Next Article in Journal
Gluten Degrading Enzymes for Treatment of Celiac Disease
Next Article in Special Issue
The Role of Human Milk Oligosaccharides and Probiotics on the Neonatal Microbiome and Risk of Necrotizing Enterocolitis: A Narrative Review
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of 120 vs. 60 and 90 g/h Carbohydrate Intake during a Trail Marathon on Neuromuscular Function and High Intensity Run Capacity Recovery
Previous Article in Special Issue
Prophylactic Intra-Uterine β-Cyclodextrin Administration during Intra-Uterine Ureaplasma parvum Infection Partly Prevents Liver Inflammation without Interfering with the Enterohepatic Circulation of the Fetal Sheep
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Perinatal Docosahexaenoic Acid Supplementation Improves Cognition and Alters Brain Functional Organization in Piglets

1
Department of Foods and Nutrition, College of Family and Consumer Sciences, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA
2
Department of Physics and Astronomy, Franklin College of Arts and Sciences, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA
3
Department of Animal and Dairy Science, College of Agricultural & Environmental Sciences, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA
4
Department of Statistics, Franklin College of Arts and Sciences, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA
5
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, College of Veterinary Medicine, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA
6
Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, Suwanee, GA 30024, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2020, 12(7), 2090; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12072090
Submission received: 10 June 2020 / Revised: 10 July 2020 / Accepted: 11 July 2020 / Published: 15 July 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Health Benefits of Pharmacological Nutrition in Neonates)

Abstract

:
Epidemiologic studies associate maternal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA)/DHA-containing seafood intake with enhanced cognitive development; although, it should be noted that interventional trials show inconsistent findings. We examined perinatal DHA supplementation on cognitive performance, brain anatomical and functional organization, and the brain monoamine neurotransmitter status of offspring using a piglet model. Sows were fed a control (CON) or a diet containing DHA (DHA) from late gestation throughout lactation. Piglets underwent an open field test (OFT), an object recognition test (ORT), and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to acquire anatomical, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), and resting-state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) at weaning. Piglets from DHA-fed sows spent 95% more time sniffing the walls than CON in OFT and exhibited an elevated interest in the novel object in ORT, while CON piglets demonstrated no preference. Maternal DHA supplementation increased fiber length and tended to increase fractional anisotropy in the hippocampus of offspring than CON. DHA piglets exhibited increased functional connectivity in the cerebellar, visual, and default mode network and decreased activity in executive control and sensorimotor network compared to CON. The brain monoamine neurotransmitter levels did not differ in healthy offspring. Perinatal DHA supplementation may increase exploratory behaviors, improve recognition memory, enhance fiber tract integrity, and alter brain functional organization in offspring at weaning.

1. Introduction

Prenatal and early postnatal time represent critical periods of brain development sensitive to nutritional status [1]. Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) is a n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (LCPUFA) commonly recommended during pregnancy and is widely added to the infant formula due to its suggested beneficial role in visual function and cognitive development [2,3]. DHA accounts for more than 40% of the total n-3 PUFA in the neuronal tissue [4], especially in the gray matter [5,6]. In the developing brain, the fastest DHA increase takes place during the third trimester of the gestation period, into the postnatal life [7,8], coinciding with the rapid myelination, dendritic outgrowth, and synaptogenesis [9], highlighting the importance of DHA during the critical window of neurodevelopment. Indeed, the perinatal deprivation of α-linolenic acid (ALA), the precursor of DHA, resulted in a 61% reduction in DHA concentration, a 65% decrease in serotonin (5-HT) levels in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [10], and disturbed hippocampal (HC)-dependent learning and cognitive behaviors in rats compared to offspring born to ALA-sufficient animals [11]. Importantly, endogenous DHA synthesis is limited in mammals, and breast milk serves as a high bioavailable source of DHA to infants [12].
In humans, compelling evidence from observational studies supports the positive association between maternal fish intake and infant DHA status in neurocognitive development [13,14,15]. However, intervention trials supplementing DHA during gestation and/or lactation yielded mixed and conflicting findings in healthy term infants [16,17,18,19,20]. For example, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) found that maternal seafood intake higher than 340 g/week is associated with better verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) and a decreased risk of suboptimal fine motor, social development, and communication scores of the children compared to those born to mothers who consumed less than 340 g seafood/week during pregnancy [14]. Nevertheless, a randomized control trial (RCT) in Australian women found that maternal supplementation of DHA (800 mg/day) during pregnancy did not influence cognitive and language development of infants at 18 months compared to the control group [21]. Similarly, DHA supplementation (220 mg/day) during gestation until 3 months after delivery did not influence the neurodevelopment of infants when they were evaluated at 18 months of age [22]; fish oil supplementation (400 mL/day) from the 28th week of gestation to 4 months lactation did not influence the visual and cognitive/psychomotor development of infants in their first year of life. These conflicting findings urge further exploration into the role of maternal DHA supplementation on the structural and functional organization of the developing brain in healthy subjects.
Pigs are a robust model for both nutritional and neurodevelopmental research in understanding nutritional programming in the human mother–infant dyad due to a myriad of similarities to humans in physiology, anatomy, pathology, and eating behavior [23,24,25,26]. Pigs have a gyrencephalic brain [27,28], unlike mice who are lissencephalic, a key architectural difference that has a direct correlation with brain connectivity and complexity [29,30,31,32]. The human and swine brain is composed of >60% white matter, while the white matter in the rodent brain is < 10% [33,34]. The anatomical similarities in different brain regions, such as the HC [35], subcortical and diencephalic nuclei [36], and cortical regions [37,38] between the pig and human brain have been illustrated. Furthermore, the pig brain exhibits a perinatal growth spurt much like human infants, during which the brain weight grows most rapidly from the last trimester of gestation to lactation [39,40]. Pig brain grows most rapidly from about 50 days pre- to 40 days postnatal [40]; the cerebrum presents two rapid growth periods between 80–100 days of conception and 6–26 days after birth [41]. In healthy human infants, the brain volume is around 25% of an adult’s brain at birth and grows rapidly to around 72 and 83% of an adult’s total brain volume at 1 year and 2 years after birth, respectively [42]. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis in pigs revealed that the total brain volume reaches 75% and 95% around 10.7 and 22.07 weeks after birth, respectively [43]. It is estimated that 1 week of piglet age is comparable to 1 month in human infants, and the 3-week-old piglets in this study are estimated to have brain development similar to that seen in human infants of around 3 months of age.
This is the first study, to our knowledge, that thoroughly examines the effect of perinatal DHA intake on the functional and cognitive development of the brain in piglets. The present study aims to investigate whether maternal supplementation of DHA during late gestation and lactation imparts advantages to cognitive development, fiber bundle maturation, anatomical and functional organization, and the monoamine neurotransmitter status of the developing brain in healthy offspring using a piglet model.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals and Study Design

The cross-bred commercial line of healthy pregnant sows (n = 8) were obtained from the University of Georgia Swine unit at approximately day 69 of gestation. Sows were assigned to the DHA group (DHA, n = 5) or the isocaloric control group (CON, n = 3) after accounting for the parity (5.38 ± 0.32) and body weight (BW, 239.7 ± 7.71 kg). After one week of acclimation in a temperature-controlled facility, sows were fed the corresponding diets from day 74 of gestation until delivery and throughout lactation. Approximately one week before expected farrowing, sows were transferred to individually house farrowing crates equipped with heat lamps. From each litter, 2–3 male and female piglets with a BW closest to the average BW were chosen within 24 h to be included in the study (n = 14 in CON, n = 20 in DHA). At weaning, piglets underwent behavior examinations, and a subset of piglets (n = 7 in both CON and DHA) underwent MRI acquisition and were sacrificed at approximately the postnatal day (PND) 20 for tissue collection (Figure 1). This study was conducted per the University of Georgia Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines (project code: A2018 04-003-Y2-A8).

2.2. Dietary Treatment

Pregnant sows were maintained on a gestation (2 kg/day) and lactation (ad libitum) diet primarily composed of corn with a total of 3300–3330 kcal/kg as recommended by National Research Council (NRC), 2012 [44]. Sows in the DHA group were supplemented with 75 mg/kg BW/day DHA as algae-produced DHASCO (contains 42.9% DHA, DSM Nutritional Products Inc., Pendergrass, GA, USA) by mixing the oil with the basal diet (Table 1). Poultry fat and corn are the primary sources of fat in the basal diet. The major fatty acids in the basal diet, based on the fatty acid composition of poultry fat and corn [45,46], are linoleic acid (C18:2n6, LA, approximately 46% in gestation diet and 43% in lactation diet), oleic acid (18:1, approximately 33% in gestation diet and 34% in lactation diet), and palmitic acid (16:0, approximately 13% in gestation and 15% in lactation diet). The current dosage of DHA was chosen based on previous findings that effectively enhances DHA level in sows and piglets and exerts beneficial health effects on the animals [47,48]. To determine whether this dose is safe, liver alanine aminotransferase was measured using a clinical chemistry analyzer that confirmed that the current dose did not cause hepatotoxicity. Sows in the CON group were on an isocaloric diet mixing the basal diet (Table 1) with safflower oil (Jedwards International Inc., Braintree, MA, USA), which contains predominantly LA [49] suggesting no additional intake of ALA in the control animals.

2.3. Colostrum Fatty Acid Composition

Colostrum was collected within 12 h postpartum from the functioning teats of the same position from all sows. The fatty acid concentration was measured using gas chromatography (Shimadzu, model 14 A, Tokyo, Japan) with a flame ionization detector as previously described [50]. Briefly, 2 mL colostrum samples were transmethylated according to the method of Park and Goins [51] and 2 mg of tridecanoic acid (C13:0) was added as an internal standard before processing. The upper hexane layer was collected, and residual water was removed by adding anhydrous sodium sulfate. Fatty acid methyl esters were separated on a Phenomenex, ZBWax Plus wide-bore capillary column (60 m × 0.53 mm, 1.00 μm film thickness; Phenomonex, Torrance, CA) with nitrogen as the carrier gas. The initial column temperature was 160 °C; the temperature was held for 10 min and increased at a rate of 5 °C/min until 220 °C. The injector temperature was 250 °C, and the detector temperature was 260 °C. Peaks were identified by comparison of the retention times of known standards (Nu-Chek Prep, Elysian, MN, USA).

2.4. Behavior Testing

In order to habituate piglets to human touch and novel environments, all piglets were handled and habituated on a daily basis from PND2 until the day of behavior tests.

2.4.1. Open Field Test

As a measurement of ambulation and exploratory behaviors, piglets underwent the open field test (OFT) at approximately PND18 (PND 18.47 ± 0.097) in a 2.7 m × 2.7 m open arena lined with black mats. White curtains were hung around the arena to eliminate any visual distraction and the floor was sanitized with 70% ethanol between every trial to reduce olfactory bias. All piglets were exposed to the open arena for the first time. The piglets were individually introduced to the arena from the entry gate and allowed to explore for 10 min. Their exploratory behaviors (sniffing the wall), ambulation behaviors (mobile time, moving time), velocity, distance moved, and time spent in the center were recorded and measured by EthoVision video tracking software (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherlands). The center zone size was designated as 0.9 m × 0.9 m in the center of the arena (Figure 2).

2.4.2. Object Recognition Test

As a measurement of memory retention, piglets underwent the object recognition test (ORT), which has been successfully applied in pigs [52]. The ORT was comprised of two trials: a sample trial and a test trial. After habituation to the arena in OF, the piglet was reintroduced to the arena with two identical objects attached in the center of the open arena and was allowed to explore both objects for 10 min. The sample trial was immediately followed by a 10-min interphase delay, during which one object was replaced with a novel object. In the test trial, the piglet was introduced back to the arena and explored both the familiar and novel object for 10 min. All objects used in the study were cleaned thoroughly with water and ethanol between trials. The time that each piglet spent with each object in the sample and test trials were measured by Etho Vision (Noldus, Wageningen, the Netherlands). Proportional time was calculated as the ratio of time spent exploring the novel object to the total time exploring both objects in the sample and test trials.

2.5. MRI Acquisition

A subset of piglets (n = 14) underwent MRI imaging at approximately PND20 (PND 20.07 ± 0.31). Piglets were sedated with propofol for intubation (0.083–0.166 mL/kg, IV) and maintained under mild anesthesia with 1.5% isoflurane during scanning. MRI scanning including T1-weighted anatomical, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), and resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) were conducted at the Bioimaging Research Center at the University of Georgia utilizing a 3.0 Tesla General Electric (GE) HDx scanner and a quadrature knee coil. Piglets were monitored throughout the scan by a veterinary technician. A 3D fast spoiled gradient echo sequence (repetition time (TR) = 5.5 s, echo time (TE) = 2.1 ms, flip angle (FA) = 9°, field of view (FOV) = 12.8 × 12.8 × 6.4 cm, slice thickness = 1 mm, acquired matrix = 256 × 256 × 112) was used to acquire T1-weighted anatomical data; a spin-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 15.5 s, TE = min-full, FOV = 12.8 × 12.8 × 6.4 cm, acquired matrix = 64 × 64 × 32, and 30 diffusion weighted images using b = 1000 s/mm2) was used for DTI acquisition; and rs-fMRI was acquired by a gradient-echo EPI sequence (TR = 3 s, TE = 30 ms, FA = 80°, FOV = 12.8 × 12.8 × 6.4 cm, acquired matrix = 96 × 96 × 32, a total volume of 300 images).

2.6. MRI Analysis

2.6.1. Anatomical and DTI Analysis

For anatomical MRI analysis, the individual brain was coregistered with a standard pig brain atlas [53] using the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) toolbox. The percentage volume of 19 regions of interest were calculated using MATLAB (MATLAB R2018b, Natick, Massachusetts: The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). For the DTI dataset, brain tissue was separated from the skull and other surrounding tissues by manual segmentation using a 3D Slicer 4.11.0, and tractography (mapping of nerve fiber tracts) was performed using the Tensor Toolkit (TTK) for tensor estimation and tensor tractography using the software MedInria (https://med.inria.fr/). A whole brain tractography was performed with fibers seeding from voxels with FA values greater than approximately double the whole-brain average and stopping at voxels with FA values less than approximately two-thirds of the whole-brain average. A more detailed, standard pig brain atlas [54] was then used to coregister each individual DTI dataset using the SPM toolbox in MATLAB. Measures of mean diffusivity (MD), fractional anisotropy (FA), and fiber length (FL) were obtained for the fibers intersecting with the region of interest from the atlas.

2.6.2. Functional Connectivity Analysis

The rs-fMRI data (n = 7 for each group) were analyzed by using sparse dictionary learning (sDL) [55], a machine learning approach that has successfully detected brain functional connectivity [56]. Specifically, the fMRI time series was temporally concatenated to form a single matrix X, which was decomposed into two matrices, i.e., X = D × α, where D is the dictionary matrix aiming to learn temporal patterns from the concatenated time series (each column referred to as an atom), and α is a weight matrix that indicates the weights of learned features in the dataset. By using a back-propagation process, the dictionary was iteratively learned, and the corresponding α-matrix was used to generate functional connectivity maps, corresponding to each atom.
Pearson correlation (PC) was calculated by using the α-matrix and an atlas of piglet brain [56]. Three atoms with the highest PC coefficients were selected for the group analysis, separately for six resting state networks (RSNs), including the executive control network (ECN), cerebellum network (CERE), visual network (VIS), sensorimotor network (SMN), auditory network (AUD), and default mode network (DMN).

2.7. Animal Sacrifice and Tissue Collection

At weaning (PND 19.97 ± 0.14), all piglets were euthanized via CO2 asphyxiation. The brains were collected and coronally sectioned using a pig brain slicer (Zivic Instruments, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) and fresh tissues of HC and PFC were collected and immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored in −80 °C for future analysis.

2.8. HPLC-ECD

Monoamine neurotransmitters, including dopamine (DA), serotonin (5-HT), and norepinephrine (NE) and their metabolites from PFC and HC were measured by the electrochemical detector for high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC-ECD) as previously described [57,58]. Frozen tissue aliquots were mixed with 0.2 N perchloric acid (10 mg/100 μL), sonicated, and centrifuged. A total of 20 μL of the supernatant was injected into HPLC to determine: (1) DA and DA metabolites dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) and homovanillic acid (HVA); (2) 5-HT and its metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA); and (3) NE in brain tissues. The data were analyzed and presented as an ng of analyte per mg of tissue.

2.9. Statistical Analysis

Data expressed as mean ± S.E. were analyzed using R (version 3.6.0, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) and GraphPad Prism (Version 7.00, GraphPad Software, Inc.; San Diego, CA, USA). The colostrum fatty acid composition from CON and DHA sows was analyzed using GraphPad Prism. For other data of piglets, a linear mixed-effect model was fitted to test the treatment while controlling the gender and maternal factors as fixed and random effects, respectively. The statistical significance of the treatment effect was determined based on t-statistics with the significance level of 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Colostrum Fatty Acids Composition

The colostrum total lipids and lipid percentage did not differ in the two groups (p > 0.05, Table 2). The fatty acid profile of the colostrum from sows is comparable to that of the human colostrum, with high contents of palmitic acid (C16:0), oleic acid (C18:1), and linoleic acid (C18:2) [59]. Compared to CON, the DHA supplementation drastically increased the relative percentage levels of colostrum DHA (C22:6, 0.04 ± 0.01 and 3.36 ± 0.20 for CON and DHA, respectively, p < 0.0001) and other n-3 PUFA, including eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA, C20:5, 0.04 ± 0.02 and 0.48 ± 0.06 for CON and DHA, respectively, p = 0.001) and docosapentaenoic acid (DPA, C22:5, 0.17 ± 0.07 and 0.36 ± 0.04 for CON and DHA, respectively, p = 0.04) without influencing ALA level (C18:2, 0.83 ± 0.03 and 0.80 ± 0.07 for CON and DHA, p > 0.05) (Table 2). Concomitantly, the relative percentage levels of colostrum n-6 PUFA were markedly reduced in sows fed a DHA diet including LA (C18:2, 35.98 ± 2.03 and 27.55 ± 1.72 for CON and DHA, respectively, p = 0.02), dihomogammalinolenic acid (C20:3, 0.38 ± 0.05 and 0.23 ± 0.01 for CON and DHA, respectively, p = 0.01), and arachidonic acid (AA, C20:4, 1.16 ± 0.08 and 0.46 ± 0.01 for CON and DHA, respectively, p < 0.0001) (Table 2). Several other minor fatty acids, including lauric acid (C12:0), myristic acid (C14:0), myristoleic acid (C14:1), and palmitoleic acid (16:1), were also elevated in DHA-fed sows compared to CON (Table 2).

3.2. Piglet Bodyweight and Brain Weight

The piglets selected for this study were born with BW 1450.10 ± 68.84 g and grew to 5939.03 ± 288.00 g before weaning with an average daily weight gain of 223.48 ± 11.77 g/d and a final brain weight of 49.22 ± 0.63 g (averages of the two groups combined). Maternal supplementation of DHA and the isocaloric CON diet from late gestation to lactation did not influence the birth weight, final BW, weight gain, daily weight gain, and brain weight of the piglets at the end of the study (Table 3).

3.3. Behavior Testing

3.3.1. Open Field Test

There was no statistically significant difference in the total distance moved, velocity, mobile time of any body part, and moving time of the body center point between CON and DHA piglets. This suggests that maternal DHA supplementation did not influence the locomotor function of healthy offspring (Figure 3A–D). Similarly, piglets from the two groups spent a comparable amount of time and similar visiting frequency in the center zone of the arena (Figure 3E–F). Interestingly, the piglets born to DHA-fed sows spent 95% more time sniffing the walls of the open arena compared to CON (p = 0.002, Figure 3G–H), demonstrating that the maternal DHA supplementation may enhance the inquisitive and exploratory behavior of the healthy offspring without affecting the normal development of motor function at weaning.

3.3.2. Object Recognition Test

During the sample trial as explained in the methods section, all piglets spent similar time and frequency exploring the two identical objects with no preference (p > 0.05). In the test trial, male and female piglets born to CON-fed sows spent similar proportional time and frequency (Figure 4A–D) exploring the familiar and novel objects (p > 0.05). Male piglets from DHA-fed sows showed a tendency to spend more proportional time with the novel object (p = 0.060, Figure 4E), but not in proportional frequency (Figure 4F). The female piglets born to DHA-fed sows exhibited significantly more interest in the novel object than the familiar object, as they spent more proportional time (p = 0.032, Figure 4G) and proportional frequency (p = 0.008, Figure 4H) engaging with the novel object. This indicates that maternal DHA supplementation may improve the hippocampal-dependent short-term learning and memory of the offspring.

3.4. Structural MRI Analysis

The volumes of different brain regions in piglets born to CON and DHA-fed sows at weaning are presented in Table 4. At approximately PND 20, MRI volumetric assessments showed that piglet brains were composed of 63.4% cortex, 11.39% cerebellum, 2.76% thalamus, 1.84% HC, and 4.63% olfactory bulb and other subcortical regions. The maternal intake of DHA during late gestation and lactation did not pose any prominent change in the brain volumes of these healthy offspring. Although we found a statistically significant 0.0035% decrease in the thalamus, the clinical significance of this observation is unclear. In addition, the left and the right cortex tended to be slightly larger in male subjects compared to that of female subjects (left cortex: 31.2615% and 31.2565% for male and female, respectively; right cortex: 32.1408% and 32.1493% for male and female, respectively, p < 0.07). Overall, DHA supplementation during normal pregnancy and lactation did not result in significant changes in brain structural development in healthy offspring.

3.5. Hippocampal DTI Analysis

Axons insulated by a myelin sheath and firmly packed axonal bundles are critical for neuronal signal transduction and information processing, which may contribute to improved cognitive performance. Whole-brain tractography was performed in piglets born to CON- and DHA-fed sows (Figure 5A1–A2). Tract-based DTI analysis showed that DHA supplementation during gestation and lactation did not influence MD in piglet HC (p > 0.05, Figure 5B1). However, there was a trend of higher FA values in piglets born to DHA-fed sows (0.21 ± 0.003 and 0.23 ± 0.005 for CON and DHA, respectively, p = 0.07, Figure 5B2). Additionally, we observed a significant increase in FL in the HC of piglets born to DHA-fed sows (27.02 ± 0.93 mm) compared to piglets born to CON-fed sows (19.84 ± 2.07 mm, p = 0.01, Figure 5B3).

3.6. Functional Connectivity Analysis

In order to test whether maternal supplementation of DHA influences the large-scale cortical networks, we examined neural network organizations in six identified RSNs (Table 5). Earlier, using sDL, we successfully detected these networks in piglets that resemble their counterparts in human brains [56]. PC analysis was performed using the sDL activation maps and RSN atlas [56]. The PC analysis results and representative brain activation maps are presented in Figure 6 and Figure 7. Perinatal DHA supplementation resulted in an 8.7% increase in functional connectivity within the CERE (r = 0.3818 and 0.4152 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6B/Figure 7B), 5.2% enhanced connectivity within the VIS (r = 0.3915 and 0.4120 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6C/Figure 7C), 9.8% increase within the DMN (r = 0.3048 and 0.3346 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6F/Figure 7F) and a minor increase within the AUD (r = 0.2523 and 0.2526 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6E/Figure 7E) compared to CON piglets. In addition, piglets born to DHA-fed sows showed a 7.1% decrease in the ECN (r = 0.5217 and 0.4849 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6A/Figure 7A) and an 8.9% decrease in functional connectivity in the SMN compared to that of CON piglets (r = 0.3149 and 0.4120 for CON and DHA, respectively, Figure 6D/Figure 7D).

3.7. Monoamine Neurotransmitters

In order to determine if the effects of DHA is through the regulation of monoamine neurotransmission, we measured key monoamine neurotransmitters and their metabolites using HPLC (Table 6). In the PFC, the levels of DA, 5-HT, NE and their metabolites did not differ between piglets born to CON and DHA-fed sows (p > 0.05). Piglets from DHA-fed sows had 32.94% higher DA, 30.48% higher 5-HT, and 45.83% higher NE in the HC relative to that of CON piglets; however, none of these reached statistical significance, likely due to the small sample size. Metabolites of DA and 5-HT in HC were not different between the two groups.

4. Discussion

DHA is commonly recommended for pregnant women and is widely added in infant formula for its potential benefits in visual and brain functions [60,61]. However, interventional trials have yielded conflicting neurocognitive outcomes. In this study, we found that maternal DHA supplementation increased exploratory behaviors, short-term object recognition memory, fiber length in the HC, and enhanced functional connectivity in key brain networks of healthy offspring.
We found that DHA supplementation during late gestation and lactation resulted in a remarkable 80-fold increase in the colostrum DHA concentration along with the other n-3 PUFA without influencing the ALA level. This is in agreement with the previous findings from humans and pigs that maternal dietary intake of DHA sufficiently modified breast milk DHA and other n-3 PUFAs, such as EPA (C20:5) and DPA (C22:5) levels, while ALA levels remained relatively stable [62,63,64,65,66]. Although we did not measure plasma DHA level in the offspring, other studies have shown that supplementing lactating mothers with DHA increased infant plasma and erythrocytes phospholipid DHA levels [64,65]. Meanwhile, breastmilk n-6 PUFAs, such as LA (C18:2) and AA (C20:4), were decreased due to dietary DHA supplementation in accordance with previous findings in humans, suggesting that reduced levels of these n-6 PUFAs in breast milk and a decreased n-6/n-3 ratio in infant plasma by DHA supplementation during pregnancy and lactation are likely due to the competitive incorporation of these two PUFAs into the plasma membrane [66,67].
Piglets born to DHA-fed sows demonstrated better cognitive performance in the behavioral testing. The OFT provides a simple and general measure of motor function and exploratory behaviors [68] and the ORT measures short-term object recognition memory, which is at least partly hippocampus dependent [69] in an enclosed and undisturbed setting. Previous studies demonstrated that selective hippocampal lesion led to impaired object recognition memory in both human and non-human primates [70,71], indicating a critical role of hippocampus in the short-term recognition memory. Additionally, the development of memory ability is suggested to be dependent partly on the progressive development of the hippocampus in a sequence of novelty preference, cognitive recall, flexible memory, and source memory [72]. Healthy offspring from DHA-fed and CON-fed sows exhibited similar locomotor functions. Interestingly, piglets from DHA-fed sows demonstrated more exploratory behaviors as they spent more time sniffing the walls of the open arena. Dietary intake of n-3-PUFA was found to be associated with increased exploratory behaviors in other models [73,74]. This may be due to decreased stress and anxiety levels and/or increased visual function of the piglets from the DHA group. In rodents, a low intake of n-3 PUFA reduced exploratory behaviors in young animals and dietary DHA decreases stress and anxiety levels [74,75,76]. In pigs, postnatal DHA deprivation depleted frontal cortex DHA and increased fear/anxiety-like behavior associated with brain DA levels [77]. The improved recognition memory formation assessed by ORT is in agreement with previous findings in which preterm piglets supplemented with DHA had increased recognition memory compared to the control [78]. The fact that the control piglets did not have a preference for the novel object at this stage indicates that a regulatory recommendation for DHA supplementation during neurodevelopment is worth considering. Nevertheless, more data and studies are needed as the memory-improving effect of DHA is widely seen in young adults [79] but not consistently observed in infants [20,80,81]. It is worth noting that these human trials commonly used the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence to assess the recognition memory with only visual stimuli [82]. In comparison, the ORT in animals provides the subjects an opportunity to explore the objects with physical contact. The enhanced exploratory behavior and memory may also be related to the improved visual acuity due to DHA treatment. Human infants fed with a DHA-enriched diet showed accelerated development and maturation of the visual system and better visual function [83,84]. We also found increased functional connectivity within the visual network in these piglets, suggesting that the increased curiosity to the surrounding environment and higher engagement during object recognition may be partly due to the advanced development of the visual system.
MRI such as structural MRI, DTI, and rs-fMRI is increasingly used as a powerful tool to evaluate the role of DHA in the cognitive development of preterm infants. In our study, the perinatal DHA supplementation increased the myelination of axonal bundles in piglets. DTI measures brain microstructure and has been largely used to assess white matter integrity indicated by a reduced FA value [85,86]. Children born with very low birth weight had lower FA values in internal and external capsules, corpus callosum, and inferior and superior fasciculus than the control group, which was associated with visual-motor deficits and lower IQ scores [87]. A lower FA score was found to be associated with language processing, reading skills, and attention and anxiety behaviors later in life of preterm-born children, and such relation was not observed in full-term infants [88,89]. Breastfed infants have higher brain DHA concentrations [90], and preterm infants fed exclusively with breastmilk demonstrated greater structural connectivity and higher FA in major white matter fasciculi compared to non-exclusively breastfed infants [91]. Moreover, postnatal DHA supplementation to premature infants for 9 weeks showed a trend of a higher FA value in the corpus callosum related to control preterm infants at 8 years of age [92]. In this study, DTI analysis revealed an increased fiber tract length and a trend of increased FA value, which is in agreement with the previous findings in which higher DTI indices in the HC was seen in DHA-fed preterm pigs [78].
Resting-state fMRI assesses intrinsic functional connectivity between different neural networks by measuring fluctuations in blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal while the subject remains at a resting state without doing any cognitive task [93]. To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study investigating the intrinsic RSN changes due to maternal intake of DHA during gestation and lactation. CERE is associated with various fundamental functions, including motor coordination, visuomotor learning, executive function, and memory formation [94,95]. Higher activity of CERE due to perinatal DHA supplementation is likely to benefit the visual guidance of movement and working memory coinciding with our observations in the behavioral testing. DMN represents the intrinsic and spontaneous neuronal activity associated with internal thought processes and is deactivated during cognitively demanding tasks [96,97]. Failure of the DMN deactivation is associated with cognitive abnormities [98,99,100]. While it is unclear why perinatal DHA supplementation increased DMN activity in piglets, the intervention is likely to pose long-term influences on the cognitive processing and neurodegenerative processes [101]. Further investigations of maternal DHA supplementation on DMN activity comparing resting and task-based fMRI in infants will be of interest.
An increased VIS activity with DHA supplementation may indicate the accelerated maturation of the visual system, which underlies the improved visual acuity observed in human trials [83,84,102]. Term infants are born with adult-like VIS and SMN networks, suggesting prenatal development within these two domains [103]. We also found a decreased activity within SMN in piglets born to DHA-fed sows, which may suggest that DHA prevents the activation of SMN at the resting state. Interestingly, in human infants, the rapid development of SMN may occur earlier than the VIS [104], and the within-network connectivity of SMN manifests an age-related decrease during the first two years [103]; this may indicate an increased synaptic and axonal pruning within the sensory and motor cortices and a shift of functional organization towards more specialized cortical networks [105]. Thus, a decreased functional connectivity within SMN may suggest an enhanced development and maturation of the network that might have resulted from maternal DHA supplementation. Our findings suggest that perinatal DHA supplementation may alter brain functional organization of the offspring and support rs-fMRI as a sensitive tool to assess DHA status on brain functional connectivity and cortical organization in healthy piglets.
Monoamine neurotransmitters are critical neurochemicals for the proper function of learning, memory, and emotions [106]. Dietary n = 3-PUFA deficiency drastically decreased DA level and increased the 5-HT2 receptor in the PFC of young animals [107]. In piglets, postnatal DHA and AA supplementation to ALA/LA-deficient animals increased DA, 5-HT, and NE in the frontal cortex [108]. Perinatal ALA deficiency decreased tyrosine hydroxylase level, the rate-limiting enzyme for DA synthesis, in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area of the dopaminergic pathway [109] and increased 5-HT turnover in the PFC in rats [10]. We found that during a healthy pregnancy, maternal DHA supplementation resulted in an insignificant increase in DA, 5-HT, and NE in the HC of the offspring. It will be of interest to determine whether perinatal DHA intake influences neurotransmitters with a larger sample size.
This is the first study to provide evidence for improvement in cognition and brain development with perinatal DHA supplementation in piglets. However, there are a few limitations to this study. A small sample size, though statistically enough, may have limited our power to detect the true effect of perinatal DHA supplementation in piglets and increased the error due to the random effects, including sow effects. The maternal factor was controlled in statistical analysis by including sow as a random effect using the mixed-effect linear model. Additionally, the present study used safflower oil as a control oil, which is high in n-6 PUFA. Thus, the observed changes may be ascribed in part to the high n-6 intake in the control animals. Although n-6-rich components like safflower oil are commonly used in the control diets to match the caloric intake between the control and test groups, safflower oil is low in oleic acid, which is the main fatty acid of myelin and could potentially influence myelination and brain functional connectivity during development [110]. Finally, the current study measured fMRI when the animals were at resting state. While it provided valuable insights into the resting-state networks influenced by the treatment, it is challenging to interpret due to its novel application in infant piglets. Future studies integrating both resting-state and task-based fMRI in a larger sample size would be of interest.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the present data provides preclinical support that the maternal DHA supplementation during late gestation and lactation may increase exploratory behavior, improve memory function, enhance fiber tract integrity, and alter brain functional organization of offspring at weaning without affecting the volume of major brain structures.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.J.P. and X.F.; Methodology, H.J.P., X.F., Q.Z., and H.K.; Investigation, H.J.P., X.F., J.J., M.A., R.S.M., N.M.F., H.K., and Q.Z.; Formal Analysis, X.F., W.S., Q.Z., J.A., H.C.C., and H.J.P.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, X.F., H.J.P., and S.R.; Writing—Review & Editing, X.F., H.J.P., S.R., W.S., J.J., M.A., H.K., J.A., H.C.C., R.S.M., N.M.F., and Q.Z.; Project Administration, H.J.P. and X.F.; Funding Acquisition, H.J.P. and S.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Georgia Experimental Agricultural Station (HATCH #GEO00795); faculty research grant from Office of Research at the UGA; and Division of Research at PCOM (19131). DHA was kindly provided by DSM, Inc.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Gregory Simchick for assisting the DTI and rs-fMRI data analysis, and Ms. Kim Mason for MRI data acquisition.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cusick, S.E.; Georgieff, M.K. The Role of Nutrition in Brain Development: The Golden Opportunity of the “First 1000 Days”. J. Pediatr. 2016, 175, 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Lien, E.L.; Richard, C.; Hoffman, D.R. DHA and ARA addition to infant formula: Current status and future research directions. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2018, 128, 26–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Lauritzen, L.; Brambilla, P.; Mazzocchi, A.; Harslof, L.B.; Ciappolino, V.; Agostoni, C. DHA Effects in Brain Development and Function. Nutrients 2016, 8, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Lacombe, R.J.S.; Chouinard-Watkins, R.; Bazinet, R.P. Brain docosahexaenoic acid uptake and metabolism. Mol. Aspects Med. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Xiao, Y.; Huang, Y.; Chen, Z.Y. Distribution, depletion and recovery of docosahexaenoic acid are region-specific in rat brain. Br. J. Nutr. 2005, 94, 544–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. McNamara, R.K.; Carlson, S.E. Role of omega-3 fatty acids in brain development and function: Potential implications for the pathogenesis and prevention of psychopathology. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2006, 75, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Coletta, J.M.; Bell, S.J.; Roman, A.S. Omega-3 Fatty acids and pregnancy. Rev. Obstet. Gynecol. 2010, 3, 163–171. [Google Scholar]
  8. Carlson, S.E. Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation in pregnancy and lactation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 89, 678S–684S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Kang, H.J.; Kawasawa, Y.I.; Cheng, F.; Zhu, Y.; Xu, X.; Li, M.; Sousa, A.M.; Pletikos, M.; Meyer, K.A.; Sedmak, G.; et al. Spatio-temporal transcriptome of the human brain. Nature 2011, 478, 483–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. McNamara, R.K.; Able, J.; Liu, Y.; Jandacek, R.; Rider, T.; Tso, P.; Lipton, J.W. Omega-3 fatty acid deficiency during perinatal development increases serotonin turnover in the prefrontal cortex and decreases midbrain tryptophan hydroxylase-2 expression in adult female rats: Dissociation from estrogenic effects. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2009, 43, 656–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Moriguchi, T.; Greiner, R.S.; Salem, N., Jr. Behavioral deficits associated with dietary induction of decreased brain docosahexaenoic acid concentration. J. Neurochem. 2000, 75, 2563–2573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Dunstan, J.A.; Mitoulas, L.R.; Dixon, G.; Doherty, D.A.; Hartmann, P.E.; Simmer, K.; Prescott, S.L. The effects of fish oil supplementation in pregnancy on breast milk fatty acid composition over the course of lactation: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatr. Res. 2007, 62, 689–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Boucher, O.; Burden, M.J.; Muckle, G.; Saint-Amour, D.; Ayotte, P.; Dewailly, E.; Nelson, C.A.; Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L. Neurophysiologic and neurobehavioral evidence of beneficial effects of prenatal omega-3 fatty acid intake on memory function at school age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 93, 1025–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Hibbeln, J.R.; Davis, J.M.; Steer, C.; Emmett, P.; Rogers, I.; Williams, C.; Golding, J. Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): An observational cohort study. Lancet 2007, 369, 578–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Braarud, H.C.; Markhus, M.W.; Skotheim, S.; Stormark, K.M.; Froyland, L.; Graff, I.E.; Kjellevold, M. Maternal DHA Status during Pregnancy Has a Positive Impact on Infant Problem Solving: A Norwegian Prospective Observation Study. Nutrients 2018, 10, 529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Simmer, K. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cohen, J.T.; Bellinger, D.C.; Connor, W.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. A quantitative analysis of prenatal intake of n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognitive development. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005, 29, 366–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Shulkin, M.; Pimpin, L.; Bellinger, D.; Kranz, S.; Fawzi, W.; Duggan, C.; Mozaffarian, D. n-3 Fatty Acid Supplementation in Mothers, Preterm Infants, and Term Infants and Childhood Psychomotor and Visual Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Nutr. 2018, 148, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Helland, I.B.; Smith, L.; Saarem, K.; Saugstad, O.D.; Drevon, C.A. Maternal supplementation with very-long-chain n-3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children’s IQ at 4 years of age. Pediatrics 2003, 111, e39–e44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Helland, I.B.; Saugstad, O.D.; Smith, L.; Saarem, K.; Solvoll, K.; Ganes, T.; Drevon, C.A. Similar effects on infants of n-3 and n-6 fatty acids supplementation to pregnant and lactating women. Pediatrics 2001, 108, E82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Makrides, M.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Yelland, L.; Quinlivan, J.; Ryan, P.; The DOMInO Investigative Team. Effect of DHA supplementation during pregnancy on maternal depression and neurodevelopment of young children: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2010, 304, 1675–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. van Goor, S.A.; Dijck-Brouwer, D.A.; Erwich, J.J.; Schaafsma, A.; Hadders-Algra, M. The influence of supplemental docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acids during pregnancy and lactation on neurodevelopment at eighteen months. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2011, 84, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Mudd, A.T.; Dilger, R.N. Early-Life Nutrition and Neurodevelopment: Use of the Piglet as a Translational Model. Adv. Nutr. 2017, 8, 92–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Lind, N.M.; Moustgaard, A.; Jelsing, J.; Vajta, G.; Cumming, P.; Hansen, A.K. The use of pigs in neuroscience: Modeling brain disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2007, 31, 728–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Thomas, J.M.; Beamer, J.L. Age-weight relationships of selected organs and body weight for miniature swine. Growth 1971, 35, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  26. Watanabe, H.; Andersen, F.; Simonsen, C.Z.; Evans, S.M.; Gjedde, A.; Cumming, P.; DaNe, X.S.G. MR-based statistical atlas of the Gottingen minipig brain. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 1089–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sun, T.; Hevner, R.F. Growth and folding of the mammalian cerebral cortex: From molecules to malformations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 217–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Schmidt, V.; Verlag, V.L. Comparative Anatomy of the Pig Brain—An Integrative Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Study of the Porcine Brain with Special Emphasis on the External Morphology of the Cerebral Cortex; VVB Laufersweiler Verlag: Staufenbergring, Germany, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  29. Nakamura, M.; Imai, H.; Konno, K.; Kubota, C.; Seki, K.; Puentes, S.; Faried, A.; Yokoo, H.; Hata, H.; Yoshimoto, Y.; et al. Experimental investigation of encephalomyosynangiosis using gyrencephalic brain of the miniature pig: Histopathological evaluation of dynamic reconstruction of vessels for functional anastomosis. Laboratory investigation. J. Neurosurg. Pediatr. 2009, 3, 488–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kuluz, J.W.; Prado, R.; He, D.; Zhao, W.; Dietrich, W.D.; Watson, B. New pediatric model of ischemic stroke in infant piglets by photothrombosis: Acute changes in cerebral blood flow, microvasculature, and early histopathology. Stroke 2007, 38, 1932–1937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Tanaka, Y.; Imai, H.; Konno, K.; Miyagishima, T.; Kubota, C.; Puentes, S.; Aoki, T.; Hata, H.; Takata, K.; Yoshimoto, Y.; et al. Experimental model of lacunar infarction in the gyrencephalic brain of the miniature pig: Neurological assessment and histological, immunohistochemical, and physiological evaluation of dynamic corticospinal tract deformation. Stroke 2008, 39, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Baltan, S.; Besancon, E.F.; Mbow, B.; Ye, Z.; Hamner, M.A.; Ransom, B.R. White matter vulnerability to ischemic injury increases with age because of enhanced excitotoxicity. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 1479–1489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Conrad, M.S.; Dilger, R.N.; Nickolls, A.; Johnson, R.W. Magnetic resonance imaging of the neonatal piglet brain. Pediatr. Res. 2012, 71, 179–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Zhang, K.; Sejnowski, T.J. A universal scaling law between gray matter and white matter of cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 5621–5626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Holm, I.E.; West, M.J. Hippocampus of the domestic pig: A stereological study of subdivisional volumes and neuron numbers. Hippocampus 1994, 4, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Larsen, M.; Bjarkam, C.R.; Ostergaard, K.; West, M.J.; Sorensen, J.C. The anatomy of the porcine subthalamic nucleus evaluated with immunohistochemistry and design-based stereology. Anat. Embryol. 2004, 208, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jelsing, J.; Hay-Schmidt, A.; Dyrby, T.; Hemmingsen, R.; Uylings, H.B.; Pakkenberg, B. The prefrontal cortex in the Gottingen minipig brain defined by neural projection criteria and cytoarchitecture. Brain Res. Bull. 2006, 70, 322–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Craner, S.L.; Ray, R.H. Somatosensory cortex of the neonatal pig: II. Topographic organization of the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII). J. Comp. Neurol. 1991, 306, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dobbing, J.; Sands, J. Comparative aspects of the brain growth spurt. Early Hum. Dev. 1979, 3, 79–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dickerson, J.W.; Dobbing, J. Prenatal and postnatal growth and development of the central nervous system of the pig. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1967, 166, 384–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Pond, W.G.; Boleman, S.L.; Fiorotto, M.L.; Ho, H.; Knabe, D.A.; Mersmann, H.J.; Savell, J.W.; Su, D.R. Perinatal ontogeny of brain growth in the domestic pig. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 2000, 223, 102–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Knickmeyer, R.C.; Gouttard, S.; Kang, C.; Evans, D.; Wilber, K.; Smith, J.K.; Hamer, R.M.; Lin, W.; Gerig, G.; Gilmore, J.H. A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 12176–12182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Conrad, M.S.; Dilger, R.N.; Johnson, R.W. Brain growth of the domestic pig (Sus scrofa) from 2 to 24 weeks of age: A longitudinal MRI study. Dev. Neurosci. 2012, 34, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. National Research Council (US.). Committee on Nutrient Requirements of Swine. In Nutrient Requirements of Swine, 11th rev. ed.; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kawakami, Y.; Yamanaka-Okumura, H.; Naniwa-Kuroki, Y.; Sakuma, M.; Taketani, Y.; Takeda, E. Flaxseed oil intake reduces serum small dense low-density lipoprotein concentrations in Japanese men: A randomized, double blind, crossover study. Nutr. J. 2015, 14, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Adhikari, C.K.; Chiba, L.I.; Brotzge, S.D.; Vieira, M.S.; Huang, C.; Bergen, W.G.; Bratcher, C.L.; Rodning, S.P.; Welles, E.G. Early dietary amino acid restrictions and flaxseed oil supplementation on the leanness of pigs and quality of pork: Growth performance, serum metabolites, carcass characteristics, and physical and sensory characteristics of pork. Livest. Sci. 2017, 198, 182–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Leonard, S.G.; Sweeney, T.; Bahar, B.; Lynch, B.P.; O’Doherty, J.V. Effect of maternal fish oil and seaweed extract supplementation on colostrum and milk composition, humoral immune response, and performance of suckled piglets. J. Anim. Sci. 2010, 88, 2988–2997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Smit, M.N.; Patterson, J.L.; Webel, S.K.; Spencer, J.D.; Cameron, A.C.; Dyck, M.K.; Dixon, W.T.; Foxcroft, G.R. Responses to n-3 fatty acid (LCPUFA) supplementation of gestating gilts, and lactating and weaned sows. Animal 2013, 7, 784–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Orsavova, J.; Misurcova, L.; Ambrozova, J.V.; Vicha, R.; Mlcek, J. Fatty Acids Composition of Vegetable Oils and Its Contribution to Dietary Energy Intake and Dependence of Cardiovascular Mortality on Dietary Intake of Fatty Acids. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2015, 16, 12871–12890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cromwell, G.L.; Azain, M.J.; Adeola, O.; Baidoo, S.K.; Carter, S.D.; Crenshaw, T.D.; Kim, S.W.; Mahan, D.C.; Miller, P.S.; Shannon, M.C.; et al. Corn distillers dried grains with solubles in diets for growing-finishing pigs: A cooperative study. J. Anim. Sci. 2011, 89, 2801–2811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Park, P.W.; Goins, R.E. In Situ Preparation of Fatty Acid Methyl Esters for Analysis of Fatty Acid Composition in Foods. J. Food Sci. 1994, 59, 1262–1266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Fleming, S.A.; Dilger, R.N. Young pigs exhibit differential exploratory behavior during novelty preference tasks in response to age, sex, and delay. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 321, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Radlowski, E.C.; Conrad, M.S.; Lezmi, S.; Dilger, R.N.; Sutton, B.; Larsen, R.; Johnson, R.W. A neonatal piglet model for investigating brain and cognitive development in small for gestational age human infants. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e91951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Saikali, S.; Meurice, P.; Sauleau, P.; Eliat, P.A.; Bellaud, P.; Randuineau, G.; Verin, M.; Malbert, C.H. A three-dimensional digital segmented and deformable brain atlas of the domestic pig. J. Neurosci. Methods 2010, 192, 102–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Mairal, J.; Bach, F.; Ponce, J.; Sapiro, G. Online dictionary learning for sparse coding. In Proceedings of the 26th Annual International Conference on Machine Learning, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, 14–18 June 2009; pp. 689–696. [Google Scholar]
  56. Simchick, G.; Shen, A.; Campbell, B.; Park, H.J.; West, F.D.; Zhao, Q. Pig Brains Have Homologous Resting-State Networks with Human Brains. Brain Connect. 2019, 9, 566–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Coban, A.; Filipov, N.M. Dopaminergic toxicity associated with oral exposure to the herbicide atrazine in juvenile male C57BL/6 mice. J. Neurochem. 2007, 100, 1177–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Krishna, S.; Lin, Z.; de La Serre, C.B.; Wagner, J.J.; Harn, D.H.; Pepples, L.M.; Djani, D.M.; Weber, M.T.; Srivastava, L.; Filipov, N.M. Time-dependent behavioral, neurochemical, and metabolic dysregulation in female C57BL/6 mice caused by chronic high-fat diet intake. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 157, 196–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Sinanoglou, V.J.; Cavouras, D.; Boutsikou, T.; Briana, D.D.; Lantzouraki, D.Z.; Paliatsiou, S.; Volaki, P.; Bratakos, S.; Malamitsi-Puchner, A.; Zoumpoulakis, P. Factors affecting human colostrum fatty acid profile: A case study. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0175817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Koletzko, B.; Cetin, I.; Brenna, J.T.; Perinatal Lipid Intake Working Group. Dietary fat intakes for pregnant and lactating women. Br. J. Nutr. 2007, 98, 873–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Koletzko, B.; Lien, E.; Agostoni, C.; Bohles, H.; Campoy, C.; Cetin, I.; Decsi, T.; Dudenhausen, J.W.; Dupont, C.; Forsyth, S.; et al. The roles of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation and infancy: Review of current knowledge and consensus recommendations. J. Perinat Med. 2008, 36, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Fidler, N.; Sauerwald, T.; Pohl, A.; Demmelmair, H.; Koletzko, B. Docosahexaenoic acid transfer into human milk after dietary supplementation: A randomized clinical trial. J. Lipid Res. 2000, 41, 1376–1383. [Google Scholar]
  63. Hawkes, J.S.; Bryan, D.L.; Makrides, M.; Neumann, M.A.; Gibson, R.A. A randomized trial of supplementation with docosahexaenoic acid-rich tuna oil and its effects on the human milk cytokines interleukin 1 beta, interleukin 6, and tumor necrosis factor alpha. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 75, 754–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Jensen, C.L.; Maude, M.; Anderson, R.E.; Heird, W.C. Effect of docosahexaenoic acid supplementation of lactating women on the fatty acid composition of breast milk lipids and maternal and infant plasma phospholipids. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 71, 292S–299S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Gibson, R.A.; Neumann, M.A.; Makrides, M. Effect of increasing breast milk docosahexaenoic acid on plasma and erythrocyte phospholipid fatty acids and neural indices of exclusively breast fed infants. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997, 51, 578–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Arbuckle, L.D.; Innis, S.M. Docosahexaenoic acid is transferred through maternal diet to milk and to tissues of natural milk-fed piglets. J. Nutr. 1993, 123, 1668–1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Sherry, C.L.; Oliver, J.S.; Marriage, B.J. Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation in lactating women increases breast milk and plasma docosahexaenoic acid concentrations and alters infant omega 6:3 fatty acid ratio. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2015, 95, 63–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Donald, R.D.; Healy, S.D.; Lawrence, A.B.; Rutherford, K.M. Emotionality in growing pigs: Is the open field a valid test? Physiol. Behav. 2011, 104, 906–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Kouwenberg, A.-L.; Walsh, C.J.; Morgan, B.E.; Martin, G.M. Episodic-like memory in crossbred Yucatan minipigs (Sus scrofa). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 117, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Pascalis, O.; Bachevalier, J. Neonatal aspiration lesions of the hippocampal formation impair visual recognition memory when assessed by paired-comparison task but not by delayed nonmatching-to-sample task. Hippocampus 1999, 9, 609–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Pascalis, O.; Hunkin, N.M.; Holdstock, J.S.; Isaac, C.L.; Mayes, A.R. Visual paired comparison performance is impaired in a patient with selective hippocampal lesions and relatively intact item recognition. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1293–1300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. de Haan, M.; Mishkin, M.; Baldeweg, T.; Vargha-Khadem, F. Human memory development and its dysfunction after early hippocampal injury. Trends Neurosci. 2006, 29, 374–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Carrie, I.; Guesnet, P.; Bourre, J.M.; Frances, H. Diets containing long-chain n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids affect behaviour differently during development than ageing in mice. Br. J. Nutr. 2000, 83, 439–447. [Google Scholar]
  74. Enslen, M.; Milon, H.; Malnoe, A. Effect of low intake of n-3 fatty acids during development on brain phospholipid fatty acid composition and exploratory behavior in rats. Lipids 1991, 26, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Takeuchi, T.; Iwanaga, M.; Harada, E. Possible regulatory mechanism of DHA-induced anti-stress reaction in rats. Brain Res. 2003, 964, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Harauma, A.; Moriguchi, T. Dietary n-3 fatty acid deficiency in mice enhances anxiety induced by chronic mild stress. Lipids 2011, 46, 409–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Ng, K.F.; Innis, S.M. Behavioral responses are altered in piglets with decreased frontal cortex docosahexaenoic acid. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3222–3227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Buddington, R.K.; Chizhikov, V.V.; Iskusnykh, I.Y.; Sable, H.J.; Sable, J.J.; Holloway, Z.R.; Blumenfeld Katzir, T.; van der Merwe, M.; Yakimkova, T.; Buddington, K.K.; et al. A Phosphatidylserine Source of Docosahexanoic Acid Improves Neurodevelopment and Survival of Preterm Pigs. Nutrients 2018, 10, 637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Stonehouse, W.; Conlon, C.A.; Podd, J.; Hill, S.R.; Minihane, A.M.; Haskell, C.; Kennedy, D. DHA supplementation improved both memory and reaction time in healthy young adults: A randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 97, 1134–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Judge, M.P.; Harel, O.; Lammi-Keefe, C.J. Maternal consumption of a docosahexaenoic acid-containing functional food during pregnancy: Benefit for infant performance on problem-solving but not on recognition memory tasks at age 9 mo. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 85, 1572–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. O’Connor, D.L.; Hall, R.; Adamkin, D.; Auestad, N.; Castillo, M.; Connor, W.E.; Connor, S.L.; Fitzgerald, K.; Groh-Wargo, S.; Hartmann, E.E.; et al. Growth and development in preterm infants fed long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: A prospective, randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 2001, 108, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Benasich, A.A.; Bejar, I.I. The Fagan test of infant intelligence: A critical review. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 1992, 13, 153–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Hoffman, D.R.; Theuer, R.C.; Castaneda, Y.S.; Wheaton, D.H.; Bosworth, R.G.; O’Connor, A.R.; Morale, S.E.; Wiedemann, L.E.; Birch, E.E. Maturation of visual acuity is accelerated in breast-fed term infants fed baby food containing DHA-enriched egg yolk. J. Nutr. 2004, 134, 2307–2313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Birch, E.E.; Hoffman, D.R.; Castaneda, Y.S.; Fawcett, S.L.; Birch, D.G.; Uauy, R.D. A randomized controlled trial of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation of formula in term infants after weaning at 6 wk of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 75, 570–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  85. McNamara, R.K. Deciphering the role of docosahexaenoic acid in brain maturation and pathology with magnetic resonance imaging. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2013, 88, 33–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Beaulieu, C. The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—A technical review. NMR Biomed. 2002, 15, 435–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Skranes, J.; Vangberg, T.R.; Kulseng, S.; Indredavik, M.S.; Evensen, K.A.; Martinussen, M.; Dale, A.M.; Haraldseth, O.; Brubakk, A.M. Clinical findings and white matter abnormalities seen on diffusion tensor imaging in adolescents with very low birth weight. Brain 2007, 130, 654–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Feldman, H.M.; Lee, E.S.; Yeatman, J.D.; Yeom, K.W. Language and reading skills in school-aged children and adolescents born preterm are associated with white matter properties on diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 3348–3362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Loe, I.M.; Lee, E.S.; Feldman, H.M. Attention and internalizing behaviors in relation to white matter in children born preterm. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2013, 34, 156–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  90. Jamieson, E.C.; Farquharson, J.; Logan, R.W.; Howatson, A.G.; Patrick, W.J.; Weaver, L.T.; Cockburn, F. Infant cerebellar gray and white matter fatty acids in relation to age and diet. Lipids 1999, 34, 1065–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Blesa, M.; Sullivan, G.; Anblagan, D.; Telford, E.J.; Quigley, A.J.; Sparrow, S.A.; Serag, A.; Semple, S.I.; Bastin, M.E.; Boardman, J.P. Early breast milk exposure modifies brain connectivity in preterm infants. Neuroimage 2019, 184, 431–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Almaas, A.N.; Tamnes, C.K.; Nakstad, B.; Henriksen, C.; Grydeland, H.; Walhovd, K.B.; Fjell, A.M.; Iversen, P.O.; Drevon, C.A. Diffusion tensor imaging and behavior in premature infants at 8 years of age, a randomized controlled trial with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. Early Hum. Dev. 2016, 95, 41–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Mongerson, C.R.L.; Jennings, R.W.; Borsook, D.; Becerra, L.; Bajic, D. Resting-State Functional Connectivity in the Infant Brain: Methods, Pitfalls, and Potentiality. Front. Pediatr. 2017, 5, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Stoodley, C.J.; Valera, E.M.; Schmahmann, J.D. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 1560–1570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  95. Vaillancourt, D.E.; Mayka, M.A.; Corcos, D.M. Intermittent visuomotor processing in the human cerebellum, parietal cortex, and premotor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2006, 95, 922–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Christoff, K.; Ream, J.M.; Gabrieli, J.D. Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 2004, 40, 623–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Raichle, M.E. The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 38, 433–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Kennedy, D.P.; Redcay, E.; Courchesne, E. Failing to deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 8275–8280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  99. Bartova, L.; Meyer, B.M.; Diers, K.; Rabl, U.; Scharinger, C.; Popovic, A.; Pail, G.; Kalcher, K.; Boubela, R.N.; Huemer, J.; et al. Reduced default mode network suppression during a working memory task in remitted major depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2015, 64, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  100. Lustig, C.; Snyder, A.Z.; Bhakta, M.; O’Brien, K.C.; McAvoy, M.; Raichle, M.E.; Morris, J.C.; Buckner, R.L. Functional deactivations: Change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 14504–14509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  101. Buckner, R.L.; Snyder, A.Z.; Shannon, B.J.; LaRossa, G.; Sachs, R.; Fotenos, A.F.; Sheline, Y.I.; Klunk, W.E.; Mathis, C.A.; Morris, J.C.; et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 7709–7717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Birch, E.E.; Hoffman, D.R.; Uauy, R.; Birch, D.G.; Prestidge, C. Visual acuity and the essentiality of docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid in the diet of term infants. Pediatr. Res. 1998, 44, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Gao, W.; Alcauter, S.; Smith, J.K.; Gilmore, J.H.; Lin, W. Development of human brain cortical network architecture during infancy. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015, 220, 1173–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Lin, W.; Zhu, Q.; Gao, W.; Chen, Y.; Toh, C.H.; Styner, M.; Gerig, G.; Smith, J.K.; Biswal, B.; Gilmore, J.H. Functional connectivity MR imaging reveals cortical functional connectivity in the developing brain. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2008, 29, 1883–1889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  105. Tau, G.Z.; Peterson, B.S. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010, 35, 147–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Suri, D.; Teixeira, C.M.; Cagliostro, M.K.; Mahadevia, D.; Ansorge, M.S. Monoamine-sensitive developmental periods impacting adult emotional and cognitive behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40, 88–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Delion, S.; Chalon, S.; Herault, J.; Guilloteau, D.; Besnard, J.C.; Durand, G. Chronic dietary alpha-linolenic acid deficiency alters dopaminergic and serotoninergic neurotransmission in rats. J. Nutr. 1994, 124, 2466–2476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. de la Presa Owens, S.; Innis, S.M. Docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acid prevent a decrease in dopaminergic and serotoninergic neurotransmitters in frontal cortex caused by a linoleic and alpha-linolenic acid deficient diet in formula-fed piglets. J. Nutr. 1999, 129, 2088–2093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Kuperstein, F.; Eilam, R.; Yavin, E. Altered expression of key dopaminergic regulatory proteins in the postnatal brain following perinatal n-3 fatty acid dietary deficiency. J. Neurochem. 2008, 106, 662–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Svennerholm, L.; Vanier, M.T. Lipid and fatty acid composition of human cerebral myelin during development. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1978, 100, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study design and experimental timeline. CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid.
Figure 1. Study design and experimental timeline. CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid.
Nutrients 12 02090 g001
Figure 2. Schematic illustration of the testing arena for open field test (A) and the object recognition test (B).
Figure 2. Schematic illustration of the testing arena for open field test (A) and the object recognition test (B).
Nutrients 12 02090 g002
Figure 3. Measures of activity in the open field test for piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 14) or DHA (n = 20) diet at weaning. (A) distance moved; (B) velocity; (C) mobile time of any part of the body; (D) moving time of the center point; (E) time spent in the center zone; (F) frequency visit the center zone; (G) sniffing wall time; (H) sniffing wall frequency. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Figure 3. Measures of activity in the open field test for piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 14) or DHA (n = 20) diet at weaning. (A) distance moved; (B) velocity; (C) mobile time of any part of the body; (D) moving time of the center point; (E) time spent in the center zone; (F) frequency visit the center zone; (G) sniffing wall time; (H) sniffing wall frequency. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Nutrients 12 02090 g003
Figure 4. Measures of activity in the test trial of novel object recognition test for piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 14) or DHA (n = 20) diet at weaning. Figures depicting proportional time and visit frequency on the familiar and novel objects. (A) proportional time of control male piglets; (B) proportional frequency of control male piglets; (C) proportional time of control female piglets; (D) proportional frequency of control female piglets; (E) proportional time of DHA male piglets; (F) proportional frequency of DHA male piglets; (G) proportional time of DHA female piglets; (H) proportional frequency of DHA female piglets; a linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: FO: familiar object; NO: novel object. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Figure 4. Measures of activity in the test trial of novel object recognition test for piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 14) or DHA (n = 20) diet at weaning. Figures depicting proportional time and visit frequency on the familiar and novel objects. (A) proportional time of control male piglets; (B) proportional frequency of control male piglets; (C) proportional time of control female piglets; (D) proportional frequency of control female piglets; (E) proportional time of DHA male piglets; (F) proportional frequency of DHA male piglets; (G) proportional time of DHA female piglets; (H) proportional frequency of DHA female piglets; a linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: FO: familiar object; NO: novel object. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Nutrients 12 02090 g004
Figure 5. Diffusion tensor imaging analysis. Representative whole brain tractography of piglets born to sows fed a CON (A1, n = 7) or DHA (A2, n = 7) diet during late gestation and lactation. Mean diffusivity (B1), fractional anisotropy (B2), and fiber length (B3) within the hippocampus of piglets. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid; MD: mean diffusivity; FA: fractional anisotropy; FL: fiber length. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Figure 5. Diffusion tensor imaging analysis. Representative whole brain tractography of piglets born to sows fed a CON (A1, n = 7) or DHA (A2, n = 7) diet during late gestation and lactation. Mean diffusivity (B1), fractional anisotropy (B2), and fiber length (B3) within the hippocampus of piglets. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid; MD: mean diffusivity; FA: fractional anisotropy; FL: fiber length. p values higher than 0.1 are not shown on the graph.
Nutrients 12 02090 g005
Figure 6. Group analysis of functional connectivity from rs-fMRI scan in six resting state networks, including the ECN (A), CERE (B), VIS (C), SMN (D), AUD (E), and DMN (F), in piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 7) or DHA (n = 7) diet. The graphs show the correlation coefficients from the best three activation maps. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. RSN: resting state network; ECN: executive control network; CERE: cerebellar network; VIS: visual network; SMN: sensorimotor network; AUD: auditory network; DMN: default mode network.
Figure 6. Group analysis of functional connectivity from rs-fMRI scan in six resting state networks, including the ECN (A), CERE (B), VIS (C), SMN (D), AUD (E), and DMN (F), in piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 7) or DHA (n = 7) diet. The graphs show the correlation coefficients from the best three activation maps. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. RSN: resting state network; ECN: executive control network; CERE: cerebellar network; VIS: visual network; SMN: sensorimotor network; AUD: auditory network; DMN: default mode network.
Nutrients 12 02090 g006
Figure 7. Color-coded regions illustrating activation within six RSNs, including ECN (A), CERE (B), VIS (C), SMN (D), AUD (E), and DMN (F), in piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 7) or DHA (n = 7) diet. Each session contains activation maps of CON (left) and DHA (right) piglets in axial, coronal, and sagittal planes. Horizontal and vertical bars on the axial image at the upper-left corner indicate locations of the coronal and sagittal images. Yellow patters are RSN atlases and orange patters are activations. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. RSN: resting state network; ECN: executive control network; CERE: cerebellar network; VIS: visual network; SMN: sensorimotor network; AUD: auditory network; DMN: default mode network.
Figure 7. Color-coded regions illustrating activation within six RSNs, including ECN (A), CERE (B), VIS (C), SMN (D), AUD (E), and DMN (F), in piglets born to sows fed a CON (n = 7) or DHA (n = 7) diet. Each session contains activation maps of CON (left) and DHA (right) piglets in axial, coronal, and sagittal planes. Horizontal and vertical bars on the axial image at the upper-left corner indicate locations of the coronal and sagittal images. Yellow patters are RSN atlases and orange patters are activations. Abbreviations: CON: control; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid. RSN: resting state network; ECN: executive control network; CERE: cerebellar network; VIS: visual network; SMN: sensorimotor network; AUD: auditory network; DMN: default mode network.
Nutrients 12 02090 g007
Table 1. Diet composition of the basal diet during gestation and lactation.
Table 1. Diet composition of the basal diet during gestation and lactation.
Ingredients, g/kgGestation Diet 1Lactation Diet 1
Corn535.4389.6
Soybean Meal32.3172.3
Distillers Dried Grains with Solubles400.0400.0
Fat 4.6
Dicalcium Phosphate2.8
Limestone17.421.6
Salt3.53.5
Vitamin pre-mix 22.52.5
Trace Mineral pre-mix 31.51.5
Sow Vitamin pre-mix 42.52.5
L-Lysine HCl2.12.0
Total1000.01000.0
Calculated Analysis
Crude Protein, %17.422.4
Metabolizable Energy, kcal/kg33303300
Crude Fiber, %4.185.10
Ether Extract, %6.236.14
Lysine, %0.701.10
TSAA, %0.680.86
Threonine, %0.640.92
Tryptophan, %0.150.25
Ca, %0.790.90
Total P, %0.520.57
Available P, %0.290.40
1 Basal diet was supplemented with 75 mg/kg BW/day DHA as DHASCO or an equivalent amount of safflower oil daily in the DHA group and CON group, respectively. 2 Supplied per kg of premix: vitamin A 4400 IU; vitamin D 660,000 IU; vitamin E 17,600 IU; vitamin K 1760 IU; riboflavin 3960 mg; niacin 22,000 mg; vitamin B12 17,600 μg. 3 Supplied per kg of premix: iron 110,000 mg; copper 11,000 mg; manganese 26,400 mg; zinc 110,000 mg; iodine 198 mg; selenium 198 mg. 4 Supplied per kg of premix: biotin, 88 mg; choline, 220.5 g; folic acid, 661.5 mg; pyridoxine, 1.98g; vitamin E8,882 IU.
Table 2. Colostrum fatty acid composition from sows fed with/without DHA during late gestation and lactation.
Table 2. Colostrum fatty acid composition from sows fed with/without DHA during late gestation and lactation.
CompoundControl (n = 3)DHA (n = 5)p-Value
lipid3.90 ± 0.903.83 ± 0.550.94
C12:00.04 ± 0.000.09 ± 0.000.01
C14:01.23 ± 0.092.29 ± 0.170.004
C14:10.02 ± 0.000.07 ± 0.010.01
C15:00.11 ± 0.010.11 ± 0.010.69
C16:018.31 ± 0.6919.50 ± 0.500.20
C16:13.25 ± 0.074.43 ± 0.340.04
C17:00.30 ± 0.010.24 ± 0.010.01
C17:10.23 ± 0.010.24 ± 0.020.90
C18:04.70 ± 0.524.30 ± 0.320.51
C18:129.80 ± 0.8132.18 ± 1.420.19
C18:235.98 ± 2.0327.55 ± 1.720.02
C18:3n-60.64 ± 0.080.39 ± 0.050.03
C18:3n-30.83 ± 0.030.80 ± 0.070.78
C20:00.18 ± 0.070.14 ± 0.030.65
C20:10.34 ± 0.030.37 ± 0.020.45
C20:20.74 ± 0.040.66 ± 0.030.14
C20:30.38 ± 0.050.23 ± 0.010.01
C20:41.16 ± 0.080.46 ± 0.01< 0.0001
C20:50.04 ± 0.020.48 ± 0.060.001
C22:20.59 ± 0.350.64 ± 0.040.84
C22:50.17 ± 0.070.36 ± 0.040.04
C22:60.04 ± 0.013.36 ± 0.20< 0.0001
Relative percent of each fatty acid is shown. The data are presented as means ± S.E. Abbreviations: DHA: docosahexaenoic acid.
Table 3. Body weight and brain weight of piglets at weaning.
Table 3. Body weight and brain weight of piglets at weaning.
ParameterControl (n = 14)DHA (n = 20)p-Value
Birth weight (g)1539.00 ± 75.561389.00 ± 103.100.67
Final weight (g)6134.00 ± 353.605790.00 ± 436.400.73
Weight gain (g)4528.00 ± 338.004333.00 ± 308.700.78
Daily weight gain (g/day)229.30 ± 16.87219.80 ± 16.280.77
Brain weight (g)49.28 ± 0.6949.17 ± 0.970.77
The data are presented as means ± S.E. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: DHA: docosahexaenoic acid.
Table 4. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis of nineteen brain regions of piglets at weaning.
Table 4. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis of nineteen brain regions of piglets at weaning.
Brain RegionControl (n = 7)DHA (n = 7)p-Value
Caudate0.860 ± 0.0010.861 ± 0.0010.46
Cerebellum11.389 ± 0.00211.388 ± 0.0010.71
Left Cortex31.262 ± 0.00231.257 ± 0.0020.09
Right Cortex32.143 ± 0.00232.144 ± 0.0010.54
Lateral Ventricle1.012 ± 0.0021.014 ± 0.00010.95
Third Ventricle0.106 ± 0.00030.105 ± 0.00050.77
Cerebral Aqueduct0.074 ± 0.00030.074 ± 0.00020.63
Fourth Ventricle0.099 ± 0.00050.100 ± 0.00020.33
Left Hippocampus0.914 ± 0.00050.916 ± 0.00060.15
Right Hippocampus0.925 ± 0.0010.926 ± 0.0010.50
Medulla3.364 ± 0.0023.3620 ± 0.0010.54
Midbrain3.414 ± 0.0013.412 ± 0.0010.31
Pons2.154 ± 0.0012.155 ± 0.0010.44
Putamen and Globus Pallidus0.731 ± 0.0010.731 ± 0.00040.78
Hypothalamus0.501 ± 0.0010.502 ± 0.0010.13
Thalamus2.771 ± 0.0012.767 ± 0.0010.04
Olfactory Bulb4.632 ± 0.0014.634 ± 0.0020.48
Corpus Callosum0.776 ± 0.0020.775 ± 0.0010.86
Internal Capsule2.875 ± 0.0022.877 ± 0.0010.55
Total (voxel)411301 ± 11.94411342 ± 25.470.23
Percentage volume of different brain regions is shown. The data are presented as means ± S.E. A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects. Abbreviations: DHA: docosahexaenoic acid.
Table 5. Resting-state network included in the analysis and their regions of interest.
Table 5. Resting-state network included in the analysis and their regions of interest.
Resting-State NetworkLocations of Regions of Interest
Executive control network (ECN)Primary somatosensory cortex
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
Anterior prefrontal cortex
Orbitofrontal cortex
Insular cortex
Ventral anterior cingulate cortex
Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
Cerebellar network (CERE)Cerebellum
Visual network (VIS)Primary visual cortex
Secondary visual cortex
Associative visual cortex
Sensorimotor network (SMN)Primary motor cortex
Somatosensory association cortex
Premotor cortex
Auditory network (AUD)Superior temporal gyrus
Auditory cortex
Default mode network (DMN)Hippocampus
Anterior prefrontal cortex
Orbitofrontal cortex
Inferior temporal gyrus
Ventral posterior cingulate cortex
Retrosplenial cingular cortex
Dorsal posterior cingular cortex
Anterior entorhinal cortex
Parahippocampal cortex
Table 6. Concentrations of monoamines and their metabolites in the PFC and HC in piglets at weaning.
Table 6. Concentrations of monoamines and their metabolites in the PFC and HC in piglets at weaning.
Brain RegionsNeurochemicalsControl (n = 7)DHA (n = 7)p-Value
PFCDA3.463 ± 0.3903.551 ± 0.4540.85
DOPAC0.090 ± 0.0120.078 ± 0.0150.64
HVA0.121 ± 0.0150.092 ± 0.0090.61
5-HT0.174 ± 0.0220.124 ± 0.0300.08
5-HIAA0.067 ± 0.0100.050 ± 0.0100.25
NE0.168 ± 0.0220.163 ± 0.0390.89
HCDA2.459 ± 0.1813.269 ± 0.3710.14
HVA0.170 ± 0.0350.155 ± 0.0240.99
5-HT0.105 ± 0.0170.137 ± 0.0240.26
5-HIAA0.096 ± 0.0040.113 ± 0.0190.36
NE0.072 ± 0.0080.105 ± 0.0140.11
The data are presented as means ± S.E; unit: ng/mg protein. Abbreviations: DHA: docosahexaenoic acid; PFC: prefrontal cortex; HC: hippocampus; DA: dopamine; DOPAC: dihydroxyphenylacetic acid; HVA: homovanillic acid; 5-HT: serotonin; 5-HIAA: 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid; NE: norepinephrine; A linear mixed-effect model was used to control for gender (fixed) and maternal (random) effects.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fang, X.; Sun, W.; Jeon, J.; Azain, M.; Kinder, H.; Ahn, J.; Chung, H.C.; Mote, R.S.; Filipov, N.M.; Zhao, Q.; et al. Perinatal Docosahexaenoic Acid Supplementation Improves Cognition and Alters Brain Functional Organization in Piglets. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2090. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12072090

AMA Style

Fang X, Sun W, Jeon J, Azain M, Kinder H, Ahn J, Chung HC, Mote RS, Filipov NM, Zhao Q, et al. Perinatal Docosahexaenoic Acid Supplementation Improves Cognition and Alters Brain Functional Organization in Piglets. Nutrients. 2020; 12(7):2090. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12072090

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fang, Xi, Wenwu Sun, Julie Jeon, Michael Azain, Holly Kinder, Jeongyoun Ahn, Hee Cheol Chung, Ryan S. Mote, Nikolay M. Filipov, Qun Zhao, and et al. 2020. "Perinatal Docosahexaenoic Acid Supplementation Improves Cognition and Alters Brain Functional Organization in Piglets" Nutrients 12, no. 7: 2090. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12072090

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop