Next Article in Journal
Effects of the COVID-19 Pandemic on the Lives of Women with Different Socioeconomic Backgrounds and Victimization Experiences in Portugal
Previous Article in Journal
Educating in History: Thinking Historically through Historical Reenactment
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

Comparing the Effects of Class Origins versus Race in the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty

1
Department of Sociology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, USA
2
Department of Sociology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2022, 11(6), 257; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11060257
Submission received: 28 February 2022 / Revised: 29 May 2022 / Accepted: 5 June 2022 / Published: 10 June 2022

Abstract

:
Building upon prior research on intergenerational income mobility, we assess class effects versus racial effects on the probability of becoming a poor adult, broken down by gender. We define the class effect (for each race-and-gender group) as the difference between the probability that a person who was born into the lowest income quintile becomes poor and the probability that a person who was born into the highest income quintile becomes poor. For each minority-by-gender group, using Whites as the baseline, the racial effect is defined as the average racial differential in the probability of becoming a poor adult, irrespective of class origins. The results indicate that, for all minority-by-gender groups, the class effect is larger than the racial effect. Our findings underscore the continuing significance of the comparatively large effects of class origins, which have not been adequately acknowledged in recent research.

1. Social Stratification in America: The Enduring Significance of Race and Poverty

Poverty continues to persist in the United States, despite the general trend towards continued economic growth (Brady and Jäntti 2016). This pattern is evident even with regard to the US Census’s official poverty measure, which is an absolute measure of poverty that depends only on the absolute (i.e., not relative) level of income (Iceland 2006). Because the benefits of economic growth tend to disproportionately accrue to the upper two quintiles of the income distribution, the official poverty rate has been relatively stagnant while household income inequality has been increasing in recent decades (Brady and Jäntti 2016; Economic Policy Institute 2021; Iceland 2006). In 2019, the Gini coefficient for household income inequality in the United States was 0.484 (Semega et al. 2020), which is higher than other developed nations (Thevenot 2017).
In addition to the persistence of poverty and a higher level of economic inequality, another endemic feature of social stratification is the income differentials by racial groups in American society (Iceland 2017). Much of the focus has been on African Americans, who have most notably faced a long history of discrimination (Wilson 1980). Other major racial categories that are often considered are Native Americans, Hispanics, and Asian Americans (Marger 2015). Of these groups, Native Americans have the longest history, while most of the growth of the Hispanic and Asian American populations has occurred in recent decades (Iceland 2017). African Americans, Native Americans, and Hispanics all have lower average socioeconomic outcomes, while Asian Americans tend to be on par with or even higher than Whites (Iceland 2017; Marger 2015; Sakamoto et al. 2009).
Because both poverty and racial minority status are notable disadvantages (with the exception of Asian Americans with regard to minority differentials), our analysis seeks to clarify which disadvantage has a greater overall total negative effect on the chances for upward intergenerational income mobility. That is, we seek to clarify whether childhood poverty or racial minority status, per se, is more consequential in reducing one’s “life chances” (Wilson 1980, p. 1), or, in other words, a favorable income position in contemporary American society. Most persons may be characterized as having been born poor (or not) and as having been born a racial minority (or not). Our objective is to assess whether having had an impoverished childhood is more or less than the disadvantage that is associated with racial minority status.
Poverty status and racial minority status are correlated because African Americans, Native Americans, and Hispanics, especially, have significantly higher poverty rates than Whites (Iceland 2017). Our analysis seeks to assess the net effects of poverty status and minority status, per se, by calculating the disadvantage of poverty for each minority group separately, and then comparing that with the disadvantage of minority status overall. In other words, for each minority group considered separately, is their general disadvantage vis-à-vis Whites larger than their disadvantage of being born poor vis-à-vis wealthy (among members of the same minority group)? For each minority group, the differential “life chances” associated with being born poor versus being born wealthy may be considered as a particular type of class effect because the differential is calculated within the given minority group so that it is inherently “controlling for” race.

2. The Resurgence of Critical Race Theory in American Sociology

Our research interest is in comparing the minority effect with the class effect (i.e., in terms of being born poor) for each racial-by-gender group because this important issue has not been adequately investigated in recent research. For example, Baker et al. (2021) emphasize “the enduring significance” of racial effects on poverty and pay little attention to the issue of class effects. While we agree that racial effects are generally significant, our investigation provides further insight by placing them into a broader context by comparing their sizes relative to the effects of class origins, rather than simply ignoring the latter (e.g., Bonilla-Silva 2004). Sociological theory and research should not be restricted to considering just one variable, such as race. A more informative analysis should go beyond ascertaining whether a certain net effect is greater than zero and also investigate which nonzero net effects are greater than other nonzero net effects.
In general, critical race theories have proliferated and have become highly influential in recent years in American sociology (e.g., Alexander 2012; Baker et al. 2021; Bonilla-Silva 2004, 2015; Bonilla-Silva and Baiocchi 2001; Bonilla-Silva and Dietrich 2011; Christian et al. 2019, 2021; Conley 2010; Feagin 2013, 2020; Ray et al. 2017). The common theme in these studies is the assumption that minority disadvantages are greater than class disadvantages. The primary focus is on minorities as being more likely to be lower income. Middle-class and wealthy minorities are only considered in terms of also having faced racism (e.g., Feagin 1991; Ladson-Billings 2009). Class effects among minority groups themselves are ignored, and are, thus, implicitly assumed to be small and substantively insignificant. Our analysis of intergenerational mobility fills an important lacuna because our study is the first to ascertain class effects among minority groups (in terms of the advantages of the affluent versus the poor), and to investigate the size of these net class effects relative to overall minority disadvantages.

3. Data and Methods

Class effects are various because different points in the distribution of income may be compared. In the following, we focus on the contrast between lower-income households versus higher-income households. The former is commonly referred to as poor, while the latter is often considered to be affluent. In focusing on this contrast, we do not mean to imply that other class contrasts are unimportant, but only that they may be further explored in future research. We investigate the contrast between poverty versus affluence because it is the clearest manifestation of the rising class inequality in the United States, as was discussed above.
The main source of data for our analysis are the reported findings of Chetty et al. (2020), including their Online Appendix. Chetty et al. (2020) is a monumental, path-breaking study that has been cited over 800 times in only two years.1 Chetty et al. (2020) is based on unprecedented access to literally hundreds of millions of records of confidential administrative data. The inordinately large quantity of data is needed in order to accurately ascertain long-term income, and especially for minorities, whose sample sizes are generally too small to be adequately represented in the available longitudinal datasets. Teams of researchers and computer programmers were needed to complete this research, and a great deal of funding for it was provided by a variety of agencies and foundations (Chetty et al. 2020, p. 203). While some prior research has investigated the available longitudinal data with much smaller samples to study White versus Black differentials (Bhattacharya and Mazumder 2011; Bloome 2014), Chetty et al. (2020) is the first (and, thus far, only) research that uses data for millions of Americans that provides detailed intergenerational income mobility statistics for other minority groups as well. Given its very high quality and notably important substantive significance, further refined consideration of Chetty et al.’s (2020) statistical findings is merited.
The three main data sources utilized by Chetty et al. (2020) include the 2000 and the 2010 US Censuses, the 2005–2015 American Community Surveys, and federal income tax records, which are maintained by the US Internal Revenue Service. Persons were first identified by their Social Security numbers, and their records were then anonymously linked across these data sources. After incorporating measures for parents’ and their children’s incomes, the final sample size investigated by Chetty et al. (2020) amounts to over twenty million Americans, including both citizens and US permanent residents.
The target population is all persons who were born in the United States between 1978 and 1983, and who were claimed as a child dependent on a US federal income tax form in at least one year from 1994 to 2015. To be included in the sample, the parents needed to be residing in the United States legally, while the offspring also needed to be linked to their parents’ tax data from 1994, 1995, and 1998 through 2000. Finally, the offspring’s own income needed to be linked to W-2 forms for 2014 and 2015, when this cohort was between the ages of 31 and 37 years.
The parental income was defined as the gross total household income (that is, pre-tax earnings), according to their federal tax filings for the years 1994, 1995, and 1998 through 2000. The offspring income was similarly defined in terms of pre-tax earnings, although, as noted above, this information was sourced from W-2 forms rather than federal income tax forms. To estimate the income mobility, the earnings of the parents and children were ranked relative to those in the same cohort group (that is, other parents and children during the collection periods). In doing so, the comparative mobility between children and parents is always framed in reference to the earnings of their peers during the same timeframe.
With regard to our operationalization of poverty, the limitations of the US Census official measure have been documented, and important research has been conducted on the conceptualization and measurement of different definitions of poverty (Brady 2003). We do not mean to gainsay the significance of this prior research, but, for our research purposes, in the following, we generically use the term “poverty” to refer to the bottom quintile of the distribution of long-term household income. Defined as having long-term income in the bottom quintile, we can ascertain poverty rates for different racial groups as directly reported by Chetty et al. (2020), including in the Online Appendix.
Poverty in contemporary developed nations has a major relative component (Brady 2003; Rainwater and Smeeding 2003; Smeeding 2016). Abundant evidence indicates that households whose money income is appreciably below the median have long been socially viewed as poor people in American society (e.g., Isenberg 2017; Lynd and Lynd 1937; Pals and Kaplan 2013; Rainwater 1974; Warner and Lunt 1941). In the contemporary United States, households in the bottom quintile of the distribution of income would typically be considered poor by many statistical measures of relative poverty (Iceland 2006; Smeeding 2016). Since the US Census official poverty hovered around from 12% to 15% for the time period of Chetty et al.’s data (1989 through 2015), many households in the bottom quintile would likely be considered poor by the official definition as well.2
While we refer to the lowest quintile of long-term household income as poverty, we use the term “affluence” to refer to the highest quintile of long-term household income. As mentioned above, we focus on class in terms of the contrast between the poor versus the affluent. Operationalizing these class positions in terms of long-term income quintiles, we define the class effect as the difference between the probability that a person who was born into the lowest income quintile becomes a poor adult and the probability that a person who was born into the highest income quintile becomes a poor adult. This calculation refers to the advantage (i.e., lower probability) that a child born into affluence has over a child born into poverty in terms of becoming a poor adult. This class effect is calculated separately for each minority group so that race is “held constant.” In other words, for each racial group separately considered, we estimate the higher probability that a person born into a poor household has on the probability of becoming a poor adult compared to a person born into an affluent household (i.e., where poverty and affluence are based on the quintile distribution of long-term incomes, as described above).
Defined in this way, the class effect may be calculated using the published findings in Chetty et al. (2020). We use their reported statistics, which are further described below. By utilizing their detailed results, which are provided in the Online Appendix of Chetty et al. (2020), we are able to furthermore break down the class effects by both gender (i.e., male versus female) and racial category simultaneously (i.e., non-Hispanic Whites, Hispanics, non-Hispanic African Americans, non-Hispanic Asian Americans, and non-Hispanic Native Americans). Hereafter, we refer to these racial categories as Whites, Hispanics, Blacks, Asians, and Native Americans, respectively.
For each minority group, a total racial effect is defined as the average racial differential in the probability of becoming a poor adult, irrespective of class origins. Using Whites as the baseline category, the racial effect is the difference in the probability that a person who was born into the jth minority group becomes a poor adult and the probability that a person who was born into the White group becomes a poor adult. This calculation thus refers to the overall racial differential between a White child and the jth minority child in the chances of becoming a poor adult.
In summary, the probability of adult poverty if born into poverty is a percentage, as reported by Chetty et al. (2020), and may be denoted as Pa|Po. We designate this quantity as Pa|Po, meaning poverty (P) as an adult (a) given poverty (P) in origins (o). The probability of affluence if born into poverty is another percentage that is also reported by Chetty et al. (2020) and may be denoted as Pa|Ao. We designate this quantity as Pa|Ao, meaning poverty (P) as an adult (a) given affluence (A) in origins (o). The class effect is then equal to (Pa|Po–Pa|Ao), which refers to the difference in the percentage (i.e., probability) of poor kids versus affluent kids becoming poor adults. This difference is calculated separately for each racial group so that minority status is not a confounding factor.
The minority effects use Whites as the baseline for comparison. As noted earlier, these effects refer to the difference for each minority compared to Whites with regard to the chances of becoming a poor adult, regardless of class origins. For each minority group considered separately, the overall probability of becoming a poor adult may be designated as Pa, and so Pa|M refers to the percentage (i.e., probability) of becoming a poor adult for minority group M.3 Then, Pa|W refers to the overall probability of becoming a poor adult for Whites. The net minority effect is calculated as (Pa|M–Pa|W).

4. Empirical Results

Table 1 shows the sample sizes for the various demographic groups, as reported by Chetty et al. (2020) in the Online Appendix, Table 5.4 The largest group is White men, with 6,891,000. The sample sizes for Hispanic men, Black men, Asian men, and Native American men are 1,312,000, 1,348,000, 350,000, and 84,000, respectively. The sample sizes for women are similar for each racial group. The smallest sample size is 82,000 for Native American women, which is still considerable for the purpose of calculating univariate statistics. Because of these very large sample sizes, we interpret Chetty et al.’s (2020) reported probability calculations as being substantively quite close to the actual population parameters.
Table 2 shows our calculated class effects for each race-by-gender group. The probabilities given in Column A refer to the percentage of persons born into poverty who become poor as an adult (i.e., Pa|Po). The probabilities shown in Column B refer to the percentage of persons born into affluence who become poor as an adult (i.e., Pa|Ao). These two columns of data were obtained directly from Chetty et al. (2020) in the Online Appendix, Table 5.
The class effect is shown in Column C in our Table 2. This class effect is equal to the difference between the probability of becoming a poor adult if born poor and the probability of becoming a poor adult if born affluent. That is, as noted above, the class effect equals (Pa|Po–Pa|Ao). Calculated for each racial group separately, the class effect thus indicates the disadvantage of persons born into poverty compared to persons born into affluence with regard to becoming a poor adult.
The class effects, as shown in Table 2, seem rather sizeable, with the possible exceptions of Asian men (i.e., 8.0%) and Asian women (i.e., 5.2%). The class effects for White men, Hispanic men, Black men, and Native American men are 21.3%, 15.1%, 27.0%, and 28.4%, respectively. The class effects for White men, Black men, and Native American men are thus each over twenty percent. By contrast, the difference in the class effect for Black men versus White men is 27.0–21.3%, which is equal to a fairly modest 5.7%. Native American men have the largest class effect, although it is only slightly greater than for Black men (i.e., 28.4% versus 27.0%).
Table 2 shows that, within racial groups, the class effects are smaller for women than for men. The class effects for White women, Hispanic women, Black women, and Native American women are 19.4%, 10.4%, 15.0%, and 25.1%, respectively. Native American women have the largest class effect, which is also the only one over twenty percent. However, the class effect for White women is also fairly high, being quite close to twenty percent. The difference in the class effect for Black women versus White women is 15.0–19.4%, which is equal to −4.4%, indicating that poor White women have a slightly greater class disadvantage than poor Black women.
Table 3 shows the racial minority effects by gender. As discussed above, these effects refer to the difference for each minority compared to Whites with regard to the chances of becoming a poor adult, regardless of class origins. Since the overall probability of becoming a poor adult is designated as Pa, then Pa|M refers to the probability of becoming a poor adult for minority group M. Because Pa|W refers to the overall probability of becoming a poor adult for Whites, the minority effect may be calculated as (Pa|M–Pa|W), as was mentioned earlier.
As is shown in Table 3, the minority effects are 6.5% for Hispanic men, 22.8% for Black men, and 20.9% for Native American men. The minority effects for Black men and for Native American men are thus fairly similar, while the minority effect for Hispanic men is smaller. According to the US Census official poverty measure, Hispanics have a substantially higher poverty level than Whites, but Chetty et al.’s results for the offspring generation refer only to a particular cohort of native born, whereas low-income Hispanics are disproportionately foreign born (Orrenius et al. 2011). With regard to Asian men, their minority effect is –0.8%, which indicates that they are actually a bit advantaged over Whites with regard to becoming a poor adult, which is consistent with prior research on native-born Asian men (Takei and Sakamoto 2011).
For women, Table 3 shows that the minority effects tend to be somewhat less disadvantageous than is the case for the corresponding male group. The minority effect for Black women is 8.1%, which is notably smaller than the 22.8% for Black men. This differential is consistent with prior research reporting more negative racial effects for Black men relative to Black women (Greenman and Xie 2008; Autor et al. 2019). Although lower, the minority effect for Native American women is 17.7%, which is fairly close to Native American men. Having a minority effect of just 2.7%, Hispanic women are only slightly disadvantaged in comparison to White women. The minority effect for Asian women is −3.2%, which indicates that they are slightly more advantaged over White women than Asian men are over White men.
Table 4 shows the comparisons between the class effect and the minority effect for each of the minority-by-gender groups. The difference between these two effects is given in Column C of Table 4. For each of the groups, the class effect is larger than the minority effect. The differences for Hispanic men, Black men, Asian men, and Native American men are 8.6%, 4.2%, 8.8%, and 7.5%, respectively. Table 4 also shows that the differences for Hispanic women, Black women, Asian women, and Native American women are 7.7%, 6.9%, 8.4%, and 7.4%, respectively. For each minority group, the class effect tends to be a little lower for women than for men, but overall, these results indicate a consistent similarity across all of these groups. That is, for all of the groups, the class effect is at least several percentage points greater than the minority effect. This consistency occurs despite more variation across the groups in the class effects and the minority effects, as summarized in Columns A and C of Table 4.

5. Discussion and Conclusions

Sakamoto and Kim (2010) analyzed the differentials on productivity versus annual earnings. Their results indicate substantial underpayment for workers in the lowest quintile of the distribution of annual earnings. That is, relative to their contributions to economic productivity, workers in the lowest quintile were underpaid by about 53%. By contrast, workers in the highest quintile were overpaid by about 162% relative to their contributions to economic productivity. Sakamoto and Kim (2010) interpret these findings as evidence for rising economic exploitation that is associated with workers in the lower versus the upper ends of the earnings distribution. The latter contributes directly to inequality in the distribution of household income, in which the upper quintile currently obtains 50.5% of the total income while the lowest quintile receives 3.6% (Semega et al. 2020, p. 45). These and other related results of class inequality (e.g., Leicht 2016; Sakamoto and Kim 2014) are completely ignored; however, there are many studies in critical race theory that emphasize race as the dominant factor relating to poverty (e.g., Alexander 2012; Bonilla-Silva 2004, 2015; Bonilla-Silva and Baiocchi 2001; Bonilla-Silva and Dietrich 2011; Christian et al. 2019, 2021; Feagin 2013, 2020; Ray et al. 2017).
In the foregoing, we have extended the study of poverty to intergenerational mobility, broken down by race-by-gender categories. Children from the lowest quintile are much more likely to become poor adults than are children from the upper quintile. As shown in Table 2, this class effect is substantial (i.e., double-digit in terms of percentage points) for all of the races by gender groups, with the slight exception of Asians, who are known for an exceptionally high level of upward mobility (Chetty et al. 2020; Sakamoto and Wang 2021). Table 2 and Table 3 further indicate that the class effects and the minority effects tend to be slightly less disadvantageous for females as compared to males for each racial category. With regard to gender differentials, the most notable group is Blacks, among whom the men are significantly more likely than White men to become poor adults, and that differential is, furthermore, significantly greater than the higher chances of Black women becoming poor compared to White women.5
Nonetheless, for all of the race-by-gender groups, the class effect is greater than the minority effect, as shown in Table 4. This conclusion also applies to Black men, among whom the class effect is larger than among White men, Hispanic men, Asian men, and any of the female groups, including Native American women. Overall, our findings indicate that the effects of class inequality on intergenerational mobility are large as well as consistently greater than the minority effects. Poverty and racial inequality are often discussed synonymously in terms of “white supremacy”, as if the class inequalities within racial groups are too trivial to mention (e.g., Bonilla-Silva 2004, p. 941). However, most income inequality is within racial groups and not between them (Sakamoto and Wang 2015), and our analysis reveals that class effects on intergenerational mobility are substantial for all race-by-gender groups, except perhaps Asians.
These conclusions are consistent with prior arguments that state that an increased focus on the expansion of civil rights laws alone will not significantly reduce poverty in the United States (Sakamoto and Wang 2015; Wilson 1980). Studies of critical race theory have become immensely popular in recent years (Douchet 2021), influencing not only sociology, but also criminal justice, popular media and entertainment, workplace training, legal and political discourse, educational reforms at all levels, and popular political movements, such as “Black Lives Matter” (Butcher and Gonzalez 2020). Our results that find large class effects for all racial groups clarify that reducing racial discrimination, per se, is clearly not sufficient to eliminate the substantial intergenerational transmission of poverty. While our research objective has not been to critique critical race theory, our analysis nonetheless demonstrates the continuing significance of large class effects in determining economic disadvantages for all racial groups in the contemporary United States.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S.; methodology, M.E.J.; software, L.H.; validation, A.S.; formal analysis, L.H; investigation, M.E.J.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review, A.S.; supervision, A.S.; project administration, A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Notes

1
According to Google Scholar, as retrieved on 28 May 2022.
2
Our use of the term “poverty” is also loose because we do not adjust for household size, as is often done (Smeeding 2016). Our research objective is based on the results of Chetty et al. (2020), who do not use any equivalence scaling.
3
Chetty et al. (2020) do not report Pa for any of the groups. These figures are instead available in Table 2 and Table 3, located on the webpage associated with their publication (https://opportunityinsights.org/data/) (accessed on 10 October 2021).
4
The Online Appendix for Chetty et al. (2020) does not contain any page numbers. Chetty et al. (2020) report these sample sizes to the nearest 1000.
5
For related discussion and analysis of this issue, see Autor et al. (2019).

References

  1. Alexander, Michelle. 2012. The New Jim Crow: Mass Incarceration in the Age of Colorblindness. New York: New Press. [Google Scholar]
  2. Autor, David, David Figlio, Krzysztof Karbownik, Jeffrey Roth, and Melanie Wasserman. 2019. Family Disadvantage and the Gender Gap in Behavioral and Educational Outcomes. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 11: 338–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Baker, Regina S., David Brady, Zachary Parolin, and Deadric T. Williams. 2021. The Enduring Significance of Ethno-Racial Inequalities in Poverty in the U.S., 1993–2017. Population Research and Policy Review 40: 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bhattacharya, Debopam, and Bhashkar Mazumder. 2011. A Nonparametric Analysis of Black–White Differences in Intergenerational Income Mobility in the United States. Quantitative Economics 2: 335–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bloome, Deirdre. 2014. Racial Inequality Trends and the Intergenerational Persistence of Income and Family Structure. American Sociological Review 79: 1196–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo. 2004. From Bi-Racial to Tri-Racial: Towards a New System of Racial Stratification in the USA. Ethnic and Racial Studies 27: 931–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo. 2015. More than Prejudice: Restatement, Reflections, and New Directions in Critical Race Theory. Sociology of Race and Ethnicity 1: 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo, and David Dietrich. 2011. The Sweet Enchantment of Color-Blind racism in Obamerica. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 634: 190–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo, and Gianpaolo Baiocchi. 2001. Anything but Racism: How Sociologists Limit the Significance of Racism. Race and Society 4: 117–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Brady, David. 2003. Rethinking the Sociological Measurement of Poverty. Social Forces 81: 715–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Brady, David, and Markus Jäntti. 2016. Economic Performance, Poverty, and Inequality in Rich Countries. In The Oxford Handbook of the Social Science of Poverty. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 555–73. [Google Scholar]
  12. Butcher, Jonathan, and Mike Gonzalez. 2020. Critical Race Theory, the New Intolerance, and Its Grip on America. The Heritage Foundation Backgrounder Paper No. 3567. December 7. Available online: https://stoplcpscrt.com/wp-content/uploads/2020/12/Heritage-Foundation-CRT-Report_12-8-2020.pdf (accessed on 10 October 2021).
  13. Chetty, Raj, Nathaniel Hendren, Maggie R. Jones, and Sonya R. Porter. 2020. Race and Economic Opportunity in the United States: An Intergenerational Perspective. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 135: 711–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Christian, Michelle, Louise Seamster, and Victor Ray. 2019. New Directions in Critical Race Theory and Sociology: Racism, White Supremacy, and Resistance. American Behavioral Scientist 63: 1731–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Christian, Michelle, Louise Seamster, and Victor Ray. 2021. Critical Race Theory and Empirical Sociology. American Behavioral Scientist 65: 1019–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Conley, Dalton. 2010. Being Black, Living in the Red: Race, Wealth, and Social Policy in America. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Douchet, Fabienne. 2021. Identifying and Testing Strategies to Improve the Use of Antiracist Research Evidence through Critical Race Lenses. William T. Grant Foundation. Available online: https://wtgrantfoundation.org/library/uploads/2021/01/Doucet_Digest_Issue-6.pdf (accessed on 10 October 2021).
  18. Economic Policy Institute. 2021. Average Family Income Growth, by Income Group, 1947–2013. The New Press. Retrieved 18 January 2022. Available online: http://www.stateofworkingamerica.org/charts/real-annual-family-income-growth-by-quintile-1947-79-and-1979-2010/ (accessed on 10 October 2021).
  19. Feagin, Joe. 2013. Systemic Racism: A Theory of Oppression. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  20. Feagin, Joe. 2020. The White Racial Frame: Centuries of Racial Framing and Counter-Framing. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  21. Feagin, Joe R. 1991. The Continuing Significance of Race: Antiblack Discrimination in Public Places. American Sociological Review 56: 101–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Greenman, Emily, and Yu Xie. 2008. Double Jeopardy? The Interaction of Gender and Race on Earnings in the United States. Social Forces 86: 1217–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ladson-Billings, Gloria. 2009. Who You Callin Nappy-Headed? A Critical Race Theory Look at the Construction of Black Women. Race, Ethnicity and Education 12: 87–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Iceland, John. 2006. Poverty in America: A Handbook. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  25. Iceland, John. 2017. Race and Ethnicity in America. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Isenberg, Nancy. 2017. White Trash: The 400-Year Untold History of Class in America. New York: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
  27. Leicht, Kevin T. 2016. Getting Serious about Inequality. The Sociological Quarterly 57: 211–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lynd, Robert S., and Helen M. Lynd. 1937. Middletown in Transition: A Study in Cultural Conflicts. New York: Harcourt Brace. [Google Scholar]
  29. Marger, Martin N. 2015. Race and Ethnic Relations. Stamford: Cengage Learning. [Google Scholar]
  30. Orrenius, Pia, Madeline Zavodny, and Agnes Scott College. 2011. Trends in Poverty and Inequality among Hispanics. In Federal Reserve Bank of Dallas, Working Paper No. 1109. Dallas: Federal Reserve Bank of Dallas. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pals, Heili, and Howard B. Kaplan. 2013. Cumulative and Relative Disadvantage as Long-Term Determinants of Negative Self-Feelings. Sociological Inquiry 83: 130–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rainwater, Lee. 1974. What Money Buys: Inequality and Social Meanings of Income. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  33. Rainwater, Lee, and Timothy M. Smeeding. 2003. Poor Kids in a Rich Country: America’s Children in Comparative Perspective. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ray, Victor Erik, Antonia Randolph, Megan Underhill, and David Luke. 2017. Critical Race Theory, Afro-Pessimism, and Racial Progress Narratives. Sociology of Race and Ethnicity 3: 147–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Sakamoto, Arthur, and Changhwan Kim. 2010. Is Rising Earnings Inequality Associated with Increased Exploitation? Evidence for U.S. Manufacturing Industries, 1971–96. Sociological Perspectives 53: 19–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sakamoto, Arthur, and ChangHwan Kim. 2014. Bringing Productivity Back In: Rising Inequality and Economic Rents in the U.S. Manufacturing Sector, 1971 to 2001. The Sociological Quarterly 55: 282–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sakamoto, Arthur, and Sharron Xuanren Wang. 2015. The Declining Significance of Race in the Twenty-First Century: A Retrospective Assessment in the Context of Rising Class Inequality. Ethnic and Racial Studies 38: 1264–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sakamoto, Arthur, Kimberly A. Goyette, and ChangHwan Kim. 2009. Socioeconomic Attainments of Asian Americans. Annual Review of Sociology 35: 255–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Sakamoto, Arthur, and Sharron Xuanren Wang. 2021. Deconstructing Hyper-Selectivity: Are the Socioeconomic Attainments of Second-Generation Asian Americans Only Due to Their Class Background? Chinese Journal of Sociology 7: 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Semega, Jessica, Melissa Kollar, Emily A. Shrider, and John F. Creamer. 2020. Income and Poverty in the United States: 2019. Retrieved. Available online: https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2020/demo/p60-270.pdf (accessed on 10 October 2021).
  41. Smeeding, Timothy M. 2016. Poverty Measurement. In The Oxford Handbook of the Social Science of Poverty. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 21–46. [Google Scholar]
  42. Takei, Isao, and Arthur Sakamoto. 2011. Poverty among Asian Americans in the 21St Century. Sociological Perspectives 54: 251–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Thevenot, Celine. 2017. Inequality in OECD Countries. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 45: 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Warner, William Lloyd, and Paul Sanborn Lunt. 1941. The Social Life of a Modern Community. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson, William J. 1980. The Declining Significance of Race. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Sample Sizes Used by Chetty et al. (2020).
Table 1. Sample Sizes Used by Chetty et al. (2020).
Racial and Gender GroupSample Size
Male
White6,891,000
Hispanic1,312,000
Black1,348,000
Asian350,000
Native American84,000
Female
White6,599,000
Hispanic1,303,000
Black1,402,000
Asian335,000
Native American82,000
Total19,706,000
Table 2. Effects of Class Origins by Race and Gender on Becoming a Poor Adult Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Table 2. Effects of Class Origins by Race and Gender on Becoming a Poor Adult Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Racial and Gender GroupProbability of Poverty If Born into Poverty (%) = Pa|PoProbability of Poverty If Born into Affluence (%) = Pa|AoClass Effect = (Pa|Po–Pa|Ao)
(A)(B)(C)
Male
White31.310.021.3
Hispanic29.114.015.1
Black48.521.527.0
Asian19.911.98.0
Native American49.320.928.4
Female
White26.77.319.4
Hispanic20.410.010.4
Black26.811.815.0
Asian13.28.05.2
Native American41.716.625.1
Note: Within each gender, the minority Pa|Po is statistically significant from the White Pa|Po at the 0.05 level using two-tailed tests. Within each gender, the minority Pa|Ao is statistically significant from the White Pa|Ao at any conventional level using two-tailed tests.
Table 3. Racial Minority Effects by Gender on Becoming a Poor Adult Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Table 3. Racial Minority Effects by Gender on Becoming a Poor Adult Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Racial and Gender GroupProbability of Poverty for Given Minority (%) = Pa|MMinority Effect = (Pa|M–Pa|W)
(A)(B)
Male
White16.6---
Hispanic23.16.5
Black39.422.8
Asian15.8−0.8
Native American37.520.9
Female
White13.6---
Hispanic16.32.7
Black21.78.1
Asian10.4−3.2
Native American31.317.7
Note: These statistics for Pa|M, based on Chetty et al. (2020), are reported by those authors in Table 2 and Table 3 on their website: https://opportunityinsights.org/data/ (accessed on 10 October 2021). Within each gender, the minority Pa|M is statistically significant from the White Pa|M at any conventional level using two-tailed tests.
Table 4. Comparing the Effects of Class Origins with Racial Minority Effects by Gender in Becoming a Poor Adult as Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Table 4. Comparing the Effects of Class Origins with Racial Minority Effects by Gender in Becoming a Poor Adult as Derived from Results of Chetty et al. (2020).
Racial and Gender GroupClass Effect from Column C of Table 2 = (Pa|Po–Pa|Ao)Minority Effect from Column B of Table 3 = (Pa|M–Pa|W)Difference between the Class Effect and the Minority Effect =
(A)(B)(C)
Male
White21.3------
Hispanic15.16.58.6
Black27.022.84.2
Asian8.0−0.88.8
Native American28.420.97.5
Female
White19.4------
Hispanic10.42.77.7
Black15.08.16.9
Asian5.2−3.28.4
Native American25.117.77.4
Note: For each minority-race-by-gender group, the class effect is statistically significant from the minority effect at any conventional level using two-tailed tests.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sakamoto, A.; Hsu, L.; Jalufka, M.E. Comparing the Effects of Class Origins versus Race in the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty. Soc. Sci. 2022, 11, 257. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11060257

AMA Style

Sakamoto A, Hsu L, Jalufka ME. Comparing the Effects of Class Origins versus Race in the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty. Social Sciences. 2022; 11(6):257. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11060257

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sakamoto, Arthur, Li Hsu, and Mary E. Jalufka. 2022. "Comparing the Effects of Class Origins versus Race in the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty" Social Sciences 11, no. 6: 257. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11060257

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop