microbiota

Among 14 dimensions and three composite scales of IBQ-R, six genera were associated with infant temperament (Figure 3). Multiple linear regressions were conducted when infant temperament was identified as the outcome, and both relevant genera and other potential confounding factors (e.g., delivery mode, feeding type, and probiotic consumption) were controlled in the model. The abundance of Bifidobacterium (β = 0.13, CI = (0.102, 0.158), p = 0.004, adjust p = 0.002) was positively related to soothability. Cuddliness was negatively correlated with abundance of Hungatella in infants (β = −2.78, CI = (−3.77, −1.79), p = 0.008, adjust p = 0. 028).

4. Discussion

Boys and girls showed no significant differences in infant temperament at 12 months old. This finding was consistent with previous studies [7,26,27]. Gartstein [7] explored the stability of infants’ temperament by gender in a sample of 315 children from South Korea, which was the only non-Western study, and found no differences in temperament between boys and girls. However, research in Western countries found that temperament ratings differed significantly by infant gender. The first study of gender differences in temperament was measured by IBQ-R in 2003 [2], and this study showed that female infants showed higher scores on the fear subscale and that male infants were rated higher on the activity and high-intensity pleasure subscales. Another study from the United States [20] found significant differences in temperament based on gender. Boys had higher ratings in high-intensity pleasure and motor activation, while girls reported higher ratings for inhibitory control and soothability. Specifically, compared to boys, girls were rated lower on the surgency/extraversion subscale, and girls were rated much higher on the effortful control subscale. Thus, the discrepancy in temperament differences between boys and girls in these various studies could be a result of cultural differences.
In this study, infant soothability in 12-month-olds increased with higher maternal education. Soothability refers to children’s reduction of fussing, crying, or distress when caregivers use soothing techniques [2]. In practice, caregivers’ soothing techniques are key factors that influence the development of greater soothability in older children. Furthermore, more highly educated mothers may have a more positive view of their parent–child interactions, thus leading to a higher level of intimacy and pleasure for both infants and mothers. Additionally, multiple studies have suggested that maternal education level contributes to positive parent–infant interaction by influencing parenting styles and timing of paternal investment. [28,29]. One study observing 139 Chinese infants aged 12–24 months found that highly educated mothers were more likely to utilize guiding behaviors, and they developed higher levels of intimacy with their infants compared to less-educated mothers [30]. When caring for their children, highly educated mothers tended to lead their children in playing or learning in a variety of ways. The ability to love and feel beloved among highly educated mothers was higher, and these mothers were more likely to perceive that they exerted a positive influence on their children through parenting. Although parenting styles and mothers’ responsiveness to children’s behaviors vary by culture [31], maternal education level is associated with infants’ physical health and psychological behaviors across different cultural contexts; this is consistent with the results of the present study. Behaviors and health outcomes associated with the mother’s education level include infants’ mental health problems [32], watching TV during meals [33], vocabulary acquisition [34], and cognitive and motor outcomes [35]. Overall, our findings indicated that infants’ temperaments were closely related to maternal education level.
This study found that high soothability was associated with a predominant relative abundance of Bifidobacterium in infants’ gut microbiota at 12 months old. Meanwhile, the temperament of cuddliness (ability to express enjoyment and the molding of one’s body to being held by caregivers) was negatively associated with an abundance of Hungatella. In accordance with our study results, previous studies have shown that the human gut microbiota is associated with temperament. In a study of 60 healthy Korean adults, Kim and Park were the first to promote the existence of temperament [19] differences between enterotypes 1 and 2 in adult populations. Compared to enterotype 2, enterotype 1 had significantly higher scores in novelty seeking and reward dependence behaviors. They suggested that differences in temperament and stability of temperament might be shaped by interactions with the gut microbiota during early life stages (e.g., infancy). Furthermore, one study of 77 children aged 18–27 months old [20] found that gut microbiota diversity and composition were associated with temperament as assessed by the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ) [36]; among these children, those with a greater phylogenetic diversity in their gut microbiome exhibited greater surgency/extraversion. Some gender differences were also found; in girls, Rikenellaceae were positively correlated with levels of fear, and in boys, sociability was positively correlated with the abundances of Ruminococcaceae phylum and Parabacteroides. Additionally, there were positive relationships between high-intensity pleasure and Dialister, Rikenellaceae phyla, as well as between activity level and Dialister, Rikenellaceae phyla. In agreement with these previous studies, our research further confirmed the possible effects of the gut microbiota on infant temperament at 12 months old.
Bifidobacterium, which accumulates in large amounts in the human gastrointestinal tract, is an integral part of the human gut microbiota. Multiple studies have verified its remarkable function in immune-modulation [37], obesity [38], infection [39], balance of gut microbiota [40], and especially in mental health disorders [41]. In fact, changes in emotional processing modulated by the gut microbiota have been established by a number of observational studies on probiotic consumption. One study in mice [42] suggested that Bifidobacterium longum 1714 and Bifidobacterium breve 1205 supplements were effective treatments for decreasing anxiety-like behaviors, and they were as effective as the anti-anxiety medication escitalopram. Additionally, a study in rats [43], using an electric shock model, found that consumption of Bifidobacterium longum R0175 over a 30-day period led to a significant decrease in anxiety. In animal models of Alzheimer’s disease [44], it was observed that Bifidobacterium breve treatment may improve cognitive function and counteract some degree of impairment. In a human study, the consumption of Bifidobacterium was shown to effectively improve depression–anxiety conditions, increase defecation frequency, and decrease body mass index in healthy elderly people [45]. Similarly, supplementation with the prebiotic Bifidobacterium longum 1714 resulted in an improvement of the frontal midline electroencephalographic mobility and hippocampus-dependent memory performance in healthy volunteers [46]. Together, these studies suggest that the human anxiety behaviors, depression behaviors, and cognitive function were altered by Bifidobacterium species. This is further supported by our results finding high soothability in infants to be associated with significantly dominant Bifidobacterium in their gut microbiome.
Hungatella is a functional genus that carries genes related to polysaccharide metabolism and is involved in the production of acetate and propionate. Numerous studies have proved that it is responsible for enhancing efficient energy harvesting and gastrointestinal mobility [47,48]. Additionally, in patients experiencing constipation, Hungatella was significantly more abundant compared to healthy controls [49]. Similarly, work in patients with Parkinson’s disease [50] also found that there was a significant positive correlation between the relative abundance of Hungatella and patients’ constipation, which was independent of their Parkinson’s disease. Meanwhile, there has been far less research on the association between mental behaviors and Hungatella. In an observational study of Chinese navy soldiers, it was found [51] that after a long voyage mission, the abundance of Hungatella in the gut microbiota of soldiers had significantly decreased. These findings opened exciting possibilities for exploring the relationship between an increased abundance of Hungatella and lower cuddliness in infants. It is important to explore the relationships between Hungatella and personality or temperament-related behaviors.
There were some limitations that reduced the generalizability of these study findings. The full study provided a small-scale test of associations between infants’ gut microbiota and temperament at the age of 12 months. Additionally, the majority of infants in our study were taking probiotics at the time of sample collection, which may affect the generalizability of the study. Our understanding of these connections could be strengthened by exploring the gut microbiota as it relates to temperament in older children. Future research should replicate this study with a larger sample, using a longitudinal study design to explore the link between infant temperament and gut microbiota and its impact on children’s later life. In addition, this study did not address the role of diet quantity or quality at the time of stool sample collection. Although parents fed their children according to standardized guidelines for infants’ diets, the potential mechanisms of food quantity may account for some of the differences in the associations.

5. Conclusions

This study examined the association between infants’ temperament and the gut microbiota at the age of 12 months. We found that soothability was positively correlated with the abundance of Bifidobacterium and maternal education level. There was a negative correlation between cuddliness and an abundance of Hungatella. This study contributed to our understanding of microbiota–behavior/physiology interactions and confirmed that this relationship is stable and robust by 12 months of age. It also provides a new approach to studying behavioral characteristics in teens, which should start in infancy. We should emphasize the intestinal health and gut microbiota balance before the transition to adult foods. The deficiencies of the study include infants’ probiotic consumption and the small sample size, which somewhat limits the study’s conclusions; consequently, further research in this area may be warranted. From the MGB viewpoint, this study was not designed to address detailed causal interaction pathways between the gut microbiota and temperament. Thus, future research should explore the possibility of incorporating metagenomics and metabolomics and analyzing these associations to understand the causes behind the appearance of this new evidence in a large infant cohort.

Author Contributions

Data curation, Y.W. and Y.Y.; Formal analysis, Y.W.; Funding acquisition, Y.L.; Methodology, J.B.; Project administration, X.C.; Resources, Y.L., Q.Z.; Supervision, X.C.; Validation, J.B.; Writing—original draft, Y.W. and X.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No.81903334).

Acknowledgments

We thank Shanghai Meiji Biomedical Technology Co. Ltd. for help in sample management and sequence production.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rothbart, M.K. Measurement of Temperament in Infancy. Child Dev. 1981, 52, 569–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gartstein, M.A.; Rothbart, M.K. Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 2003, 26, 64–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Cozzi, P.; Putnam, S.P.; Menesini, E.; Gartstein, M.A.; Aureli, T.; Calussi, P.; Montirosso, R. Studying cross-cultural differences in temperament in toddlerhood: United States of America (US) and Italy. Infant Behav. Dev. 2013, 36, 480–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Enlow, M.B.; White, M.T.; Hails, K.; Cabrera, I.; Wright, R.J. The Infant Behavior Questionnaire-Revised: Factor structure in a culturally and sociodemographically diverse sample in the United States. Infant Behav. Dev. 2016, 43, 24–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. de Lauzon-Guillain, B.; Wijndaele, K.; Clark, M.; Acerini, C.L.; Hughes, I.A.; Dunger, D.B.; Wells, J.C.; Ong, K.K. Breastfeeding and Infant Temperament at Age Three Months. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e29326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Gartstein, M.A.; Gonzalez, C.; Carranza, J.A.; Ahadi, S.A.; Ye, R.; Rothbart, M.K.; Yang, S.W. Studying Cross-cultural Differences in the Development of Infant Temperament: People’s Republic of China, the United States of America, and Spain. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2006, 37, 145–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gartstein, M.A.; Putnick, D.; Kwak, K.; Hahn, C.S.; Bornstein, M.H. Stability of temperament in South Korean infants from 6 to 12 to 18 months: Moderation by age, gender, and birth order. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015, 40, 103–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Turkheimer, E.; Waldron, M. Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 78–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Keightley, P.; Pavli, P.; Platten, J.; Looi, J.C.L. Gut feelings 1. Mind, mood and gut in irritable bowel syndrome: Approaches to psychiatric care. Australas. Psychiatry 2015, 23, 403–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Keightley, P.; Pavli, P.; Platten, J.; Looi, J.C.L. Gut feelings 2. Mind, mood and gut in inflammatory bowel disease: Approaches to psychiatric care. Australas. Psychiatry 2015, 23, 407–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dinan, T.G.; Cryan, J.F. The Microbiome-Gut-Brain Axis in Health and Disease. Gastroenterol. Clin. N. Am. 2017, 46, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Kennedy, P.J.; Cryan, J.F.; Dinan, T.G.; Clarke, G. Kynurenine pathway metabolism and the microbiota-gut-brain axis. Neuropharmacology 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Foster, J.; Neufeld, K.A. Gut-brain axis: How the microbiome influences anxiety and depression. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014, 17, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Steenbergen, L.; Sellaro, R.; van Hemert, S.; Bosch, J.A.; Colzato, L.S. A randomized controlled trial to test the effect of multispecies probiotics on cognitive reactivity to sad mood. Brain Behav. Immun. 2015, 48, 258–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Messaoudi, M.; Violle, N.; Bisson, J.F.; Desor, D.; Javelot, H.; Rougeot, C. Beneficial psychological effects of a probiotic formulation (Lactobacillus helveticus R0052 and Bifidobacterium longum R0175) in healthy human volunteers. Gut Microbes 2011, 2, 256–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Hsiao, E.Y.; McBride, S.W.; Hsien, S.; Sharon, G.; Hyde, E.R.; McCue, T.; Codelli, J.A.; Chow, J.; Reisman, S.E.; Petrosino, J.F.; et al. Microbiota modulate behavioral and physiological abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Cell 2013, 155, 1451–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Tillisch, K.; Labus, J.; Kilpatrick, L.; Jiang, Z.; Stains, J.; Ebrat, B.; Guyonnet, D.; Legrain-Raspaud, S.; Trotin, B.; Naliboff, B.; et al. Consumption of fermented milk product with probiotic modulates brain activity. Gastroenterology 2013, 144, 1394–1401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Gao, W.; Salzwedel, A.P.; Carlson, A.L.; Xia, K.; Azcarate-Peril, M.A.; Styner, M.A.; Thompson, A.L.; Geng, X.; Goldman, B.D.; Gilmore, J.H.; et al. Gut microbiome and brain functional connectivity in infants-a preliminary study focusing on the amygdala. Psychopharmacology 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kim, H.; Park, Y.J. The Association between Temperament and Microbiota in Healthy Individuals: A Pilot Study. Psychosom. Med. 2017, 79, 898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Christian, L.M.; Galley, J.D.; Hade, E.M.; Schoppe-Sullivan, S.; Dush, C.K.; Bailey, M.T. Gut microbiome composition is associated with temperament during early childhood. Brain Behav. Immun. 2015, 45, 118–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Putnam, S.P.; Rothbart, M.K.; Gartstein, M.A. Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 2008, 17, 387–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Li, Y.; Faden, H.S.; Zhu, L. The Response of the Gut Microbiota to Dietary Changes in the First Two Years of Life. Front. Pharmacol. 2020, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Wang, Y.; Liu, Y.; Bai, J.; Chen, X. The Effect of Maternal Postpartum Practices on Infant Gut Microbiota: A Chinese Cohort Study. Microorganisms 2019, 7, 511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Yu, J.; Feng, Q.; Wong, S.H.; Zhang, D.; Liang, Q.Y.; Qin, Y.W.; Tang, L.Q.; Zhao, H.; Stenvang, J.; Li, Y.L.; et al. Metagenomic analysis of faecal microbiome as a tool towards targeted non-invasive biomarkers for colorectal cancer. Gut 2017, 66, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. R Core Team. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2019; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 12 January 2019).
  26. Lerner, R.M. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science; WILEY: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  27. Coll, C.T.G.; Halpern, L.F.; Vohr, B.R.; Seifer, R.; Oh, W. Stability and Correlates of Change of Early Temperament in Preterm and Full-Term Infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 1992, 15, 137–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Livas-Dlott, A.; Fuller, B.; Stein, G.L.; Bridges, M.; Mangual Figueroa, A.; Mireles, L. Commands, competence, and carino: Maternal socialization practices in Mexican American families. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 566–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Friend, M.; DeAnda, S.; Arias-Trejo, N.; Poulin-Dubois, D.; Zesiger, P. Developmental changes in maternal education and minimal exposure effects on vocabulary in English- and Spanish-learning toddlers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 164, 250–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lai, J. The Effects of Socioeconomic Status and Parent-Infant Interactions on Early Lexical Development of 12-to 24-Month-Old Infants. Ph.D. Thesis, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  31. Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Kuchirko, Y.; Song, L.L. Why Is Infant Language Learning Facilitated by Parental Responsiveness? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 23, 121–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Meyrose, A.K.; Klasen, F.; Otto, C.; Gniewosz, G.; Lampert, T.; Ravens-Sieberer, U. Benefits of maternal education for mental health trajectories across childhood and adolescence. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 202, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Thompson, A.L.; Adair, L.S.; Bentley, M.E. Maternal Characteristics and Perception of Temperament Associated With Infant TV Exposure. Pediatrics 2013, 131, E390–E397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Deanda, S.; Arias-Trejo, N.; Poulin-Dubois, D.; Zesiger, P.; Friend, M. Minimal second language exposure, SES, and early word comprehension: New evidence from a direct assessment. Biling.-Lang. Cogn. 2016, 19, 162–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Patra, K.; Greene, M.M.; Patel, A.L.; Meier, P. Maternal Education Level Predicts Cognitive, Language, and Motor Outcome in Preterm Infants in the Second Year of Life. Am. J. Perinatol. 2016, 33, 738–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Putnam, S.P.; Gartstein, M.A.; Rothbart, M.K. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 2006, 29, 386–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Ruiz, L.; Delgado, S.; Ruas-Madiedo, P.; Sanchez, B.; Margolles, A. Bifidobacteria and Their Molecular Communication with the Immune System. Front. Microbiol. 2017, 8, 2345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Kondo, S.; Kamei, A.; Xiao, J.Z.; Iwatsuki, K.; Abe, K. Bifidobacterium breve B-3 exerts metabolic syndrome-suppressing effects in the liver of diet-induced obese mice: A DNA microarray analysis. Benef. Microbes 2013, 4, 247–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Wang, Z.H.; Gao, Q.Y.; Fang, J.Y. Meta-analysis of the efficacy and safety of Lactobacillus-containing and Bifidobacterium-containing probiotic compound preparation in Helicobacter pylori eradication therapy. J. Clin. Gastroenterol. 2013, 47, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kondo, J.; Xiao, J.Z.; Shirahata, A.; Baba, M.; Abe, A.; Ogawa, K.; Shimoda, T. Modulatory effects of Bifidobacterium longum BB536 on defecation in elderly patients receiving enteral feeding. World J. Gastroenterol. 2013, 19, 2162–2170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sampson, T.R.; Mazmanian, S.K. Control of Brain Development, Function, and Behavior by the Microbiome. Cell Host Microbe 2015, 17, 565–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Savignac, H.M.; Kiely, B.; Dinan, T.G.; Cryan, J.F. Bifidobacteria exert strain-specific effects on stress-related behavior and physiology in BALB/c mice. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2014, 26, 1615–1627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Messaoudi, M.; Lalonde, R.; Violle, N.; Javelot, H.; Desor, D.; Nejdi, A.; Bisson, J.F.; Rougeot, C.; Pichelin, M.; Cazaubiel, M.; et al. Assessment of psychotropic-like properties of a probiotic formulation (Lactobacillus helveticus R0052 and Bifidobacterium longum R0175) in rats and human subjects. Br. J. Nutr. 2011, 105, 755–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Kobayashi, Y.; Sugahara, H.; Shimada, K.; Mitsuyama, E.; Kuhara, T.; Yasuoka, A.; Kondo, T.; Abe, K.; Xiao, J.Z. Therapeutic potential of Bifidobacterium breve strain A1 for preventing cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 13510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Inoue, T.; Kobayashi, Y.; Mori, N.; Sakagawa, M.; Xiao, J.Z.; Moritani, T.; Sakane, N.; Nagai, N. Effect of combined bifidobacteria supplementation and resistance training on cognitive function, body composition and bowel habits of healthy elderly subjects. Benef. Microbes 2018, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Allen, A.P.; Hutch, W.; Borre, Y.E.; Kennedy, P.J.; Temko, A.; Boylan, G.; Murphy, E.; Cryan, J.F.; Dinan, T.G.; Clarke, G. Bifidobacterium longum 1714 as a translational psychobiotic: Modulation of stress, electrophysiology and neurocognition in healthy volunteers. Transl. Psychiatry 2016, 6, e939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Ohara, T. Identification of the microbial diversity after fecal microbiota transplantation therapy for chronic intractable constipation using 16s rRNA amplicon sequencing. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0214085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Kaur, S.; Yawar, M.; Kumar, P.A.; Suresh, K. Hungatella effluvii gen. nov., sp. nov., an obligately anaerobic bacterium isolated from an effluent treatment plant, and reclassification of Clostridium hathewayi as Hungatella hathewayi gen. nov., comb. nov. Int. J. Syst. Evol. Microbiol. 2014, 64, 710–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Huang, L.S.; Gao, R.Y.; Yan, X.B. Structure analysis of the gut microbiota in chronic functional constipation patients. Chin. J. Colorectal Dis. 2017, 6, 121–126. [Google Scholar]
  50. Zao, A.B. Analysis of Gut Microbiota in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Ph.D. Thesis, Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, China, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  51. Wei, L. Diversity of Gut Flora in Navy Officers and Soldiers Involved in Long Voyage. Ph.D. Thesis, The Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, China, 2017. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Distribution of microbial community for each sample at genus level. The data were visualized by Circos, and the width of the bars from each genus indicate the relative abundance of that genus in the sample.
Figure 1. Distribution of microbial community for each sample at genus level. The data were visualized by Circos, and the width of the bars from each genus indicate the relative abundance of that genus in the sample.
Microorganisms 08 00753 g001
Figure 2. Principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) of infant gut microbiota using Bray–Curtis distance matrix. A two-dimensional PCoA was used to describe the relative abundance of infant gut microbiota based on sex, delivery mode, feeding type, and probiotic consumption. Each point represents a single sample and is color-coded into different groups.
Figure 2. Principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) of infant gut microbiota using Bray–Curtis distance matrix. A two-dimensional PCoA was used to describe the relative abundance of infant gut microbiota based on sex, delivery mode, feeding type, and probiotic consumption. Each point represents a single sample and is color-coded into different groups.
Microorganisms 08 00753 g002aMicroorganisms 08 00753 g002bMicroorganisms 08 00753 g002c
Figure 3. Relationships between bacterial composition and infants’ temperament. The figure shows a heatmap in which samples have been clustered according to their compositional profiles. Bacterial genera appear color-coded according to their under (green) or over-representation (red) in the samples (* 0.01 < p ≤ 0.05, ** 0.001 < p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001).
Figure 3. Relationships between bacterial composition and infants’ temperament. The figure shows a heatmap in which samples have been clustered according to their compositional profiles. Bacterial genera appear color-coded according to their under (green) or over-representation (red) in the samples (* 0.01 < p ≤ 0.05, ** 0.001 < p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001).
Microorganisms 08 00753 g003
Table 1. Demographic and birth characteristics (N = 51).
Table 1. Demographic and birth characteristics (N = 51).
ItemsN (%)/Median (IQR)
Maternal education
 <12 a4 (7.8%)
 12 a31 (60.8%)
 >12 a16 (31.4%)
Sex
 Male20 (39.2%)
 Female31 (60.8%)
Mode of delivery
 Caesarean 29 (56.9%)
 Vaginal 22 (43.1%)
Feeding mode
 Breast 32 (62.7%)
 Mixed 19 (37.3%)
Probiotic consumption
 Yes 42 (82.4%)
 No 9 (17.6%)

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wang, Y.; Chen, X.; Yu, Y.; Liu, Y.; Zhang, Q.; Bai, J. Association between Gut Microbiota and Infant’s Temperament in the First Year of Life in a Chinese Birth Cohort. Microorganisms 2020, 8, 753. https://doi.org/10.3390/microorganisms8050753

AMA Style

Wang Y, Chen X, Yu Y, Liu Y, Zhang Q, Bai J. Association between Gut Microbiota and Infant’s Temperament in the First Year of Life in a Chinese Birth Cohort. Microorganisms. 2020; 8(5):753. https://doi.org/10.3390/microorganisms8050753

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wang, Ying, Xiaoli Chen, Yun Yu, Yanqun Liu, Qing Zhang, and Jinbing Bai. 2020. "Association between Gut Microbiota and Infant’s Temperament in the First Year of Life in a Chinese Birth Cohort" Microorganisms 8, no. 5: 753. https://doi.org/10.3390/microorganisms8050753

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop