Next Article in Journal
Education-to-Work Transitions in Former Communist Countries after 30-Plus Years of Transformation
Previous Article in Journal
Towards Quality Education: An Entrepreneurship Education Program for the Improvement of Self-Efficacy and Personal Initiative of Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Prejudice towards Immigrants: A Conceptual and Theoretical Overview on Its Social Psychological Determinants

by
Flavia Albarello
1,*,
Silvia Moscatelli
2,
Michela Menegatti
2,
Fabio Lucidi
1,
Elisa Cavicchiolo
3,
Sara Manganelli
4,
Pierluigi Diotaiuti
5,
Andrea Chirico
1 and
Fabio Alivernini
1
1
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat 5, 40126 Bologna, Italy
3
Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, 00133 Rome, Italy
4
National Institute for the Evaluation of the Education System (INVALSI), 00153 Rome, Italy
5
Department of Human Sciences, Society and Health, University of Cassino and Southern Lazio, 03043 Cassino, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2024, 13(1), 24; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13010024
Submission received: 19 October 2023 / Revised: 20 December 2023 / Accepted: 21 December 2023 / Published: 26 December 2023

Abstract

:
Immigration processes and the possible marginalization of ethnic minorities in the receiving countries are essential issues in contemporary societies. Prejudice and discrimination can be critical obstacles to immigrants’ integration into the host country and can severely affect their well-being and mental health. This theoretical and conceptual overview aims to highlight the critical social–psychological processes underlying attitudes toward immigrants. First, it tackles the social psychological roots of social prejudice by focusing on the role of individual (ideological, motivational, and cultural) factors and categorization processes. Second, it examines how contextual factors such as intergroup perceptions and structural relations can lead to high levels of prejudice and discrimination towards immigrants. This review highlights how prejudice against immigrants can be driven by various factors at the individual and contextual level, suggesting that programs aimed at facilitating harmonious relations in contemporary multi-ethnic societies should consider such different determinants. Accordingly, the conclusion discusses possible interventions that can promote better relations between the majority and immigrant groups and counteract the negative impact of discrimination.

1. Introduction

The immigration process and the possible marginalization of ethnic minorities in the receiving countries are essential issues that societies have to care about. Social prejudice and discrimination are severe obstacles for immigrant groups to develop a sense of belonging to the new society and are critical for their mental health (The APA Presidential Task Force on Immigration 2013). Thus, prejudice and discrimination1—whether against economic migrants, asylum seekers, or refugees—must be regarded as major concerns for public health.
The issue of immigration is often intensely debated and divisive within society. Many people see immigrants as a threat to their country’s resources, stability, and cultural character (e.g., Esses 2021; ICMPD 2019; Nese 2022), and therefore display their prejudice against them or even behave in a discriminatory way. Socio-psychological research has pointed out that prejudice and its behavioral facet—namely, discrimination–can take different forms and has revealed that, in the last decades, overt intolerance and prejudice seem to have given way to subtler or ambivalent expressions of negative intergroup attitudes (Dovidio et al. 2013; Quillian 2006). Even though immigrants continue to be victims of major discrimination (e.g., being denied housing or being denied a promotion) or hate speech (e.g., Bilewicz and Soral 2020), they often experience more “routine” forms of discrimination: For instance, they are avoided or disrespected in public places, monitored in stores, or treated with condescendence or paternalism (e.g., Taylor et al. 2019). Even immigrant pupils are often harassed and bullied by their peers (e.g., Alivernini et al. 2019; Bayram Özdemir et al. 2016) and risk being socially isolated in the classroom (e.g., Cavicchiolo et al. 2022, 2023). Moreover, teachers frequently evaluate students with immigrant backgrounds as less academically competent than native students (Kleen and Glock 2018; Menegatti et al. 2017), undermining their academic performance, motivation, and well-being (Manganelli et al. 2021; Paletta et al. 2017).
Based on these considerations, the present conceptual and theoretical overview focuses on the main, consolidated, social–psychosocial accounts of the origins of prejudiced attitudes against immigrants to provide a detailed picture of the state of the art on factors that might account for prejudice towards this stigmatized social group. Given that systematic reviews on the social psychological accounts of prejudice already exist (e.g., Brown 2010; Esses 2021), the goal of the present contribution was to provide a comprehensive illustration of the variety of individual (ideological, motivational, and cultural) and contextual (intergroup perceptions and structural relations) factors that can be called into play to explain prejudice and discrimination against immigrants. In this respect, and differently from available reviews on the same topic (cf. Esses 2021), this contribution also aimed to enlarge the analysis of the individual-level factors associated with high levels of prejudice (e.g., epistemic cognitive motivations, moral motivations, and cultural motivations). By doing that, we aim to offer new insights into how multiple factors (e.g., social cognitive processes and individual-level motivations) that previous reviews have considered in isolation might have joint effects on prejudice.
Indeed, acknowledging this fact might completely change how scholars think about intervention tools (e.g., Paluck et al. 2021) since it might reveal—for instance—that intergroup contact works better for individuals with specific motivational mindsets (e.g., in terms of motivated closed-mindedness; Baldner and Pierro 2019b; Dhont et al. 2011).
In the conclusion, we will briefly reason on possible interventions that can promote better relations between the majority and immigrant groups and counteract the negative impact of discrimination.

2. Social Psychological Accounts of Prejudice

Social prejudice has been a crucial issue for social psychological research for decades. Allport (1954) pivotally defined it as “an antipathy based upon a faulty and inflexible generalization (…) directed towards a group as a whole or towards an individual because he is a member of that group»” (p. 10). Importantly, as stressed by Brown (2010), although prejudice can be both positive and negative, its negative, derogatory, and hostile form afflicts many societies worldwide and, therefore, asks for a deep understanding. As underlined in such definitions, the social cognitive roots of prejudice can be found in the social categorization process that leads individuals to distinguish between “us” (the groups they belong to) and “them” (Tajfel and Turner 1979).
Besides paving the way for social cognitive accounts of prejudice based on the process of social categorization, Allport (1954) also provided the idea of a generalized tendency towards prejudice, stating that someone who has prejudicial attitudes towards one group refuses all other (minority) outgroups (cf. Roets and Van Hiel 2011; Roets et al. 2012). This calls into play the problem of whether individual differences explain the endorsement of prejudice or if situational (i.e., contextual) cues shape it. In this regard, we will first outline the person-based approaches that explain differences in prejudice in terms of personal traits, ideologies, or motivations. Second, we will briefly review critical studies concerning the automatic nature of prejudice. Then, we will describe the contextual cues highlighted in the literature as factors eliciting prejudicial views of others.

2.1. The Role of Individual Differences, Ideologies, and Motivations

Some theoretical approaches have provided explanations of prejudicial attitudes and discrimination focusing on individual-level factors. Among these, we can distinguish individuals’ personality traits (e.g., authoritarian personality, Big Five Traits, etc.), ideologies (e.g., right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation), epistemic motivations (e.g., need for cognitive closure), moral motivations (e.g., binding moral foundations), and cultural motivations (e.g., desired cultural tightness).

2.1.1. Personality Traits

Prejudice and intergroup hostility can be predicted based upon stable and enduring personal characteristics. Adorno et al. (1950) were the first to consider prejudice as an expression of a personality trait, namely, the authoritarian personality. People with this trait suffer from the psychopathological consequences of hierarchical parent–child relations and express their hostility by attacking others. Also, there is evidence that the Big Five traits (Goldberg 1990) of agreeableness (i.e., being king and gentle vs. rude and harsh) and openness to experience (i.e., being innovative and unconventional vs. shallow and conventional) predict more positive attitudes towards immigrants; whereas neuroticism (i.e., being moody and anxious vs. relaxed and calm) is associated with more negative attitudes (for a review, see Hodson and Dhont 2015). Moreover, Sibley et al. (2010) highlighted that high scores on personality factors were predictive of prejudice to a low extent if the nature of the target group was not considered (e.g., derogated/low-status outgroups and dangerous groups). More recently, Ashton et al. (2004) proposed the HEXACO model of the personality structure, differently representing Agreeableness and Emotionality. This model also added a sixth dimension, Honesty–Humility, tapping sincerity, fairness, and modesty versus deception, greed, and slyness. This additional dimension is very relevant to prejudice (cf. Hodson and Dhont 2015) since individuals high in Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy show high levels of prejudice (Hodson et al. 2009). These latter clinical constructs (known as the “dark triad”; Jonason and Webster 2010) underline a malicious and antisocial nature and thus are easily associated with prejudice. Overall, it must be acknowledged that these trait-based approaches to prejudice have been criticized since they provide relatively inflexible explanations of intergroup discrimination/prejudice; whereas, in reality, intergroup antipathy can arise and dissipate within dramatically short spaces of time (Abrams and Hogg 1988).

2.1.2. Ideologies

Among person-based variables, the positive association between ideological constructs and prejudice has been underlined (e.g., Duckitt and Sibley 2009). The idea that individuals with authoritarian personalities are more prejudiced has been renewed and updated by Altemeyer (1981), who introduced the concept of Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA). Altemeyer stressed that the roots of RWA are not related to psychodynamic explanations but to teaching and modeling, particularly during adolescence. More specifically, RWA is conceived as an individual’s ideology expressing adherence to conventional norms and values, uncritical submission to authorities, and aggressive feelings towards people violating the norms (Altemeyer 1981). In other words, it expresses the motivational goal of establishing and maintaining societal security, order, and cohesion. This theorization has been recently encompassed in the dual motivational model of ideology, politics, and prejudice formulated by Duckitt and Sibley (2009), who stressed that the path through which RWA leads to high levels of prejudice is rooted in the perception that the social/group context is dangerous and threatening.
Another individual ideology associated with higher prejudicial attitudes and legitimization of inequalities is the social dominance orientation (SDO; Sidanius and Pratto 1999). The SDO indicates an individual’s worldview about social hierarchies and the groups that deserve to be superior to others and are not directly related to a specific political ideology. People high in SDO are less sensitive to moral violations and the welfare of others. In contrast, people with low SDO are motivated by egalitarianism and altruistic social concerns, prioritizing fairness and harm avoidance. Given that SDO is also strongly linked with perceived competition by immigrants (Duckitt and Sibley 2010)—in line with predictions of the ethnic competition theory (Scheepers et al. 2002)—evidence has consistently shown that individuals with high SDO have more negative attitudes toward immigrants (e.g., Küpper et al. 2010).
Developmental studies have underlined this association (e.g., Bratt et al. 2016). In this respect, it is essential to underline that children’s sensitivity to intergroup inequality is associated with parents’ SDO (Tagar et al. 2017). Children of parents with low SDO were more fairness-oriented towards outgroups, whereas those of parents high in SDO favored the ingroup. Interestingly, Albarello et al. (2020) provided longitudinal evidence on the associations between adolescents’ SDO, prejudice towards migrants, and social inclusiveness across time. They found that SDO was associated with higher prejudice at Time 1, which in turn led to higher levels of SDO at a later time (Time 3), thus stressing that SDO is malleable, at least in adolescence.
Among ideologies, Hodson and Dhont (2015) highlighted further “rationalization constructs” or legitimizing myths that provide moral or intellectual legitimization to inequalities in society. People endorsing ideologies that justify the status quo, for instance, the belief that “prejudice is justified, normative, and ‘understandable’” (p. 21), are more likely to accept prejudice towards outgroups.
Similarly, beliefs in a just world (Lerner and Miller 1978) allow people to cope with inequality as they stress an individual’s motivation to believe that good things happen to good people and bad things happen to bad people. Interestingly, such beliefs are related to the justification of the status quo, as encompassed in the system justification theory (Jost 2020; Jost and Hunyady 2003). On the contrary, individual endorsement of values, such as self-transcendence values (Schwartz 2010), can be (negatively) associated with individuals’ prejudicial tendencies.

2.1.3. Epistemic Cognitive Motivations

The need for cognitive closure (NCC; Kruglanski 1989; Kruglanski et al. 2009) is another individual-level difference associated with intergroup biases and prejudice (e.g., Roets et al. 2012, 2015). Even though it can be conceptualized as an individual trait (cf. Webster and Kruglanski 1994), the NCC is a motivation to search for epistemic certainty and avoid the ambiguity that starts when individuals are confronted with a question they do not have an answer to and stops when the answer is found. Individuals with a high NCC tend to prefer stable environments and secure knowledge while disliking change (Kruglanski 2004). For these reasons, they refer to their groups as sources of knowledge stability (i.e., group centrism hypothesis; Kruglanski et al. 2006). As a consequence, high NCC individuals will be more likely to have negative attitudes toward outgroups such as immigrants (Dhont et al. 2011) since they represent a change that might threaten natives’ realistic and symbolic resources (Stephan and Stephan 2000). Interestingly, a recent contribution (Albarello et al. 2023b) related the NCC with the so-called prejudice-prone personality theorized by Allport (1954), providing the first empirical test that individuals with high NCC are prejudiced towards multiple outgroups to the same extent.
The NCC might also be a feature of environments, which can be raised by threatening societal/ecological conditions causing uncertainty. As a consequence, during wars, worldwide pandemics, and economic and environmental crises, the increased NCC might, in turn, intensify prejudice against immigrants (Albarello et al. 2023a; Mula et al. 2022).

2.1.4. Moral Motivations

Other motivational factors that lead to enhanced prejudice towards immigrants have been highlighted by research on moral foundations (e.g., Baldner and Pierro 2019a, 2019b). According to the moral foundations theory (Graham et al. 2011; Haidt 2012), people judge what is right or wrong through moral intuitions rooted in the culture. Specifically, while the individualizing foundations of Fairness and Care emphasize protecting individuals and guaranteeing individual rights and prosperity, the binding foundations emphasize preserving larger groups (e.g., overall culture) through duties, loyalty, and purity. Studies have consistently shown that the binding moral foundations predicted prejudice (e.g., Baldner and Pierro 2019a, 2019b; Bianco et al. 2021; Federico et al. 2016), whereas the individualizing foundations are negatively associated with intergroup prejudice (e.g., Federico et al. 2013; Forsberg et al. 2019). Importantly, research has underlined that these moral motivations can be prompted by other individual-level factors such as RWA, SDO, or NCC. For instance, Hadarics and Kende (2018) showed that binding foundations mediated the effect of RWA on prejudice towards dissident groups, including immigrants. In contrast, individualizing foundations mediated the effect of SDO on the same groups.
Interestingly, Strupp-Levitsky et al. (2020) argued that the epistemic and existential motivations to reduce uncertainty and threat that underlie political ideologies (e.g., conservativism and system justification; Jost et al. 2003) elicit the endorsement of binding moral foundation in terms of preference for ingroup loyalty, defense to authority, and enforcement of purity sanctions (Kugler et al. 2014), thus stressing their role as antecedents of individuals’ higher endorsement of moral motivations to protect their groups.
Overall, these contributions suggest that it is crucial to deepen knowledge of the intertwined effects of different kinds of individual-level antecedents of prejudice and to address the complex psychological processes underlying anti-immigrant prejudice.

2.1.5. Cultural Motivations

Recent contributions bridging insights from cultural psychology and social psychology of prejudice have underlined the role of desired cultural tightness–looseness in affecting prejudice. Cultural tightness is defined as “the strength of social norms, or how clear and pervasive norms are within societies, and the strength of sanctioning, or how much tolerance there is for deviance from norms within societies” (Gelfand et al. 2011, p. 1226). In this vein, strengthening social norms in response to threats serves as an adaptive mechanism that helps individuals coordinate to survive, but it can also lead to intolerant attitudes towards outgroups (e.g., immigrants, Dhont et al. 2011, and homosexuals, Brandt and Reyna 2010). Consistent evidence has been collected on the relation between individuals’ desire for their culture/society to be tight and high levels of anti-immigrant prejudice (e.g., Gelfand 2018; Mula et al. 2021, 2022) since people with high desired cultural tightness view immigrants as sources of chaos disrupting the social order they seek for (cf. Goffman 2009; Jackson et al. 2019).
This individual motivation is indeed fostered by situational uncertainty. For instance, Albarello et al. (2023d) showed that the situational threat of COVID-19 led people with a high NCC (Kruglanski 1989) to desire that their society endorse high cultural tightness through stricter definitions of allowed and forbidden behaviors in order to fight the spread of the virus. This, in turn, led to increased negative attitudes towards immigrants, who were perceived as disturbing the social order or competing with the ingroup of natives in terms of access to resources.

2.2. Categorization Processes and Automatic Ethnic Prejudice

The idea that (ethnic) prejudice stems from the ordinary process of social categorization helps explain why it is so difficult to eradicate it even from societies based on egalitarian principles. In particular, race is an extremely salient dimension of person categorization, which quickly occurs in information processing. For instance, studies conducted using event-related potentials (ERPs) of White people have shown that race effects appeared in the N100 component, which occurred with a mean latency of 122 msec, with larger amplitude for black than white faces (for a review, see Kubota and Ito 2015). Given the association of the N100 with selective attention, the larger ERP responses for black faces could reflect orienting to the more threatening or salient social group. However, other studies have reported divergent findings examining other components. For instance, research involving both Black and White and both Asian and White participants has observed larger P200 responses–which reflects goal-directed attention and perceptual matching–to outgroup faces, irrespective of their race (Dickter and Bartholow 2007; Dickter and Kittel 2012; Willadsen-Jensen and Ito 2008). These results suggest that attentional processes reflected in the P200 are sensitive to the target group membership, with greater attention directed to the threatening or novel cues represented by outgroup targets. Even more interesting is the case of the processing of children’s faces. Indeed, studies have shown that infant faces, whether of own ethnicity or another, capture attention equally as they possess certain features, such as a large head, chubby cheeks, and big eyes, which make them appear cute and trigger the observer’s caregiving system (Proverbio and De Gabriele 2019). Therefore, this “baby-schema” would take precedence over the processing of ethnic affiliation (Dickter and Bartholow 2007).
Similarly, people categorize others more rapidly when they can use race than when they cannot (McCann et al. 1985). This ability appears very early in life: many studies have shown that by three years of age, children effortlessly sort people into racial categories and use membership in these categories to interpret behaviors in accord with the stereotype (Aboud 1989; Katz 1986). Thus, during socialization, cultural beliefs about social groups become well learned and are activated, without conscious awareness or intention, in the presence of members of stereotyped groups, and can consequently influence social thought and behavior (e.g., Brewer 1988; Fiske and Neuberg 1990). The consequences of these automatic activations have been shown by research conducted within the Weapon Identification paradigm. In early experiments (e.g., Payne 2001), participants made visual discriminations between guns and harmless objects right after a black or white face appeared. Results showed that guns were detected faster and were seen more often when the face was black rather than white. Additional studies revealed a similar bias in the decision to “shoot.” In a video game simulation, participants were instructed to “shoot” anyone holding a gun but not to shoot targets carrying anything else. They decided to shoot Black armed targets more quickly and frequently than White targets and not to shoot unarmed targets more quickly and more frequently when they were White rather than Black (Correll et al. 2002).
Despite the above discouraging results, consistent evidence shows that ethnic prejudice is not inevitable. Although virtually everyone knows ethnic stereotypes, their influence can be diminished through controlled processing. Non-prejudiced individuals can intentionally inhibit stereotypes and replace them with belief-based responses when they are motivated to respond without bias, are aware that the stereotype has been activated, and have cognitive resources (Devine 1989). In recent years, several research programs have shown that situational or contextual factors play a central role in this inhibition process. For instance, participants who viewed videos showing Black individuals at an outdoor barbecue or in a church displayed less automatic bias than those exposed to videos depicting Black individuals in a gang or ghetto street context (Mitchell et al. 2003; Wittenbrink et al. 2001). Subsequent studies demonstrated that individuals motivated to control prejudice showed an automatic bias favoring Blacks when the context (e.g., jail) implied that the targets were threatening (Maddux et al. 2005). Other findings showed that participants with high internal and low external (normative) motivation to respond without prejudice and little external motivation to respond without prejudice displayed lower levels of implicit racial bias (Devine et al. 2002). The same was found for those who reported chronically accessible egalitarian values (Moskowitz et al. 2000).
In sum, research has documented that stereotypes and biases are not unconditionally automatically activated and that prejudiced responses can be avoided. For some individuals, negative evaluations are automatically activated when encountering a person of a different ethnicity. However, individuals can be motivated, sincerely or strategically, to monitor and avoid the effects of such activation. Moreover, there are “truly non-prejudiced” individuals who do not experience the activation of the negative ethnic stereotype or may even experience an activation of a positive evaluation (Fazio et al. 1995). Thus, it is possible to think and act without prejudice.

2.3. Contextual Factors Leading to Prejudice

Since the first studies with the minimal group paradigm (which involves laboratory groups whose members are unknown by other participants; Tajfel et al. 1971), scholars have pointed out that categorization—i.e., the mere awareness of belonging to a group distinct from an outgroup—is generally sufficient for individuals to favor their ingroup over others (e.g., Brewer 1979; Rubini et al. 2014). Such a finding has been explained by relying on the concept of social identity, intended as the part of an individual’s self-image that derives from group memberships (Tajfel and Turner 1979). According to social identity theory (SIT), individuals wish to maintain a positive social identity, and, to this aim, search out various forms of positive distinctiveness for their ingroups: for instance, they favor the ingroup and show prejudice against outgroups. Accordingly, whenever individuals think of themselves as being of one national or ethnic group, they are motivated to stress the superiority of such a group over other groups in order to win social comparisons.
Whereas categorization processes are at the roots of intergroup discrimination, research has highlighted the crucial role of contextual cues in determining prejudice against members of outgroups, especially the stigmatized ones. In this section, we will discuss the role of intergroup perceptions in terms of threat, structural differences, and specific contextual cues that affect ethnic prejudice.

2.3.1. Perceived Intergroup Threat

Convergent evidence has stressed that intergroup threat (which can be defined as economic threat, cultural/values threat, safety/security threat, competition, threat to well-being/security, demographic threat, etc.; Esses 2021; for reviews, see Riek et al. 2006; Rios et al. 2018) is an antecedent of negative attitudes towards outgroups (e.g., Salvati et al. 2020), as well as the exclusion of minority groups (e.g., Albarello et al. 2017, 2019). Such relationships should be understood through the lens of various social psychological theorizations emphasizing the role of threat and competition as antecedents of prejudice. Among them, the ethnic competition theory (Scheepers et al. 2002) assumes that mixing different groups will likely elicit intergroup tensions due to competition over resources.
Accordingly, the natives can perceive immigrants as a threat to the ingroup’s welfare (Stephan and Stephan 2000; Scheepers et al. 2002; see also Stephan et al. 2016), representing resource stress (i.e., the perception that, within a society, access to desired resources is limited; Esses et al. 2001, 2010) that leads to perceived group competition for resources (Esses 2021). Support for this contention comes also from sociological studies, which assumed that ethnic heterogeneity works as a threat to ingroup identity and showed that the higher the immigration rate in a country, the lower the support for social welfare expenditure (Eger 2010).
Indeed, one of the most successful theorizations has been formulated by Stephan and Stephan (2000), who distinguished between realistic threat (i.e., a threat to the ingroup’s existence, economic, and political power, or physical or material well-being) and symbolic threat (i.e., threat related to outgroups’ differential morals, values, beliefs, and standards) and showed that both types of threat predict social prejudice towards immigrants. In this respect, Pereira et al. (2010) found that realistic threats due to immigrants (operationalized as threats to well-being/safety and economic threat) mediated the relationship between prejudice and opposition to immigration. In contrast, symbolic threat (i.e., a threat to natives’ culture/identity) mediated the effects of prejudice on opposition to the naturalization of outgroups. Interestingly, the impact of intergroup threat in enhancing prejudice and discrimination towards an ethnic outgroup was also found with an implicit measure of discrimination, such as the level of abstraction of terms used to describe the outgroup (see Rubini et al. 2014). Albarello and Rubini (2018) showed that both realistic (i.e., a threat to ingroup’s resources) and symbolic threats (i.e., a threat to ingroup identity and culture) enhanced linguistic derogation and affective prejudice toward Roma. A similar process can be detected in teachers, who tend to implicitly provide biased evaluations of immigrant pupils (representing a demographic threat) by describing their achievements with more abstract negative and more concrete positive terms than those of native pupils (Menegatti et al. 2017).
Moreover, Albarello et al. (2019) investigated the role of threat to ingroup’s resources and threat to ingroup’s identity and culture on the projection of negative prejudice from one minority, negatively evaluated outgroup (e.g., Roma and Islamic terrorists) onto another super-inclusive and partially overlapping outgroup (e.g., Romanians and Arabs). They found that both realistic and symbolic intergroup threats led to higher affective prejudice towards the super-inclusive outgroup and the perception that the members of the minority outgroup and the super-inclusive one “were all alike,” thus extending negative attitudes from one outgroup onto another. A further study (Albarello and Rubini 2022) showed that both kinds of threat enhanced the denial of human rights to the stigmatized outgroup of migrants via increasing perceived relative deprivation of the ingroup of natives.
Taking a different stance and considering demographical threat related to the (perceived) size of the immigrant population or the rate of arrivals of immigrants in the receiving country (cf. Esses 2021), various contributions highlighted the association between rising numbers of immigrants and the display of negative attitudes toward them (e.g., Hopkins 2010; Strabac 2011).

2.3.2. Uncertainty as Situational Threat

Besides intergroup threats related to the outgroups, research has shown that the uncertainty raised by various situational threats (e.g., economic threats, environmental threats, pandemic-related threats, immigrant threats, etc.) works as a proxy for individual-level factors outlined above as antecedents of prejudice, such as the NCC, binding moral foundations, system justification, etc. (Jost et al. 2003; Strupp-Levitsky et al. 2020). For instance, as argued by Liaquat et al. (2023), threat due to uncertainty explains why Republicans in the U.S. are more resistant to equality-promoting policies, irrespective of their race and ethnicity.
Franks et al. (2022) provided similar evidence by examining the effect of racial framing of COVID-19 outcomes (i.e., stressing the disproportionate impact of the pandemic on minorities) on decreasing support for COVID-19 mitigating policies. They showed that racial framing of the pandemic decreased support for COVID-19 mitigation policies in respondents who were high in racial bias through the mediation of perceived realistic threat related to the coronavirus (Study 1a). Symbolic threat related to the coronavirus did not mediate this relationship. Importantly, this outcome was more pronounced in those with higher beliefs in a just world (Lerner and Miller 1978), thus showing that such beliefs are associated with higher prejudice and lower approval of policies supporting minorities.
Similar findings have been highlighted by research on the NCC. Situational uncertainty operationalized in terms of individual’s concern for the COVID-19 threat led individuals with high NCC to desire their country to be tighter, which in turn increased prejudice towards immigrants (Albarello et al. 2023d; Mula et al. 2022).

2.3.3. Structural Features of Intergroup Relations

Social psychologists have underlined that structural features of intergroup relations can act as antecedents of prejudice and discrimination. Studies adopting the “minimal group paradigm” (Tajfel et al. 1971) identified a series of conditions under which ingroup love/favoritism (e.g., preference for ingroup members) or outgroup hate/derogation (e.g., the allocation of “penalties” to outgroup members) are likely to occur (e.g., Moscatelli et al. 2017; Rubini et al. 2014). For instance, Rubini and colleagues (for a review, see Rubini et al. 2014) showed that both low- and high-power groups attributed more penalties to the outgroup compared to groups in an equal-power situation. Moreover, they also showed that high- and low-status groups described the outgroup in a more biased fashion than equal-status groups, suggesting that status asymmetries enhance outgroup derogation conveyed through language. In a similar vein, Moscatelli et al. (2014) examined the consequences of experiencing relative deprivation (i.e., the perception that the ingroup is worse off than an outgroup) or relative gratification (i.e., the feeling that the ingroup is better off than an outgroup). They found that both experiences increased behavioral and linguistic discrimination towards the outgroup (cf. Rubini et al. 2014).
Even though conducted in the laboratory, these studies demonstrated that contextual structural societal differences could enhance discrimination and prejudicial attitudes towards outgroups by advantaged and disadvantaged social groups. More ecological research showed converging results. For instance, Pettigrew et al. (2008) found that fraternal relative deprivation was linked to prejudice against immigrants in Europe. Guimond and Dambrun (2002), who manipulated relative deprivation and relative gratification by bogus information on respondents’ job prospects compared to an outgroup, found that both conditions increased prejudice against North African immigrants in France compared to a control condition. Dambrun et al. (2006) confirmed this evidence by examining prejudice against African and Western immigrants in a representative sample of South Africans. Interestingly, Postmes and Smith (2009) pointed out that the effects of relative gratification can be explained by increased conformism to the dominant group’s norms: namely, economic and status improvements are connected to support for anti-immigration policies only when the elite’s norms against immigrants are negative rather than positive. Moreover, when the ingroup advantage is portrayed as legitimate (i.e., based on merit), individuals are more prone to rely on traditional prejudice toward ethnic minorities—which implies the belief that such minorities are inherently inferior to the native majority—as a means to justify the intergroup inequalities (LeBlanc et al. 2015).
More recently, Jetten et al. (2021), integrating macro-economic and political science findings with socio-psychological processes, showed that all the wealth groups within a society (i.e., the poor, the middle, and the more affluent groups) become more opposed to immigrants when economic inequality is growing rather than declining. Moreover, extreme-right-wing–anti-immigrant movements can develop not only when the national economy is contracting but also when the economy is booming (Mols and Jetten 2016) since the leaders of those movements are able to turn objective relative gratification into perceived relative deprivation.
In the attempt to examine the contextual factors that determine prejudice, it is crucial to stress that stereotypes and prejudice are socially created and transmitted and that different types of prejudice target different groups. In this respect, Fiske et al. (2002) (see also Cuddy et al. 2008) formulated the stereotype content model (SCM), which provides a heuristic explanation of the peculiar and different contents of prejudice against social groups based on the perception of the groups in terms of warmth (i.e., trustworthiness and sociability) and competence (i.e., how capable or agentic groups are). This model also allows the detection of the multiple dimensions underlying prejudicial portrayals of a group by considering the combination of warmth (high and low) and competence (high and low). As a consequence, four types/clusters of prejudice have been theorized: (a) admiration prejudice (the most positive pattern of prejudice, usually referring to ingroups) targets highly competent and warm groups who do not compete with other groups; (b) paternalistic prejudice targets low competence and high warmth groups (e.g., older people); (c) envious prejudice portrays groups as competent but not warm, that is, as having no positive intentions toward the ingroup (e.g., Asians); and (d) finally, contemptuous prejudice targets low competence and low warmth groups (e.g., homeless people). Notably, the last pattern of prejudice is the worst one since—depending on the connection between contempt/disgust and dehumanization (Harris and Fiske 2007)—it usually conveys a less human perception of the groups.
Considering these variegated socially shared perceptions of specific social groups is of utmost importance if we aim to address the roots of intergroup prejudice thoroughly. Among these, research has highlighted the crucial role of dehumanization (i.e., the denial of full humanness to individuals or groups as a specific facet of prejudice and discrimination; Albarello and Rubini 2008). In the last two decades, a considerable amount of research tackled the denial of full humanness to outgroups as a specific form of prejudice (for reviews, see Haslam 2006; Vaes et al. 2012). With respect to the aim of this contribution, the most critical finding collected in such a field is that the attribution of lesser humanness to outgroups is a factor that enhances the extent to which the group is discriminated. Interestingly, Albarello and Rubini (2015) experimentally manipulated the perceived humanity of a target and found higher linguistic derogation of the less human target expressed in the use of more abstract negative terms (Rubini et al. 2014), as well as the use of derogatory insults (Rubini et al. 2017).

3. Conclusions

Prejudice is a crucial issue that profoundly affects targeted individuals’ and groups’ well-being and mental health. Understanding the factors leading to prejudice and discrimination is critical to developing interventions to reduce their detrimental effects.

3.1. The Multiple Social Psychological Roots of Prejudice

In this contribution, we illustrated the numerous person-based and contextual factors discussed in the inherent literature as leading to higher prejudice towards social groups. Overall, we highlighted the multiple sources of prejudicial attitudes towards stigmatized outgroups such as immigrants. On the one hand, research on contextual determinants of prejudice pointed out how structural features of society (e.g., wealth distribution and economic status) and the perception of immigrants as a threat have predictable effects, suggesting that political and media communication can build on such factors to influence, at least to some degree, individuals’ attitudes and proneness toward integration efforts. On the other hand, studies on person-based determinants can help clarify which individual characteristics result in relatively high and stable levels of prejudice and the necessary interventions. An intertwined analysis of the role of different variables and integration of perspectives can increase the understanding of the multifaceted origins of prejudice (Duckitt and Sibley 2009) and allow practitioners to implement more efficient interventions to challenge the detrimental outcomes of prejudice and discrimination.

3.2. The Good News: Prejudice Can Be Challenged

If we have reviewed the multiplicity of factors associated with prejudice, we should also stress that prejudice can still be challenged. Even though we have shown that person-based factors predispose people to be prejudiced and treat all outgroups as if they were “all alike” (Allport 1954), studies suggest that intervention aimed to reduce prejudice can be effective even for these individuals (cf. Baldner and Pierro 2019b; Roets et al. 2015). For instance, intergroup contact (Pettigrew and Tropp 2006) has been shown to reduce, under precise conditions (e.g., institutional support and monitoring; equal status of groups engaging in contact; common goals; and intergroup cooperation), prejudice towards immigrants in various contexts and with different groups (for a recent review, see Vezzali et al. 2014).
It is also important to underline that intergroup contact (direct or extended) has been consistently employed to develop interventions to reduce prejudice and discrimination at school. Nonetheless, empirical evidence supporting the contact theory in schools, particularly in European settings, is quite controversial (cf. Thijs and Verkuyten 2014; Vervoort et al. 2011). In this respect, Vezzali et al. (2019)—with a sample of Italian elementary school students—unexpectedly showed that direct contact increased negative implicit attitudes toward immigrants, whereas extended contact reduced implicit prejudice only among those with less direct contact experiences.
In addition, studies on the secondary transfer effect of intergroup contact showed that contact might lead to lower prejudice not only towards the specific outgroup (i.e., primary outgroup) met at school, but also towards further outgroups (i.e., secondary outgroups), especially in majority youth (Vezzali et al. 2019). Such evidence suggests that contact and its consequences (e.g., secondary transfer effect and reduction in intergroup anxiety) are most effective for majority members when structural inequalities characterize intergroup relations. Thus, contact cannot be conceived as a panacea for challenging prejudice when the optimal conditions theorized by Allport (1954) (e.g., equal status of the groups) for its efficacy are not met in the specific intergroup context at stake.
Besides contact, other strategies have been outlined as social cognitive tools aimed at challenging prejudice towards immigrants. For instance, relying on the multiple categorization paradigm (i.e., providing more than four categorical dimensions to define outgroups; Crisp and Hewstone 2007) and the common ingroup identity model (Gaertner and Dovidio 2000), Albarello et al. (2018) showed that salience of human identity (Morton and Postmes 2011; Wohl and Branscombe 2005; see also Turner et al. 1987) and providing multiple categorical dimensions to define a Black immigrant target led to the highest reduction in target dehumanization. Likewise, interventions relying on cognitive creativity (Crisp and Turner 2011), such as counterstereotypes or surprising category combinations (Hutter and Crisp 2008; Vasiljevic and Crisp 2013), also reduce prejudice towards others.
Not less importantly, studies in the school domain have shown a negative association between student’s objective socio-economic status (SES) and anti-immigrant prejudice; that is, the higher the SES, the more individuals report positive feelings toward immigrants (Alivernini et al. 2019), and the lower they show anti-immigrant prejudice. Such evidence has been explained by stressing that students from more affluent families have more opportunities than their less advantaged peers to visit different countries and understand more about foreign cultures. As a consequence, on the one hand, this increased contact with different outgroupers contributes to broadening adolescents’ views about their society and increases their tolerance toward it; on the other hand, those adolescents who have less access to socioeconomic resources might perceive immigrants as a possible threat to their interests and as competitors for the same resources, thus leading to less favorable attitudes toward immigrants. This suggests the importance of carefully disentangling the specific role of the multiplicity of factors accounting for prejudice within each specific domain of analysis.

3.3. The Reduction in Prejudice in the School-Related Domain

This contribution uniquely attempted to reach this goal by keeping the analytical view as broad as possible to reach a thorough understanding of the phenomenon at stake. In this respect, recent research allows us to be optimistic: in the very same context wherein youth belonging to stigmatized minority outgroups are exposed to discrimination and prejudice—that is, school—beneficial processes can be stimulated.
Albarello et al. (2023c) pointed out that the contents of prejudice toward immigrants can be affected by the extent to which adolescents are exposed to a classroom context characterized by high levels of classroom openness to discussion (i.e., a climate in which teachers motivate students to feel free to bring up issues to the class, express their own opinions, explore diverse perspectives, and respect the opinions of each other; Gniewosz and Noack 2008). These findings suggest that the classroom, with its specific compositional characteristics, can profoundly affect the pattern of adolescents’ prejudice towards immigrants, as it represents a context wherein very peculiar social processes happen, such as social tuning of attitudes. Thus, interventions to foster an open-to-discussion classroom climate can be effectively implemented to challenge anti-immigrant prejudice. The role of teachers is also fundamental: Karataş et al. (2023) showed that perceived equal treatment by teachers was related to higher positive and lower negative contact over time.
In conclusion, besides stressing the detrimental factors that can lead to prejudice and its automatic nature, it is important to underline that the adverse effects of prejudice can also be challenged in youth. Various interventions are available and can be applied in contexts such as the classroom (e.g., promotion of classroom open-to-discussion climate, Albarello et al. 2023c; inter-ethnic contact institutionally supported by school administration, Vezzali et al. 2014; imagined intergroup positive contact, Vezzali et al. 2014; and making chronically salient multiple categorization dimensions to define outgroupers, cf. Albarello et al. 2020), the domain wherein youth spend most of their time and meet different others who belong to groups varying in terms of ethnicity, religion, socioeconomic status, sexual preference, etc. By reducing the experience of being prejudiced in school, young people can feel that they are respected members of the national society, and such a feeling can increase their well-being (Liebkind and Jasinskaja-Lahti 2000). This virtuous circle might have positive outcomes in society when young people grow to the age of adults. Moreover, it has to be underlined that the intervention strategies that might be implemented in the school, as well as in other domains (e.g., work domain, sports domain, etc.), all require support by institutions—for instance, through the definition of laws aimed at punishing the display of prejudicial attitudes and enactment of discriminatory behaviours (e.g., social exclusion; hate speech; bullying, etc.)—institutionalizing and guaranteeing their actualization, as Gordon Allport (1954) contended in his pivotal contribution on intergroup contact as a resource for promoting more harmonious relations with different groups. Even more importantly, as underlined in this contribution, it has to be acknowledged that the intervention strategies aimed at challenging prejudice highlighted in the literature (cf. Paluck et al. 2021) need to be carefully implemented considering the intertwined effects of individual-level variables (e.g., NCC, binding moral foundations, and desired cultural tightness) that might moderate or augment their effectiveness.

3.4. Recommendations

This contribution highlights the multiplicity and complexity of the origins of prejudice. At the same time, it raises the question of how to implement interventions that target the different factors involved. For example, community interventions aimed to promote acceptance of a refugee settlement in a certain area must be able to take into account both the possible feelings of threat, deprivation, and competition raised by such settlement and the ideological variables that can fuel prejudice towards immigrants. At the same time, outreach actions should not disregard the “natural” reactions triggered by meeting people from different and unfamiliar groups, as well as the beneficial effects of exposure to multicultural environments. Having in mind the various factors that feed distrust, fear, or negative views of immigrants—as illustrated in this review—might help maximize the effectiveness of the intervention. Similarly, this review highlights the need for research to consider simultaneously different determinants of negative attitudes towards immigrants (as well as negative attitudes of immigrants towards the native population; e.g., Tropp and Pettigrew 2005), exploring how factors at different levels intertwine and reinforce each other. As Allport (1954) stated, “plural causation is the primary lesson we wish to teach” (p. xii). Nearly seventy years later, such a claim still represents a key reminder to researchers and practitioners who wish to ameliorate reciprocal attitudes between natives and immigrants and facilitate the success of interventions in different domains.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.A. (Flavia Albarello), S.M. (Silvia Moscatelli), M.M., and F.A. (Fabio Alivernini); writing-original draft preparation F.A. (Flavia Albarello), S.M. (Silvia Moscatelli) and M.M.; writing—review and editing F.A. (Flavia Albarello), S.M. (Silvia Moscatelli), M.M., F.L., E.C., S.M. (Sara Manganelli), P.D., A.C. and F.A. (Fabio Alivernini). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Note

1
In line with existing literature, from now on we will broadly refer to prejudice as an attitude, thus having cognitive, affective, and behavioral components (cf. Hovland and Rosenberg 1960) and to discrimination as its behavioral facet.

References

  1. Aboud, Frances E. 1989. Children and Prejudice. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  2. Abrams, Dominic, and Michael Hogg. 1988. Social Identifications: A Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations and Group Processes. Hoboken: Taylor and Francis. [Google Scholar]
  3. Adorno, Theodor W., Else Frenkel-Brunswik, Daniel J. Levinson, Nevitt Sanford, and Peter Eli Gordon. 1950. The Authoritarian Personality. London and New York: Verso. [Google Scholar]
  4. Albarello, Flavia, and Monica Rubini. 2008. Relazioni intergruppi e fenomeni di deumanizzazione. Psicologia Sociale, Social Psychology Theory & Research 3: 67–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Albarello, Flavia, and Monica Rubini. 2015. The Role of reduced humanity in producing linguistic discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41: 224–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Albarello, Flavia, and Monica Rubini. 2018. Linguistic discrimination toward Roma: Can intergroup threat enhance bias? Journal of Language and Social Psychology 37: 350–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Albarello, Flavia, and Monica Rubini. 2022. At the roots of attribution of human rights to migrants. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 1046616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Albarello, Flavia, Elisabetta Crocetti, and Monica Rubini. 2020. Prejudice and inclusiveness in adolescence: The role of social dominance orientation and multiple categorization. Child Development 91: 1183–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Albarello, Flavia, Elisabetta Crocetti, Francesca Golfieri, and Monica Rubini. 2023a. The Language of adolescents in depicting migrants. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 42: 183–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Albarello, Flavia, Federico Contu, Roberto Vecchione, Molly Ellenberg, Arie W. Kruglanski, and Antonio Pierro. 2023b. At the roots of Allport’s “prejudice-prone personality”: The impact of need for cognitive closure on prejudice towards different outgroups and the mediating role of binding moral foundations. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 97: 101885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Albarello, Flavia, Francesco Foroni, Miles Hewstone, and Monica Rubini. 2017. Generalisation of Roma onto Romanians: Evidence of the outgroup projection effect. Psicologia Sociale, Social Psychology Theory & Research 12: 239–49. Available online: https://www.rivisteweb.it/doi/10.1482/87249 (accessed on 20 November 2023).
  12. Albarello, Flavia, Francesco Foroni, Miles Hewstone, and Monica Rubini. 2019. ‘They are all alike’: When negative minority outgroups are generalized onto superordinate inclusive outgroups. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 73: 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Albarello, Flavia, Richard J. Crisp, and Monica Rubini. 2018. Promoting beliefs in the inalienability of human rights by attributing uniquely human emotions. Journal of Social Psychology 158: 309–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Albarello, Flavia, Sara Manganelli, Elisa Cavicchiolo, Fabio Lucidi, Andrea Chirico, and Fabio Alivernini. 2023c. Addressing adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants: The role of the classroom context. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 52: 951–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Albarello, Flavia, Silvana Mula, Federico Contu, Conrad Baldner, Arie W. Kruglanski, and Antonio Pierro. 2023d. Addressing the effect of concern with COVID-19 threat on prejudice towards immigrants: The sequential mediating role of need for cognitive closure and desire for cultural tightness. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 93: 101755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Alivernini, Fabio, Elisa Cavicchiolo, and Sara Manganelli. 2019. Brothers, ants or thieves: Students’ complex attitudes towards immigrants and the role of socioeconomic status and gender in shaping them. Social Psychology of Education 22: 629–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Allport, Gordon W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Reading: Addison-Wesley. [Google Scholar]
  18. Altemeyer, Bob. 1981. Right-Wing Authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ashton, Michael C., Kibeom Lee, Marco Perugini, Piotr Szarota, Reinout E. De Vries, Lisa Di Blas, Kathleen Boies, and Boele De Raad. 2004. A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86: 356–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Baldner, Conrad, and Antonio Pierro. 2019a. Motivated prejudice: The effect of need for closure on anti-immigrant attitudes in the United States and Italy and the mediating role of binding moral foundations. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 70: 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Baldner, Conrad, and Antonio Pierro. 2019b. The trials of women leaders in the workforce: How a need for cognitive closure can influence acceptance of harmful gender stereotypes. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research 80: 565–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bayram Özdemir, Sevgi, Metin Özdemir, and Håkan Stattin. 2016. What Makes youth harass their immigrant peers? Understanding the risk factors. The Journal of Early Adolescence 36: 601–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bianco, Fleur, Ankica Kosic, and Antonio Pierro. 2021. COVID-19 and prejudice against migrants: The mediating roles of need for cognitive closure and binding moral foundations. A comparative study. The Journal of Social Psychology 161: 477–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bilewicz, Michał, and Wiktor Soral. 2020. Hate speech epidemic. The dynamic effects of derogatory language on intergroup relations and political radicalization. Political Psychology 41: 3–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Brandt, Mark J., and Christine Reyna. 2010. The role of prejudice and the need for closure in religious fundamentalism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36: 715–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bratt, Christopher, Jim Sidanius, and Jennifer Sheehy-Skeffington. 2016. Shaping the development of prejudice: Latent growth modeling of the influence of social dominance orientation on outgroup affect in youth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 42: 1617–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Brewer, Marilynn B. 1979. In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin 86: 307–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Brewer, Marilynn B. 1988. A dual process model of impression formation. In Advances in Social Cognition. Edited by Thomas K. Srull and Robert S. Wyer. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, vol. 1, pp. 1–36. [Google Scholar]
  29. Brown, Rupert. 2010. Prejudice: Its Social Psychology, 2nd ed. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cavicchiolo, Elisa, Fabio Lucidi, Pierluigi Diotaiuti, Andrea Chirico, Federica Galli, Sara Manganelli, Monica D’Amico, Flavia Albarello, Laura Girelli, Mauro Cozzolino, and et al. 2022. Adolescents’ characteristics and peer relationships in class: A population study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 8907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Cavicchiolo, Elisa, Sara Manganelli, Dora Bianchi, Valeria Biasi, Fabio Lucidi, Laura Girelli, Mauro Cozzolino, and Fabio Alivernini. 2023. Social Inclusion of immigrant children at school: The impact of group, family and individual characteristics, and the role of proficiency in the national language. International Journal of Inclusive Education 27: 146–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Correll, Joshua, Bernadette Park, Charles M. Judd, and Bernd Wittenbrink. 2002. The Police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83: 1314–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Crisp, Richard J., and Miles Hewstone. 2007. Multiple social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 39: 163–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Crisp, Richard J., and Riannon N. Turner. 2011. Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity. Psychological Bulletin 137: 242–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cuddy, Amy J. C., Susan T. Fiske, and Peter Glick. 2008. Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 40: 61–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Dambrun, Michaël, Donald M. Taylor, David A. McDonald, Jonathan Crush, and Alain Méot. 2006. The relative deprivation-gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans toward Immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 1032–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Devine, Patricia G. 1989. Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56: 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Devine, Patricia G., Ashby E. Plant, David M. Amodio, Eddie Harmon-Jones, and Stephanie L. Vance. 2002. The Regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82: 835–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Dhont, Kristof, Arne Roets, and Alain Van Hiel. 2011. Opening closed minds: The combined effects of intergroup contact and need for closure on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37: 514–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Dickter, Cheryl L., and Bruce D. Bartholow. 2007. Racial ingroup and outgroup attention biases revealed by event-related brain potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2: 189–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Dickter, Cheryl L., and Julie A. Kittel. 2012. The effect of stereotypical primes on the neural processing of racially ambiguous faces. Social Neuroscience 7: 622–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Dovidio, John F., Miles Hewstone, Peter Glick, and Victoria M. Esses. 2013. The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination. Los Angeles: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  43. Duckitt, John, and Chris G. Sibley. 2009. A dual-process motivational model of ideology, politics, and prejudice. Psychological Inquiry 20: 98–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Duckitt, John, and Chris G. Sibley. 2010. Right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation differentially moderate intergroup effects on prejudice. European Journal of Personality 24: 583–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Eger, Maureen A. 2010. Even in Sweden: The effect of immigration on support for welfare state spending. European Sociological Review 26: 203–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Esses, Victoria M. 2021. Prejudice and discrimination toward immigrants. Annual Review of Psychology 72: 503–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Esses, Victoria M., John F. Dovidio, Lynne M. Jackson, and Tamara L. Armstrong. 2001. The immigration dilemma: The role of perceived group competition, ethnic prejudice, and national identity. Journal of Social Issues 57: 389–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Esses, Victoria M., Lynne M. Jackson, and Caroline Bennett-AbuAyyash. 2010. Intergroup competition. In The Sage Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination. Edited by John F. Dovidio, Miles Hewstone, Peter Glick and Victoria M. Esses. London: Sage, pp. 225–40. [Google Scholar]
  49. Fazio, Russell H., Joni R. Jackson, Bridget C. Dunton, and Carol J. Williams. 1995. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69: 1013–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Federico, Christopher M., Christopher R. Weber, Damla Ergun, and Connie Hunt. 2013. Mapping the connections between politics and morality: The multiple sociopolitical orientations involved in moral intuition. Political Psychology 34: 589–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Federico, Christopher M., Pierce Ekstrom, Michal R. Tagar, and Allison L. Williams. 2016. Epistemic motivation and the structure of moral intuition: Dispositional need for closure as a predictor of individualizing and binding morality. European Journal of Personality 30: 227–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Fiske, Susan T., Amy J. C. Cuddy, Peter Glick, and Jun Xu. 2002. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82: 878–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Fiske, Susan T., and Steven L. Neuberg. 1990. A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 23: 1–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Forsberg, Erik, Artur Nilsson, and Øyvind Jørgensen. 2019. Moral dichotomization at the heart of prejudice: The role of moral foundations and intolerance of ambiguity in generalized prejudice. Social Psychological and Personality Science 10: 1002–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Franks, Andrew, Y. Jenny Xiao, and Farhang Hesami. 2022. Racial framing of pandemic outcomes has conditional indirect effects on support for COVID-19 mitigation policies: Examining moral and threat-based mediating mechanisms. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 2: 130–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Gaertner, Samuel L., and John F. Dovidio. 2000. Reducing Intergroup Bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  57. Gelfand, Michele J. 2018. Rule Makers, Rule Breakers: Tight and Loose Cultures and the Secret Signals That Direct Our Lives. New York: Scribner Book Company. [Google Scholar]
  58. Gelfand, Michele J., Jana L. Raver, Lisa Nishii, Lisa M. Leslie, Janetta Lun, Beng Chong Lim, Lili Duan, Assaf Almaliach, Soon Ang, Jakobina Arnadottir, and et al. 2011. Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-Nation Study. Science 332: 1100–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Gniewosz, Burkhard, and Peter Noack. 2008. Classroom climate indicators and attitudes towards foreigners. Journal of Adolescence 31: 609–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Goffman, Erving. 2009. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. New York: Simon and Schuster. [Google Scholar]
  61. Goldberg, Lewis. R. 1990. An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59: 1216–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Graham, Jesse, Brian A. Nosek, Johnathan Haidt, Ravu Iyer, Spassena Koleva, and Peter H. Ditto. 2011. Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101: 366–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Guimond, Serge, and Michaël Dambrun. 2002. When prosperity breeds intergroup hostility: The effects of relative deprivation and relative gratification on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28: 900–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hadarics, Márton, and Anna Kende. 2018. The dimensions of generalized prejudice within the dual-process model: The mediating role of moral foundations. Current Psychology 37: 731–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Haidt, Johnathan. 2012. The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion. New York: Pantheon. [Google Scholar]
  66. Harris, Lasana T., and Susan T. Fiske. 2007. Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2: 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Haslam, Nick. 2006. Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10: 252–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Hodson, Gordon, and Kristof Dhont. 2015. The Person-based nature of prejudice: Individual difference predictors of intergroup negativity. European Review of Social Psychology 26: 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hodson, Gordon, Sarah M. Hogg, and Cara C. MacInnis. 2009. The role of “dark personalities” (narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), Big Five personality factors, and ideology in explaining prejudice. Journal of Research in Personality 43: 686–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Hopkins, Daniel D. 2010. Politicized places: Explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition. American Political Science Review 104: 40–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hovland, Carl I., and Milton J. Rosenberg. 1960. Attitude Organization and Change: An Analysis of Consistency Among Attitude Components. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  72. Hutter, Russell C., and Richard J. Crisp. 2008. Reduced constituent category application in surprising combinations. The Journal of Social Psychology 148: 247–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. ICMPD. 2019. Vienna Migration Conference Report 2019. Available online: https://www.icmpd.org/about-us/vienna-migration-conference/vienna-migration-conference-2019 (accessed on 20 November 2023).
  74. Jackson, Joshua Conrad, Marieke van Egmond, Virginia K. Choi, Carol R. Ember, Jamin Halberstadt, Jovana Balanovic, Inger N. Basker, Klaus Boehnke, Noemi Buki, Ronald Fischer, and et al. 2019. Ecological and cultural factors underlying the global distribution of prejudice. PLoS ONE 14: e0221953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Jetten, Jolanda, Kim Peters, Belén Álvarez, Bruno G. S. Casara, Michael Dare, Kelly Kirkland, Ángel Sánchez-Rodríguez, Hema Preya Selvanathan, Stefanie Sprong, Porntida Tanjitpiyanond, and et al. 2021. Consequences of economic inequality for the social and political vitality of society: A social identity analysis. Political Psychology 42: 241–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Jonason, Peter K., and Gregory D. Webster. 2010. The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment 22: 420–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Jost, John T. 2020. A Theory of System Justification. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  78. Jost, John T., and Orsolya Hunyady. 2003. The psychology of system justification and the palliative function of ideology. European Review of Social Psychology 13: 111–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Jost, John T., Jack GLase, Arie W. Kruglanski, and Frank J. Sulloway. 2003. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin 129: 339–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Karataş, Savaş, Katharina Eckstein, Peter Noack, Monica Rubini, and Elisabetta Crocetti. 2023. Meeting in school: Cultural diversity approaches of teachers and intergroup contact among ethnic minority and majority adolescents. Child Development 94: 237–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Katz, Phyllis A. 1986. Modification of children’s gender-stereotyped behavior: General issues and research considerations. Sex Roles 14: 591–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kleen, Hannah, and Sabine Glock. 2018. A further look into ethnicity: The Impact of stereotypical expectations on teachers’ judgments of female ethnic minority students. Social Psychology of Education 21: 759–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Kruglanski, Arie W. 1989. Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases. Perspectives in Social Psychology. New York: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar]
  84. Kruglanski, Arie W. 2004. The Psychology of Closed Mindedness. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  85. Kruglanski, Arie W., Antonio Pierro, Lucia Mannetti, and Eraldo De Grada. 2006. Groups as epistemic providers: Need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. Psychological Review 113: 84–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kruglanski, Arie W., Mark Dechesne, Edward Orehek, and Antonio Pierro. 2009. Three decades of lay epistemics: The why, how, and who of knowledge formation. European Review of Social Psychology 20: 146–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Kubota, Jennifer T., and Tiffany A. Ito. 2015. You were always on my mind: How event-related potentials inform impression formation research. In Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. Edited by Todd D. Nelson. London: Psychology Press, pp. 295–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Kugler, Matthew, John T. Jost, and Sharareh Noorbaloochi. 2014. Another look at moral foundations theory: Do authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain liberal-conservative differences in “moral” intuitions? Social Justice Research 27: 413–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Küpper, Beate, Carina Wolf, and Andreas Zick. 2010. Social status and anti-immigrant attitudes in Europe: An examination from the perspective of social dominance theory. International Journal of Conflict and Violence 4: 205–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. LeBlanc, Josée, Ann M. Beaton, and Iain Walker. 2015. The Downside of being up: A new look at group relative gratification and traditional prejudice. Social Justice Research 28: 143–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Lerner, Melvin J., and Dale T. Miller. 1978. Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin 85: 1030–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Liaquat, Usman, John T. Jost, and Emily Balcetis. 2023. System justification motivation as a source of backlash against equality-promoting policies—And what to do about it. Social Issues and Policy Review 17: 131–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Liebkind, Karmela, and Inga Jasinskaja-Lahti. 2000. Acculturation and psychological well-being among immigrant adolescents in Finland: A Comparative study of adolescents from different cultural backgrounds. Journal of Adolescent Research 15: 446–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Maddux, William W., Jamie Barden, Marilynn B. Brewer, and Richard E. Petty. 2005. Saying no to negativity: The effects of context and motivation to control prejudice on automatic evaluative responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41: 19–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Manganelli, Sara, Elisa Cavicchiolo, Fabio Lucidi, Federica Galli, Mauro Cozzolino, Andrea Chirico, and Fabio Alivernini. 2021. Differences and similarities in adolescents’ academic motivation across socioeconomic and immigrant backgrounds. Personality and Individual Differences 182: 111077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. McCann, C. Douglas, Thomas M. Ostrom, Linda K. Tyner, and Mark L. Mitchell. 1985. Person perception in heterogeneous groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49: 1449–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Menegatti, Michela, Elisabetta Crocetti, and Monica Rubini. 2017. Do gender and ethnicity make the difference? Linguistic evaluation bias in primary school. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 36: 415–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Mitchell, Jason P., Brian A. Nosek, and Mahzarin R. Banaji. 2003. Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132: 455–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Mols, Frank, and Jolanda Jetten. 2016. Explaining the appeal of populist right-wing parties in times of economic prosperity: Economic prosperity and populist right-wing parties. Political Psychology 37: 275–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Morton, Thomas A., and Tom Postmes. 2011. Moral duty or moral defence? The effects of perceiving shared humanity with the victims of ingroup perpetrated harm. European Journal of Social Psychology 41: 127–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Moscatelli, Silvia, Flavia Albarello, Francesca Prati, and Monica Rubini. 2014. Badly off or better off than them? The impact of relative deprivation and relative gratification on intergroup discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107: 248–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Moscatelli, Silvia, Miles Hewstone, and Monica Rubini. 2017. Different Size, Different Language? Linguistic Ingroup favoritism and outgroup derogation by majority and minority groups. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 20: 757–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Moskowitz, Gordon B., Amanda R. Salomon, and Constance M. Taylor. 2000. Preconsciously controlling stereotyping: Implicitly activated egalitarian goals prevent the activation of stereotypes. Social Cognition 18: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Mula, Silvana, Daniela Di Santo, Elena Resta, Farin Bakhtiari, Conrad Baldner, Erica Molinario, Antonio Pierro, Michele J. Gelfand, Emmy Denison, Maximilian Agostini, and et al. 2022. Concern with COVID-19 Pandemic threat and attitudes towards immigrants: The mediating effect of the desire for tightness. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3: 100028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Mula, Silvana, Daniela Di Santo, Michele J. Gelfand, Cristina Cabras, and Antonio Pierro. 2021. The mediational role of desire for cultural tightness on concern with COVID-19 and perceived self-control. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 713952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Nese, Annamaria. 2022. Migrations in Italy and perceptions of ethnic threat. Journal of International Migration and Integration 24: 939–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Paletta, Angelo, Fabio Alivernini, and Sara Manganelli. 2017. Leadership for learning: The relationships between school context, principal leadership and mediating variables. International Journal of Educational Management 31: 98–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Paluck, Elisabeth Levy, Roni Porat, Chelsey S. Clark, and Donald P. Green. 2021. Prejudice reduction: Progress and challenges. Annual Review of Psychology 72: 533–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Payne, Keith B. 2001. Prejudice and Perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 181–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Pereira, Cícero, Jorge Vala, and Rui Costa-Lopes. 2010. From Prejudice to discrimination: The legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants. European Journal of Social Psychology 40: 1231–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Pettigrew, Thomas F., and Linda R. Tropp. 2006. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 751–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Pettigrew, Thomas F., Oliver Christ, Ulrich Wagner, Roel W. Meertens, Rolf van Dick, and Andreas Zick. 2008. Relative deprivation and intergroup prejudice. Journal of Social Issues 64: 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Postmes, Tom, and Laura G. E. Smith. 2009. Why do the privileged resort to oppression? A look at some intragroup factors. Journal of Social Issues 65: 769–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Proverbio, Alice Mado, and Valeria De Gabriele. 2019. The other-race effect does not apply to infant faces: An ERP attentional study. Neuropsychologia 126: 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Quillian, Lincoln. 2006. New approaches to understanding racial prejudice and discrimination. Annual Review of Sociology 32: 299–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Riek, Blake M., Eric W. Mania, and Samuel L. Gaertner. 2006. Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10: 336–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Rios, Kimberly, Nicholas Sosa, and Hannah Osborn. 2018. An experimental approach to intergroup threat theory: Manipulations, moderators, and consequences of realistic vs. symbolic threat. European Review of Social Psychology 29: 212–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Roets, Arne, Alain Van Hiel, and Kristof Dhont. 2012. Is sexism a gender issue? A motivated social cognition perspective on men’s and women’s sexist attitudes toward own and other gender. European Journal of Personality 26: 350–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Roets, Arne, and Alain Van Hiel. 2011. Allport’s prejudiced personality today: Need for closure as the motivated cognitive basis of prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science 20: 349–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Roets, Arne, Arie W. Kruglanski, Malgorzata Kossowska, Antonio Pierro, and Ying-yi Hong. 2015. The motivated gatekeeper of our minds. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 52: 221–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Rubini, Monica, Alessandra Roncarati, Marcella Ravenna, Flavia Albarello, Silvia Moscatelli, and Gün R. Semin. 2017. Denying psychological properties of girls and prostitutes: The role of verbal insults. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 36: 226–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Rubini, Monica, Michela Menegatti, and Silvia Moscatelli. 2014. The Strategic role of language abstraction in achieving symbolic and practical goals. European Review of Social Psychology 25: 263–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Salvati, Marco, Nicola Carone, Valeria De Cristofaro, Mauro Giacomantonio, and Roberto Baiocco. 2020. Support for discriminatory behaviours against immigrants in Italy: Perceived threat and positive beliefs mediate the effect of contact with immigrants. International Journal of Psychology 55: 543–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Scheepers, Peer, Mérove Gijsberts, and Marcel Coenders. 2002. Ethnic exclusionism in European countries. Public opposition to civil rights for legal migrants as a response to perceived ethnic threat. European Sociological Review 18: 17–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Schwartz, Shalom H. 2010. Basic values: How they motivate and inhibit prosocial behavior. Prosocial Motives Emotion and Behavior 14: 221–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Sibley, Chris G., Jessica Harding, Ryan Perry, Frank Asbrock, and John Duckitt. 2010. Personality and prejudice: Extension to the HEXACO personality model. European Journal of Personality 24: 515–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Sidanius, Jim, and Felicia Pratto. 1999. Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Stephan, Walter G., and C. White Stephan. 2000. An integrated threat theory of prejudice. In Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination. Edited by Stuart Oskamp. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate, pp. 23–45. [Google Scholar]
  129. Stephan, Walter G., Oscar Ybarra, and Kimberly Rios. 2016. Intergroup threat theory. In Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. Edited by Stuart Oskamp. London: Psychology Press, pp. 255–78. [Google Scholar]
  130. Strabac, Zan. 2011. It is the eyes and not the size that matter: The real and the perceived size of immigrant populations and anti-immigrant prejudice in Western Europe. European Societies 13: 559–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Strupp-Levitsky, Michael, Sharareh Noorbaloochi, Andrew Shipley, and John T. Jost. 2020. Moral “foundations” as the product of motivated social cognition: Empathy and other psychological underpinnings of ideological divergence in “individualizing” and “binding” concerns. PLoS ONE 15: e0241144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Tagar, Michal R., Chelsea Hetherington, Deborah Shulman, and Melissa Koenig. 2017. On the path to social dominance? Individual differences in sensitivity to intergroup fairness violations in early childhood. Personality and Individual Differences 113: 246–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Tajfel, Henri, and John C. Turner. 1979. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Intergroup Relations: Essential Readings. Edited by William G. Austin and Stephen Worchel. Monterey: Brooks/Cole, pp. 33–47. [Google Scholar]
  134. Tajfel, Henri, Michael G. Billig, Robert P. Bundy, and Claude Flament. 1971. Social Categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology 1: 149–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Taylor, Robert Joseph, Ivy Forsythe-Brown, Dawne M. Mouzon, Verna M. Keith, David H. Chae, and Linda M. Chatters. 2019. Prevalence and correlates of everyday discrimination among Black Caribbeans in the United States: The Impact of nativity and country of origin. Ethnicity & Health 24: 463–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. The APA Presidential Task Force on Immigration. 2013. Crossroads: The Psychology of immigration in the new century. Journal of Latina/o Psychology 1: 133–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Thijs, Jochem, and Maykel Verkuyten. 2014. School ethnic diversity and students’ interethnic relations. British Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Tropp, Linda R., and Thomas F. Pettigrew. 2005. Relationships between intergroup contact and prejudice among minority and majority status groups. Psychological Science 16: 951–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Turner, John C., Micheal A. Hogg, Penellaphe J. Oakes, Stephen D. Reicher, and Margareth S. Wetherell. 1987. Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory. New York: Basil Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  140. Vaes, Jeroen, Jacques P. Leyens, Maria P. Paladino, and Mariana P. Miranda. 2012. We are human, they are not: Driving forces behind outgroup dehumanisation and the humanisation of the ingroup. European Review of Social Psychology 23: 64–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Vasiljevic, Milica, and Richard J. Crisp. 2013. Tolerance by surprise: Evidence for a generalized reduction in prejudice and increased egalitarianism through novel category combination. PLoS ONE 8: e57106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Vervoort, Miranda H. M., Ron H. J. Scholte, and Peer L. H. Scheepers. 2011. Ethnic composition of school classes, majority–minority friendships, and adolescents’ intergroup attitudes in the Netherlands. Journal of Adolescence 34: 257–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Vezzali, Loris, Gian A. Di Bernardo, Sofia Stathi, Emilio P. Visintin, and Miles Hewstone. 2019. Using intercultural videos of direct contact to implement vicarious contact: A school-based intervention that improves intergroup attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 22: 1059–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Vezzali, Loris, Miles Hewstone, Dora Capozza, Dino Giovannini, and Ralf Wölfer. 2014. Improving intergroup relations with extended and vicarious forms of indirect contact. European Review of Social Psychology 25: 314–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Webster, Donna M., and Arie W. Kruglanski. 1994. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 1049–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Willadsen-Jensen, Eve C., and Tiffany A. Ito. 2008. A foot in both worlds: Asian Americans’ perceptions of Asian, White, and racially ambiguous faces. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 11: 182–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Wittenbrink, Bernd, Charles M. Judd, and Bernadette Park. 2001. Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 815–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Wohl, Michael J., and Nyla R. Branscombe. 2005. Forgiveness and collective guilt assignment to historical perpetrator groups depend on level of social category inclusiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 288–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Albarello, F.; Moscatelli, S.; Menegatti, M.; Lucidi, F.; Cavicchiolo, E.; Manganelli, S.; Diotaiuti, P.; Chirico, A.; Alivernini, F. Prejudice towards Immigrants: A Conceptual and Theoretical Overview on Its Social Psychological Determinants. Soc. Sci. 2024, 13, 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13010024

AMA Style

Albarello F, Moscatelli S, Menegatti M, Lucidi F, Cavicchiolo E, Manganelli S, Diotaiuti P, Chirico A, Alivernini F. Prejudice towards Immigrants: A Conceptual and Theoretical Overview on Its Social Psychological Determinants. Social Sciences. 2024; 13(1):24. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13010024

Chicago/Turabian Style

Albarello, Flavia, Silvia Moscatelli, Michela Menegatti, Fabio Lucidi, Elisa Cavicchiolo, Sara Manganelli, Pierluigi Diotaiuti, Andrea Chirico, and Fabio Alivernini. 2024. "Prejudice towards Immigrants: A Conceptual and Theoretical Overview on Its Social Psychological Determinants" Social Sciences 13, no. 1: 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13010024

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop