Next Article in Journal
Prenatal Nutrition Containing Bisphenol A Affects Placenta Glucose Transfer: Evidence in Rats and Human Trophoblast
Next Article in Special Issue
Discrimination of Isointense Bitter Stimuli in a Beer Model System
Previous Article in Journal
The Mediating Effects of Eating Disorder, Food Addiction, and Insomnia in the Association between Psychological Distress and Being Overweight among Iranian Adolescents
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Study on the Relationship between Type 2 Diabetes and Taste Function in Patients with Good Glycemic Control
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Gender Differences in Fat-Rich Meat Choice: Influence of Personality and Attitudes

1
Department of Agricultural, Food, Environment and Forestry (DAGRI), University of Florence, 50144 Florence, Italy
2
Alma Mater Studiorum, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
3
School of Agricultural, Forest, Food, and Environmental Sciences, University of Basilicata, Via dell’Ateneo Lucano 10, 85100 Potenza, Itay
4
Department of Food, Environmental and Nutritional Sciences (DeFENS), University of Milan, 20133 Milan, Italy
5
Sensory and Consumer Science, University of Gastronomic Sciences, Piazza Vittorio Emanuele II, 9, 12042 Pollenzo, Italy
6
Department of Agricultural Sciences, University of Naples Federico II, 80055, Portici, Italy
7
Center Agriculture Food Environment, University of Trento/Fondazione Edmund Mach, Via E. Mach 1, 38010 San Michele All’adige, Italy
8
Dipartimento di Agricoltura, Alimentazione e Ambiente (Di3A), University of Catania, Via Santa Sofia 100, 95123 Catania, Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2020, 12(5), 1374; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12051374
Submission received: 28 March 2020 / Revised: 29 April 2020 / Accepted: 3 May 2020 / Published: 11 May 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Taste Perception and Food Preferences)

Abstract

:
The innate liking of fats may be due to one or more orosensory, post-ingestive, and metabolic signals; however, individuals differ in their preference for fat in meat. One of the variables that mainly impacts eating behaviors and thus should be carefully analyzed is sex/gender, and while sex (female/male, in a binary approximation) refers only to biological characteristics, gender (woman/man, in a binary approximation) refers to cultural attitudes and behavior. This study aimed at exploring the role of gender, age, taste responsiveness (measured as sensitivity to the bitterness of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP)), personality traits, attitudes, and liking of and familiarity with meat on the choice of fat-rich meat products in 1208 women and men aged 18–66. Both a between- and a within-gender approach were adopted. Results showed that gender had a major impact on liking of and familiarity with meat and choice for fat-rich meat compared to age. A lower liking meat in general was found in women, independently of fat content. Women also reported a lower familiarity than men with fatty meat and cold meat and a lower choice of fat-rich meat. Genders differed in the influence of personality and attitudes about fat-rich meat choice. In both genders, the choice of meat higher in fat was associated with liking cold and fatty meat and with age and negatively with liking low-fat meat. Women were in general more interested in health than men, and this may explain the main difference in the choice of fat-rich meat between genders. However, when we look at each gender separately, general health interest was significantly correlated with a lower choice of fat-rich meat only in men. In addition, in men food neophobia was negatively correlated with choice of fat-rich meat. In women, the emotional dimension was found to play an important role, with sensitivity to disgust that was negatively associated with fat-rich meat choice and emotional eating that was positively associated with it. Thanks to the large sample and the gender-sensitive approach adopted, this study showed that different factors affect choice of fat-rich meat by gender, in addition to liking of and familiarity with fat-rich and cold meat and age. This suggests that strategies personalized by gender to reinforce or activate barriers to this type of consumption may be more effective at reducing fat intake, promoting the consumption of meat lower in fat.

1. Introduction

It is well known that overconsumption of fat has negative health impacts [1,2] and increases the risk of incidence of diseases, such as obesity [3], diabetes [4] coronary heart disease [5], and cancer [6,7,8]. The innate liking of fats may be due to one or more of the orosensory, post-ingestive, and metabolic signals; however, it is evidenced that individuals differ in their preference for fat in meat [9,10]. The fat content of meat from most species has been mainly associated with texture, comprising tenderness and juiciness, and flavor [11].
Sex is a biological quality or classification of sexually-reproducing organisms, generally female, male, and/or intersex, according to functions that derive from the chromosomal complement, reproductive organs, or specific hormones or environmental factors that affect the expression of phenotypic traits that are strongly associated with females or males within a given species, while gender is a socio-cultural process that refers to cultural and social attitudes that together shape and sanction “feminine” and “masculine” behaviors, products, technologies, environments, and knowledges (http://genderedinnovations.stanford.edu/terms/distinct.html). Gender has been seen as a proxy for other measures, as it often “defines differences in perceived expectancies, environments, opportunities, income level, interaction with children, and experience in food selection and preparation, and many other variables in addition to genetic, hormonal and anatomical difference” [12]. On this premise, in the present study, which takes into account factors affecting liking of, familiarity with and choice of fat-rich meat, gender was considered instead of sex. Furthermore, a gender-sensitive approach was adopted with the aim of reporting similarities and difference between the genders, but also within each gender (namely, between women and men, respectively).
Gender has the largest impact on sensory response and food preferences [13] and was found to affect liking and consumption of meat and fat products, with women liking meat [14,15,16] and high-fat products [17] less than men. Furthermore, women reported more avoidance of fats from meat than men did [10,18,19], and a study across 22 countries showed that gender had the most consistent influence on fat preference in pork meat among other factors, with a greater proportion of women than men preferring pork with less fat cover [20]. Women tend to be more concerned about animal welfare compared to men [21,22,23], and this might explain gender differences in liking of meat [24]. Other factors play a role in preference for and consumption of fatty foods, ranging from sensory sensitivity to psychological traits and motives. Texture, odor, and flavor contribute to the liking of fatty foods [25]. Fatty acid taste sensitivity, defined as hypersensitivity to the taste of oleic acid, was negatively associated with greater consumption of fatty foods, specifically butter, meat, dairy, and with increasing BMI [26]. The positive relationship between 6-n-propyl-2-thiouracile (PROP) responsiveness and perception of fat was also suggested, but the results are controversial, as discussed by Tepper et al. [27]. Most studies [28,29,30,31,32,33] reported that PROP non-tasters had a lower ability to distinguish fat content and creaminess in certain fatty food, compared to those who taste PROP as more bitter. On the other hand, other studies reported that PROP responsiveness and both sensory response and preference for fat were unrelated [34,35] Furthermore, sensitivity to the taste of PROP and fatty acid taste sensitivity were found to be unrelated [26].
Greater fat preference was found to be inversely related to restrained eating and thus more an aspect of eating behavior than of personality [36]. Other studies with female students [37] and patients with diagnosed type 2 diabetes [38] confirmed that fat intake was negatively associated with restrained eating, while external eating was positively associated, and emotional eating was not significantly associated with it. On the other hand, reward sensitivity was found to be associated with fat and sweet food intake and alcohol consumption [39] and with fat and sweet food liking [40], suggesting that reward sensitivity may lead to preferential intake of foods rich in calories.
The literature on the relationship between personality and preference and choice of meat is quite limited: high levels of openness to experience (which characterizes curious, imaginative and openminded people who like new ideas) were associated with lower meat consumption in a large study in Germany [41]. In addition, studies supported that this relationship depends on meat types: openness was negatively associated with red meat consumption, whereas it was unrelated to poultry consumption and overall meat consumption; by contrast, extraversion was associated with higher consumption of each individual type of meat and more overall meat consumption [42].
These findings warrant further investigations to overcome some limitations of the previous studies, such as the small sample size and the focus on overweight and obese individuals. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, the role of gender has been under-investigated. It is unclear how different factors such as personality traits, attitudes, and taste responsiveness affect liking and choice of meat rich in fat in men and women. We might in fact hypothesize that a variable is very significant for a gender, but not for the other one, while some variables are relevant for both genders. This may be due to the fact that genders differ in personality traits and taste responsiveness. Analyses of similarities and differences both between and across genders have been encouraged [43]. Within-gender approaches, namely approaches in which each gender is investigated separately, have been proven to be very effective [44,45], in addition to between-gender studies.
The aim of this study was, therefore, to investigate, in a large sample, the factors affecting choice for fat-rich meats, including socio-demographics, taste responsiveness, personality traits, attitudes, liking of and familiarity with fat-rich meats, adopting a gender-sensitive approach. In addition, the study aimed at testing if PROP responsiveness significantly affected liking of and familiarity with fat-rich meats and if gender moderated this relationship.
Differences and similarities between genders were investigated through both a between-gender and a within-gender analysis. The former was used to study the effect of gender, while the latter was used to investigate each gender separately in order to highlight the importance that each variable had for each gender, pointing out the differences within women and men, respectively.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The subjects (n = 1208) who participated in the study were 58.36% women (n = 705), with a mean age of 35.5 (SD 12.9). The characteristics of the participants are reported in Table 1. Data were collected in 8 cities in different geographical areas of Italy (north: Trento, Milan, Pollenzo (Cuneo); central: Bologna and Florence; south: Napoli, Potenza, and Catania). Exclusion criteria were pregnancy, breastfeeding, not being born in Italy or having lived less than 20 years in Italy, and age not included in the range 18–66 years old.
Participants were recruited by means of announcements, social networks, participant universities and research centers’ websites, and national newspapers and magazines.
All testing involving human subjects was in compliance with the principles laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all subjects provided informed, written consent prior to participation in agreement with the Italian ethical requirements on research activities and personal data protection (Law Decree 30.6.03 n. 196). The protocol of the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Florence and Trieste University.

2.2. Overview of Data Collection

Participants were asked to fill in an online questionnaire before attending two sessions at the laboratory. Socio-demographic information (self-reported gender, age, education) and familiarity with foods were collected before the test sessions. In the lab sessions, participants were asked to fill in a set of questionnaires to measure personality and psychological traits and attitudes towards food, liking, and choice. The study included sensory tests, questionnaires, and the collection of other data (see Monteleone et al. [46] for a complete overview of data collection), but only a selection of variables will be presented here (Figure 1).

2.3. PROP Responsiveness

A 3.2 mM solution of 6-n-propyl-2-thiouracile (European Pharmacopoeia Reference Standard, Sigma Aldrich, Milano, IT) was prepared by dissolving 0.5447 g/L in deionized water [47]. To ensure consistency, respondents evaluated the bitter intensity of two identical samples (10 mL), presented monadically in white plastic cups and coded with two different three-digit codes [48]. Subjects were instructed to hold each sample into their mouth for 10 s, then, after expectorating, wait 20 more seconds, and subsequently, evaluate the bitterness using the Generalized Labeled Magnitude Scale (gLMS scale [49] ). In order to control the carry-over effect after the first evaluation, a break of 90 s was established between the two evaluations; during the break, respondents rinsed their mouth with water (30 s), had a cracker (30 s), then rinsed their mouth again. Mean scores between the two replicates were calculated, and participants were then grouped according to their PROP status on the basis of cut-offs reported in previous studies [46,50,51]. Respondents were classified as non-taster (NT; gLMS ≤ moderate, 17), supertaster (ST; gLMS ≥ very strong, 53), and medium taster (MT; gLMS >17 and <53).
Before the evaluation, participants were instructed on the use of the gLMS scale. Instructions were given that the top of the scale represented the most intense sensation that subjects could ever imagine experiencing. A variety of sensations from different modalities, including loudness and oral pain/irritation, were recalled to provide examples [52]. To practice the use of the scale, subjects rated intensities of the brightest light they had ever seen following the procedure described in Dinnella et al. [53]. The task was performed individually, and the criterion to conclude that the subjects correctly used the scale was that ratings must have been higher than “very strong”, but lower than “the strongest imaginable”. In the case of ratings out of this range, a short individual interview was carried out to understand the reason for the ratings, and the use of the scale was explained again. In a limited number of cases when subjects were unable to use the scale properly even after the second explanation, their evaluations were discarded from the analysis.

2.4. Psychological and Personality Traits

2.4.1. Toronto Alexithymia Scale

Alexithymia is a construct characterized by the difficulty identifying subjective emotional feelings and distinguishing between feelings and the bodily sensations of emotional arousal, difficulty describing feelings to other people, an impoverished fantasy life, and a stimulus-bound, externally oriented cognitive style. The trait was measured using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) questionnaire, structured into 3 domains: DIF, difficulty identifying feeling; DDF, difficulty describing feeling; EOT, externally oriented thinking. The questionnaire included a total of 20 items evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = disagree strongly, 7 = agree strongly) [54], validated in Italian by Bressi et al. [55]. The individual score for each domain corresponded to the sum of ratings (ranging from 20 to 100), with higher scores indicating a greater level of alexithymia reflected in the lower capabilities of identifying feelings (DIF, range 7–35), describing feelings (DDF, range 5–25), and externally-oriented thinking (EOT, range 8–40).

2.4.2. Private Body Consciousness

The disposition to focus on internal bodily sensations (awareness of internal sensations) was quantified using the 5 item Private Body Consciousness (PBC) questionnaire [56], validated in Italian by Spinelli et al. [45] on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = extremely uncharacteristic to 5 = extremely characteristic. The final individual score for PBC was the sum of the scores (ranging from 5 to 25).

2.4.3. Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward

This Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) was used for the responsiveness of the two brain systems that control the behavioral activation system (BAS) and the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) [57], validated in Italian by Spinelli et al. [45]. The questionnaire included two scales: the sensitivity to punishment scale (SP) was built with items that reflected situations related to individual differences in reaction and responsiveness to BIS, while the sensitivity to reward scale (SR) was represented by items that measured the BAS functionality dealing with specific rewards (i.e., money, praising, social power). Each scale was rated with a yes/no format, and the total score for each subject was represented by the sum of “yes” answers. Based on Spinelli et al. [45], we removed 7 items from the Italian version (4, 8, 16, 25, 32, 24, 26); thus, the scores ranged from 0 to 23 for SP and from 0 to 18 in SR, with higher scores reflecting, respectively, higher sensitivity to punishment and to reward.

2.4.4. Food Neophobia Scale

The evaluation of food neophobia was quantified by the scale developed by [58], validated in Italian by Laureati et al. [59] This psychological trait describes the reluctance to eat and try new and unfamiliar products. It was investigated by means of 10 items, represented by 10 statements evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale (range: 1 = disagree strongly; 7 = agree strongly). The final score for the Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) was represented by the sum of ratings (ranging from 10 to 70).

2.4.5. Sensitivity to Disgust

The individual sensitivity to core disgust was evaluated with the DS-SF questionnaire; this questionnaire is a short form of the Disgust Scale [60,61,62,63] validated in Italian by Spinelli et al [45], represented by 8 items divided into 2 subscales. Specifically, a 5-point scale was used, and in the case of Subscale 1, scores rated from 1 (strongly disagree-very untrue about me) to 5 (strongly agree-very true about me); rates for Subscales 2 ranged from 1 (not at all disgusting) to 5 (extremely disgusting). The total score for sensitivity to disgust was given by the sum of scores (ranging from 8 to 40).

2.5. Eating Behaviors, Food-Related Lifestyles, and Attitude Measurements

2.5.1. Food-Related Lifestyle

The Food-Related Lifestyles (FRL) questionnaire [64], validated in Italian by Saba et al. [65], was used to measure the lifestyle, defined as the system of cognitive categories, scripts, and their associations, which associate a set of products to a set of values, related to food. The FRL is organized into 5 domains (and their relative 23 subscales) including ways of shopping (6 subscales), importance of quality aspects (6 subscales), cooking methods (6 subscales), consumption situations (2 subscales), and purchasing motives (3 subscales). The 69 items (7 of them required reverse scoring) were evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = disagree strongly; 7 = agree strongly). The total score for each subscale was calculated as the mean of scores given by respondents.

2.5.2. Health and Taste Attitudes Scale

The individual orientation towards the hedonic and health characteristics of food was measured with the Health and Taste Attitudes Scale (HTAS) questionnaire [66] validated in Italian by Saba et al. [65], organized into 6 different domains: three domains were health-related (the general health interest (GHI), i.e., interest in eating healthily; light product interest (LPI), i.e., interest in eating reduced-fat foods; and natural product interest (NPI), i.e., the interest in eating foods that do not contain additives and are unprocessed), and three domains were taste-related (craving for sweet foods (CSF), food as a reward (FR), and pleasure (P)}). The HTAS included a total of 38 items, rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = disagree strongly; 7 = agree strongly). The HTAS for each domain was calculated as the mean of the ratings.

2.5.3. Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire

The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) was used to measure individual differences in emotional eating (eating in response to internal emotional factors), external (eating in response to external factors such as the sight and smell of food), and restrained eating (eating less than desired to lose or maintain a particular body weight) [67], validated in Italian by Dakanalis et al. [68]. DEBQ consisted of 33 items rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never; 2 = seldom; 3 = sometimes; 4 = often, and 5 = very often). The total score for each domain was calculated as the mean.

2.6. Stated Liking, Familiarity with, and Choice of Meat-Based Products

2.6.1. Familiarity with and Stated Liking of Meat-Based Products

Familiarity with and liking of meat-based fat-rich products were measured using a selection of the IT-Food Familiarity Questionnaire (IT-FFQ) and of the IT-Food Preference Questionnaire (IT-FPQ; [46], developed within the Italian Taste project to collect information about familiarity with and liking of foods among Italians. The IT-FFQ and IT-FPQ included 15 items referred to meat, part of a larger group of 184 items. IT-FFQ was assessed using a 5-point labelled scale (1 = I do not recognize it; 2 = I recognize it, but I have never tasted it; 3 = I have tasted it, but I do not eat it; 4 = I occasionally eat it; 5 = I regularly eat it; [69]). Stated liking of meat-based fat-rich products was assessed using a 9-point hedonic labelled scale [70] with the addition of the option “never tasted it”. The presentation order of the items in both questionnaires was randomized across participants.
Meat products were classified as fat and low-fat based on macro-composition. Fat products were further divided into meat and cold meat based on processing (raw and cured). Three groups were then obtained: fat-rich meat, fatty cold meat, and low-fat meat items (Table 2). Indices of familiarity and liking were calculated for each of the three as the sum (for familiarity) and the mean (for liking) of the individual scores of each subject.

2.6.2. Choice of Fat-Rich/Not Fat-Rich Meat-Based Foods and Validation of the Oppositions

Choice of fat-rich/not fat-rich meat-based foods was quantified using a selection of items taken from the IT-Food Choice Questionnaire [46], a tool developed in order to evaluate preference within a pair of foods selected among the ones included in the IT-FFQ/FPQ. For each pair, respondents were asked to indicate which meat they would choose in a main meal (either lunch or dinner). The presentation order of the food items within each pair and of the pairs was randomized across participants. A choice index for fat-rich meat (FCI) was calculated as the sum of the choices of the fattier and more caloric option assigning to each a value of 1, with higher scores reflecting a greater choice of the fattier options.
The options were selected based on a preliminary study conducted at the University of Florence with 181 subjects (mean age 40.5, range 18–68; 74.86% women) using a check-all-that-apply methodology to evaluate sensory properties (only results about “fatty” and “caloric” will be presented here) though an online questionnaire. Products were presented monadically in a randomized order. Attributes were presented in a randomized order.

2.7. Data Analysis

The Cochran’s Q test was applied to check for significant differences among products in the preliminary study for the choice questionnaire validation.
Cronbach’s α was calculated for all questionnaires. The effect of gender, age, and gender * age was calculated on all the variables (PROP status, psychological traits, attitudes, stated liking of and familiarity with fatty meat, cold meat, and low-fat) using a two-way ANOVA. The Chi-squared test was applied to check the difference in the distribution of PROP status by gender.
A three-way ANOVA model was applied to test the effect of PROP, age, gender and their intercation on liking of and familiarity with fatty meat, cold meat, and low-fat meat.
Indices of liking, familiarity with, and choice of fat-rich meat were calculated, respectively, as the mean (for liking) and the sum (for familiarity and choice) of the ratings.
Individuals higher and lower in fat-rich meat choice were identified, respectively, as those higher (high FCI) and lower (low FCI) than the median of the choice index. Subjects whose values corresponded to the median were not considered. One-way ANOVA models were applied to test the effect of choice for fat-rich meat (high/low) on each variable considered in the study by gender. The significance level was set at 0.05, but results <0.1 are discussed, for the exploratory purposes of the study.
A partial least squares discriminant analysis (PLS-DA) regression model was computed on the whole dataset and separately for each gender assuming a low and high fat choice index as the response variable (Y) and 39 explanatory variables (X): age; seven personality traits (FN, SR, SP, DS, and TAS subscales, see Table 3); PROP responsiveness; attitudes (selected subscales reported in Table 4); familiarity with and liking of fat-rich meat, cold meat, and low-fat meat.
All PLS regression models were run on standardized mean centered input variables, using cross-validation on 5 (for men) and 6 (for women) random segments and performing a jack-knife uncertainty test with a 95% confidence interval for the detection of significant variables [71] (Due to the large amount of information collected, a two-step procedure was used [72]. In the first step, all the individual attributes were included in the model. Then, in further steps, a new model was run only including as active variables those that were found to be significant in the model according to the uncertainty test. The other variables were included in the model as downweighted. This resulted in a better suited and more parsimonious model.
All data were analyzed using XLSTAT 19.4.1, with the exception of the PLS-DA, which was conducted using Unscrambler®X 10.3.

3. Results

3.1. PROP Responsiveness by Gender and Age

Men and women significantly differed in PROP responsiveness (Χ2 = 32.48, p < 0.0001). The distribution of men into NT, MT, and ST reflected quite well the expected distribution of 25%, 50%, and 25% respectively, while in women, a higher number of supertasters was found (41.28%). PROP responsiveness decreased with age (F = 13.60, p < 0.0001), with an interaction with gender close to significance (p = 0.052).

3.2. Psychological and Personality Traits by Gender and Age

The internal reliability of all the questionnaires measuring psychological traits was satisfactory (Table 3). An interaction between gender and age was found in the case of private body consciousness (F = 7.85; p = 0.0004) and sensitivity to disgust (F = 3.17; p = 0.042), with women higher in these traits than men independent of age, with the exception of individuals aged 18 to 30, for which the two genders did not differ in PBC. Women were found also to have more difficulty in identifying the feeling dimension (alexithymia-DIF) and less difficulty in externally oriented thinking (alexithymia-EOT), a higher private body consciousness, a higher sensitivity to punishment, and a lower sensitivity to reward than men.
Alexithymia and its subscales DIF, DDF, and EOT, and sensitivity to punishment and to reward were higher in the younger group. Inversely, neophobia and sensitivity to disgust increased with age.

3.3. Eating Behaviors, Food-Related Lifestyles, and Attitude Towards Health and Taste by Gender and Age

Nine (out of the twenty-three) subscales of the food-related lifestyle scale (APA2—price/quality relation, APA3—novelty, APA5—taste, CS4—whole family, CS5—planning, US1—snacks versus meal, US2—social event, CO2—security, CO3—social relationship) and the pleasure domain of the Health and Taste Attitude Scale showed a low internal validity (α < 0.6) and were removed from the analysis. Age and gender effects were found on some subscales, without a significant interaction between these two variables with only one exception (Table 4): the belief that cooking was a woman’s task (CS6) was higher in women and in men aged 45–60. Women found the price criteria in food purchasing (SC5) and the use of a shopping list (SC6) more important than men. Women were more interested in looking for new ways of cooking (CS2) and declared more self-fulfillment with food (CO1), while men declared using more convenience foods than women (CS3). No difference was found between genders in the importance of product information (SC1), attitudes toward advertising (SC2), enjoyment from food shopping (SC3), interest in specialty shops (SC4), interest in organic products (APA4), importance of product freshness (APA6), and interest in cooking (CS1).
Enjoyment from food shopping (SC3), the importance of the price criteria (SC5), and interest in cooking decreased with age (CS1), while the interest in specialty shops (SC4) and organic products (APA4) increased with age. The consumption of convenience foods was lower among the 30–35-year-olds, but with older age, this tended to increase (CS3).
Health interest was higher for women than men; this was confirmed both by the FRL Health subscale (APA1) and by the HTAS (GHI). Health interest also increased with age. Women were also more interested in the naturalness of the products (namely, absence of additives), and they had a greater attitude for craving sweet foods. Interest in light products was higher among the 18–30 age class, while the interest in natural products increased with age. The younger used more food as a reward than older individuals.
Women had a greater attitude toward emotional eating with no age effect. External eating did not differ by gender and decreased with age. A significant interaction between gender and age was found for restrained eating, which was higher for women and increased with age only in men (F = 5.06; p = 0.006).

3.4. Liking of, Familiarity with, and Choice of Meat-Based Products

3.4.1. Stated Liking of and Familiarity with Meat-Based Products

The percentage of individuals who had never tasted meat products was generally low, and lower in men than women. Lamb and carpaccio were the two products with the highest percentage of individuals who declared to have never tasted the product; 6.67% of women and 3.58% of men in the case of lamb; 8.51% of women and 4.77% of men for carpaccio. Only 3.31% of the participants declared a vegetarian diet (3.83% among women and 2.58% among men).
In general, all the products were liked with average ratings ranging from 6.70 to 7.88 for men and 5.43 to 7.78 for women. Men stated a liking higher than women for lamb, pork, beef rib, steak, carpaccio, grilled beef cutlet, and pork sausage, while no difference between genders was found for breaded cutlet and chicken breast. A similar trend was found also in fatty cold meat products with men reporting a higher liking of mortadella, spicy salami, and salami. No gender difference was found for ham (cooked or dry-cured) and bacon.
Indices of familiarity with and liking of meat were calculated. Internal reliability, gender, and age effects are reported in Table 5. Internal reliability was acceptable, with Cronbach’s α ranging from 0.60 to 0.84. Gender affected the liking of and familiarity with fatty meat and fatty cold meat indices and the liking of low-fat meat index, with women reporting a lower liking and familiarity index than men. Liking of meat independently of fat and familiarity with fatty cold meat and low-fat meat decreased with age. The familiarity with fatty meat index remained stable with age. No interaction between gender and age was observed.

3.4.2. Stated Liking of and Familiarity with Meat-Based Products by PROP Status

PROP status was found to interact with gender with a significant effect on liking of fatty meat (F = 3.36, p = 0.035), liking of cold meat (F = 3.09, p = 0.046), liking of low-fat meat (F = 3.32, p = 0.036), and familiarity with low-fat meat (F = 3.17, p = 0.042). Post hoc tests showed that men declared a higher liking of fat and cold meat compared to women and that medium and non-taster men expressed a higher liking compared to medium and non-taster women, respectively. No gender difference in liking fat and cold meat was found in supertasters. Men and women did not differ in liking of low-fat meat, with the exception of non-tasters: female non-tasters declared a lower liking than male non-tasters (Figure 2). A further interaction between age, gender, and PROP status was found on liking of fatty meat (F = 3.35, p = 0.010): results showed that liking for fatty meat was lower in women aged 30–45 non-tasters and in men aged 45–60 supertasters. A post hoc test did not indicate significant difference in the case of familiarity with low-fat meat.

3.4.3. Fat-Rich Meat Choice Index

Preliminary Study: Validation of the Fat-Rich Meat Choice Index: Cochran’s Q test applied on each attribute showed a significant difference between the two options of each choice pair both in fatty and in caloric (Table 6).
Correlation analysis showed that the choice in the pair 2 (grilled vs. breaded cutlet) and 6 (carpaccio vs. sliced steak) was not significantly correlated with the choice in other pairs, and factor loading was lower than 0.3 for these pairs. Internal reliability improved upon removing these items with Cronbach’s α passing from 0.58 to 0.68; thus, only five pairs (bold in Table 7) were retained to calculate the fat-rich meat choice index.
Factors Affecting Fat-Rich Meat Choice by Gender: An effect on the fact-rich meat choice index of gender (F = 62.11, p < 0.0001) and age (F = 6.86, p = 0.001) not further characterized by interaction was found. The fat-rich meat choice index was lower in women and in individuals aged 18–30. Given the higher variability in choice by gender, analyses were conducted separated in women and men.
Individuals low and high in the fatty meat choice index differed mainly in familiarity with and liking of fat-rich meat and fatty cold meat, both in men and women (Table 7). In addition, individuals low in fat choice declared a higher general health interest, a lower susceptibility to external eating, higher neophobia, and tended to be younger in both genders. Some variables affected choice only within one gender: women reporting a lower fat-rich meat choice index resulted in being more sensitive to disgust and to punishment, less sensitive to emotional eating, less self-fulfilled in food, and enjoyed food shopping less. On the contrary, men who reported a lower fat-rich meat choice index gave a higher importance to product information and to organic products, declared a higher light product interest, and resulted in being less prone to use food as a reward.
The partial least squares discriminant analysis regression on the whole sample showed that gender was the most relevant variable impacting choice of meat low and high in fat. Gender was found to affect significantly the choice of fat-rich meat, with men choosing systematically the fattiest option compared to women. For this reason, we proceeded with an analysis separated by gender.
In both models, the cross-validation indicated that three factors had a significant prediction ability and were used in the jack-knife test for estimating the uncertainty of the model parameters.
The explained variance for the first three components was 34%, 16%, and 5% for X and 18%, 7%, and 2% for Y in men (Figure 3) and 27%, 11%, and 6% for X and 16%, 12%, and 2% for Y in women (Figure 4).
In men (n = 393), on the first component, the liking of cold meat was positively associated with the choice of fatty meat, while general health interest and neophobia were negatively correlated with it. On the second and third component, also age and the liking of fatty meat were positively associated with fatty meat choice, while liking of low-fat meat was negatively associated with it (Figure 3).
In women (n = 522), on the first component, liking fat-rich meat and cold meat and familiarity with fat-rich meat, enjoyment from food shopping (SC3), and emotional eating were positively associated with a higher fat choice index, while sensitivity to disgust was negatively associated with it. On the second component, liking low-fat meat was negatively associated with the fat choice index, while age was positively associated with it (Figure 4).

4. Discussion

The gender-sensitive approach adopted in this study allowed analyzing differences and similarities between and across women and men. First, through a between-gender approach, the effect of gender was investigated together with age, both on the variables characterizing the subjects (personality traits, attitudes, and taste responsiveness) and on liking of, familiarity with, and choice of meat varying in fat. Secondly, a within-gender approach was adopted. This allowed pointing out similarities and differences in women and men, respectively, that remain often underestimated as they are covered by the larger differences between genders. The advantage of this latter approach is that it provides information that can be used to develop gender-specific interventions to promote healthier food behaviors.
Gender was found to have a major impact on liking of and familiarity with meat and choice of fat-rich meat. A lower liking of meat in general was found in women, independent of fat content. Women also reported a lower familiarity than men with fatty meat and cold meat. These results were in line with previous studies that reported a lower intake of meat in women [15,73]. In addition, this study showed that this was true independent of fat content. Looking at behaviors, even if women expressed a lower liking also for low-fat meat compared to men, in terms of consumption, they did not differ from men, with a low-fat meat familiarity index that was low in both genders. This suggested that men, even if they liked all types of meat, tended to consume less meat lower in fat, such as chicken breast. In the choice between a meat option lower or higher in fat, women systematically chose the option that was lower in fat.
Age was differently associated with liking of, familiarity with, and choice of fat-rich meat. In fact, while younger individuals declared a higher liking of meat higher in fat, when they had to choose between two options, higher and lower in fat, they preferred the option lower in fat. These results suggested that in older individuals, there was a higher coherence between liking, consumption, and choice, while in younger individuals, the conflict between liking and choice was greater. In general, younger individuals liked all meat more independently of its fat content and were more familiar with cold and low-fat meat, but when they could freely choose, they chose more consistently the option lower in fat. Older individuals reported a lower liking of all meat independently of fat and did not differ from the younger in familiarity with fat-rich meat; however, when they could freely choose, they systematically chose the option richer in fat. This meant that for these subjects, consuming the option lower in fat was more difficult, notwithstanding that for them, adopting healthier food behaviors was even more relevant and recommended.
Univariate analysis showed that many, but not all the variables that were found to affect choice of meat higher in fat significantly played a role in both genders. Liking of and familiarity with meat and cold meat higher in fat were positively associated with choice of meat higher in fat, as could be expected, in both genders. Health interest was found to be higher in women, in line with previous findings [73], and was associated in both genders with a lower choice of fat-rich meat. Neophobia was negatively associated with fat-rich meat choice both in men and women. Several studies reported neophobics to have a lower diet variety, lower liking [74], and lower intake [75,76] of meat and fish. Furthermore, fattier meat such as lamb may be less familiar, namely more novel, and thus less appealing for neophobics. We may also hypothesize that this result is related to an association of fat with more flavorful options, while neophobics usually prefer milder tastes [59].
Different from previous studies, we did not find any association of restrained eating with fat-rich meat choice, while we found in both men and women a positive association of external eating, defined as eating in response to external food-related cues, such as the sight and smell of food, regardless of physical need, in line with [38]. The lack of a relationship with restrained eating may be explained by the fact that in our study, the range of age was wider compared to [37], and individuals were not selected for specific health conditions; in addition in the choice task, individuals were encouraged to freely choose as they were in a condition without restriction (“if you are on a diet, please answer as if you are not on a diet”).
In women, we also found a positive association with emotional eating, which suggested that choice of meat rich in fat was higher in those women that eat more in response to negative emotions. This may be explained by the nutritional and rewarding value of fat that helps to cope with stress [25,77,78].
Sensitivity to disgust and sensitivity to punishment as a tendency were associated negatively, only in women, with higher fat-rich meat choice. We may hypothesize that the impact of sensitivity to disgust was related to visceral aspects of how the product looks: it is common that meat, especially when fat is visible, is perceived as more disgusting, particularly by those individuals more sensitive to this. Sensitivity to punishment, compared to sensitivity to disgust, seemed to be more related to the value that was associated with the food more than with how the food looked like. This trait refers, in fact, to the degree to which an individual’s behavior is inhibited by punishment-relevant stimuli and was found to be associated with food avoidance [79]. We may hypothesize that choice for higher fatty meats was associated with punishment, as an unhealthier behavior. Individuals more sensitive to punishment might choose more systematically the lower fat option to avoid punishment, as a sort of protective and prudent behavior.
We did not find an association between sensitivity to reward and higher fat-rich meat choice, while we found an association with the attitude to use food as a reward only in men. Sensitivity to reward refers to the degree to which an individual’s behavior is motivated by reward-relevant stimuli. Previous studies [39,40] that highlighted the association between sensitivity to reward and fat liking focused mainly on fat in sweet foods. Our results showed that further studies are required to extend these considerations to fat in salty foods, and particularly in meat.
Although current results about PROP responsiveness and fat perception are controversial [27], several studies found that PROP status was negatively associated with preference for dietary fat in women and girls [80,81] or independently of the gender [29,30,82,83]. We did not observe any difference according to PROP status in liking of, familiarity with, or choice of fat-rich meat, neither in women nor in men. When we found an effect, on liking, gender moderated this relationship. An interaction between PROP responsiveness and personality has been reported [84,85]. This may suggest that PROP status is interlinked with personality traits, thus moderating its effect. Another possible explanation for this result is the specific food product considered, meat, which is more liked by men. In this food matrix, fat content could be less relevant compared to the specificity of the food matrix (meat).
In men, a lower choice of fat-rich meat was associated also with a higher attitude toward light products, organic food, and importance assigned to product information. This suggested that cognitive motivations, that is knowledge about product nutritional and health characteristics, were very relevant for this gender in choosing the option lower in fat, compared to women.
The multivariate models separated by gender allowed pointing out the main factors that acted as barriers or facilitators of the consumption of fat-rich meat. In both genders, liking of and familiarity with meat confirmed their relevant role in choice. Liking of fat-rich meat and cold meat was positively associated with higher fat-rich meat choice, while familiarity with low-fat meat was negatively associated with it. Apart from liking of and familiarity with meat, women and men differed substantially in the main factors that affected fat-rich meat choice. Women were in general more interested in health than men, and this may explain the main difference in choice of fat-rich meat between genders. However, when we look at each gender separately, general health interest was significantly correlated with a lower choice of fat-rich meat only in men. This difference may be explained by the fact that women shared their interest in health, and thus, this factor did not play a significant role in choosing the option lower in fat. In men, factors that discouraged the choice of fat-rich meat seemed mainly represented by a higher general health interest and food neophobia. This meant that for men, cognitive motivations were very relevant as a deterrent for choosing the fat-rich option, both in terms of importance attributed to the information available to the individual (e.g., in terms of health benefits) and in terms of past experience, which they referred to when trying to make sense of information presently available and in determining how to respond or relate to the current situation. This latter aspect is very relevant to neophobia, which is defined as reluctance to eat unfamiliar foods.
On the contrary, in women, greater fat-rich meat choice was found to be inversely related to sensitivity to disgust and positively to emotional eating and enjoyment of food shopping and, thus, more an aspect of emotions than of attitudes towards health.
The large sample of individuals that participated in this study warranted the reliability of the results. However, the study was conducted on self-reported preferences on food items presented using words. The extension of the approach to tasted food differing in fat content could be helpful to confirm these results.

5. Conclusions

The study showed that women like meat less than men and chose the option lower in fat more compared to men. In addition, thanks to the large sample and the gender-sensitive approach adopted, this study showed that different factors affected the choice of fat-rich meat by gender, in addition to liking of and familiarity with fat-rich and cold meat and age. These findings have important health implications. They suggest that strategies personalized by gender to reinforce or activate barriers to the choice of fat-rich meat may be more effective to reduce fat intake, promoting the consumption of meat lower in fat. Men were found to be more responsive to cognitive motivations, such as health benefits, neophobia, and information about the product. This type of motivation did not seem to be very effective for women, who were in general more interested in health independently of the choice of fat-rich meat. Barriers to choose meats richer in fat in women were more psychological, related to sensitivity to disgust and punishment, which may lead to inhibitory behaviors, while emotional eating may act as a facilitator.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.S., C.D., T.G.T., and E.M.; formal analysis, S.S. and F.T.; funding acquisition, T.G.T. and E.M.; investigation, S.S., F.T., A.B. (Alessandra Bendini), A.B. (Ada Braghieri), C.P., L.T., N.A.M., E.A., and A.M.; methodology, S.S.; supervision, E.M.; visualization, S.S.; writing, original draft, S.S.; writing, review and editing, C.D., F.T., A.B. (Alessandra Bendini), A.B. (Ada Braghieri), C.P., L.T., N.A.M., E.A., A.M., T.G.T., and E.M. All authors read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della Ricerca (MIUR), Italy Research Project: 20158YJW3W Programmi di Ricerca Scientifica di Rilevante Interesse Nazionale-PRIN 2015: “Individual differences in the acceptability of healthy foods: focus on phenol and fat content”. The dissemination of the results of the research, i.e., the present publication, was supported by the H2020 project “PLOTINA-Promoting gender balance and inclusion in research, innovation and training” (www.plotina.eu), Grant Agreement No. 666008.

Acknowledgments

The gender analysis followed in this study is promoted by the H2020 project “PLOTINA-Promoting gender balance and inclusion in research, innovation and training” (www.plotina.eu).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. Fats and Fatty Acids in Human Nutrition; World Health Organization: Rome, Italy, 2014; Volume 91. [Google Scholar]
  2. World Health Organization. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases: Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2014; Volume 916. [Google Scholar]
  3. Swinburn, B.A.; Sacks, G.; Hall, K.D.; McPherson, K.; Finegood, D.T.; Moodie, M.L.; Gortmaker, S.L. The global obesity pandemic: Shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet 2011, 378, 804–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Ravussin, E.; Smith, S.R. Increased Fat Intake, Impaired Fat Oxidation, and Failure of Fat Cell Proliferation Result in Ectopic Fat Storage, Insulin Resistance, and Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2006, 967, 363–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Howard, B.V.; Van Horn, L.; Hsia, J.; Manson, J.A.E.; Stefanick, M.L.; Wassertheil-Smoller, S.; Kuller, L.H.; LaCroix, A.Z.; Langer, R.D.; Lasser, N.L.; et al. Low-fat dietary pattern and risk of cardiovascular disease: The Women’s Health Initiative randomized controlled dietary modification trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2006, 295, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Prentice, R.L.; Caan, B.; Chlebowski, R.T.; Patterson, R.; Kuller, L.H.; Ockene, J.K.; Margolis, K.L.; Limacher, M.C.; Manson, J.A.E.; Parker, L.M.; et al. Low-fat dietary pattern and risk of invasive breast cancer: The Women’s Health Initiative randomized controlled dietary modification trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2006, 295, 629–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Beresford, S.A.A.; Johnson, K.C.; Ritenbaugh, C.; Lasser, N.L.; Snetselaar, L.G.; Black, H.R.; Anderson, G.L.; Assaf, A.R.; Bassford, T.; Bowen, D.; et al. Low-fat dietary pattern and risk of colorectal cancer: The Women’s Health Initiative randomized controlled dietary modification trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2006, 295, 643–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Rothstein, W.G. Dietary fat, coronary heart disease, and cancer: A historical review. Prev. Med. 2006, 43, 356–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Frank, D.; Joo, S.T.; Warner, R. Consumer acceptability of intramuscular fat. Korean J. Food Sci. Anim. Resour. 2016, 36, 699–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Font-i-Furnols, M.; Guerrero, L. Consumer preference, behavior and perception about meat and meat products: An overview. Meat Sci. 2014, 98, 361–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Dransfield, E. The taste of fat. Meat Sci. 2008, 80, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Stevens, D.A. Individual differences in taste perception. Food Chem. 1996, 56, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ueland, Ø. Gender differences in food choice. In Understanding Consumers of Food Products; Frewer, L., van Trijp, H.C.M., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2007; pp. 316–328. ISBN 9781845690090. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kubberød, E.; Ueland, Ø.; Rødbotten, M.; Westad, F.; Risvik, E. Gender specific preferences and attitudes towards meat. Food Qual. Prefer. 2002, 13, 285–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lombardo, M.; Aulisa, G.; Padua, E.; Annino, G.; Iellamo, F.; Pratesi, A.; Caprio, M.; Bellia, A. Gender differences in taste and foods habits. Nutr. Food Sci. 2019, 50, 229–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kiefer, I.; Rathmanner, T.; Kunze, M. Eating and dieting differences in men and women. J. Men’s Heal. Gend. 2005, 2, 194–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Nakamura, K.; Shimai, S.; Kikuchi, S.; Tanaka, M. Correlation between a liking for fat-rich foods and body fatness in adult Japanese: A gender difference. Appetite 2001, 36, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Girolami, A.; Napolitano, F.; Faraone, D.; Di Bello, G.; Braghieri, A. Image analysis with the computer vision system and the consumer test in evaluating the appearance of Lucanian dry sausage. Meat Sci. 2014, 96, 610–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Goldberg, L.R.; Strycker, L.A. Personality traits and eating habits: The assessment of food preferences in a large community sample. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2002, 32, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ngapo, T.M.; Martin, J.F.; Dransfield, E. International preferences for pork appearance: II. Factors influencing consumer choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 2007, 18, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Herzog, H.A. Gender differences in human-animal interactions: A review. Anthrozoos 2007, 20, 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Graça, J.; Calheiros, M.M.; Oliveira, A.; Milfont, T.L. Why are women less likely to support animal exploitation than men? The mediating roles of social dominance orientation and empathy. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2018, 129, 66–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Amiot, C.E.; Bastian, B. Toward a psychology of human-animal relations. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 6–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Blanc, S.; Massaglia, S.; Borra, D.; Mosso, A.; Merlino, V.M. Animal welfare and gender: A nexus in awareness and preference when choosing fresh beef meat? Ital. J. Anim. Sci. 2020, 19, 410–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Drewnowski, A.; Almiron-Roig, E. Human Perceptions and Preferences for Fat-Rich Foods. In Fat Detection: Taste, Texture, and Post Ingestive Effects; Montmayeur, J.-P., le Coutre, J., Eds.; CRC Press/Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2010; pp. 265–291. ISBN 9781420067750. [Google Scholar]
  26. Stewart, J.E.; Newman, L.P.; Keast, R.S.J. Oral sensitivity to oleic acid is associated with fat intake and body mass index. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 30, 838–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Tepper, B.J.; Banni, S.; Melis, M.; Crnjar, R.; Tomassini Barbarossa, I. Genetic sensitivity to the bitter taste of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) and its association with physiological mechanisms controlling body mass index (BMI). Nutrients 2014, 6, 3363–3381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Duffy, V.; Lucchina, L.; Bartoshuk, L.; Duffy, V.; Lucchina, L.; Bartoshuk, L. Genetic variation in taste: Potential biomarker for cardiovascular disease risk? In Genetic Variation in Taste Sensitivity; Prescott, J., Tepper, B.J., Eds.; Marcel Dekker Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 195–228. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayes, J.E.; Duffy, V.B. Revisiting Sugar-Fat Mixtures: Sweetness and Creaminess Vary with Phenotypic Markers of Oral Sensation. Chem. Senses 2007, 32, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Hayes, J.E.; Duffy, V.B. Oral sensory phenotype identifies level of sugar and fat required for maximal liking. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 95, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Kirkmeyer, S.V.; Tepper, B.J. Understanding creaminess perception of dairy products using free-choice profiling and genetic responsivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil. Chem. Senses 2003, 28, 527–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Prescott, J.; Bartoshuk, L.M.; Prutkin, J. 6-n-Propylthiouracil Tasting and the Perception of Nontaste Oral Sensations. In Genetic variation in taste sensitivity; Prescott, J., Bartoshuk, L.M., Eds.; Marcel Dekker: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 75–87. [Google Scholar]
  33. Tepper, B.J.; Nurse, R.J. Fat Perception is Related to PROP Taster Status. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 61, 949–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Drewnowski, A.; Henderson, S.A.; Barratt-Fornell, A. Genetic sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil and sensory responses to sugar and fat mixtures. Physiol. Behav. 1998, 63, 771–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Drewnowski, A.; Krahn, D.D.; Demitrack, M.A.; Nairn, K.; Gosnell, B.A. Taste responses and preferences for sweet high-fat foods: Evidence for opioid involvement. Physiol. Behav. 1992, 51, 371–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Elfhag, K.; Erlanson-Albertsson, C. Sweet and fat taste preference in obesity have different associations with personality and eating behavior. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 88, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Anschutz, D.J.; Van Strien, T.; Van De Ven, M.O.M.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Eating styles and energy intake in young women. Appetite 2009, 53, 119–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Van Strien, T.; Van de Laar, F.A. Intake of energy is best predicted by overeating tendency and consumption of fat is best predicted by dietary restraint: A 4-year follow-up of patients with newly diagnosed Type 2 diabetes. Appetite 2008, 50, 544–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Tapper, K.; Baker, L.; Jiga-Boy, G.; Haddock, G.; Maio, G.R. Sensitivity to reward and punishment: Associations with diet, alcohol consumption, and smoking. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015, 72, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Davis, C.; Patte, K.; Levitan, R.; Reid, C.; Tweed, S.; Curtis, C. From motivation to behaviour: A model of reward sensitivity, overeating, and food preferences in the risk profile for obesity. Appetite 2007, 48, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Keller, C.; Siegrist, M. Does personality influence eating styles and food choices? Direct and indirect effects. Appetite 2015, 84, 128–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Pfeiler, T.M.; Egloff, B. Personality and meat consumption: The importance of differentiating between type of meat. Appetite 2018, 130, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. European Commission. Meta-Analysis of Gender and Science Research: Synthesis Report; Caprile, M., Ed.; Publications Office of the European Union: Luxembourg, 2012.
  44. Byrnes, N.K.; Hayes, J.E. Gender differences in the influence of personality traits on spicy food liking and intake. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 42, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Spinelli, S.; De Toffoli, A.; Dinnella, C.; Laureati, M.; Pagliarini, E.; Bendini, A.; Braghieri, A.; Gallina, T.; Sinesio, F.; Torri, L.; et al. Personality traits and gender influence liking and choice of food pungency. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 66, 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Monteleone, E.; Spinelli, S.; Dinnella, C.; Endrizzi, I.; Laureati, M.; Pagliarini, E.; Sinesio, F.; Gasperi, F.; Torri, L.; Aprea, E.; et al. Exploring influences on food choice in a large population sample: The Italian Taste project. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 59, 123–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Prescott, J.; Soo, J.; Campbell, H.; Roberts, C. Responses of PROP taster groups to variations in sensory qualities within foods and beverages. Physiol. Behav. 2004, 82, 459–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Masi, C.; Dinnella, C.; Monteleone, E.; Prescott, J. The impact of individual variations in taste sensitivity on coffee perceptions and preferences. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 138, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Bartoshuk, L.M. Comparing Sensory Experiences Across Individuals: Recent Psychophysical Advances Illuminate Genetic Variation in Taste Perception. Chem. Senses 2000, 25, 447–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Fischer, M.E.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Schubert, C.R.; Pinto, A.; Klein, R.; Pankratz, N.; Pankow, J.S.; Huang, G.H. Factors related to fungiform papillae density: The beaver dam offspring study. Chem. Senses 2013, 38, 669–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Hayes, J.E.; Sullivan, B.S.; Duffy, V.B. Explaining variability in sodium intake through oral sensory phenotype, salt sensation and liking. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 100, 369–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Bajec, M.R.; Pickering, G.J. Thermal taste, PROP responsiveness, and perception of oral sensations. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 95, 581–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dinnella, C.; Monteleone, E.; Piochi, M.; Spinelli, S.; Prescott, J.; Pierguidi, L.; Gasperi, F.; Laureati, M.; Pagliarini, E.; Predieri, S.; et al. Individual Variation in PROP Status, Fungiform Papillae Density, and Responsiveness to Taste Stimuli in a Large Population Sample. Chem. Senses 2018, 43, 697–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D.; Taylor, G.J.; Nemiah, J.; Sifneos, P.; Nemiah, J.; Freyberger, H.; Sifneos, P.; Acklin, R.; Alexander, G.; et al. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale--I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 1994, 38, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bressi, C.; Taylor, G.; Parker, J.; Bressi, S.; Brambilla, V.; Aguglia, E.; Allegranti, I.; Bongiorno, A.; Giberti, F.; Bucca, M.; et al. Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: An Italian multicenter study. J. Psychosom. Res. 1996, 41, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Miller, L.C.; Murphy, R.; Buss, A.H. Consciousness of body: Private and public. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1981, 41, 397–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Torrubia, R.; Ávila, C.; Moltó, J.; Caseras, X. The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2001, 31, 837–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Pliner, P.; Hobden, K. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1992, 19, 105–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Laureati, M.; Spinelli, S.; Monteleone, E.; Dinnella, C.; Prescott, J.; Cattaneo, C.; Proserpio, C.; De Toffoli, A.; Gasperi, F.; Endrizzi, I.; et al. Associations between food neophobia and responsiveness to “warning” chemosensory sensations in food products in a large population sample. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 68, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Haidt, J. The Disgust Scale Home Page. Available online: http://people.virginia.edu/%22jdh6n/%0Ddisgustscale.html (accessed on 1 February 2018).
  61. Haidt, J.; McCauley, C.; Rozin, P. Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1994, 16, 701–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Olatunji, B.O.; Williams, N.L.; Tolin, D.F.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Sawchuk, C.N.; Lohr, J.M.; Elwood, L.S. The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 19, 281–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Inbar, Y.; Pizarro, D.A.; Bloom, P. Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cogn. Emot. 2009, 23, 714–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Grunert, K.G.; Brunsø, K.; Bisp, S. Food-related life style: Development of a cross-culturally valid instrument for market surveillance. In Values, Lifestyles, and Psychographics; Kahle, L., Chiagouris, C., Mahwah, N., Eds.; Psychology Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1998; pp. 337–354. [Google Scholar]
  65. Saba, A.; Sinesio, F.; Moneta, E.; Dinnella, C.; Laureati, M.; Torri, L.; Peparaio, M.; Saggia Civitelli, E.; Endrizzi, I.; Gasperi, F.; et al. Measuring consumers attitudes towards health and taste and their association with food-related life-styles and preferences. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 73, 25–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Roininen, K.; Lähteenmäki, L.; Tuorila, H. Quantification of Consumer Attitudes to Health and Hedonic Characteristics of Foods. Appetite 1999, 33, 71–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Van Strien, T.; Frijters, J.E.R.; Bergers, G.P.A.; Defares, P.B. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 1986, 5, 295–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Dakanalis, A.; Zanetti, M.A.; Clerici, M.; Madeddu, F.; Riva, G.; Caccialanza, R. Italian version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire. Psychometric proprieties and measurement invariance across sex, BMI-status and age. Appetite 2013, 71, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Tuorila, H.; Lähteenmäki, L.; Pohjalainen, L.; Lotti, L. Food neophobia among the Finns and related responses to familiar and unfamiliar foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2001, 12, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Peryam, D.R.; Pilgrim, F.J. Hedonic scale method of measuring food preferences. Food Technol. 1957, 11, 9–14. [Google Scholar]
  71. Martens, H.; Martens, M. Modified Jack-knife estimation of parameter uncertainty in bilinear modelling by partial least squares regression (PLSR). Food Qual. Prefer. 2000, 11, 5–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Asioli, D.; Almli, V.L.; Næs, T. Comparison of two different strategies for investigating individual differences among consumers in choice experiments. A case study based on preferences for iced coffee in Norway. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 54, 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Wardle, J.; Haase, A.M.; Steptoe, A.; Nillapun, M.; Jonwutiwes, K.; Bellisie, F. Gender differences in food choice: The contribution of health beliefs and dieting. Ann. Behav. Med. 2004, 27, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Jaeger, S.R.; Rasmussen, M.A.; Prescott, J. Relationships between food neophobia and food intake and preferences: Findings from a sample of New Zealand adults. Appetite 2017, 116, 410–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Cooke, L.; Wardle, J.; Gibson, E.L. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite 2003, 41, 205–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Yuan, W.L.; Rigal, N.; Monnery-Patris, S.; Chabanet, C.; Forhan, A.; Charles, M.-A.; de Lauzon-Guillain, B. Early determinants of food liking among 5y-old children: A longitudinal study from the EDEN mother-child cohort. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2016, 13, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Wardle, J.; Gibson, E.L. Diet and stress: Interactions with emotions and behavior. In Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior: Handbook of Stress; Fink, G., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 9780128009512. [Google Scholar]
  78. Rolls, E.T. Emotion Explained; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009; ISBN 9780191693793. [Google Scholar]
  79. Vandeweghe, L.; Vervoort, L.; Verbeken, S.; Moens, E.; Braet, C. Food Approach and Food Avoidance in Young Children: Relation with Reward Sensitivity and Punishment Sensitivity. Front. Psychol. 2016, 07, 928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Duffy, V.B.; Bartoshuk, L.M. Food acceptance and genetic variation in taste. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2000, 100, 647–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Keller, K.L.; Steinmann, L.; Nurse, R.J.; Tepper, B.J. Genetic taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil influences food preference and reported intake in preschool children. Appetite 2002, 38, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Tepper, B.J.; Nurse, R.J. PROP taster status is related to fat perception and preference. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1998, 855, 802–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Keller, K.L.; Tepper, B.J. Inherited taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil in diet and body weight in children. Obes. Res. 2004, 12, 904–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Ullrich, N.V.; Touger-Decker, R.; O’Sullivan-Maillet, J.; Tepper, B.J. PROP taster status and self-perceived food adventurousness influence food preferences. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2004, 104, 543–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Bajec, M.R.; Pickering, G.J. Association of thermal taste and PROP responsiveness with food liking, neophobia, body mass index, and waist circumference. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 589–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Overview of the study.
Figure 1. Overview of the study.
Nutrients 12 01374 g001
Figure 2. Differences by gender and PROP status (MT = medium taster; NT = non-tasters; ST = supertasters) in mean liking of fatty meat, cold meat, and low-fat meat.
Figure 2. Differences by gender and PROP status (MT = medium taster; NT = non-tasters; ST = supertasters) in mean liking of fatty meat, cold meat, and low-fat meat.
Nutrients 12 01374 g002
Figure 3. Correlation loadings with significant consumer attributes from the PLS-DA model in men. Significant variables according to the uncertainty test are circled.
Figure 3. Correlation loadings with significant consumer attributes from the PLS-DA model in men. Significant variables according to the uncertainty test are circled.
Nutrients 12 01374 g003
Figure 4. Correlation loadings with significant consumer attributes from the PLS-DA model in women. Significant variables according to the uncertainty test are circled.
Figure 4. Correlation loadings with significant consumer attributes from the PLS-DA model in women. Significant variables according to the uncertainty test are circled.
Nutrients 12 01374 g004
Table 1. Details of the research participants.
Table 1. Details of the research participants.
Characteristics of the Participants%
Gender (women)58.36
Age range%
18–3045.6
31–4527.8
46–6026.6
Education level%
none0.08
elementary school0.33
middle school4.39
high school49.25
degree32.17
post degree13.76
Marital status%
not married58.69
married35.79
divorced4.50
widowed1.02
Family members (n)3.37 (1.27 SD)
Expense for food (monthly, €)%
up to 20018.82
from 201 to 40043.78
from 400 to 60028.77
more than 6008.62
Diet%
none91.13
hypocaloric diet6.52
specific diet for health reasons2.35
Table 2. List of meat-based products selected for the fatty meat, fatty cold meat, low-fat meat stated liking and familiarity indices.
Table 2. List of meat-based products selected for the fatty meat, fatty cold meat, low-fat meat stated liking and familiarity indices.
Stated Liking/Familiarity
Fatty MeatFatty Cold MeatLow-Fat Meat
Beef ribBaconCarpaccio
Breaded cutletCooked hamChicken breast
LambCured hamGrilled cutlet
PorkMortadella
Pork sausageSalami
SteakSpicy salami
Table 3. Effect of gender (women and men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on personality traits. For each trait, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported. SF, Short Form.
Table 3. Effect of gender (women and men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on personality traits. For each trait, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported. SF, Short Form.
Personality TraitCronbach’s αGenderAge
WomenMenFp18–3031–4546–60Fp
Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20)0.8246.2145.980.120.73449.75 a43.44 b45.09 b37.11<0.0001
Identifying feelings dimension (DIF)0.8215.35 a14.49 b6.70.01016.59 a13.23 c14.94 b37.86<0.0001
Describing feelings dimension (DDF)0.7812.6712.940.940.33314.06 a12.16 b12.20 b23.97<0.0001
Externally oriented thinking (EOT)0.6117.96 b18.78 a8.330.00419.11 a18.05 b17.94 b8.080.000
Private Body Consciousness (PBC) *0.7218.68 a17.36 b33.69<0.000118.2218.1117.741.630.197
Sensitivity to punishment (SP)0.919.86 a8.05 b36.54<0.000110.52 a8.15 b8.20 b32.04<0.0001
Sensitivity to reward (SR)0.875.13 b6.84 a74.03<0.00017.64 a5.57 b4.75 b85.9<0.0001
Food neophobia (FN)0.8627.2427.650.360.54826.08 b26.64 b29.62 a9.92<0.0001
Sensitivity to disgust (SD-SF) *0.7030.63 a27.56 b90.42<0.000128.04 b29.22 a30.03 a14.26<0.0001
Significant differences are in bold. a,b,c Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).
* Variable for which a significant interaction (p < 0.05) between gender and age was found.
Table 4. Effect of gender (women, men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on attitudes toward foods. For each subscale, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported. Only subscales with Cronbach’s α > 0.6 α are reported.
Table 4. Effect of gender (women, men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on attitudes toward foods. For each subscale, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported. Only subscales with Cronbach’s α > 0.6 α are reported.
Food-Related Lifestyle (FRL) Questionnaire
SubscaleCronbach’s αGenderAge
WomenMenFp18–3031–4546–60Fp
Way of shopping
SC1Importance of product information0.745.495.290.290.7465.305.425.460.080.972
SC2Attitudes toward advertising0.603.053.050.010.9863.1233.020.030.993
SC3Enjoyment from food shopping0.645.545.470.920.3375.59 a5.61 a5.32 b6.360.002
SC4Specialty shops0.744.554.560.010.9264.37 b4.51 b4.79 a9.120.000
SC5Price criteria0.694.83 a4.62 b6.690.0104.91 a4.65 b4.61 b5.950.003
SC6Shopping list0.704.98 a4.73 b9.670.0024.774.934.881.630.196
Quality aspects
APA1Health0.825.68 a5.51 b6.050.0145.26 c5.59 b5.94 a32.11<0.0001
APA4Organic product0.774.524.470.330.5684.20 b4.38 b4.91 a25.93<0.0001
APA6Freshness0.756.266.191.810.1796.176.246.271.440.238
Cooking methods
CS1Interest in cooking0.785.415.360.40.5475.44 a5.53 a5.19 b5.340.005
CS2Looking for new ways0.785.43 a5.24 b7.00.0085.425.375.212.710.067
CS3Convenience0.712.35 b2.67 a19.1<0.00012.62 a2.38 b2.53 a,b4.170.016
CS6Woman’s task *0.642.40 a1.98 b33.72<0.00012.07 b2.11 b2.39 a7.50.001
Purchasing motives
CO1Self-fulfilment in food0.675.44 a5.21 b10.920.0015.355.325.300.250.781
Health and Taste Attitudes Scale (HTAS)
DomainCronbach’s αGenderAge
WomenMenFp18–3031–4546–60Fp
General health interest0.784.90 a4.64 b20.04<0.00014.52 c4.72 b5.07 a31.87<0.0001
Light product interest0.813.403.420.100.7533.61 a3.41 b3.21 b12.26<0.0001
Natural product interest0.744.62 a4.42 b9.090.0034.15 c4.46 b4.96 a54.59<0.0001
Craving for sweet foods0.855.09 a4.37 b73.43<0.00014.874.664.673.070.047
Using food as a reward0.814.494.391.680.1954.68 a4.54 a4.11 b20.87<0.0001
Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ)
DomainCronbach’s αGenderAge
WomenMenFp18–3031–4546–60Fp
Restrained eating *0.882.952.681.540.2162.73 b2.74 b2.98 a5.250.001
Emotional eating0.942.44 a1.99 b4.210.0152.432.152.060.880.45
External eating0.823.193.210.040.9613.39 a3.20 b3.02 c5.490.001
Significant differences are in bold. a,b,c Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05). * Variable for which a significant interaction (p < 0.05) between gender and age was found.
Table 5. Effect of gender (women, men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on the familiarity with (FAM) and liking (LIK) of fatty meat, fatty cold meat, and low-fat meat indices. For each index, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported.
Table 5. Effect of gender (women, men) and age (age classes: 18–30, 31–45, 46–60) on the familiarity with (FAM) and liking (LIK) of fatty meat, fatty cold meat, and low-fat meat indices. For each index, Cronbach’s α, mean scores by gender and age class, and F- and p-values are reported.
IndicesCronbach’s αGenderAge
WomenMenFp18–3031–4546–60Fp
FAM fatty meat (range: 1–30)0.8223.32 b23.97 a11.340.00123.7223.8823.332.570.077
FAM fatty cold meat (range: 1–30)0.8223.72 b24.23 a7.570.00624.21 a24.12 a,b23.59 b4.310.014
FAM low-fat meat (range: 1–15)0.6012.2212.240.030.86812.28 a12.42 a11.99 b5.390.005
LIK fatty meat (range: 1–9)0.846.77 b7.29 a36.91<0.00017.23 a7.07 a6.79 b9.74<0.0001
LIK fatty cold meat (range: 1–9)0.816.70 b7.10 a23.31<0.00016.97 a7.07 a6.67 b7.260.001
LIK low-fat meat (range: 1–9)0.696.90 b7.16 a9.590.0027.26 a7.12 a6.71 b15.56<0.0001
Significant differences are in bold. a,b Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).
Table 6. Proportion of subjects who checked fat and caloric to describe the low- and high-fat option between each pair. The amount of lipids for 100 g is reported (source: food composition tables; CREA—Centro di Ricerca per gli Alimenti e la Nutrizione, Ministero per le Politiche Agricole, Alimentari e Forestali, http://sapermangiare.mobi/tabelle_alimenti.html). Items selected for the fat-rich meat index are in bold.
Table 6. Proportion of subjects who checked fat and caloric to describe the low- and high-fat option between each pair. The amount of lipids for 100 g is reported (source: food composition tables; CREA—Centro di Ricerca per gli Alimenti e la Nutrizione, Ministero per le Politiche Agricole, Alimentari e Forestali, http://sapermangiare.mobi/tabelle_alimenti.html). Items selected for the fat-rich meat index are in bold.
ChoiceFatCaloricLipids (g/100 g)
Low-Fat (0)High Fat (1)p-Value01p-Value01
1Calf ribLamb rib0.0000.436 a0.646 b0.0000.470 a 0.624 b6.14.2
2Grilled cutletBreaded cutlet0.0000.182 a0.840 b0.0000.271 a0.956 b--
3Chicken breastSausage0.0000.083 a0.950 b0.0000.105 a0.978 b0.926.1
4ChickenLamb<0.00010.083 a0.646 b<0.00010.105 a0.624 b10 *14.2
5Cooked hamMortadella0.0000.442 a0.923 b0.0000.436 a0.912 b14.728.1
6CarpaccioSliced steak (tagliata)0.0000.160 a0.354 b0.0000.260 a0.475 b2.76.1
7Cooked hamCured ham0.0050.442 a0.564 b0.0240.436 a0.536 b14.723
* Without skin. a, b Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).
Table 7. Effect of fatty meat choice index (low FCI, high FCI) on age, personality traits, and attitudes toward food by gender. F, p, and mean scores. Only variables with p < 0.1 are reported.
Table 7. Effect of fatty meat choice index (low FCI, high FCI) on age, personality traits, and attitudes toward food by gender. F, p, and mean scores. Only variables with p < 0.1 are reported.
WomenMen
VariablesFpLow FCIHigh FCIFpLow FCIHigh FCI
FAM fatty meat index33.20<0.000122.45 b24.03 a20.32<0.000122.87 b24.45 a
FAM fatty cold meat index25.05<0.000123.05 b24.34 a23.61<0.000123.01 b24.71 a
LIK fatty meat index67.72<0.00016.28 b7.33 a46.09<0.00016.73 b7.60 a
LIK fatty cold meat index50.04<0.00016.24 b7.10 a39.84<0.00016.52 b7.36 a
Age3.610.05834.0436.203.170.07634.1236.67
Food neophobia7.700.00628.08 a25.30 b12.180.00130.25 a25.90 b
Sensitivity to disgust6.270.01331.09 a29.96 b----
Sensitivity to punishment2.880.09010.579.79----
SC1 (importance of product information)----10.750.0015.56 a5.08 b
SC3 (enjoyment from food shopping)8.600.0045.41 b5.73 a----
APA4 (organic products)----3.230.0734.584.30
CO1 (self-fulfillment in food)5.370.0215.32 b5.55 a----
Emotional eating3.600.0582.392.53----
External eating6.700.0103.16 b3.29 a6.660.0103.13 b3.31 a
General health interest6.920.0095.02 a4.79 b25.42<0.00014.96 a4.39 b
Light product interest----6.780.0103.68 a3.35 b
Using food as a reward----4.900.0274.20 b4.51 a
a, b Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Spinelli, S.; Dinnella, C.; Tesini, F.; Bendini, A.; Braghieri, A.; Proserpio, C.; Torri, L.; Miele, N.A.; Aprea, E.; Mazzaglia, A.; et al. Gender Differences in Fat-Rich Meat Choice: Influence of Personality and Attitudes. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1374. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12051374

AMA Style

Spinelli S, Dinnella C, Tesini F, Bendini A, Braghieri A, Proserpio C, Torri L, Miele NA, Aprea E, Mazzaglia A, et al. Gender Differences in Fat-Rich Meat Choice: Influence of Personality and Attitudes. Nutrients. 2020; 12(5):1374. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12051374

Chicago/Turabian Style

Spinelli, Sara, Caterina Dinnella, Federica Tesini, Alessandra Bendini, Ada Braghieri, Cristina Proserpio, Luisa Torri, Nicoletta A. Miele, Eugenio Aprea, Agata Mazzaglia, and et al. 2020. "Gender Differences in Fat-Rich Meat Choice: Influence of Personality and Attitudes" Nutrients 12, no. 5: 1374. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12051374

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop