Next Article in Journal
Multi-Technique Approach by Traditional and Innovative Methodologies to Support the Restoration of a Wall Painting from the 16th Century at Palazzo Gallo in Bagnaia, Viterbo, Central Italy
Next Article in Special Issue
Development of a Low-Cost Luminance Imaging Device with Minimal Equipment Calibration Procedures for Absolute and Relative Luminance
Previous Article in Journal
Long-Term Impacts of Temperature Gradients on a Concrete-Encased Steel I-Girder Experiment—Field-Monitored Data
Previous Article in Special Issue
Studying Response to Light in Offices: A Literature Review and Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Mathematical Model for the Action Spectrum of Steady-State Pupil Size in Photopic Vision with Insight into Healthful Lighting

1
School of Architecture, Tianjin University, 92nd Weijinlu, Nankai District, Tianjin 300072, China
2
China Academy of Building Research, 30th Beisanhuandonglu, Chaoyang District, Beijing 100033, China
3
School of Architecture, Design and Planning, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
4
Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Buildings 2023, 13(3), 781; https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings13030781
Submission received: 7 February 2023 / Revised: 8 March 2023 / Accepted: 14 March 2023 / Published: 16 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Lighting in Buildings)

Abstract

:
The pupillary light reflex, which has been seen as an important noninvasive and objective indicator of autonomic nervous system function, can be used for evaluating the impact of different lighting conditions in buildings on circadian behaviors, assessing ipRGC function in healthy and diseased retinas, and explaining luminance adaptation. However, the mechanism by which the intrinsic and extrinsic signals of ipRGCs regulate the steady-state pupil size under continuous lighting stimuli is still not clearly understood after decades of exploration. This paper presents a new experimental protocol with a large hemisphere LED screen as the stimulation device, allowing for a more realistic and comprehensive study in architectural spaces, which can potentially inform the design of lighting systems in buildings that promote healthy vision and comfort. Results reveal that both intrinsic and extrinsic signals participated in the process of regulating pupil size under continuous lighting conditions. Based on the findings, a new mathematical model was further proposed to calculate the contribution of these two signal sources to the changing intensity of melanopic radiance. The research outcomes also provide new insight into the mechanism of visual perception and adaptation and the nonvisual effect of eyes under different light conditions. Results suggest that the contribution of extrinsic signals may have been underestimated in previous studies since the extrinsic signal increases with reducing intensity in photopic conditions with lower melanopic radiance.

1. Introduction

The pupillary light reflex (PLR) is a reflex that controls the size of the pupil in response to the intensity of light falling on the retina. The pupil diameter mainly depends on the adaptation luminance [1]. The PLR has been seen as an important noninvasive indicator of autonomic nervous system function [2]. Historically, it was assumed that the PLR is driven by pathways originating in conventional photoreceptors [3]. Extensive efforts have been made, during the last century, to develop equations and mathematical models to predict pupil sizes under different lighting conditions. Based on a number of equations proposed, Watson and Yellott developed a unified formula for steady-state pupil size, considering photopic luminance as a controlling variable [4]. However, several observations have shown that V(λ)-weighted indicators do not predict the PLR very well [5,6,7]. It is still unclear how the visual system is able to detect absolute changes in environmental irradiance to mediate the PLR [8].
In the early 2000s, the discovery of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), which have extensive projections in the shell of the olivary pretectal nucleus (OPN) for mediating the PLR, may provide us with new insight into explaining this question [9,10,11,12]. Similar to other types of RGCs, ipRGCs also receive extrinsic signals from rods and cones, in addition to their own intrinsic signal [13,14,15]. For longer stimuli durations, during the first 200 ms after the onset of incremental light stimuli, the initial constriction of the pupil is mediated solely by classical photoreceptors [16], after which the pupil gradually increases to a relatively stable state because of the light adaptation for classical photoreceptors and the involvement of melanopsin if the light intensity is higher than the threshold of melanopsin [17]. The contribution of melanopsin is the determining factor for the post-illumination pupil response (PIPR), i.e., sustained pupil constriction no less than 1.7 s after offset stimuli, when the stimuli are higher than the threshold of melanopsin [18]. Rao, Chan, and Zhu pointed out a limitation in earlier studies that did not account for the contribution of ipRGCs on pupil responses in those equations. To address this issue, Rao’s team conducted an experiment to predict pupil size according to photopic and cirtopic luminance. Cirtopic luminance is the summation of the spectral irradiance of the light weighted by the cirtopic spectral sensitivity, which represents the spectral sensitivity of the third retinal input in addition to photopic and scotopic vision [19]. As Rao, Chan, and Zhu’s experiment was designed for typical computer and smartphone screens, the luminance tested was limited to a range from 50 cd/m2 to 300 cd/m2. In architectural spaces, it is not uncommon that luminance could be lower than this range. Spitschan also suggested that a larger range of luminance under natural behavior with conjoint spectral measurement needs to be considered in future studies [20]. Additionally, the light stimuli used in their research were relatively small (covering a circular field of 20 deg of the participants’ visual field), while larger stimuli are usually expected in a typical lighting environment in architectural spaces, which plays a crucial role in influencing human visual comfort, performance, productivity, and overall wellbeing Therefore, this present research explored the relationship between pupil sizes and different photoreceptors with a wider luminance range, color-mixing LED spectra, and much larger stimulus size.
The individual contribution of different photoreceptors to the sustained state of pupillary constriction during continuous light stimuli is still unclear. Some research supports that the melanopsin signal is the sole source of the sustained response 30 s after the onset of the stimuli [21,22,23], and some results suggest that in primates, the role of the melanopsin signal is to combine with the cone signal over the photopic range, serving to maintain pupil constriction during continuous daylight illumination [24,25,26]. Spitschan developed a function of pupil size with melanopic retinal intensity, which has a much better prediction than Watson and Yellott’s formula. He reported that there is a notable and systematic deviation for the 670 nm data points [20]. However, his research was based on an extant data set described in Bouma’s research [6]. In this paper, we present experiments for exploratory research to develop a mathematical model of the individual contributions of different photoreceptors to the sustained state of pupillary constriction. The data collected in this research could provide new empirical evidence.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

It has been demonstrated that pupillary diameter can be significantly influenced by non-photic factors such as diseases such as glaucoma [27], sleep quality [28], state of arousal [5], type of cognitive activities [29], and environmental factors [30,31].
Five male participants (mean age = 30, SD = 4.2) took part in this research. They all reported having normal corrected visual acuity and normal color vision. None of the participants had health issues, such as sleep disorders influencing pupil constriction. Written informed consent was obtained from each observer prior to this study. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, and followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. During the experiment, all participants were asked to avoid caffeine and alcohol one day before the experiment, a prescription medication known to influence the PLR. All the participants were asked to have a sleep duration of no less than 7 h the day before the experiment and to keep their mood in peace during the experiment.

2.2. Apparatus

In this research, we developed a simulation facility with a hemisphere LED screen to present uniform stimuli with different spectra and intensities for the participants, as shown in Figure 1. The technical characteristics of the LED screen are listed in Table 1, and Figure 2 presents the relative spectral intensity of R/G/B chips used in the LED screen, with a comparison to the melanopic action spectrum given in CIE/S 026 [10]. Compared to the stimulation devices used in related research, the LED hemisphere can tune the luminance and spectrum in any direction in the full visual field of the participants. During the experiments, participants positioned their chins on the adjustable chin rest so that one of their eyes was located on the center point of the hemisphere, with the eye tracker monitoring the pupil size during the experiment, as shown in Figure 1.
An infrared eye tracker aSee Pro F90 (produced by 7 invensun Technology Co., Ltd., Beijing, China) was used in this research, the sampling rate of which was 100 Hz. The claimed test error of the pupil diameter of this instrument is no more than 1.5%. A SPIC-300 Spectral Irradiance Colorimeter produced by EVERFINE Cooperation (Hangzhou, China), an illuminance meter, and an L1009 luminance meter produced by LMT LICHTMESSTECHNIK GMBH (Berlin, Germany) were used to calibrate the stimulus of the experiment. All these instruments were calibrated by the National Institute of Metrology, China.

2.3. Experiment Protocol

The baseline pupil diameter (BPD) was defined as the average pupil diameter over 30 s pre-stimulus phase. The relative pupil diameter was the measured pupil diameter normalized to the BPD, avoiding any individual difference in pupil size [32]. The PIPR was determined as the relative pupil diameter 6 s after the offset of stimuli, which has a strong dependence on the melanopsin signal.
All experiments were conducted at 10 am and 5 pm to minimize the potential error due to the circadian phase [33]. The PLR was measured under undilated conditions. The light stimuli described in 2.4 were presented sequentially. The measurements of every stimulus were preceded by 20 min of dark adaptation in the laboratory (the vertical illuminance on the eye of the participant was less than 0.001 lux) to guarantee the full dark adaptation of rods and cones [17]. The baseline pupil diameter was measured in the dark during 30 s of fixation before the onset of light stimuli. The participants were instructed to gaze at the center point of the screen, and no specified cognitive task was required. The pupillary light response to the stimuli presented for one minute in duration was monitored simultaneously. We set the time the stimulus offset as 0 s and the stimulus onset as −60 s. The PIPR was recorded for 30 s after the stimulus offset.

2.4. Stimuli

To develop the mathematical model of the individual contributions of intrinsic signals and external signals to the steady-state PLR under a continuous stimulus, eight types of spectra were used, including red, green, blue, and polychromatic white light, with five different CCTs, as shown in Figure 3. Nine levels and twelve levels of intensity were set for monochromatic light and white light, respectively, as shown in Table 2.

3. Results

3.1. Verifying the Contribution of the Intrinsic Signal and External Signal to the Steady State of PLR

As Gooley claimed, during exposure to a 30–60 min continuous light stimulus, because of the slower decreasing relative contribution of cones to the PLR, pupillary size continued to dilate to a steady state after the initial constriction, which was an order of magnitude longer than previously reported. The time course of the PLR under continuous stimulus was first examined to develop a method of calculating the steady-state pupil size. Results showed that pupil sizes could reach the balance state in seconds after light onset, and the slope of the response curve vs. time before the stimuli offset became flat, as shown in Figure 4.
Comparisons of relative pupil diameters (RPDs) at different times after the stimuli presentation showed that the mean difference of RPDs was not significant after the first 10 s of exposure to light. Table 3 demonstrates an example of RPD comparisons under red stimuli with a luminance of 366 cd/m2. The RPD at each time point was compared with the RPD at any other time point to identify any statistically significant differences. Interestingly, the phenomenon of ‘pupillary escape’ reported by Gooley was not observed in this experiment, even for the red stimulus without activating melanopsin [21]. In our experiment, there is a significant difference between the RPD at 1 s after stimulus onset and other subsequent RPDs, but no significant difference was found for RPDs with light exposure longer than 11 s. This suggests that at the very beginning of stimulus onset, there is a transient response that is followed by a gradual relaxation in the pupil to a more sustained and balanced state after 11 s of light exposure. Therefore, the average RPD from 11 s after the stimulus onset until light-off can be taken as the steady-state pupil size.
As shown in Figure 5, the steady-state pupil size under red and blue stimuli both decreased significantly with increasing stimulus intensity (for red stimuli: F = 506.955, P = 0, blue stimuli: F = 2169.339, P = 0). There was an interesting phenomenon that the steady-state pupil size under the red stimuli had a saturation effect, which means that the pupil diameter did not decrease with the increasing intensity when the mean difference in pupil size had no significant difference between stimuli with a luminance of 189 cd/m2 and 366 cd/m2 (Table 4). However, the minimum pupil size of the blue stimulus (mean value = 0.3107) was significantly smaller than that of the red stimulus (mean value = 0.4331) (F = 2707.161, P = 0).

3.2. Development of a Mathematical Model for the Action Spectrum of Steady-State Pupil Size in Photopic Vision

Both McDougal and Gooley supported that the pupil size dilated after the initial constriction over time because of the light adaptation of cones in a relatively long-time course, and the contribution of cones to the PLR could be ignored after a specified time of exposure [3,34]. In this experiment, the response of pupils to red stimuli of different intensities was investigated with the blue stimulus as a reference. According to Table 2, red spectra only stimulated the cones in photopic conditions without activating melanopsin. From the data, unexpectedly, we did not find a slow recovery process, instead, a fast establishment of the steady-state PLR in seconds was found. Therefore, melanopsin might not be the sole source for mediating the PLR function but in cooperating with cones.
Another phenomenon found in this research was the saturation effect of the PLR under red stimulus when a pupil was not reached its minimum state. A possible reason for this was that cones reach their saturation state without adjusting their sensitivity by light adaptation [35]. This phenomenon might further confirm that melanopsin functions analogously to a photographer’s light meter, providing a measure to regulate functional adaptation in the mammalian retina [36,37]. According to Prigge and Mark, M1 ipRGCs send signals retrogradely to dopaminergic amacrine cells (DACs) within the retina, providing a possible mechanism by which ipRGCs may influence light adaptation via the retinal dopaminergic system [36,38]. Therefore, we hypothesized that the melanopsin signal might have two functions: one is that the signal could be sent to the brain to mediate pupil size directly; the other is that it could mediate the extrinsic signal of cones by adjusting their sensitivity and deliver it to the brain to regulate the pupil indirectly, as shown in Figure 6.
Most existing models developed from sensory to light are based on S-shaped curves, which are mathematically more accurately referred to as a hyperbolic tangent (tanh) curve [35,39,40]. Therefore, the PLR under continuous light stimulus may be expressed as the following formula:
P L R = k 1 f 1 ( m e l ) + k 2 f 2 ( m e l , L ) + k 3
where k1, k2, and k3 are constants.
f 1 ( m e l ) is the response of melanopsin on melanopic radiance mel as a function of the direct component of the intrinsic signal on the PLR, which can be expressed as follows:
f 1 ( m e l ) = a 1 + c e b m e l + d
where a, b, c, and d are constants.
f 2 ( m e l , L ) is the response of cones on stimulus luminance L with the gain control changing as determined by the intrinsic signal on the PLR, and f 1 ( m e l ) can be seen as a proportionality constant under the specific adaptation state [41]. So, it can be expressed as Equation (3):
f 2 ( m e l , L ) = g 1 + h e m L f 1 ( m e l )
where g, h, and m are constants.
As the PIPR is highly dependent on the intrinsic signal from melanopsin, we can take the PIPR as the dependent variable, perform regression of Equation (2) first, and then substitute Equation (1) as:
P L R = k 1 P I P R + k 2 g 1 + h e m L P I P R + k 3
From the steady-state pupil diameter data collected under 87 scenarios listed in Table 2, we found that the V(λ)-weighted luminance did not predict the steady-state pupil size well, and there was a significant difference in pupil sizes under stimuli with the same luminance of different spectra (Figure 7). Melanopic radiance could provide a much better explanation, especially for the higher melanopic radiance situations, but deviation increased in the lower melanopic radiance region (melanopic radiance lower than 0.01 W/m2/Sr), particularly with red spectra (Figure 8). This reconfirmed that both cones and melanopsin contributed to the PLR activities under continuous stimulus in photopic conditions, as Manuel stated [20]. Manuel’s findings were based on the reanalysis of Bouma’s data set, which was published in 1962 when ipRGCs were not yet discovered.
A new model to establish the relationship between the PIPR and melanopic radiance was proposed by performing a nonlinear regression model using SPSS, Version 23. The constants in Equation (2) were determined, and the model can be expressed as:
P I P R = 0.481 1 + 0.649 e 10.095 m e l + 0.501
The best model was identified for its ability to fit the result of the PIPR very well with a high value of R2 = 0.818 (as shown in Table 5). It can be seen from the mathematical model of the PIPR that melanopsin has a higher sensation threshold value, approximately 0.001 W/m2, in a situation with uniform luminance distribution in the visual field of participants on the level of 1 cd/m2 with a 6000 K spectrum, as specified in Figure 9.
With the function of SPSS nonlinear regression, the constant in Equation (4) was determined for predicting the steady-state PLR with PIPR calculated using Equation (5) and measured luminance as follows:
P L R = 0.552 P I P R 1120.801 1 24690.191 e ( L × P I P R ) + 0.051
The best model was identified for its ability to fit the result of the relative steady-state PLR with a high value of R2 = 0.930 (Table 6). This also reconfirmed the hypothesis that both intrinsic signals from melanopsin and mediated extrinsic signals from cones mediated by intrinsic signals co-mediate PLR function under continuous light stimulus higher than the threshold value of melanopsin.
More than 75% of the absolute values of errors between the computed results and measured values are less than 4%, and the average value is 3% (see Figure 10).

3.3. Explanation for the Mathematic Model

With Equation (6), the relative steady-state PLR can be calculated under a continuous light stimulus with different intensities and spectra (Figure 11). For stimuli with lower intensities (less than 10 cd/m2), the calculated PLR values for different spectra are very close, but the deviation grows with the increasing luminance of the stimulus. In this paper, we proposed a new term daylight (D65) coefficient of the PLR, which is defined as the ratio of the luminance of test stimuli and the luminance produced by radiation conforming to standard daylight (D65) that provides an equal PLR with the stimuli. The daylight (D65) coefficients of the PLR for different spectra with different luminance levels were further calculated. For a higher intensity of the stimulus, the daylight (D65) coefficient of the PLR is a constant with a value the same as the melanopic daylight (D65) efficacy ratio determined according to CIE S/026:2018, which means that the intrinsic signal from melanopsin can be seen as the sole source for steady-state PLR mediation. However, when the intensity of the stimulus is lower than a critical value, the daylight (D65) coefficient of the PLR changes with decreasing stimulus intensity, approaching 1. This means that both intrinsic and extrinsic signals contribute to the function of mediating the PLR under continuous light presentation, but their respective contributions dynamically change with the intensity of the stimulus, which is very similar to the situation of mesopic vision.

4. Discussion and Conclusions

The PLR can be seen as a valuable indicator of autonomic nervous system function and is widely used as a noninvasive tool for basic neuroscience research, physiological and psychological clinical diagnosis, etc. The PLR has been widely investigated in humans [42], and some consensus has been made on signal sources for mediating the initial constriction of pupils and the PIPR, but we are still uncertain about the mechanism by which the intrinsic and extrinsic signals of ipRGCs regulate the steady-state pupil size under continuous lighting stimuli. In this paper, two experiments were conducted, and we confirmed the following:
(1)
We did not find continued dilation of the pupil size after the initial constriction under continuous light presentation for a relatively long time (up to 30 min) but found that the pupil size could reach balance in no more than 10 s, even under a red stimulus that only activated the cones. This suggests that both intrinsic and extrinsic signals participated in the process of regulating pupil size under continuous lighting conditions, unlike the previous report indicating an underestimated contribution of extrinsic signals [21,22,23].
(2)
The contribution of the signals might change with the intensity of melanopic radiance. For melanopic radiance with higher intensity, the intrinsic signal solely determines the pupil size under sustained light presentation, which is consistent with the data from Manuel [20]; however, for photopic conditions with lower melanopic radiance, the contribution of the intrinsic signal decreases, and the extrinsic signal increases with reducing intensity (see Figure 12).
(3)
The hypothesis for the potential function of melanopsin in regulating the sensitivity of cones was proposed and verified according to the two experiments. This hypothesis is also supported by the research of Prigge and Mark, who found that M1 ipRGCs, which also regulate the function OPN for the PLR and SCN for behavior, send signals retrogradely to dopaminergic amacrine cells (DACs) within the retina, providing a possible mechanism by which ipRGCs may influence light adaptation via the retinal dopaminergic system [36,38,43]. This may provide new insight into how to explain the mechanism of visual perception.
In this research, we have drawn some preliminary conclusions from the two experiments with a relatively small number of male participants, but there is still a long way to go to develop a complicated model of the PLR under different spectra and luminance distributions for people of different ages and genders. The current findings provide new insights into the mechanisms underlying visual perception, such as light adaptation, and the contribution of signals to light perception.
Additionally, this research presents a novel non-invasive method for investigating the non-visual effect of light. As an easily measurable physiological marker, pupil responses can be a promising tool for further research and accelerate the implementation of research findings into practical solutions for improving human health and wellbeing.
Research on the PLR may provide some new implications for the following topics:
(1)
As M1 ipRGCs are responsible for both the circadian clock and the PLR, research on the PLR might offer us a quick and easy tool to evaluate the impact of different lighting conditions on the nonvisual effect of eyes, such as circadian behaviors.
(2)
Many studies have shown that ipRGC dysfunction may have strong relevance to human diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, diabetic retinopathy, glaucoma, etc. [44]. Research on the PLR may develop a standard protocol for pupillometry as an emerging method for the direct assessment of ipRGC function in healthy and diseased retinas, which may be used for early detection and clinical examination of these ipRGC-related diseases.
(3)
The measurement or estimation of adaptation levels in natural environments with a complex luminance distribution is a very important topic, but there have been no studies directly dealing with this issue [45]. Historically, the steady-state PLR has been seen as an indicator for luminance adaptation, but recent studies on ipRGCs offered us a better mechanistic explanation of the relationship between light adaptation of the visual system and the PLR. This suggests that the same class of ipRGCs for the PLR also functions analogously to a photographer’s light meter, providing a measure to regulate luminance adaptation by synapsing with bipolar and amacrine cells [36,37,46]. Research on the PLR might give us a new perspective on developing a model for light adaptation in a natural environment.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and Project Administration, S.W. and F.Y.; Methodology, J.Z. and L.W.; Validation, W.H.; Writing—original draft, S.W.; writing—review and editing, W.H., J.Z. and L.W.; funding acquisition, J.Z.; supervision, L.W. and J.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Key R&D Program of China (2018YFC0705100), Space Medical Experiment Project of China Manned Space Program (HYZHXM03004).

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Kees Teunissen for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. David, L.D.; Kevin, W.H.; Richard, G.M.; Steffy, G.R. The Lighting Handbook, 10th ed.; Illuminating Engineering Society: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  2. Hall, C.A.; Chilcott, R.P. Eyeing up the Future of the Pupillary Light Reflex in Neurodiagnostics. Diagnostics 2018, 8, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. McDougal, D.H.; Gamlin, P.D. The influence of intrinsically-photosensitive retinal ganglion cells on the spectral sensitivity and response dynamics of the human pupillary light reflex. Vis. Res. 2010, 50, 72–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Watson, A.B.; Yellott, J.I. A unified formula for light-adapted pupil size. J. Vis. 2012, 12, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Berman, S.; Fein, G.; Jewett, D.; Saika, G.; Ashford, F. Spectral Determinants of Steady-State Pupil Size with Full Field of View. J. Illum. Eng. Soc. 1992, 21, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bouma, H. Size of the Static Pupil as a Function of Wave-length and Luminosity of the Light Incident on the Human Eye. Nature 1962, 193, 690–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lucas, R.J.; Douglas, R.H.; Foster, R.G. Characterization of an ocular photopigment capable of driving pupillary constriction in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 621–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Joyce, D.S. Temporal, Spatial and Adaptation Characteristics of Melanopsin Inputs to the Human Pupil Light Reflex. Ph.D. Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane City, QLD, Australia, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berson, D.M. Strange vision: Ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26, 314–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. CIE S 026/E:2018; CIE System for Metrology of Optical Radiation for IpRGC-Influenced Responses to Light. Commission Internationale De L’eclairage(Cie) CIE: Vienna, Austria, 2018.
  11. Dacey, D.; Liao, H.; Peterson, B.; Robinson, F.; Smith, V.; Pokorny, J.; Yau, K.-W.; Gamlin, P.D. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal color and irradiance and project to the lgn. Nature 2005, 433, 749–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wang, S.; Zhao, J. New prospectives on light adaptation of visual system research with the emerging knowledge on non-imageforming effect. Front. Built Environ. 2022, 8, 1019460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Berson, D.M.; Dunn, F.A.; Takao, M. Phototransduction by Retinal Ganglion Cells That Set the Circadian Clock. Science 2002, 295, 1070–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Hattar, S.; Liao, H.-W.; Takao, M.; Berson, D.M.; Yau, K.-W. Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: Architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science 2002, 295, 1065–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Hattar, S.; Lucas, R.J.; Mrosovsky, N.; Thompson, S.; Douglas, R.H.; Hankins, M.W.; Lem, J.; Biel, M.; Hofmann, F.; Foster, R.G.; et al. Melanopsin and rod–cone photoreceptive systems account for all major accessory visual functions in mice. Nature 2003, 424, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Zele, A.J.; Feigl, B.; Adhikari, P.; Maynard, M.L.; Cao, D. Melanopsin photoreception contributes to human visual detection, temporal and colour processing. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 3842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Münch, M.; Léon, L.; Crippa, S.V.; Kawasaki, A. Circadian and Wake-Dependent Effects on the Pupil Light Reflex in Response to Narrow-Bandwidth Light Pulses. Investig. Opthalmolo. Vis. Sci. 2012, 53, 4546–4555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Adhikari, P.; Feigl, B.; Zele, A.J. Rhodopsin and Melanopsin Contributions to the Early Redilation Phase of the Post-Illumination Pupil Response (PIPR). PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0161175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Rao, F.; Chan, A.; Zhu, X.-F. Effects of photopic and cirtopic illumination on steady state pupil sizes. Vis. Res. 2017, 137, 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Spitschan, M. Photoreceptor inputs to pupil control. J. Vis. 2019, 19, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Gooley, J.J.; Mien, I.H.; Hilaire, M.A.S.; Yeo, S.C.; Chua, E.C.P.; Van Reen, E.; Hanley, C.J.; Hull, J.T.; Czeisler, C.A.; Lockley, S.W. Melanopsin and Rod-Cone Photoreceptors Play Different Roles in Mediating Pupillary Light Responses during Exposure to Continuous Light in Humans. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 14242–14253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Park, J.C.; Moura, A.L.; Raza, A.S.; Rhee, D.W.; Kardon, R.H.; Hood, D.C. Toward a Clinical Protocol for Assessing Rod, Cone, and Melanopsin Contributions to the Human Pupil Response. Investig. Opthalmology Vis. Sci. 2011, 52, 6624–6635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Gamlin, P.D.; McDougal, D.H.; Pokorny, J.; Smith, V.C.; Yau, K.-W.; Dacey, D.M. Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells. Vis. Res. 2007, 47, 946–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Drouyer, E.; Rieux, C.; Hut, R.A.; Cooper, H.M. Responses of Suprachiasmatic Nucleus Neurons to Light and Dark Adaptation: Relative Contributions of Melanopsin and Rod–Cone Inputs. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 9623–9631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Wong, K.Y.; Dunn, F.A.; Graham, D.M.; Berson, D.M. Synaptic influences on rat ganglion-cell photoreceptors. J. Physiol. 2007, 582, 279–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lucas, R.J. Diminished Pupillary Light Reflex at High Irradiances in Melanopsin-Knockout Mice. Science 2003, 299, 245–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Zhang, H.; Guo, J.; Wang, S. Clinical Nursing of Correlation between Pupil Changes and Diseases. Chin. Gen. Pract. Nurs. 2014, 12, 1123–1124. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  28. McDougal, D.H.; Gamlin, P.D.R. Pupillary Control Pathways. In The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 521–536. [Google Scholar]
  29. Yu, Y.; Jiang, Y.; Wang, Y.; Yu, M. Pupil size as a biomarker of Memory Processing. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 28, 10. Available online: http://kns.cnki.net/kcms/detail/11.4766.R.20200117.1029.012.html (accessed on 5 January 2023). (In Chinese).
  30. Dabbs, J.M. Testosterone and Pupillary Response to Auditory Sexual Stimuli. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 62, 909–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Akashi, Y.; Muramatsu, R.; Kanaya, S. Unified Glare Rating (UGR) and subjective appraisal of discomfort glare. Light. Res. Technol. 1996, 28, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sperandio, I.; Bond, N.; Binda, P. Pupil Size as a Gateway Into Conscious Interpretation of Brightness. Front. Neurol. 2018, 9, 1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Zele, A.J.; Adhikari, P.; Cao, D.; Feigl, B. Melanopsin and Cone Photoreceptor Inputs to the Afferent Pupil Light Response. Front. Neurol. 2019, 10, 529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. William, A.H.; Rushton, F.R.S. The Plotting of Dark-Adaptation Curves in Rod-Cone Investigations. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 1963, 56, 748–751. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hung, G.K.; Ciuffreda, K.J. Model of Visual System; Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 125–127. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hankins, M.W.; Hughes, S. Vision: Melanopsin as a novel irradiance detector at the heart of vision. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, R1055–R1057. [Google Scholar]
  37. Allen, A.E.; Storchi, R.; Martial, F.P.; Petersen, R.S.; Montemurro, M.A.; Brown, T.M.; Lucas, R.J. Melanopsin-Driven Light Adaptation in Mouse Vision. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, 2481–2490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Prigge, C.L.; Yeh, P.-T.; Liou, N.-F.; Lee, C.-C.; You, S.-F.; Liu, L.-L.; McNeill, D.S.; Chew, K.S.; Hattar, S.; Chen, S.-K.; et al. M1 ipRGCs Influence Visual Function through Retrograde Signaling in the Retina. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 7184–7197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Brainard, G.C.; Hanifin, J.P.; Greeson, J.; Byrne, B.; Glickman, G.; Gerner, E.; Rollag, M.D. Action Spectrum for Melatonin Regulation in Humans: Evidence for a Novel Circadian Photoreceptor. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 6405–6412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. van Bommel, W. Interior Lighting: Fundamentals, Technology and Application; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; p. 114. [Google Scholar]
  41. Shapley, R.; Enroth-Cugell, C. Chapter 9 Visual adaptation and retinal gain controls. Prog. Retin. Res. 1984, 3, 263–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Clarke, R.J.; Zhang, H.; Gamlin, P.D.R. Characteristics of the Pupillary Light Reflex in the Alert Rhesus Monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 2003, 89, 3179–3189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Schmidt, T.M.; Chen, S.-K.; Hattar, S. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: Many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends Neurosci. 2011, 34, 572–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Esquiva, G.; Hannibal, J. Melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in aging and disease. Histol. Histopathol. 2019, 34, 1299–1311. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. International Commission On Illumination (CIE). CIE 135/5:1999 Visual Adaptation to Complex Luminance; CIE: Vienna, Austria, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  46. Besenecker, U.C. Investigating Melanopsin Contribution to Scene Brightness Perception. Ph.D. Thesis, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, Troy, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the apparatus during the experiment (unit: mm).
Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the apparatus during the experiment (unit: mm).
Buildings 13 00781 g001
Figure 2. Relative spectral intensity of R/G/B chips used in the LED screen.
Figure 2. Relative spectral intensity of R/G/B chips used in the LED screen.
Buildings 13 00781 g002
Figure 3. The eight spectra of the experimental scenarios.
Figure 3. The eight spectra of the experimental scenarios.
Buildings 13 00781 g003
Figure 4. Average pupillograms of the five participants under the red stimulus, as an example, with different luminance.
Figure 4. Average pupillograms of the five participants under the red stimulus, as an example, with different luminance.
Buildings 13 00781 g004
Figure 5. Comparison of steady-state pupil size under different luminance of the red (shown as green circles) and blue stimuli (shown as blue circles).
Figure 5. Comparison of steady-state pupil size under different luminance of the red (shown as green circles) and blue stimuli (shown as blue circles).
Buildings 13 00781 g005
Figure 6. Hypotheses for the mechanism of pupil mediation with different signals.
Figure 6. Hypotheses for the mechanism of pupil mediation with different signals.
Buildings 13 00781 g006
Figure 7. Steady-State of Pupil Diameter vs. Luminance of Stimulus (log scale).
Figure 7. Steady-State of Pupil Diameter vs. Luminance of Stimulus (log scale).
Buildings 13 00781 g007
Figure 8. Measured Steady-State of Pupil Diameter vs. Melanopic Radiance of Stimulus (log scale).
Figure 8. Measured Steady-State of Pupil Diameter vs. Melanopic Radiance of Stimulus (log scale).
Buildings 13 00781 g008
Figure 9. A regression model was developed using PIPR measurements as a function of melanopic radiance (in a logarithmic scale). The normalized PIPR values are represented with blue dots and the regression curve is shown in red. The model yields a high goodness of fit (0.818).
Figure 9. A regression model was developed using PIPR measurements as a function of melanopic radiance (in a logarithmic scale). The normalized PIPR values are represented with blue dots and the regression curve is shown in red. The model yields a high goodness of fit (0.818).
Buildings 13 00781 g009
Figure 10. Distribution of computation error between the regressed model and measured data.
Figure 10. Distribution of computation error between the regressed model and measured data.
Buildings 13 00781 g010
Figure 11. Calculated relative steady-state PLR with Formula (6) vs. luminance of stimulus with different spectra.
Figure 11. Calculated relative steady-state PLR with Formula (6) vs. luminance of stimulus with different spectra.
Buildings 13 00781 g011
Figure 12. Calculated Steady-State of Pupil Diameter with Formula (6) vs. Melanopic Radiance of Stimulus (log scale).
Figure 12. Calculated Steady-State of Pupil Diameter with Formula (6) vs. Melanopic Radiance of Stimulus (log scale).
Buildings 13 00781 g012
Table 1. Technical characteristics of the hemisphere LED Screen.
Table 1. Technical characteristics of the hemisphere LED Screen.
Characteristics Performance
Diameter2.4 m
Number of pixels723,360 pixels
ColorR/G/B
Maximum luminance>1000 cd/m2
Luminance uniformity>95%
Refresh rate≥2000 Hz
Bit depth12 bit
Table 2. Detailed information on the scenarios for the experiment.
Table 2. Detailed information on the scenarios for the experiment.
Scenario
NO.
Color/
CCT
Measured
Luminance
(cd/m2)
Melanopic
Radiance
(W/m2/Sr)
Scenario
No.
Color/
CCT
Measured
Luminance
(cd/m2)
Melanopic
Radiance
(W/m2/Sr)
1Red50.0004536421040.085
2Red130.0004635941510.117
3Red280.0004735742080.158
4Red520.0004835992660.217
5Red820.0004936193230.250
6Red1310.0005037554480.326
7Red1890.0015137445050.389
8Red2610.001526685110.010
9Red3660.001536462180.017
10Green180.017546148260.025
11Green460.045555833440.040
12Green1010.098565429690.063
13Green1870.180575385950.095
14Green3100.2995852701530.135
15Green4770.4605951682200.187
16Green6910.6656051882850.246
17Green9610.9266151923540.300
18Green13531.3036252805030.466
19Blue50.0356353506490.619
20Blue70.057647095100.010
21Blue160.124656959190.019
22Blue300.230666406290.031
23Blue500.382676026500.051
24Blue770.587685749680.067
25Blue1120.8516957571020.103
26Blue1561.1847056481470.147
27Blue2201.6727155712020.196
284336100.0077256712630.264
293193190.0127358323020.302
302951290.0197457404460.467
312734530.0347558745360.533
322440720.0447611,09190.011
3321231220.0627712,717140.019
3421901530.0867814,339210.028
3521522110.1107916,877350.049
3621262700.1488019,003550.079
3720993070.1678119,739740.106
3820804620.2468219,5601230.177
3921015080.2688319,4301750.254
405545100.0098419,4572290.337
414979180.0198519,7562870.403
424302320.0278619,6524030.591
433836530.0398719,6135400.804
443854700.059————————
Table 3. Paired comparisons of RPDs at different times after stimuli presentation (the luminance of red stimuli is 366 cd/m2). Dependent factor: Relative pupil diameter.
Table 3. Paired comparisons of RPDs at different times after stimuli presentation (the luminance of red stimuli is 366 cd/m2). Dependent factor: Relative pupil diameter.
LSD
(I) Time after the Stimulus Onset(J) Time after the Stimulus OnsetMean Difference (I–J)Sig.
1.0011.000.42839 *0.000
21.000.42766 *0.000
31.000.42221 *0.000
41.000.42215 *0.000
55.000.41890 *0.000
11.001.00−0.42839 *0.000
21.00−0.000730.980
31.00−0.006180.831
41.00−0.006240.829
55.00−0.009490.743
21.001.00−0.42766 *0.000
11.000.000730.980
31.00−0.005450.851
41.00−0.005510.849
55.00−0.008750.762
311.00−0.42221 *0.000
11.000.006180.831
21.000.005450.851
41.00−0.000060.998
55.00−0.003310.909
41.001.00−0.42215 *0.000
11.000.006240.829
21.000.005510.849
31.000.000060.998
55.00−0.003240.911
55.001.00−0.41890 *0.000
11.000.009490.743
21.000.008750.762
31.000.003310.909
41.000.003240.911
* The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level.
Table 4. Comparisons of steady-state RPDs for the red stimulus with different luminance.
Table 4. Comparisons of steady-state RPDs for the red stimulus with different luminance.
Dependent Factor: Relative Pupil Diameter
LSD
(I) Luminance of Stimulus (cd/m2)(J) Luminance of Stimulus (cd/m2)Mean Difference (I–J)Std. ErrorSig.95% Confidence Interval
Lower BoundUpper Bound
5280.05306 *0.00304400.0000.047080.05903
820.08648 *0.0030330.0000.080520.09243
1890.11041 *0.0030330.0000.104460.11636
3660.11060 *0.0030370.0000.104640.11655
285−0.05306 *0.0030440.000−0.05903−0.04708
820.03342 *0.0031290.0000.027280.03956
1890.05736 *0.0031290.0000.051220.06349
3660.05754 *0.0031320.0000.051400.06369
825−0.08648 *0.0030330.000−0.09243−0.08052
28−0.03342 *0.0031290.000−0.03956−0.02728
1890.02394 *0.0031180.0000.017820.03005
3660.02412 *0.0031220.0000.018000.03025
1895−0.11041 *0.0030330.000−0.11636−0.10446
28−0.05736 *0.0031290.000−0.06349−0.05122
82−0.02394 *0.0031180.000−0.03005−0.01782
3660.000190.0031220.953−0.005940.00631
3665−0.11060 *0.0030370.000−0.11655−0.10464
28−0.05754 *0.0031320.000−0.06369−0.05140
82−0.02412 *0.0031220.000−0.03025−0.01800
189−0.000190.0031220.953−0.006310.00594
* The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level.
Table 5. ANOVA of the PIPR Regression.
Table 5. ANOVA of the PIPR Regression.
SourceSum of SquaresdfMean Squares
Regression38.36449.591
Residual0.231830.003
Uncorrected Total38.59487
Corrected Total1.27186
Dependent variable: PIPR. R2 = 1 − (Residual Sum of Squares)/(Corrected Sum of Squares) = 0.818.
Table 6. ANOVA of the PLP Regression.
Table 6. ANOVA of the PLP Regression.
SourceSum of SquaresdfMean Squares
Regression15.14543.786
Residual0.024830.000
Uncorrected Total15.16987
Corrected Total0.34286
Dependent variable: PIPR. R2= 1 − (Residual Sum of Squares)/(Corrected Sum of Squares) = 0.930.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wang, S.; Zhao, J.; Wang, L.; Hu, W.; Yan, F. A Mathematical Model for the Action Spectrum of Steady-State Pupil Size in Photopic Vision with Insight into Healthful Lighting. Buildings 2023, 13, 781. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings13030781

AMA Style

Wang S, Zhao J, Wang L, Hu W, Yan F. A Mathematical Model for the Action Spectrum of Steady-State Pupil Size in Photopic Vision with Insight into Healthful Lighting. Buildings. 2023; 13(3):781. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings13030781

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wang, Shuxiao, Jianping Zhao, Lixiong Wang, Wenye Hu, and Fanfang Yan. 2023. "A Mathematical Model for the Action Spectrum of Steady-State Pupil Size in Photopic Vision with Insight into Healthful Lighting" Buildings 13, no. 3: 781. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings13030781

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop