Next Article in Journal
Exercise Intervention Changes the Perceptions and Knowledge of Non-Communicable Disease Risk Factors among Women from a Low-Resourced Setting
Next Article in Special Issue
A Circular Economy Approach to Restoring Soil Substrate Ameliorated by Sewage Sludge with Amendments
Previous Article in Journal
“Precious Time Together Was Taken Away”: Impact of COVID-19 Restrictive Measures on Social Needs and Loneliness from the Perspective of Residents of Nursing Homes, Close Relatives, and Volunteers
Previous Article in Special Issue
M-GRCT: A Dynamic Circular Economy Model for the Optimal Design of Waste Management Systems in Low-Income Municipalities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Does Social Capital Affect Residents’ Waste-Separation Behavior? Evidence from China

1
Center for Innovation and Development Studies, Beijing Normal University, Zhuhai 519087, China
2
School of Economics and Resource Management, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
3
Beijing Key Lab of Study on Sci-Tech Strategy for Urban Green Development, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
4
School of Economics and Management, Inner Mongolia University, Hohhot 010021, China
5
Management Academy of China Cooperatives, Beijing 100028, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(6), 3469; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063469
Submission received: 31 January 2022 / Revised: 6 March 2022 / Accepted: 12 March 2022 / Published: 15 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Circular Economy in Waste and Wastewaters)

Abstract

:
The increasing amount of waste produced has been a challenge for human health and the environment, causing a call for effective waste management measures in which household waste separation is of great significance. Although an expanding body of literature has examined the impact of social capital on individual waste-separation behavior, few studies have explicitly discussed the endogeneity problem and the influence mechanisms. Accordingly, our study investigates the effect of social capital on waste-separation behavior and corresponding mechanisms using a national survey dataset of China. The study also reveals the heterogeneity of the influence of individual characteristics on waste-separation behavior. Our results demonstrate that social capital casts a significant positive impact on waste-separation behavior, providing opportunities for individuals’ social learning and strengthening the reputation effect. The heterogeneous effects of social capital reveal that women, higher-educated individuals, and political party members present better waste-separation behavior. Besides, the impact of social capital varies between urban and rural areas and among different age groups. Our study provides empirical evidence for policy making of household waste-separation management in developing countries from the perspective of informal institutions.

1. Introduction

With the rapid global economic and social development in the past few decades, the amount of waste produced has been growing at an increasing rate. The amount of global waste is predicted to grow from about 2.02 billion tons in 2016 to 3.40 billion tons by 2050, and the amount in low- and middle-income countries is expected to increase by about 40% [1]. China became the most significant global waste producer in 2004, and it currently produces 70% of the waste in East Asia and the Pacific Ocean area [2]. The waste produced by 196 large- and medium-sized cities in China reached 0.236 billion tons in 2019 [3]. It also predicts that China will more than double the U.S. solid waste by 2030 [2]. To address the urgent waste-management problem, China and many other counties have adopted waste-management policies and regulations [4,5]. Twelve out of the 17 UN Sustainable Development Goals are directly linked to solid waste management [6].
In the process of waste management, waste separation is of great importance. Effective waste separation can significantly improve resource utilization and reduce environmental pollution, contributing to public health, carbon emission reduction, and sustainable development [7,8]. However, there is still a significant gap in waste separation between developed and developing countries [9,10]. For example, without a sound waste-management system available in many developed countries, the process of waste separation and recycling in China remains inefficient: some profitable household waste is sold to scrap dealers, while the rest is often disposed of with landfills or incineration. Therefore, the efficiency of waste-separation facilities in many cities is relatively low [11].
Household behavior is at the crux of the waste-separation process, which is important to the success or failure of waste management as a whole. Studies have disclosed that as the marginal benefit of waste-separation investment presents a downward trend, future investment focus should shift from adding separation facilities to encouraging voluntary household waste separation [12]. Nevertheless, it is challenging to put the rule of waste separation into residents’ practice, for individuals are often inclined to free-rider behavior [13]. Formal law cannot function well alone to promote household waste separation, as informal institutions are necessarily complementary and indispensable to the formal institutions [14,15]. As one of the informal institutions, social capital plays a vital role in waste-separation behavior [16]. Particularly, when implementing waste separation in a relational society such as China [17,18], residents’ social environment and social capital cannot be ignored. Therefore, this paper investigates whether social capital can contribute to forming waste-separation behavior and explores the channels of social capital influencing waste-separation behavior, contributing to finding more effective measures in the waste-separation practice.
Existing literature has identified that social capital can promote public environmental protection and management [19,20]. Furthermore, social capital is significantly associated with waste-separation behavior in urban areas [16,21]. Most of the existing studies apply case study methods or employ regional survey data to analyze the impact of social capital on waste separation in China. For instance, some use the survey data in a couple of provinces or cities in China to examine the waste-treatment method [22,23,24], and some discuss the waste separation and recycling patterns only in a specific city [25]. There are also a few studies using national survey data. However, they have not discussed the endogenous problem of social capital [26,27]. In addition, as far as we know, no previous study has employed an empirical test to reveal the mechanisms of social capital influencing an individual’s waste-separation behavior. Although Li and Wang (2021) theoretically analyzed the mechanisms of how social capital affects the performance of household waste separation in urban communities, the study did not perform any empirical analysis.
Different from the previous literature that uses regional data or case studies, our study, utilizing the data of the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS), otherwise applied the instrumental variable (IV) technique to dealing with the potential endogeneity problem in assessing the impact of social capital on waste separation and further explored the influence mechanisms. Referring to Shi et al. [28] and Jia and Zhao [24], we measured social capital from three perspectives: social networks, social trust, and social participation. Based on the previous literature, we chose the parental education level as the instrumental variable to discuss and deal with the endogenous problem between social capital and waste separation. Compared with the existing literature, our study further empirically proposes and analyzes two influence mechanisms. The results reveal that social capital encourages individuals’ waste-separation behavior via increasing social learning opportunities and forming external constraints. Furthermore, the study discloses the heterogeneity of social capital effects regarding communities, education levels, age cohorts, gender, and urban–rural areas. Our findings for China are constructive for implementing effective household waste-separation policies and might benefit other developing countries confronting similar problems.
The study contributes to the existing literature in the following three aspects. First, instead of adopting small-scale survey data or case studies, our study adopted national representative data to examine the impact of social capital on waste-separation behavior. Second, we employed the IV technique to correct the endogeneity problem. Third, this paper further analyzes the mechanisms and provides empirical evidence for implementing waste-separation policies.

2. Hypotheses

Social capital is a key to solving the dilemma of collective action [29,30]. As an informal institution, social capital posts a particular impact on individual decision making and behavior and plays an essential role in inhibiting air or water pollution [31,32,33]. It can significantly raise people’s awareness of environmental protection, thus encouraging their pro-environment behavior. For example, residents with better community relationships are more environmentally friendly [21]. Several regional studies have found that peasants with more social capital show stronger environmental willingness when dealing with straw waste [23]. Specifically, farmers with a higher frequency of social exchanges, higher institutional trust, or more active participation in rural waste management attain a higher frequency of waste separation [24]. However, it is also found that the impact of individual social capital (including trust, justice, and participation) on environmental governance performance is not significant because an atmosphere of protecting the environment has not been formed in society [34]. Based on the above evidence-adopting case studies or local survey data, we speculate that social capital may generally positively impact individual waste-separation behavior when using national representative survey data. We posit the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1.
Social capital can promote individual waste-separation behavior.
Different dimensions of social capital (social networks, social trust, and social participation) may have various effects on waste separation behavior. Zheng et al. [35] conducted an online survey of 259 subjects and found that social network connections encourage waste-separation behavior. Halkos and Jones’s [36] study also suggested that social trust produces significant positive effects on individual waste-separation behavior in Sweden. Furthermore, Nguyen et al. [37] found that social trust serves as a decisive factor in residents’ willingness to segregate waste in Delhi, India. Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1a.
Social networks can promote individual waste-separation behavior.
Hypothesis 1b.
Social trust can promote individual waste-separation behavior.
Social participation can contribute to the emergence of pro-social behavior among community residents [38], and those who spend more time on volunteer activities are more likely to spend time on waste management [39]. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1c.
Social participation can promote individual waste-separation behavior.
Social capital can decrease the cost of cooperation [20]. The quality and quantity of social capital determine the state of trust and interaction among group members [40]. Specifically, social networks and social participation can provide the opportunity for cooperation, and social trust plays a crucial role in promoting cooperation [41,42]. Furthermore, cooperation can promote social learning, including gaining knowledge from peers and learning during the cooperation process [43]. Therefore, social capital may facilitate learning waste separation knowledge and behavior by promoting cooperation. It is also argued that the lack of knowledge about waste separation become a major obstacle to effective waste separation [44]. By participating in public education activities, individuals can obtain environmental protection knowledge and form norms that affect personal behavior [45]. For example, a study of plastic bag recycling behavior among Ecuadorian residents found that participation in relevant social learning can effectively improve the utilization rate of plastic bags [46]. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 2.
Social capital can promote individual waste-separation behavior by providing social learning opportunities.
Social capital produces reputation effect, which provides instrumental benefits (including obtaining valuable resources and the potential to influence others) and symbolic benefits (e. g., meeting basic respect needs) [36]. Driving by these gains, individuals may develop more positive waste-separation behavior. Chen and Gao [47] found that mental satisfaction, a kind of symbolic benefit, is one of the essential drivers to motivate residents to segregate waste effectively. At the same time, individuals tend to avoid reputation damage from negative waste-separation behavior. Existing literature has shown that peasants’ reputation demands significantly promote their pro-environmental behavior [48]. We speculate that individuals with more social capital would care more about reputation, thereby showing better waste-separation behavior. We propose the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 3.
Social capital can influence individuals’ waste-separation behavior through the reputation effect.
Based on the literature review, we mainly analyzed social capital in three dimensions: social networks, social trust, and social participation. As shown in Figure 1, we propose a research framework based on the above hypotheses to explain the impacts of social capital and its various dimensions on waste separation and the influence mechanisms.

3. Data and Empirical Strategy

3.1. Data

We utilized the data from the Chinese General Social Survey (CGSS) 2013 conducted by Renmin University of China to explore the effects of social capital on the waste-separation behavior of residents. The sample size is about 12,000 from 134 municipal districts and counties. The questionnaire includes variables at the levels of community, family, and especially the individual in detail. Finally, we retained a sample of 11,282 individuals after cleaning the data.
To reflect individual social networks, we drew on three questions from the CGSS 2013 questionnaire: (1) “How often did you get together with relatives who do not live with you over the last year?”; (2) “How often have you met with your friends over the past year?”; and (3) “The frequency of having entertainment activities with other friends.” We employed the factor analysis method to construct the component index of social networks and extract a factor whose KMO (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin) test result is 0.60, indicating the factor analysis can be performed. To measure social trust, we selected two questions: “Do you agree that the majority of people in the society can be trusted?” and “Do you trust the strangers in the society?” Adding together these two questions, we generated the social trust variable. Social participation is a dummy variable equal to one if the individual participated in the last neighborhood/village committee elections or has been a labor union member; otherwise, the variable was equal to zero. Finally, using factor analysis again and all the original indicators in the questionnaire, we extracted three common factors, generating the social capital variable. Table 1 provides further details relating to the factor analysis. We show the exploratory factor analysis process of social capital in Appendix A.
Previous studies have verified the impact of demographic characteristics, such as gender, income, and education level, on individual environmental behavior and household waste-management behavior [49,50,51]. This study controlled respondents’ age, gender, marital status, education level, and per capita annual family income. In addition, members of the Communist Party of China (CPC) and eight democratic parties have a stronger sense of social responsibility and more advanced ideological awareness. They are likely to exhibit more pro-environmental behavior, so we controlled for the political status of individuals. Considering that the community environment of respondents varies, and the household waste-management facilities and rules also affect the individual waste-separation behavior, this paper controls the type of community where individuals live. According to the completeness of waste separation facilities, the communities are divided into three categories: poor, general, and good. Specifically, communities with poor waste-sorting facilities refer to rural communities. Communities with average waste sorting facilities include old city communities and new urban communities transformed from rural communities. Furthermore, communities with good waste-sorting facilities include apartments, indemnificatory housings, common commercial housings, senior villa areas, and top-grade residential quarters. The descriptive statistics of the main variables are illustrated in Table 2. In terms of individual characteristics, about 50% of respondents are males, and 79% are married. The samples we use are consistent with the data of the China Statistical Yearbook 2013.

3.2. Empirical Strategy

As individual waste-separation behavior is measured using ordered discrete variables, we used the ordered probit model as the benchmark in the following form:
y i * = α + β S C i + θ X i + P δ P + ε i    
y i = 0 ,     y i * μ 1 1 ,     μ 1 < y i * 2 ,     y i * > μ 2 μ 2
y i * represents the latent variable, which represents the frequency of individuals’ waste-separation behavior. S C i is the set of variables representing the level of social capital, including social network, social trust, and social participation. y i is the dependent variable measuring the frequency of waste-separation behavior of individual i in the past year. In Equation (2), μ 1 and μ 2 are two boundary points classifying the household waste separation level into three categories. y i * μ 1 ( y i = 0 ) indicates that individual i never segregates waste; y i = 1 means that individual i shows separating-waste behavior occasionally; and y i = 2 means that individual i often segregates waste. X i is the characteristic vector at the individual and household levels. Given that waste facilities and management levels vary in different regions, which can influence individual waste-separation behavior, we controlled for community types of respondents X i . We also added the provincial fixed effects p δ p in the regression to control for the disparity among regions. ε i is a random disturbance term.

4. Results

Table 3 shows the baseline regression results of model 1. Individuals’ social capital positively impacts the frequency of waste separation, which is significant at the 1% level (column 4 of Table 3). Columns 1–3 of Table 3 show that an individual’s social network, social trust, and social participation substantially affect waste-separation behavior. The possible reason is that individuals with higher social capital interact more often with their relatives, friends, or neighbors and are thus more familiar with their living environment and have a stronger sense of social identity. Thereby, individuals may pay more attention to environmental conditions and have a more positive attitude toward environmental protection; therefore, they are more willing to segregate waste. The results provide empirical evidence to support Hypothesis 1.
We further report the marginal effects of each explanatory variable on waste-separation behavior in Table 4. If we take the mean of all variables, for each unit increment in individual social capital, the probability of “never segregating waste” would decrease by 0.036, the probability of “occasionally segregating waste” would increase by 0.020, and the likelihood of “often segregating waste” would increase by 0.016. Among the three dimensions of social capital, the marginal effect of social networks is the most significant. For each unit increment in an individual’s social network, the probability of “never segregating waste” decreases by 0.024, and the probabilities of occasionally and often segregating waste both increase by 0.012, with all of them being significant at the 1% level. Compared to those without social participation, residents involved in public activities are 0.023 less likely never to segregate waste and 0.011 more likely to occasionally or often segregate waste. It can be seen from the regression results in Table 3 and Table 4 that social trust would also promote individual waste separation, but the effect is smaller than social networks and social participation. Controlling for all other variables, for each unit increment in social trust, the probability of “never segregating waste” decreases by 0.007, and the probability of occasionally or often segregating waste increases by 0.003.
The regression results of individual characteristics on waste separation in Table 4 are consistent with the literature. Men are less likely to segregate waste [52]. Men’s probability of “never segregating waste” is 0.031 higher than women’s, and men are 0.014 less likely to segregate waste often. Besides, it is generally believed that more educated individuals are more sensitive to social welfare and would exhibit more pro-environmental behaviors [46,53,54]. As seen in Table 4, compared to illiterate respondents, the probability of “often segregating waste” is on average 0.016 times higher for those who have attended primary school, 0.039 times higher for the group who have attended junior high school, 0.065 times higher for those who have attended high school, and 0.071 times higher for those with a college degree or above.
Having a political identity, such as being a member of the Chinese Communist Party or eight democratic parties, would increase the probability of “often segregating waste” by 0.022, which is significant at the 1% level and consistent with related studies based on the values-beliefs-norms (VBN) model [55,56]. The literature on pro-environmental behavior suggests that the sense of morality and obligation have a significant impact on individual behavior [57,58]. According to universal values, Schwartz’s model of human values divides people into four groups [59,60], among which the altruistic group would engage in pro-environmental behavior more actively [61,62,63]. In addition, the community environment of individuals is associated with waste-separation habits. Residents of communities with average waste-separation facilities have a 0.028 higher probability of “often segregating waste” than rural residents. Residents living in communities with sound waste-sorting facilities will have a 0.061 higher probability of “often segregating waste” than rural residents. The reason may be that people’s waste-separation behavior is related to local environmental institutions and the prevalence of waste-separation facilities.
We found that social capital can significantly promote individual waste-separation behavior. Nevertheless, there may be endogeneity problems as social capital is influenced by some unobservable omitted variables, such as customs and culture [64]. Since the waste classification is a discrete variable and the two-stage least squares method based on continuous variables may fail, we adopted the Conditional Mixed Process (CMP) proposed by Roodman [65] for regression analysis to deal with the endogenous problem. CMP is a two-stage regression, where the first stage finds the instrumental variables of the core explanatory variables and evaluates their interdependency. The second stage performs regression analysis with the instrumental variables in the model, using the joint likelihood estimation method. Education is the primary channel for intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status, and parents’ educational experience has a significant positive impact on their offspring’s human capital and socioeconomic status [66]. Therefore, we used “parent_edu” as the instrumental variable for social participation.
We tested the validity of using “parent_edu” as an instrumental variable. First, the correlation between “social participation” and “parent_edu” was tested (Table 5, column 4), and the regression coefficient of “parent_edu” is significant at the 1% level. It means that individuals with higher “parent_edu” are more likely to participate in social activities. In addition, the F-test value of the first-stage regression is 10.9, passing the weak instrument test.
Columns 2–3 of Table 5 report the CMP estimation results and find that social participation still has a positive effect on individual waste-separation behavior after controlling for the endogeneity. We further estimated Equation (1) using the OLS model, as shown in column 1 of Table 5. Moreover, columns 4–5 of Table 5 show the results of the first and second stages of the two-stage least squares (2SLS) estimation. The results of the instrumental variable regression indicate that social participation significantly increases the probability of an individual segregating waste, which is consistent with the conclusion of our OLS regression, suggesting that the estimated coefficients of the OLS regression are underestimated.

5. Discussion

5.1. Mechanism Tests

The above results have suggested that social capital casts a significant positive impact on individual waste-separation behavior, and we will further discuss the influence mechanisms. First, social capital can increase cooperation opportunities with which one’s environmental protection willingness may be enhanced by social learning [67,68]. Second, social capital may promote environmental behavior by reputation effect [48]. Thus, we speculate that social capital will affect the environmental behavior of residents through the two mechanisms and examine Hypotheses 2 and 3.

5.1.1. Social Learning

We measured social learning by individual participation in various environmental protection activities. Since the product-term test of the mediating effect coefficient method proposed by Sobel [69] is only applicable to the linear mediating effect model, while this paper is a non-linear probability model, we used the OLS method to test the mediating effects after standardizing the dependent variables. Column 1 of Table 6 shows the baseline regression results; columns 2, 4, and 6 show the results of the regression of social capital on social learning; and columns 3, 5, and 7 show the results of the regression after adding the mediator variables. The results reveal that social capital significantly increases the frequency at which individuals discuss environmental issues with their relatives and friends and motivates people to participate more actively in environmental activities organized by the government, affiliate, or private environmental groups, which significantly increases their social learning opportunities. After adding a mediator variable in columns 3, 5, and 7, respectively, the effect of social capital on waste separation is still significantly positive, but the degree of the effect decreases. The mediating effect of social learning is 0.63 (0.143 × 0.293/0.066 = 0.63) when measured by “discussing environmental issues with friends and relatives”, 0.26 when measured by “participating in government environmental activities”, 0.63 when measured by “discussing environmental issues with friends and family”, 0.26 when using “participating in government environmental activities”, and 0.22 when using “participating in private environmental activities”, indicating that by providing social learning opportunities, social capital can increase the frequency of waste separation.
We further tested the social learning mechanism using individuals’ environmental knowledge scores. Individuals’ group learning promotes information exchanges, and the more social learning opportunities individuals have, the more knowledge they would acquire [70]. Therefore, if social capital promotes waste-separation behavior by facilitating social learning, individuals will gain more environmental protection knowledge. From Table 7, it can be seen that there is a significant positive effect of social capital on individuals’ environmental protection knowledge level, and the mediating effect is about 0.18, indicating that social capital does improve individuals’ relevant environmental protection knowledge by increasing the opportunities of social learning to improve the frequency of individual waste-separation behavior. The results support Hypothesis 2.

5.1.2. Reputation Effect

In society, individuals are subject to the external constraints of informal institutions such as reputation, which may lead to more pro-environmental behavior. A study found that peasants’ reputation demand significantly affects their pro-environmental behavior [48]. Furthermore, we deduced that individuals with more social capital care more about personal reputation and thus present better waste-separation behavior. The results in column 2 of Table 8 show that individuals with more social capital more greatly value reputation. Furthermore, the coefficients of “social capital” and “reputation” in column 3 of Table 8 are significantly positive, and the magnitude of the mediating effect is about 0.018, indicating that social capital promotes waste-separation behavior by reputation effect. The results support Hypothesis 3.

5.2. Heterogeneous Effects

According to the baseline regression results in Table 3, the coefficient of age on waste-separation behavior is negative and significant, indicating that older groups show less waste-separation behavior than younger groups. To further examine the cohort effect, the sample was divided into five age cohorts: 30 and below, 30–40, 40–50, 50–60, and over 60 years old. As can be seen in panel A of Table 9, the influence of the social network on waste-separation behavior gradually decreases with age. This effect may be because the older cohorts have relatively lower environmental awareness and knowledge; also, they focus more on themselves and their families, which can lead to lower social frequency. The results of panel B and panel C in Table 9 show that the influence of social trust on waste-separation behavior is mainly concentrated in the 50–60-year-old cohort, while the influence of social participation is primarily detected in the 30–40-year-old cohort. In general, the impact of social capital on waste-separation behavior is in cohorts over 30–40 years old, and the impact decreases with increasing age until 60 years old.
As there are significant differences in the social environment, lifestyle, and behavior habits of residents between urban and rural areas in China, the impact of social capital on waste-separation behavior of urban and rural residents may also be different. In the CGSS survey data, respondents’ residences consist of central urban areas, non-central urban areas, urban fringe, towns, and rural areas. This study classified all the respondents living in rural areas to rural residence and the rest to urban residence. According to the results in Table 10, the influence of social networks on waste-separation behavior in rural areas is 0.088, which is larger than 0.059 in urban areas, although both are significant. Social trust exposes a significant impact on the waste-separation behavior of rural residents, whereas social participation otherwise influences urban residents more.

6. Conclusions

Based on the national representative survey data CGSS in China, our study demonstrates that social capital is a critical factor affecting individual waste-separation behavior. Specifically, social networks, social trust, and social participation promote individual waste-separation action. To deal with the endogenous problem, we utilized the instrumental variable and found that social participation exerts a causal effect on waste-separation behavior. Furthermore, our examination of influence mechanisms evidences that social capital can improve environmental protection knowledge by providing opportunities for individuals’ social learning and strengthening the reputation effect to encourage residents’ waste-separation behavior. Heterogeneous effects of social capital exist among individuals with different characteristics of gender, age, education level, political identity, and living area.
Our results provide valuable policy implications on improving waste-separation management, especially for developing countries plagued by environmental pollution. In addition to the formal institution construction and technological innovation, the government should also give full play to informal institutions, such as social capital, in promoting residents’ waste-separation behavior by enhancing the interaction, communication, and general trust among residents. First, the government can organize various publicity activities and improve the individual waste-separation knowledge. Second, the government can establish a people supervision system to integrate the concept of environmental protection into social values, thus improving residents’ sense of responsibility for environmental protection and stimulating their motivation to participate in waste separation. Besides, the relevant administration can pay more attention to young and highly educated individuals and give full play to the role model of CPC members and other cadres. At the same time, urban–rural differences should be considered. We should pay attention to improving the social trust of residents in rural areas, making them believe that others also strictly carry out waste separation and be confident to participate in collective activities. For urban residents, more attention should be paid to improving social participation, enhancing ownership, and internalizing environmental protection awareness.
There are some limitations to our study. The national representative data (CGSS2013) needs to be improved to provide more adequate information on waste-separation attitudes and intentions. First, the specific municipal district and county information is not disclosed. Second, this survey lacks variables that can be used to measure social norms accurately. In addition, analysis of the influence mechanisms of social capital is not sufficient. Social capital can lower the threshold of social information interaction and has a significant positive impact on information exchange [71]. Furthermore, social capital can improve the dissemination of waste-separation information, thus increasing the frequency of waste-separation behavior among urban residents [47]. Nevertheless, we have not empirically tested the information mechanism for the lack of appropriate measurement variables. Further studies can be conducted after obtaining the improved data.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization Y.Z.; methodology, Y.Z. and H.S.; validation, Y.Z., H.C. and W.W.; formal analysis, Y.Z., H.S., X.H., H.C. and W.W.; data curation, H.S.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.Z., H.S. and X.H.; writing—review and editing, Y.Z., H.S., X.H., H.C. and W.W.; visualization, Y.Z., H.S., X.H., H.C. and W.W.; supervision, Y.Z., H.C. and W.W.; project administration, Y.Z., H.C. and W.W.; funding acquisition, Y.Z. and H.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China, grant number 71773010; and the National Social Science Fund of China, grant number 21BJY013; and the Beijing Natural Science Foundation, grant number 9222016.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The CGSS 2013 database is available in Renmin University of China: http://cgss.ruc.edu.cn/index.htm (accessed on 30 January 2022).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Exploratory Factor Analysis on Social Capital

The essence of factor analysis is dimensionality reduction. It attributes multiple variables with complex relationships, such as correlations or dependencies, to several common factors through specific statistical methods. In this study, we adopted exploratory factor analysis to process the following items included in social capital variables.
We selected variables potentially related to social capital from the questionnaire, as shown in Table A1, and conducted a preliminary factor analysis of all the following variables. Using principal component analysis for factor extraction, we extracted all the factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, and the common factor was extracted basing on the correlation matrix. We adopted the KMO test, which compares the simple and partial correlation coefficients between the variables, and the Bartlett test generated from the correlation coefficient matrix.
Table A1. Survey Questions about Social Capital.
Table A1. Survey Questions about Social Capital.
VariablesOriginal Question in the QuestionnaireMeanStd. Dev.MinMax
Together_relativeHow often did you get together with relatives who do not live with you over the last year?2.250.7215
Together_friendHow often have you met with your friends over the past year?2.410.9315
Activity_neighborThe frequency of having entertainment activities with your neighbors?3.702.0717
Activity_friendThe frequency of having entertainment activities with other friends?4.131.7817
ContactThe frequency of contact with relatives and friends?0101
Trust_societyDo you agree that the vast majority of people in the society can be trusted?3.281.0315
Trust_strangerDo you trust the strangers in society?2.590.9015
VoteHave you voted in the last neighborhood committee/village committee election?0.44-01
UnionAre you a labor unionist?0.19-01
The result of the KMO test is 0.61, and the result of Bartlett’s Sphericity Test is 11,905.822 (df = 36, p < 0.001), indicating that there is a suitable common factor for factor analysis. In this study, a common factor was extracted by principal component analysis, and varimax orthogonal rotation was used to obtain the matrix of factor loads (e.g., Table A2), in which factor 1 contains the most indicators. As “Contact” has low factor loadings and factor score coefficient (e.g., Table A3), we eliminated it before further study.
Table A2. Rotated Factor Matrix (1).
Table A2. Rotated Factor Matrix (1).
VariablesFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4
Together_friend0.814
Activity_friend0.712
Together_relative0.711
Contact0.526
Trust_society 0.814
Trust_stranger 0.803
Vote 0.783
Activity_neighbor −0.619
Union 0.873
Table A3. Factor Score Coefficient Matrix (1).
Table A3. Factor Score Coefficient Matrix (1).
VariablesFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4
Trust_stranger−0.0030.608−0.032−0.129
Union0.1400.0180.7000.246
Together_friend0.403−0.0430.047−0.155
Together_relative0.365−0.0450.195−0.112
Activity_neighbor0.0850.009−0.4760.363
Trust_society−0.0440.6160.0620.077
Vote−0.055−0.0350.1230.781
Contact0.2480.0620.0750.140
Activity_friend0.317−0.031−0.2740.067
We conducted the factor analysis again to obtain the rotated factor matrix of Table A4. It can be seen that “Activity_neighbor” appears in two common factors. We regarded it as an invalid variable and eliminated it to explore further.
Table A4. Rotated Factor Matrix (2).
Table A4. Rotated Factor Matrix (2).
VariablesFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4
Together_friend0.843
Activity_friend0.742
Together_relative0.715
Trust_society 0.813
Trust_stranger 0.810
Vote 0.846
Activity_neighbor 0.588−0.506
Union 0.838
Three common factors were finally extracted from seven original indicators, and the social capital index was generated. The result of the KMO test is 0.60, and the result of Bartlett’s Sphericity Test is 11,905.822 (df = 36, p < 0.001). It can be seen from Table A5 and Table A6 that “Together_friend”, “Activity_friend”, and “Together_relative” belong to the first public factor, “Social network”; “Trust_ society” and “Trust_stranger” belong to the second public factor, “Social trust”; and “Union” and “Vote” are strong interpretations of the third public factor, “Social participation”.
Table A5. Rotated Factor Matrix (3).
Table A5. Rotated Factor Matrix (3).
VariablesFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3
Together_friend0.860
Activity_friend0.734
Together_relative0.723
Trust_society 0.815
Trust_stranger 0.805
Union 0.728
Vote 0.699
Table A6. Factor Score Coefficient Matrix (2).
Table A6. Factor Score Coefficient Matrix (2).
VariablesFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3
Trust_stranger0.0160.609−0.097
Union0.072−0.0340.691
Activity_friend0.3970.028−0.068
Together_friend0.468−0.021−0.019
Together_relative0.395−0.0450.092
Trust_society−0.0490.6200.097
Vote−0.0640.0300.662

Appendix B. Results of Marginal Effects Analysis

This section reports more detailed results of the marginal effects analysis. Table A7, Table A8 and Table A9 show the results of the marginal effects of each control variable with the social network, social trust, and social participation as the explanatory variables.
Table A7. Marginal Effects of Social Networks on Waste-Separation Behavior.
Table A7. Marginal Effects of Social Networks on Waste-Separation Behavior.
Variables(1)(2)(3)
NeverOccasionallyOften
Networks index−0.024 ***0.012 ***0.012 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Age0.002 ***−0.001 ***−0.001 ***
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Male0.031 ***−0.016 ***−0.016 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Marry−0.0050.0030.003
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Primary education−0.043 **0.021 **0.022 **
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Junior high school−0.091 ***0.045 ***0.046 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
High school−0.147 ***0.073 ***0.075 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
College and above−0.159 ***0.078 ***0.081 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Party membership−0.051 ***0.025 ***0.026 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Lnincome−0.0010.0010.001
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Community_average−0.062 ***0.030 ***0.031 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Community_good−0.134 ***0.066 ***0.068 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Note: The values in parentheses indicate the standard error of county/street level clustering; clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table A8. Marginal Effects of Social Trust on Waste-Separation Behavior.
Table A8. Marginal Effects of Social Trust on Waste-Separation Behavior.
Variables(1)(2)(3)
NeverOccasionallyOften
Trust index−0.007 **0.003 **0.003 **
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Age0.003 ***−0.001 ***−0.001 ***
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Male0.031 ***−0.015 ***−0.015 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Marry−0.0040.0020.002
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Primary education−0.045 ***0.022 ***0.023 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Junior high school−0.096 ***0.047 ***0.049 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
High school−0.155 ***0.076 ***0.078 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
College and above−0.168 ***0.083 ***0.085 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Party membership−0.052 ***0.026 ***0.026 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Lnincome−0.0020.0010.001
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Community_average−0.064 ***0.031 ***0.032 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Community_good−0.139 ***0.069 ***0.070 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Note: The values in parentheses indicate the standard error of county/street level clustering; clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table A9. Marginal effects of social participation on waste-separation behavior.
Table A9. Marginal effects of social participation on waste-separation behavior.
Variables(1)(2)(3)
NeverOccasionallyOften
Participation index−0.023 **0.011 **0.011 *
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Age0.003 ***−0.001 ***−0.001 ***
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Male0.031 ***−0.015 ***−0.016 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Marry−0.0020.0010.001
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Primary education−0.041 **0.020 **0.021 **
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Junior high school−0.091 ***0.045 ***0.046 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
High school−0.150 ***0.073 ***0.075 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
College and above−0.164 ***0.081 ***0.083 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Party membership−0.049 ***0.024 ***0.025 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Lnincome−0.0020.0010.001
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Community_average−0.065 ***0.032 ***0.033 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Community_good−0.141 ***0.070 ***0.071 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Note: The values in parentheses indicate the standard error of county/street level clustering; clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

References

  1. Kaza, S.; Yao, L.C.; Bhada-Tata, P.; Van Woerden, F. What a Waste 2.0: A Global Snapshot of Solid Waste Management to 2050; Urban Development Series; World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2018; pp. 19–24. [Google Scholar]
  2. Hoornweg, D.; Bhada-Tata, P. What a Waste: A Global Review of Solid Waste Management; The World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  3. China Ministry of Ecology and Environment. Available online: http://www.mee.gov.cn (accessed on 5 January 2022).
  4. Meng, X.; Tan, X.; Wang, Y.; Wen, Z.; Tao, Y.; Qian, Y. Investigation on decision-making mechanism of residents’ household solid waste classification and recycling behaviors. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2019, 140, 224–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Nguyen, T.T.; Watanabe, T. Win-win outcomes in waste separation behavior in the rural area: A case study in vietnam. J. Clean. Prod. 2019, 230, 488–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rodić, L.; Wilson, D. Resolving Governance Issues to Achieve Priority Sustainable Development Goals Related to Solid Waste Management in Developing Countries. Sustainability 2017, 9, 404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Song, Q.; Li, J.; Zeng, X. Minimizing the increasing solid waste through zero waste strategy. J. Clean. Prod. 2015, 104, 199–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Varotto, A.; Spagnolli, A. Psychological strategies to promote household recycling. A systematic review with meta-analysis of validated field interventions. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 168–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Matter, A.; Dietschi, M.; Zurbrügg, C. Improving the informal recycling sector through segregation of waste in the household—The case of Dhaka Bangladesh. Habitat Int. 2013, 38, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chen, S.J.; Li, R.C.; Ma, Y.B. The paradox between willingness and behavior: Classification mechanism of urban residents on household waste. China Pop. Resour. Environ. 2015, 25, 168–176. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hoornweg, D.; Lam, P.; Chaudhry, M. Waste Management in China: Issues and Recommendations; The World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2005; pp. 1–163. [Google Scholar]
  12. Zhang, Y.; Huang, W. Analysis on the Effectiveness of the Input in Household Waste Classification of Residents—Taking S City in China as an Example. Sustainability 2021, 13, 11632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Chen, F.; Chen, H.; Guo, D.; Han, S.; Long, R. How to achieve a cooperative mechanism of MSW source separation among individuals—An analysis based on evolutionary game theory. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 195, 521–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Majeed, A.; Batool, S.A.; Chaudhry, M.N. Informal Waste Management in the Developing World: Economic Contribution Through Integration with the Formal Sector. Waste Biomass Valoriz. 2017, 8, 679–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Vollan, B.; Landmann, A.; Zhou, Y.; Hu, B.; Herrmann-Pillath, C. Cooperation and authoritarian values: An experimental study in China. Eur. Econ. Rev. 2017, 93, 90–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Knickmeyer, D. Social factors influencing household waste separation: A literature review on good practices to improve the recycling performance of urban areas. J. Clean. Prod. 2020, 245, 118605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fei, X. From the Soil: The Foundations of Chinese Society; The Joint Publishing Company: Shanghai, China, 1948. [Google Scholar]
  18. Gold, T.; Guthrie, D.; Wank, D. Social Connections in China: Institutions, Culture, and the Changing Nature of Guanxi; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  19. Anderson, C.; Schirmer, J. An Empirical Investigation of Social Capital and Networks at Local Scale through Resistance to Lower-Carbon Infrastructure. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2015, 28, 749–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pretty, J.; Ward, H. Social Capital and the Environment. World Dev. 2001, 29, 209–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hua, Y.; Dong, F.; Goodman, J. How to leverage the role of social capital in pro-environmental behavior: A case study of residents’ express waste recycling behavior in China. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 280, 124376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Han, H.; Zhang, Z.; Peng, W. Analysis of the influence mechanisms of social capital on the household waste separation be-havior. J. Zhejiang Univ. 2016, 46, 164–179. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  23. Zhu, Q. The impact of social capital on environmental behavior on straw disposal-based on the survey data of peasant households in L County, Hubei Province. J. Arid Land Resour. Environ. 2018, 32, 15–21. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  24. Jia, Y.; Zhao, M. The impact of waste pollution perception and social capital on peasant household waste separation—Based on 1374 peasant households survey data in Shaanxi Province. Resour. Sci. 2020, 42, 2370–2381. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  25. Li, W.; Wang, J. Household waste management in Shanghai and its implications for the second-tier cities in China. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 321, 128980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. He, X. Social interaction and public environmental behavior—An empirical analysis based on CGSS 2013. Soft Sci. 2016, 30, 4. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  27. Luo, H.; Zhao, L.; Zhang, Z. The impacts of social interaction-based factors on household waste-related behaviors. Waste Manag. 2020, 118, 270–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Shi, H.; Sui, D.; Wu, H.; Zhao, M. Influence of social capital on farmers’ participation in watershed ecological governance: Take the Heihe River basin as example. Chin. Rural Econ. 2018, 1, 34–45. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  29. Putnam, R.D.; Leonardi, D.R. Making democracy work: Civic traditions in modern Italy. Contemp. Sociol. 1994, 26, 306–308. [Google Scholar]
  30. Ostrom, E. Beyond Markets and States: Polycentric Governance of Complex Economic Systems. Am. Econ. Rev. 2010, 100, 641–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Ibrahim, M.; Law, S.H. Social capital and CO2 emission—output relations: A panel analysis. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2014, 29, 528–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Keene, A.; Deller, S.C. Evidence of the Environmental Kuznets’ Curve among US Counties and the Impact of Social Capital. Int. Reg. Sci. Rev. 2015, 38, 358–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Paudel, K.P.; Schafer, M.J. The Environmental Kuznets Curve Under a New Framework: The Role of Social Capital in Water Pollution. Environ. Resour. Econ. 2009, 42, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Liu, X. Empirical analysis of the impact of social capital on environmental governance performance in China. China Pop. Resour. Environ. 2011, 21, 20–24. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  35. Zheng, J.; Ma, G.; Wei, J.; Wei, W.; He, Y.; Jiao, Y.; Han, X. Evolutionary process of household waste separation behavior based on social networks. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2020, 161, 105009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Halkos, G.E.; Jones, N. Modeling the effect of social factors on improving biodiversity protection. Ecol. Econ. 2012, 78, 90–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Nguyen, T.T.P.; Zhu, D.; Le, N.P. Factors influencing waste separation intention of residential households in a developing country: Evidence from Hanoi, Vietnam. Habitat Int. 2015, 48, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Tu, C.; Rieger, M. Community-Driven Development and Social Capital: Evidence from Morocco. World Dev. 2017, 91, 28–52. [Google Scholar]
  39. Matsumoto, S. Do individuals free ride on participation in environmental policies? Personal values and waste management practices. Ecol. Econ. 2020, 174, 106669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Yan, J. Community governance and social capital investment: Theoretical explanation of China’s community governance in-novation. Tianjin Soc. Sci. 2010, 3, 59–64. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  41. Van Lange, P.A.; Joireman, J.; Parks, C.D.; Van Dijk, E. The psychology of social dilemmas: A review. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2013, 120, 125–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. De Cremer, D.; Snyder, M.; Dewitte, S. ‘The less I trust, the less I contribute (or not)?’ the effects of trust, accountability and self-monitoring in social dilemmas. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 31, 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ribeiro-Soriano, D.; del Mar Benavides-Espinosa, M.; Mohedano-Suanes, A. Cooperative Learning and Learning of Knowledge Through a Joint Venture: A Study from the Entrepreneurial Firm Perspective. Rev. Econ. 2013, 35, 67–85. [Google Scholar]
  44. Dai, X.; Han, Y.; Zhang, X.; Hu, W.; Huang, L.; Duan, W.; Li, S.; Liu, X.; Wang, Q. Comparison between students and residents on determinants of willingness to separate waste and waste separation behavior in Zhengzhou, China. Waste Manag. Res. 2017, 35, 949–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Babcock, H.M. Assuming personal responsibility for improving the environment: Moving toward a new environmental norm. Harv. Environ. Law Rev. 2009, 33, 117–175. [Google Scholar]
  46. Manuel, A.; Maria, A. Do you need a bag? Analyzing the consumption behavior of plastic bags of households in Ecuador. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2020, 152, 104489. [Google Scholar]
  47. Chen, L.; Gao, M. Novel information interaction rule for municipal household waste classification behavior based on an evolving scale-free network. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2021, 168, 105445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Xu, Z.; Zhang, J.; Chou, H. Research on the influence of reputation demand on farmers’ environmental behavior—Take the pollutant treatment method of poultry peasant households as example. China Popul. Resour. Environ. 2016, 26, 9. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  49. Enid, K.; Svetlana, E.; Melinda, S. Gender, Social Capital and Information Exchange in Rural Uganda. J. Int. Dev. 2008, 20, 35–52. [Google Scholar]
  50. Nainggolan, D.; Pedersen, A.B.; Smed, S.; Zemo, K.H.; Hasler, B.; Termansen, M. Consumers in a circular economy: Economic analysis of household waste seperation behavior. Ecol. Econ. 2019, 166, 106402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Rijal, S.; Huang, Y.H.; Lin, H.Y. An integrated approach to municipal solid waste recycling performance evaluation by in-corporating local demographic features. Sustainability 2021, 13, 10446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hansmann, R.; Laurenti, R.; Mehdi, T.; Binder, C.R. Determinants of pro-environmental behavior: A comparison of university students and staff from diverse faculties at a Swiss University. J. Clean. Prod. 2020, 268, 121864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Meyer, A. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 116, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Gradus, R.; Homsy, G.C.; Liao, L.; Warner, M.E. Which US municipalities adopt Pay-As-You-Throw and curbside recycling? Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2019, 143, 178–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Abel, T.; Guagnano, G.A.; Kalof, L. A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 1999, 6, 81. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kaiser, F.G.; Hübner, G.G.; Bogner, F.X. Contrasting the Theory of Planned Behavior with the Value-Belief-Norm Model in Explaining Conservation Behavior1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 35, 2150–2170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Blok, V.; Wesselink, R.; Studynka, O.; Kemp, R. Encouraging sustainability in the workplace: A survey on the pro-environmental behavior of university employees. J. Clean. Prod. 2015, 106, 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Shi, J.G.; Xu, K.; Si, H.; Song, L.; Duan, K. Investigating intention and behavior towards sorting household waste in Chinese rural and urban-rural integration areas. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 298, 126827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Schwartz, S.H. Are There Universal Aspects in the Structure and Contents of Human Values? J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 19–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Schwartz, S.H. Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 25, 1–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Dietz, T.; Stern, P.C.; Guagnano, G.A. Social Structural and Social Psychological Bases of Environmental Concern. Environ. Behav. 1998, 30, 450–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Karp, D.G. Values and their Effect on Pro-Environmental Behavior. Environ. Behav. 1996, 28, 111–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Wall, R.; Devine-Wright, P.; Mill, G.A. Comparing and combining theories to explain pro-environmental intentions: The case of commuting-mode choice. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 731–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Zhang, Z.; Wang, X.; Zhao, L. The influence of social capital on household waste reduction and its influence mechanism. J. Bus. Econ. 2019, 2, 85–97. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  65. Roodman, D. Fitting Fully Observed Recursive Mixed-Process Models with CMP. Stata J. Promot. Commun. Stat. Stata 2011, 11, 159–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Blanden, J.; Gregg, P.; Macmillan, L. Accounting for Intergenerational Income Persistence: Noncognitive Skills, Ability and Education. Econ. J. 2007, 117, C43–C60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Fairbrother, M. Trust and Public Support for Environmental Protection in Diverse National Contexts. Sociol. Sci. 2016, 3, 359–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Bluffstone, R.; Dannenberg, A.; Martinsson, P.; Jha, P.; Bista, R. Cooperative behavior and common pool resources: Experimental evidence from community forest user groups in Nepal. World Dev. 2020, 129, 104889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Sobel, M.E. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol. Methodol. 1982, 13, 290–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Ellison, G.; Fudenberg, D. Word-of-Mouth Communication and Social Learning. Q. J. Econ. 1995, 110, 93–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Li, Y.; Ye, F.; Sheu, C. Social capital, information sharing and performance: Evidence from China. Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 2014, 34, 1440–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The analytical framework of the social capital impact on waste-separation behavior.
Figure 1. The analytical framework of the social capital impact on waste-separation behavior.
Ijerph 19 03469 g001
Table 1. The results of factor analysis.
Table 1. The results of factor analysis.
Panel A: Social Capital
VariableOriginal Question in the QuestionnaireFactor LoadingKMOBartlett Test
Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3
Together_relativeHow often did you get together with relatives who don’t live with you over the last year?0.72−0.020.100.607637.4 ***
Together_friendHow often have you met with your friends over the past year?0.860.02−0.01
Activity_friendThe frequency of having entertainment activities with other friends?0.730.08−0.06
Trust_societyDo you agree that the majority of people in the society can be trusted?−0.030.820.11
Trust_strangerDo you trust the strangers in the society?0.090.80−0.10
VoteHave you voted in the last neighborhood committee/village committee election?−0.130.040.70
unionAre you a labor unionist?0.12−0.020.73
Panel B: Social networks
VariableOriginal question in the questionnaireFactor loadingKMOBartlett test
Factor 1
Together_relativeHow often did you get together with relatives who don’t live with you over the last year?0.720.606023.8 ***
Together_friendHow often you have met with your friends over the past year?0.86
Activity_friendThe frequency of having entertainment activities with other friends?0.75
Note: The data in Table 1 are all quoted from the CGSS2013; the data were positively processed; ***, **, * denotes significant levels at 1%, 5%, and 10%, respectively.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of variables in the model.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of variables in the model.
VariableVariable DescriptionsMeanStd. Dev.MinMax
Social networks
Together_relativeHow often did you get together with relatives who do not live with you over the last year?2.250.7215
Together_friendHow often have you met with your friends over the past year?2.410.9315
Activity_friendThe frequency of having entertainment activities with other friends?4.131.7817
Networks indexSocial networks index = factor loading × variance contribution ratio3.731.161.287.22
Social trust
Trust_societyDo you agree that the vast majority of people in the society can be trusted?3.281.0315
Trust_strangerDo you trust the strangers in society?2.590.9015
Trust indexAn ordered variable summed by subindexes from 0 to 83.881.5708
Social participation
VoteHave you voted in the last neighborhood/village committee elections?0.44-01
UnionAre you a labor union member?0.19-01
Participation indexSocial participation index0.54-01
Social
capital index
Social capital index = factor loading × variance contribution ratio2.730.840.825.54
SeparationThe frequency of waste-separation behavior in the past year (Never = 0, sometimes = 1, often = 2)0.570.7002
Ageage48.5616.371797
MaleMale = 1, female = 00.50-01
MarriedMarried with spouse = 1; other marital status = 00.79-01
Education level
IlliterateNo education = 1, otherwise = 00.14-01
Primary educationPrimary school = 1, otherwise = 00.21-01
Junior high schoolJunior high school = 1, otherwise = 00.29-01
High schoolHigh school = 1, otherwise = 00.19-01
College and aboveCollege and above = 1, otherwise = 00.16-01
Party membershipMember of Chinese Communist Party or eight democratic parties in China = 1, otherwise = 00.10-01
Lnincome (1)Yuan, log of annual household income per capita10.292.31016.12
Type of community
Community_poorRural communities.0.41-01
Community_averageOld city communities and new urban communities transformed from rural communities.0.25-01
Community_goodApartments, indemnificatory housings, common commercial housings, senior villa areas, and top-grade residential quarters.0.33-01
Parent_eduThe highest educational level of parents (years).4.954.85019
Social learning
DiscussionThe frequency of discussing environmental issues with relatives and friends?0.560.6302
Act_govThe frequency of participating in environmental activities organized by the government and affiliations?0.270.5302
Act_groupThe frequency of participating in environmental activities organized by private environmental groups?0.190.4502
KnowledgeThe correct number of ten environmental protection knowledge judgment questions.3.091.96010
ReputationDo you think it is important for you to be respected in society? (The demand degree ranging from low to high is divided into 8 options)2.460.9018
Notes: (1) Annual household income per capita is calculated by dividing total household income in a year by the number of people in the household who live with the respondent and are not economically independent of each other, and 57 samples have zero annual household income per capita; the variables in Table 2 are all quoted from the data of CGSS 2013. The mean and standard deviation of each variable were generated after summing up the 11,282 validated samples.
Table 3. Ordered probit model results of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
Table 3. Ordered probit model results of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Networks index0.070 ***
(0.01)
Trust index 0.020 **
(0.01)
Participation index 0.065 *
(0.03)
Social capital index 0.174 ***
(0.03)
Age−0.007 ***−0.008 ***−0.008 ***−0.012 ***
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Male−0.091 ***−0.088 ***−0.088 ***−0.150 ***
(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)(0.04)
Married0.0150.0130.0050.020
(0.03)(0.03)(0.03)(0.05)
Primary education0.124 **0.129 ***0.117 **0.174 **
(0.05)(0.05)(0.05)(0.08)
Junior high school0.264 ***0.276 ***0.259 ***0.411 ***
(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)(0.10)
High school0.424 ***0.445 ***0.428 ***0.683 ***
(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)(0.10)
College and above0.459 ***0.482 ***0.470 ***0.747 ***
(0.07)(0.07)(0.07)(0.12)
Party membership0.146 ***0.150 ***0.142 ***0.236 ***
(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.07)
Lnincome0.0030.0040.0050.009
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Community_average0.178 ***0.183 ***0.186 ***0.296 ***
(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)(0.10)
Community_good0.386 ***0.400 ***0.405 ***0.643 ***
(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)(0.10)
Province DummyYESYESYESYES
N11,28211,28211,28211,282
Pseudo R20.1000.0990.0990.104
Note: The values in parentheses indicate the standard error of county/street level clustering. Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 4. Ordered probit model results of marginal effects of social capital on waste separation behavior.
Table 4. Ordered probit model results of marginal effects of social capital on waste separation behavior.
(1)(2)(3)
NeverOccasionallyOften
Networks index−0.024 ***0.012 ***0.012 ***
(0.01)(0.00)c0)
Trust index−0.007 **0.003 **0.003 **
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Participation index−0.023 **0.011 **0.011 *
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Social capital index−0.036 ***0.020 ***0.016 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Age0.002 ***−0.001 ***−0.001 ***
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Male0.031 ***−0.017 ***−0.014 ***
(0.01)(0.00)(0.00)
Married−0.0040.0020.002
(0.01)(0.01)(0.00)
Primary school−0.036 **0.020 **0.016 **
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Junior high school−0.085 ***0.046 ***0.039 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
High school−0.142 ***0.077 ***0.065 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
College and above−0.155 ***0.084 ***0.071 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Party membership−0.049 ***0.027 ***0.022 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Lnincome−0.0020.0010.001
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Community_average−0.061 ***0.033 ***0.028 ***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Community_good−0.134 ***0.073 ***0.061 ***
(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)
Note: The marginal effects reported in Table 4 are based on the regression results in column (4) of Table 3, basically consistent with the marginal effects using social networks, social trust, and social participation as explanatory variables. The marginal effect results based on columns (1), (2) and (3) of Table 3 are presented in Appendix B. The values in parentheses indicate the standard error of town/sub-district level clustering. clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 5. Result of the instrumental variable regression.
Table 5. Result of the instrumental variable regression.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)
OLSCMP-probitCMP-oprobit2SLS-first2SLS-second
SeparationParticipation indexSeparationParticipation indexSeparation
Participation index0.036 ** 0.268 ** 2.602 **
(0.02) (0.15) (0.70)
Parent_edu 0.011 ** 0.009 ***
(0.00) (0.00)
ControlsYESYESYESYESYES
N11,28211,28211,28210,89010,890
R20.1770.1290.0990.159-
F-statistic---10.902-
Note: Values in parentheses indicate clustering standard errors. The control variables in the table are consistent with those in the baseline regression in Table 3. Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 6. Mediating effect of social learning: environmental protection activities.
Table 6. Mediating effect of social learning: environmental protection activities.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)
Dependent VariablesSeparationDiscussionSeparationAct_GovSeparationAct_GroupSeparation
Social capital index0.066 ***0.143 ***0.024 *0.103 ***0.049 ***0.095 ***0.051 ***
(0.01)(0.02)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.02)(0.01)
Discussion 0.293 ***
(0.01)
Act_gov 0.168 ***
(0.01)
Act_group 0.155 ***
(0.01)
ControlsYESYESYESYESYESYESYES
N11,28211,28211,28211,28211,28211,28211,282
R20.1760.1960.2450.1630.2000.1490.196
Note: Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 7. Mediating effect of social learning: environmental knowledge score.
Table 7. Mediating effect of social learning: environmental knowledge score.
(1)(2)(3)
SeparationKnowledgeSeparation
Social capital index0.066 ***0.241 ***0.054 ***
(0.01)(0.03)(0.01)
Knowledge 0.050 ***
(0.01)
ControlsYESYESYES
N11,28211,28211,282
R20.1760.2700.183
Note: Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 8. Mediating effect of reputation.
Table 8. Mediating effect of reputation.
(1)(2)(3)
SeparationReputationSeparation
Social capital index0.066 ***0.025 *0.065 ***
(0.01)(0.02)(0.01)
Reputation 0.050 ***
(0.01)
ControlsYESYESYES
N11,28211,28211,282
R20.1740.0250.178
Note: Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 9. The heterogeneous effects of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
Table 9. The heterogeneous effects of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)
Below 30(30, 40)(40, 50)(50, 60)Over 60
Networks index0.0350.074 **0.072 ***0.055 **0.075 ***
(0.03)(0.03)(0.03)(0.03)(0.02)
Trust index0.0060.0120.032 *0.043 **0.013
(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)
Participation index−0.0480.173 ***0.0840.0040.041
(0.07)(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)(0.06)
Social capital index0.0430.112 ***0.101 ***0.076 **0.110 ***
(0.05)(0.04)(0.03)(0.04)(0.03)
N18112046244221402843
Note: The control variables in Table 9 are the same as those in the baseline regression in Table 3. Clustered standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 10. The heterogeneous effects of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
Table 10. The heterogeneous effects of social capital on waste-separation behavior.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
UrbanRuralUrbanRuralUrbanRuralUrbanRural
Networks index0.059 ***0.088 ***
(0.02)(0.02)
Trust index 0.0150.030 **
(0.01)(0.02)
Participation index 0.093 **0.013
(0.04)(0.05)
Social capital index 0.087 ***0.121 ***
(0.03)(0.03)
ControlsYESYESYESYESYESYESYESYES
N67094573670945736709457367094573
Pseudo R20.0840.0530.0830.0500.0830.0500.0840.053
Note: Robust standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhou, Y.; Song, H.; Huang, X.; Chen, H.; Wei, W. How Does Social Capital Affect Residents’ Waste-Separation Behavior? Evidence from China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3469. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063469

AMA Style

Zhou Y, Song H, Huang X, Chen H, Wei W. How Does Social Capital Affect Residents’ Waste-Separation Behavior? Evidence from China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(6):3469. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063469

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhou, Yexin, Hongke Song, Xiaopei Huang, Hao Chen, and Wei Wei. 2022. "How Does Social Capital Affect Residents’ Waste-Separation Behavior? Evidence from China" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 6: 3469. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063469

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop