Next Article in Journal
Associations between Patient-Reported and Clinician-Reported Outcome Measures in Patients after Traumatic Injuries of the Lower Limb
Next Article in Special Issue
Culinary Solitude in the Diet of People with Functional Diversity
Previous Article in Journal
Age, Cognitive Factors, and Acceptance of Living with the Disease in Rheumatoid Arthritis: The Short-Term Perspective
Previous Article in Special Issue
In the Labyrinth of Dietary Patterns and Well-Being—When Eating Healthy Is Not Enough to Be Well
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Central Persons in Sustainable (Food) Consumption

by
Carolin V. Zorell
School of Humanities, Education and Social Sciences, Örebro University, 701 82 Örebro, Sweden
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(5), 3139; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19053139
Submission received: 3 January 2022 / Revised: 19 February 2022 / Accepted: 28 February 2022 / Published: 7 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Public Awareness of Food Products, Preferences and Practices)

Abstract

:
What people eat has become a highly political issue, closely intertwined with public health, environmental concerns, and climate change. Individuals’ consumption decisions tend to be greatly influenced by the people that surround them, and this seems to be especially true when it comes to food. In recent years, alongside close contacts, such as family and friends, a myriad of social influencers have appeared on the screens, sharing opinions on what (not) to eat. Presenting results from a youth survey conducted in Sweden in 2019 (N = 443), this paper shows that social media have become the primary source of information about food and eating for youths, followed by schools and families. However, primary sources of influence continue to be parents and the family at large. Furthermore, the study shows that it is possible to identify ‘central persons’, i.e., relatively clear-cut groups of people whose food choices—measured as tendency to eat climate friendly—is mirrored by the youths, both in their everyday food preferences and in their broader political awareness as expressed through political consumerism. A conclusion from this is that certain people can be particularly successful at inspiring larger numbers of other people to engage with healthier and environmentally friendlier (food) consumption in a society.

1. Introduction

What food is consumed, in what way, and with whom, has fundamentally changed over the last decades. On average, today individuals eat more [1], more out of home [2], and more sugary, processed, and in general unhealthy food [1]. This is a major and continuously increasing problem, causing a rise in preventable diseases and premature deaths in countries worldwide [3,4]. Likewise, it is causing great harm to the environment and climate [3,5,6]. The food sector has thus been identified as one of the most important sectors to be urgently changed in order to reach sustainability goals and climate targets [3,5,6].
A key ambition stated across countries is therefore to encourage citizens to choose more ecological, climate-friendly, and healthy food. Especially children and adolescents seem to have been identified as key target group for such endeavour [7,8,9]. They are the adults of tomorrow, and thus whether they develop environmentally- and climate-friendly food habits today will determine the food sector’s long-term success at performing within the Earths’ “planetary boundaries” [10,11].
A common finding is that food choices are related to individual-level factors (e.g., attitudes, tastes, biological and demographic factors) and purchasing contexts (e.g., food availability). It is also increasingly recognised that food choices and behaviours are very much related to what peers and parents eat and drink [12,13], and, in a broader sense, to those sharing a social identity [14,15]. However, in a massively growing media landscape, the sources of information and the ways of communicating about food have changed fundamentally. B/vlogs, YouTube channels, (pseudo-)documentaries, social media posts, and food influencers have mushroomed, spreading recommendations about what to eat or not to eat. Digital spheres seem to have become an ever more important source of information about food, diets, and their implications for both health and the environment, especially—though by no means exclusively—for adolescents and young adults [16,17].
Hence, the number of potential sources of influence on when, where, and what a person eats seems to have exploded. However, just because people get information from certain sources, this must not necessarily mean that it translates into behaviour [18]. Initial studies suggest that digital media influence food choice and intake of children and youths [16,19]. How their influence compares to non-digital sources, such as parents or friends, remains unclear though. A strand of literature in social psychology and economics highlights that certain people have more influence on their peers than others in making them adopt certain attitudes or behaviours. Crucially, these studies suggest that on the basis of certain relationship characteristics or attributes, one can identify such “central persons” [20] or “social referents” [21]. Additionally, having such central persons changing behaviour and disseminating information about it seems to effectively achieve behaviour change of individuals and in larger populations [20,21].
This paper aims to study if such ‘central persons’ can also be identified in the context of sustainable consumption and eating (meaning, ethical, environmentally- and climate-friendly). This is done through addressing three sets of questions which stem from the above-mentioned:
RQ1: To what degree do digital sources of information about food influence the food choices of youths, and how does their influence compare to that of close ones such as parents or friends?
RQ2: Can the idea of ‘central persons’ be applied to the sphere of food, that is, can we identify certain kinds of people that are particularly central to (youths’) everyday food choices and influence what they eat, and if so, how can they be identified?
From this, a third question follows. As described above, food is tied to great socio-political challenges. Analogous, eating ethical, ecological, and ‘climate-friendly’ food has become political, expressed among other things through so-called political consumerism [22,23]. Additionally, the individual responsibility to be considerate about the wider societal and environmental implications of consumption choices is hotly debated, highlighted, and gradually acknowledged across spheres and people [24]. This raises the question:
RQ3: Can the behaviours and recommendations of central persons be ‘catalysts’ of a broader political engagement, such as boycotting and buycotting products for ethical, environmental, or other political reasons?
In keeping with this last question, the paper focuses on environmental and climate aspects of food. To investigate into the various questions, it uses data from a high school survey conducted in Sweden in 2019 (N = 443). Next to questions about everyday food habits and sources of information about food, health, and ecology/climate, it contains an original measure to identify ‘central’ persons and their perceived eating habits. However, before presenting the data, methods, and results in detail, the paper starts with a review of current knowledge on the roots of eating patterns and food influence(r)s. Afterwards, it proceeds with presenting the empirical results. The paper concludes by discussing its contribution to the understanding of the role of social influence(r)s in defining eating patterns in the context of acute needs for more ecological and, generally, sustainable food choices.

2. Theoretical Background

2.1. Food Information and Influence

Individual preferences, attitudes, tastes, biological traits, and demographic backgrounds play a major role in determining when, what, and how much a person eats. Considerations such as convenience, price, taste, certain dietary requirements, health objectives or environmental values can guide what an individual is interested in knowing about food, where they search for information about food, and what they eventually buy and eat [13,25]. However, there are multiple other sources from the social and physical environments which inform and direct individual food choices [26,27,28,29,30,31,32] (see for a review [12]).
Parents play a particularly crucial role. By way of how and what parents provide for eating, what they encourage (not) to eat, and what they eat themselves, parents influence what their children eat, from early childhood throughout adolescence into adulthood [2,33,34,35,36]. Peer groups are another place where individuals discuss, get informed about, and mimic food preferences and habits. What and how much peers eat and weigh, especially close friends, has been shown to be tightly correlated with individual eating habits and weight [32,37,38] (but see also [33]). This points to much time being spent together consuming food, possibly talking about food, and influencing each other’s food habits.
A third important factor shaping where and how food is purchased and consumed is physical and socio-cultural contexts. Such contexts cultivate socio-cultural norms, values, and traditions, and they typically build on certain policy and regulatory frameworks. These shape what is produced and imported in a country [12] and how individuals and media communicate about food (e.g., through advertising laws [39,40]). Thus, they determine what kind of food is made available, in which quantities, and what is socially accepted and encouraged to buy and eat (e.g., [40]). Furthermore, cultural belongings and social identities can influence, e.g., customs and tastes, and thus choices [14].
In modern everyday lives, these individual predispositions and immediate social and physical influences compete with multiple other aspects. In print and digital media, schools, and other outlets, individuals encounter a wide array of news and information about impacts of food on health and the environment. Some advocate certain diets for reasons of, e.g., attaining a certain body shape or look, or on how to ‘comfort’ the self through food. Others connect the advertised foods or diets to certain social images and groups [41], conveying the impression that for becoming or remaining a certain person or part of a social group, one should eat this or that [42]. Simultaneously, food advertisements surround individuals everywhere, most commonly for snacks and food that can be considered unhealthy [19,43,44,45]. Given their usually processed and plastic-packaged nature, they typically cannot be considered environmentally friendly either. Taking the multitude of messages about diets and lifestyles together, they are often contradictory and give rise to conflicting recommendations, desires, and needs.
As it becomes difficult to ascertain what food choice is the ‘right’ one, individuals find themselves in recurrent situations of uncertainty. Here, they likely look for orientation. Numerous studies show that exposure to advertisements, recommendations, and to less obvious marketing of products, especially in social media, has a discernible impact on attitudes towards the products and the likelihood to consume them [42,46,47], see also [12] (p. S66). This is found even when individuals disapprove or are unwilling to acknowledge such influence [47]. The interaction between different users in their roles as individuals seems to be an important driver of such influences [48,49]. In a recent randomised experimental study, Coates and colleagues [16] show how seeing social media influencers that eat unhealthy food successfully purports consumption of unhealthy food (measured in terms of calories) among children. Other research comes to the same conclusion. Media and advertisements, including user-generated content, predominantly focus on food that can be considered unhealthy; and, as a whole, it is mainly the intake of unhealthy food which is purported successfully in (social) media [43,50]. The information and messages surrounding an individual in these spheres thus seem to serve as anchors that give orientation in the decision what to eat.
Research yet also suggests that for being granted attention and successfully influencing an audience, individuals, brands, and their relationship towards a person need to satisfy certain criteria. For instance, they need to be able to raise feelings of accessibility and a “warm personal relationship” [49] (p. 5). Additionally, the ability to establish and maintain a trusting relationship seems to be of crucial importance [51], which can enable and be enabled through interpersonal contacts [52]. Correspondingly, personal closeness (family, close friends) and trust in the counterparts’ competence in food matters (schools, teachers, doctors) play a key role in developing food preferences and choice [35,53].
However, social media very much build on such principles, too. Certain styles of language and images are used to raise feelings of closeness and shared social identities, with the ambition to influence peers and followers. Groups are created (e.g., in Facebook) or imagined (e.g., the following community of an Instagram influencer) and some seem to influence thousands (or millions) of followers to adopt certain diets, try recipes, or purchase certain (food) products.
A general contention thus is that social media is increasingly outdoing parents and other close ones as sources of information and influence about what to eat, especially for young people. Yet, how the various ‘channels’ of information and potential influence fare compared to each other remains largely uncharted. The knowledge gap is even greater when one includes other sources that spread information about food, health, or climate-friendly eating, such as TV, magazines, and civil society actors. One may agree with the contention and expect that (hypothesis h1) social media have become the most important source of information about food. Yet, parents, schools, and friends are the daily interaction partners of youths. An alternative hypothesis thus is that (ha1) parents, friends, and schools are the most important sources of information about food.

2.2. Central Persons and Food Habits

Transmitting information to people does not necessarily entail getting them to do something [54]. Some researchers studying social influence and imitation differentiate between different kinds of persons and strengths of influence. As they highlight, some persons are more influential—or ‘central’—in opinion and behaviour formation than others are [21,54,55,56,57].
Crucially, Banjeree and colleagues [20] show that it is possible to identify concrete persons in a community who are better at spreading messages (e.g., gossips) and calls for action (i.e., getting people to get inoculated). Similarly, Paluck and her colleagues [21,56,58] could identify certain individuals that were more apt than others at instigating changes in attitudes and behaviours (in their case, harassment behaviour at schools) by changing perceptions of collective norms. Food consumption and the development and change of food preferences and habits tend to occur in social settings. Hence, the food individuals choose in their everyday lives may be similarly influenced.
Importantly, observing ‘central persons’ engaging with aspects such as ecology and climate change in relation to their food choices could motivate an individual—be it an adolescent or an adult—to do the same. Central persons can work as a source of information about food, food habits, and food norms in general, as well as about sustainable and climate-friendly eating in particular. In conversations and as living examples, they may inspire others to reflect upon their food habits, and to try and adopt new ones. In other words, observing central persons engaging with sustainable consumption and eating, e.g., eating climate friendly, could be an effective way through which more people get drawn into sustainable consumption.
Hence, the insights by Banjeree, Paluck, and their colleagues suggest that some people may be more powerful at influencing others’ food preferences than others, and with this, at drawing people into sustainable eating. Hence, hypothesis 2 is that (h2) it is possible to identify certain kinds of persons that are more ‘central’ than others at influencing people’s food preferences.
Moreover, this could work as an entry point into broader engagements with politics. By raising interest in the political implications of food, it may stir a broader socio-political awareness. In this vein, political scientists have consistently shown that political participation—mainly studied in terms of voting—is connected to what surrounding people say and do (e.g., [59,60,61,62,63]). Correspondingly, what central persons eat may not only affect individual food preferences. If individuals perceive their central person focus on political issues such as climate friendliness of food, they may become interested in politics in a broader sense. That is, they may engage with the politics of consumption, e.g., through deliberately choosing to buy or boycotting certain foods, diets, and other products for ecological, ethical, or other political reasons [64,65]. Put simply, a last hypothesis is that (h3) an individual’s boycotting and buycotting engagement correlates with the perceived tendency of their central persons to ground food choices on political considerations.

3. Materials and Methods

The subsequent empirical study uses data collected as part of a survey of Swedish pupils in their last semester at senior high in 2019. The selection of schools was guided by a combined sample strategy of strategic sampling, where the aim was to cover classes with different subject specialisations (natural sciences, social sciences), orientations (aiming for further education or practical work after school), and from towns of different sizes in mid-Sweden (five in total, ranging from Stockholm as a big city, to Hallsberg municipality as a small town area). Yet, regardless of this strategic approach, the sample needs to be seen as a convenience sample.
Before the survey, the teachers made available the information about the study aim and ethical issues in their school’s internal information system, so the pupils had time to go through the material. Data collection took place in the classrooms, for anonymity and quality reasons without the teachers. Trained study assistants distributed again information about the study and ethics and collected informed consent before students would receive a link to the electronic questionnaire (a few answered in paper format). The assistants remained in the classroom while the students filled in the questionnaire, which took about 90 min (with a break in between). The students did not get any reward for answering the questionnaire. Those who did not want to participate did schoolwork instead. The final sample consisted of 443 pupils aged 16–22; this excludes ca. 40 respondents who provided answers that gave reason to suspect systematic misreporting. In total, 59% of the respondents were girls, 39% boys, and 1% youth identifying with another gender.
To measure the most important source of information (hypothesis 1), the youths were asked from where they usually get information about food, eating, and ecology/climate. To this end, they were presented a list of six kinds of social ties and media (family, friends, school, social media, TV, newspaper) plus an open answer field and asked to assign to each of them a rank describing to what extent they usually get information about food from that source. Given the setup of the questionnaire, the participants assigned to each of the seven potential sources of information a number between 1 (least usual) and 7 (most usual). This resulted in seven variables with seven categories, i.e., scale values. For each value, the frequency of responses indicates the number of people who ranked the source of information on the respective rank. Comparing these frequencies permits drawing conclusions about the comparative importance of the distinct kinds of social ties and media as sources of information for the youth. However, in many cases, the ranking was not entirely ‘neatly’ done, but respondents assigned same ranks to more than one source and no source to single ranks. Since the focus of measurement is on identifying the most important source of information, the analysis focuses on the frequency to which each source was ranked first (i.e., as main/most important); if single individuals ranked more than one with “1”, all of them are counted as “most important”. This renders a comparative assessment of the various sources’ importance.
The dependent variable for hypothesis 2 grounds on a validated questionnaire capturing everyday food preferences. It asks respondents what is important to them that the food they eat on a typical day fulfils. Through 24 response items, individuals can indicate on a 7-point scale whether a trait is not at all (1) to very (7) important to them [66,67,68]. The items include price, various health-related, sensory, and popularity aspects, as well as ecological and ethical criteria. A factor analysis provides five distinct factors, of which one discernibly relates to ‘sustainable’ eating (Cronbach’s α = 0.934; see Appendix A). It covers that the food should be organic, fair traded, produced and packaged environmentally friendly, be in harmony with one’s ethical values, and climate friendly. On its basis, an additive index is created that captures the degree to which an individual puts weight on the fact that their everyday food choices are ‘sustainable’.
To capture central persons, a measure was adapted from Åmna and colleagues [69] (see Appendix B). Respondents were asked whether certain kinds of persons have a greater influence on their food choices than others, and if so, to freely mention one or two such persons. To make the question simpler to understand, the wording further specified “who this person is, for example a person [they] follow on social media, [their] best friend, [their] mom or dad, a celebrity, the leader of an organization”.
As a second independent variable (climate-friendly eating of central persons), the youths were asked if and to what extent one or the two of these persons themselves “buy/eat climate-friendly food”. Here, the answer options included (1) “No, they do not”, “Yes, one does so (2) sporadically/(3) most of the time”, “Yes, the two do so (4) sporadically/(5) most of the time”, as well as a (6) “I do not know” (see also Appendix B). Despite the question referring to the prior question, examining the data revealed that the youths did not necessarily refrain from answering this question despite previously having answered that there is no central person. In total, 19 individuals answered that one or two central persons are eating climate friendly, despite not having named any central person beforehand. This might be due to the additional effort that writing down a central person implies (first question), whereas ticking a box (second question) requires less cognitive effort. A check showed that these responses do not change the general conclusions. Nonetheless, in the following analysis the two variables are treated as separated from each other.
For hypothesis 3, two questions gauged the regularity of political consumerism. Adapted from commonly employed measures [63] (p. 9), they ask how often it occurred in the last 12 months that the person (1) boycotted and (2) bought food or other products for ecological or ethical reasons. Respondents then indicated on a 6-point scale whether they did so never (1) to every day (6). The boycotting and buycotting variables are correlated (rp = 0.546; p = 0.000; N = 432), yet there are differences across individuals and thus the two are considered as separate dependent variables.
Finally, age (measured in years of age) and gender (girl (0), boy (1), other (0)) were included in all models as control variables. Table 1 summarises the descriptive statistics for all variables.
The methodological approach starts with descriptive and correlation analysis. This is combined with non-parametric group tests (Kruskal–Wallis H test) to compare groups of people with and without central persons that eat in a certain way (i.e., climate friendly).

4. Results

4.1. Identifying Central Persons Influencing Food Preferences

To map the terrain, the enquiry starts with a look at the principle factors that the youths mention as guiding their food choices. Figure 1 reports the mean values for the five overarching categories (which, according to a non-parametric Friedman test, are statistically different from each other; χ2(4) = 492.77, p < 0.001). The principal factor are sensory and emotional aspects, i.e., taste, texture, and mood. This is closely followed by price and, with some distance, health criteria. All three are not different for girls and boys. For the third gender, given the small number of respondents, a discussion would be speculative in character and is therefore excluded from this comparison.
Ecological criteria range lowest in influence on food choice. However, they are notably different between girls and boys. Girls show a greater tendency to consider sustainability important than boys. This difference fits in with multiple other studies on environmental behaviours, showing that across all age groups, women tend to be more considerate of ecological and ethical consumption aspects than men (even though the gap has narrowed over the past years; cf. [70] p. 132). Overall, however, it seems clear that the issue of food choices remains complex, and the youths are far from all being “Greta”, that is, young environmental activists who have changed their lifestyles entirely.
Such differences in the priorities can be rooted in various kinds of personal circumstances. A main one as suggested in this paper is where individuals get information about food and the people with whom they interact discussing food-related matters. Looking into this question, Figure 2 summarises the ranking provided by the respondents of the sources through which they usually receive information about “food, eating, and climate matters”. Their comparative ranking is interpreted as an indication of their comparative importance; that is, the sources were the youths state to get information most (usually) are here understood as being the most important sources of information.
Visibly, social media headed the ranking with a mean value of 5.16 on a 7-point scale. With notable distance, this is followed by the family and school. After this, TV and friends follow. Newspapers and other sources rank as least usual sources of information. Some respondents also specified what they mean with “other” sources; those named by several respondents include statistics, (online) documentaries, and podcasts. A single respondent named politicians. According to a non-parametric Friedman test, these mean values are different from each other to a statistically significant extent (χ2(6) = 189.87, p < 0.001).
Digital media outlets, especially social media, are thus the principal ‘channel’ through which youths receive information about matters relating to food. This confirms hypothesis 1. Of the non-remote sources, schools, teachers, and the family can be considered important, while friends less so. Overall, however, they do not seem to be the most important sources of information about food, which disproves the alternative hypothesis (ha1).
Influencers on social media and teachers at school may advocate certain healthy diets. However, this does not automatically and necessarily mean that the receivers of the messages adopt their dietary recommendations. This leads to the question whether there are certain kinds of people who are particularly central at influencing a person’s everyday food choices. This can be gauged in two ways. By directly asking about the existence of central persons; and indirectly, by observing if a potentially central person has an influence on what a person eats.
The survey includes a direct question. Furthermore, while it provides only cross-sectional data, which does not allow for studying a causal connection between eating patterns, the questionnaire includes questions that allow for comparing the eating patterns of a group of youth whose central persons eat climate friendly with that of a group whose central persons do not eat climate friendly. This way, we can study if the patterns of climate-friendly eating of central persons correspond with the eating patterns of the respondents.
About 38% of the youth state that there is a specific person that has especially affected what they buy and choose when it comes to food. Correspondingly, a majority states that they cannot name anyone that has a central role in influencing their food choices. While this does not necessarily mean that there is no one affecting their choices, it yet means that they have no one who consciously and distinctively affects them, and who can be named off the top of their head.
Figure 3 reports the sorted answers to the open question about who the central persons are (i.e., responses given by 38% of respondents). Instagram, influencers, etc., are not directly grouped under the label of social media to illustrate the variation in answers. Besides, it gives an impression of the various sub-types and variety that the label ‘social media’ actually covers.
The graph renders a relatively clear picture, which slightly diverges from the results regarding sources of information. When it comes to actual influence on food choice, it seems that family members, and especially parents, range similarly high in importance as social media, even after subsuming the different sub-types of social media into one category. Hence, parents may not be considered primary sources of information about food, but they are primary sources of influence. Admittedly, this is not entirely surprising as most youths live at home (91% of the respondents), and hence may find their parents choosing what food is bought and served to begin with. This also likely influences the food tastes and habits in situations where the youths make choices on their own. However, parents are also central for those who do not live at home, thus pointing to an additional kind of influence besides that of owning the decision-making authority.
Best friends are important too, ranging on the same level as influencers. Schools and teachers, in turn, seem to be important sources of information, but according to this descriptive result, they appear to lack influence on what is eaten.
Table 2 reports the responses to whether one or two central persons eat climate friendly. A clear majority reports perceiving at least one doing so. What is more, they are perceived to be doing it more regularly than sometimes. This is surprising, since it suggests a disproportionately large fraction of people eating climate friendly. An alternative reason may be that the respondents observe it consciously precisely because it stands out. In any of the two cases, the figures should not compromise the later conclusions, but they will be kept in mind when drawing general conclusions from the results.
According to the theory of central persons, if an individual perceives that their central person(s) focuses on something such as the climate friendliness of what they eat, the individual would be expected to reproduce it in their everyday food choices. To test this theory, one can look at the overlap between the frequency with which an individual perceives their central person(s) is (are) grounding food choices on certain aspects (i.e., climate-friendly eating) and the individual’s own frequency of doing so. The variable assessing perceived central persons’ climate-friendly eating is treated as an ordinal variable in the analysis, i.e., the influence of two persons eating sometimes climate friendly is considered to be stronger than one person doing so regularly. This assumption is based on research into social influence and behaviour spread, suggesting that several influences tend to reinforce each other and therewith strengthen the influencing effect [54]).
The test statistics of a Kruskal–Wallis H test indicate that there is a statistically significant difference in the median focus that respondents put on sustainability aspects of their food depending on the degree to which they perceive their central persons to be doing so (i.e., the independent variable as reported in Table 2) (χ2(4) = 24.934, p < 0.001). Hence, the youths’ focus on sustainable eating seems to indeed vary along with what they perceive their central persons are doing. Non-parametric correlation analysis further specifies that what central persons are perceived to be doing is positively related to what the youths themselves do (rτ = 0.235, p < 0.001, N = 194). However, when differentiating according to gender, the effect is again different among girls and boys. For girls, the correlation coefficient does not reach statistical significance (rτ = 0.099, p = 0.163, N = 120), while for boys it is moderately strong (rτ = 0.312, p < 0.001, N = 69).
A bar chart visualises this difference (Figure 4). Girls’ inclination to consider it important that their everyday food choices are sustainable is high throughout. The mean values are slightly higher among those reporting that one or two of the central persons are eating climate friendly on a regular basis. Yet, the confidence intervals overlap with the other categories. Thus, their inclination seems to be relatively independent of what their central persons are perceived to be doing. In contrast, boys who do not perceive any central person to be eating climate friendly do not either. Yet, if one or two central persons sometime or regularly eat climate friendly, they tend to do so as well. Moreover, the inclination increases quasi-linear along with the increased number and frequency of central persons eating climate friendly (although the confidence intervals overlap, suggesting that this last interpretation must be taken with caution).
In sum, the results suggest that there are central persons. These central persons seem to belong to relatively clear-cut groups of people, who can be characterised by a distinct type of affiliation to the individual (e.g., family, friends, social media influencer). Yet, perceived centrality does not necessarily mean behavioural centrality. For girls, influence seems to happen mainly if central persons eat climate friendly on a notable, i.e., regular basis. Boys, in turn, the more frequently they perceive their central persons to be grounding food choices on climate considerations, the more frequent they ground their everyday food choices on such criteria as well. This association can be interpreted as a potential influence coming from the central person, which allows for confirming hypothesis 2.

4.2. Central Persons and Political Consumerism

The last hypothesis (h3) states that what central persons are perceived to be doing can affect more than eating habits. If they are perceived to be grounding food choices on climate considerations, this may get the ones to whom they are central to engage with the politics of food and consumption more widely. In other words, central persons’ behaviour may instigate broader political awareness and motivate political engagements, especially political consumerism. Two dependent variables are used to measure such engagement: boycotting and buycotting. The independent variable is the degrees of climate-friendly eating of central persons.
The results of a Kruskal–Wallis H test indicate that there is a statistically significant difference in buycotting between the youth within the five different degrees of perceived climate-friendly eating of their central persons (χ2(4) = 43.370, p < 0.000). Non-parametric correlation analysis further points to a positive relationship, for both girls (rτ = 0.212, p = 0.006, N = 120) and boys (rτ = 0.431, p < 0.000, N = 67). For boycotting, the group test also reveals a statistically significant difference between the five degrees of what central persons are perceived to be doing (χ2(4) = 19.122, p < 0.000). Yet, according to correlation analysis, this holds only for boys (rτ = 0.273, p = 0.006, N = 66), while not for girls (rτ = 0.100, p = 0.187, N = 120).
Figure 5 specifies the patterns. Girls’ tendency to buycott is connected to central persons’ engagement with climate-friendly eating if the central persons do so regularly, but not if they are perceived to be doing so only sometimes or not at all. An explanation may be that the baseline for girls is higher as they have generally a higher tendency towards being already engaged with the politics of food and consumption. Hence, they may note and be influenced only by those cases that stand out through great regularity of climate-friendly consumption. Girls’ boycotting engagement does not appear to be related to what their central persons do.
Conversely, boys’ engagement with buycotting is more closely related to what their central persons are perceived to be doing. If they are perceived to never eat climate friendly, the boys do the same. If they are perceived to focus on climate-friendly eating sporadically or frequently, the boys respectively buycott sporadically or frequently. Boycotting, however, seems to need a stronger engagement on the central persons’ side to uncover an effect. That is, a potential influence seems to be observable only if central persons, be it one or two, are perceived to regularly engage with climate-friendly eating. Overall, it seems reasonable to confirm the part of hypothesis 3 relating to buycotting. The youths’ buycotting engagement is closely related to what they observe of their central persons, and this is especially strong for boys. Boycotting, in turn, seems to be related only for boys and only provided central persons send strong signals through regular engagements with the politics of eating.

5. Conclusions

The consumption of unhealthy and unecological food is a major and continuously increasing problem, causing a rise in preventable diseases, premature deaths, and ecological disasters in countries worldwide [3,4,5,6]. With the ambition to contribute knowledge to advance ambitions to get citizens choose more ecological food, this paper provides insights which suggest that gaining certain kinds of people adopting climate-friendly diets can act as multiplier inspiring other people to do the same.
In a study of Swedish high school students, the paper could identify relatively clear-cut groups of people who show to be particularly central for what youths prefer to eat. These groups are characterised by a distinct type of affiliation to the individual: the family, friends, or influencer on social media. Depending on whether these central persons are seen to eat climate friendly or not, the youth respectively consider environmental and ethical aspects important in their food choices as well, or they do not. This influence of central persons seems to be particularly great on boys, and although to a lesser extent, also on girls. Moreover, their influence seems to extend beyond everyday food habits and come along with broadened political awareness that is expressed through political consumerism.
From a more general perspective, adults tend to like to see themselves as less vulnerable to others’ influences, yet research suggests otherwise (e.g., [48,71]). Hence, while these findings rest on a youth sample, we can expect that similar tendencies would be observable in other age groups. Nonetheless, a limitation is that the youth may not always buy food for themselves and need to eat what is served. This can imply that we are observing a larger influence of especially parents in this study as what would be observed in a study of the general population. In addition, it carries the possibility that there are various kinds of influence observed in the present study, again especially of parents on their kids, as they may not only ‘inspire’ their kids towards certain food choices, but also choose the food they get to begin with. Future studies, therefore, need to explore more in detail what happens within families, and to what extent the influence of the family is temporary.
The findings illustrate two other things. First, there seems to be an important distinction between sources of information and sources of influence. Schools and teachers, for example, are important sources of information for the youth, while they seem to have little actual influence on food choice. The reverse holds for parents and friends. Social media, in turn, fare high on both aspects, but more so on the provision of information.
A common contention is that social media are more relevant in youths’ lives than in that of adults. However, the findings suggest that even among the youth, social media influence seems to be on average lower than that of family and friends. This allows for concluding that findings regarding influence may be quite similar in a study of the general population. That is, close ties such as family and friends are principal influencers. This includes the possibility of a recursive link where youths influence their parents.
The findings further suggest that to successfully promote changes in diets, it is important to focus on addressing and convincing those who can actually influence others to change what they eat, i.e., central persons, rather than only disseminate information through, e.g., leaflets and educational programmes. The paper also provides a methodological tool for it: one can identify certain kinds of ‘central’ people based on easily observable characteristics (e.g., being a parent or a v/blogger). Hence, knowing about such ties can work as a methodological shortcut to identify persons who can influence eating patterns particularly well. Campaigns and broader strategies aimed at promoting ecological and healthy eating may then not need to start each time by categorising and assessing each individual’s very personal conception of their social influencers. Instead, they can build on those easily identifiable relationship characteristics and explicitly target those groups, who can act as multipliers.
Second, the observations underscore how supposedly small everyday actions such as food activism can multiply and trigger broader engagements with the politics of consumption, climate change, and political issues more in general. Thinking one step further, this process may not only unfold at the level of the individual who engages in gradually more ways with such issues, but also at the level of a society, where engagements of some can ‘tip over’ to others, inspiring them to engage with the politics of, e.g., food and climate change. Future studies may apply social network approaches to dive deeper into this idea and study whether the influence of certain people can especially effectively advance sustainable consumption in a larger population.
These future studies may then also cover aspects which represent limitations of this study: the cross-sectional nature of the data, the small sample size, and the focus on youths and only one country. Additionally, there is the possibility of reporting bias due to the potential desire of respondents to put themselves in a better light when reporting what they care for and tend to consume. Future studies may include more systematic measures to control for such biases. Likewise, they may focus on further age groups (also to study who are the central persons of those central persons) and include more socio-demographic and economic background variables, as well as longitudinal measures.
This paper made a first effort to compare roles of different kinds of people in the process of changing consumption patterns. It reconfirms that eating is deeply connected to social environments. Yet, it adds the insight that not all social ties are equally relevant. Additionally, while the findings are based on a youth sample, there are reasons to expect that it also applies to adults. This is an invaluable insight for those aiming to transform current food consumption patterns. Specifically, gaining central persons for the purpose may be a key to reach social tipping points at which ‘sustainable’ consumption choices become the ‘new normal’.

Funding

The collection of data for this research was funded by the Swedish Research Council for Sustainable Development Formas through project 2017-00880, led by Maria Ojala (Örebro University).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Swedish National Ethics Committee national ethics committee under protocol number 2019-03857.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank the participants of the 2021 paper workshop from the Centre for Environmental and Sustainability Social Science (CESSS) at Örebro University for their valuable comments. Furthermore, my special thanks go to Maria Ojala, for offering me to add questions to her survey study.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Factor analysis for important aspects in everyday food consumption. Principal Component Analysis. Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. Only loadings > 0.400 are shown. KMO: 0.906, Chi-sq.: 5,484,406. Explained Variance: 69.40%, Cronbach’s Alpha: 0.934, 0.895, 0.731, 0.492, and 0.431, for components 1–5, respectively. Components 1 and 2 are moderately-strong correlated (0.504), while all other correlations range between 0.043 (components 1 and 4) and 0.293 (components 2 and 5).
Table A1. Factor analysis for important aspects in everyday food consumption. Principal Component Analysis. Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. Only loadings > 0.400 are shown. KMO: 0.906, Chi-sq.: 5,484,406. Explained Variance: 69.40%, Cronbach’s Alpha: 0.934, 0.895, 0.731, 0.492, and 0.431, for components 1–5, respectively. Components 1 and 2 are moderately-strong correlated (0.504), while all other correlations range between 0.043 (components 1 and 4) and 0.293 (components 2 and 5).
Component
SustainableHealthyPriceConvenienceSensationsMood
Produced in an environmentally friendly way0.894
Climate friendly0.885
Packaged in an environmentally friendly way0.868
Environmentally friendly0.849
Organic0.848
Fairtrade0.810
Animal friendly0.712
In line with my ethical values0.661
Natural0.469
Nutritious 0.827
Much protein 0.816
Keeps me healthy 0.809
Much fibre 0.806
Vitamins and minerals 0.780
Good for the teeth/
hair/nails etc.
0.690
Healthy 0.639
Helps me control my weight 0.506
Not too expensive 0.851
Affordable 0.833
Well-known 0.805
Convenient/simple 0.649
Pleasant sensations 0.805
Tasty 0.739
Mood 0.869

Appendix B

Questions gauging central persons:
Q1: Is there someone who has particularly influenced what you buy and choose when it comes to food and drink? If so, please state who this person is, e.g., a person you follow on social media, your best friend, your mom or dad, a celebrity, the leader of an organisation.
Please specify one or two of the most important persons.
0 = No one
1 = Yes, my (1) __________________________
(2) __________________________
Q2: If you have specified at least one person, does any of them buy/eat climate friendly food?
1 = No, they do not
2 = Yes, one does so sporadically
3 = Yes, one does so most of the time
4 = Yes, the two do so sporadically
5 = Yes, the two do so most of the time
6 = I do not know

References

  1. Popkin, B.M.; Adair, L.S.; Ng, S.W. Global Nutrition Transition and the Pandemic of Obesity in Developing Countries. Nutr. Rev. 2012, 70, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Savage, J.S.; Fisher, J.O.; Birch, L.L. Parental Influence on Eating Behavior. J. Law Med. Ethics 2007, 35, 22–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Clark, M.A.; Springmann, M.; Hill, J.; Tilman, D. Multiple Health and Environmental Impacts of Foods. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 23357–23362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Forouzanfar, M.H.; Alexander, L.; Anderson, H.R.; Bachman, V.F.; Biryukov, S.; Brauer, M.; Burnett, R.; Casey, D.; Coates, M.M.; Cohen, A.; et al. Global, Regional, and National Comparative Risk Assessment of 79 Behavioural, Environmental and Occupational, and Metabolic Risks or Clusters of Risks in 188 Countries, 1990–2013: A Systematic Analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 2015, 386, 2287–2323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Clark, M.A.; Domingo, N.G.G.; Colgan, K.; Thakrar, S.K.; Tilman, D.; Lynch, J.; Azevedo, I.L.; Hill, J.D. Global Food System Emissions Could Preclude Achieving the 1.5° and 2 °C Climate Change Targets. Science 2020, 370, 705–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Willett, W.; Rockström, J.; Loken, B.; Springmann, M.; Lang, T.; Vermeulen, S.; Garnett, T.; Tilman, D.; DeClerck, F.; Wood, A.; et al. Food in the Anthropocene: The EAT–Lancet Commission on Healthy Diets from Sustainable Food Systems. Lancet 2019, 393, 447–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Galli, F.; Brunori, G.; Di Iacovo, F.; Innocenti, S. Co-producing sustainability: Involving parents and civil society in the governance of school meal services. A case study from Pisa, Italy. Sustainability 2014, 6, 1643–1666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Morgan, K.; Sonnino, R. Empowering consumers: The creative procurement of school meals in Italy and the UK. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2007, 31, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. UNICEF. The State of the World’s Children 2019. Children, Food and Nutrition: Growing Well in a Changing World; UNICEF: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  10. Steffen, W.; Richardson, K.; Rockström, J.; Cornell, S.E.; Fetzer, I.; Bennett, E.M.; Biggs, R.; Carpenter, S.R.; De Vries, W.; De Wit, C.A.; et al. Planetary Boundaries: Guiding Human Development on a Changing Planet. Science 2015, 347, 1259855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Rockström, J.; Steffen, W.; Noone, K.; Persson, Å.; Stuart Chapin, F.; Lambin, E.F.; Lenton, T.M.; Scheffer, M.; Folke, C.; Schellnhuber, H.J.; et al. A Safe Operation Space for Humanity. Nature 2009, 461, 472–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Larson, N.; Story, M. A Review of Environmental Influences on Food Choices. Ann. Behav. Med. 2009, 38, 56–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Neumark-Sztainer, D.; Story, M.; Perry, C.; Casey, M.A. Factors Influencing Food Choices of Adolescents: Findings from Focus-group Discussions with Adolescents. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1999, 99, 929–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hackel, L.M.; Coppin, G.; Wohl, M.J.A.; Van Bavel, J.J. From Groups to Grits: Social Identity Shapes Evaluations of Food Pleasantness. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 74, 270–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Oyserman, D.; Fryberg, S.A.; Yoder, N. Identity-Based Motivation and Health. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 1011–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Coates, A.E.; Hardman, C.A.; Halford, J.C.G.; Christiansen, P.; Boyland, E.J. Social Media Influencer Marketing and Children’s Food Intake: A Randomized Trial. Pediatrics 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Potvin Kent, M.; Pauzé, E.; Roy, E.A.; de Billy, N.; Czoli, C. Children and Adolescents’ Exposure to Food and Beverage Marketing in Social Media Apps. Pediatr. Obes. 2019, 14, e12508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Brown, K.; Mcilveen, H.; Strugnell, C. Nutritional Awareness and Food Preferences of Young Consumers. Nutr. Food Sci. 2000, 30, 230–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Coates, A.E.; Hardman, C.A.; Halford, J.C.G.; Christiansen, P.; Boyland, E.J. Food and Beverage Cues Featured in YouTube Videos of Social Media Influencers Popular With Children: An Exploratory Study. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Banjeree, A.V.; Chandrasekhar, A.G.; Duflo, E.; Jackson, M.O. Using Gossips to Spread Information: Theory and Evidence from two Randomized Controlled Trials. MIT Department of Economics Working Paper. 2018. Available online: https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2425379 (accessed on 2 January 2022).
  21. Paluck, E.L.; Shepherd, H.S. The Salience of Social Referents: A Field Experiment on Collective Norms and Harassment Behavior in a School Social Network. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 899–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Stolle, D.; Hooghe, M.; Micheletti, M. Politics in the Supermarket: Political Consumerism as a Form of Political Participation. Int. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2005, 26, 245–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Theocharis, Y.; van Deth, J.W. The Continuous Expansion of Citizen Participation: A New Taxonomy. Eur. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2018, 10, 139–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Schnaudt, C.; van Deth, J.W.; Zorell, C.V.; Theocharis, Y. Revisiting norms of citizenship in times of democratic change. Politics 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Aertsens, J.; Verbeke, W.; Mondelaers, K.; van Huylenbroeck, G. Personal Determinants of Organic Food Consumption: A Review. Br. Food J. 2009, 111, 1140–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Cioffi, C.E.; Levitsky, D.A.; Pacanowski, C.R.; Bertz, F. A nudge in a healthy direction. The effect of nutrition labels on food purchasing behaviors in university dining facilities. Appetite 2015, 92, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Higgs, S.; Thomas, J. Social Influences on Eating. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 9, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Miller, G.F.; Gupta, S.; Kropp, J.D.; Grogan, K.A.; Mathews, A. The Effects of Pre-ordering and Behavioral Nudges on National School Lunch Program Participants’ Food Item Selection. J. Econ. Psychol. 2016, 55, 4–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Robinson, E.; Tobias, T.; Shaw, L.; Freeman, E.; Higgs, S. Social Matching of Food Intake and the Need for Social Acceptance. Appetite 2011, 56, 747–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Rozin, P.; Scott, S.; Dingley, M.; Urbanek, J.K.; Jiang, H.; Kaltenbach, M. Nudge to Nobesity I: Minor Changes in Accessibility Decrease Food Intake. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2011, 6, 323–332. [Google Scholar]
  31. Salvy, S.J.; de la Haye, K.; Bowker, J.C.; Hermans, R.C.J. Influence of Peers and Friends on Children’s and Adolescents’ Eating and Activity Behaviors. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 106, 369–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Wansink, B.; Sobal, J. Mindless Eating: The 200 Daily Food Decisions we Overlook. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 106–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Feunekes, G.I.J.; De Graaf, C.; Meyboom, S.; Van Staveren, W.A. Food Choice and Fat Intake of Adolescents and Adults: Associations of Intakes Within Social Networks. Prev. Med. 1998, 27, 645–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Sharps, M.; Higgs, S.; Blissett, J.; Nouwen, A.; Chechlacz, M.; Allen, H.A.; Robinson, E. Examining Evidence for Behavioural Mimicry of Parental Eating by Adolescent Females. An Observational Study. Appetite 2015, 89, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. van der Horst, K.; Oenema, A.; Ferreira, I.; Wendel-Vos, W.; Giskes, K.; Van Lenthe, F.; Brug, J. A Systematic Review of Environmental Correlates of Obesity-related Dietary Behaviors in Youth. Health Educ. Res. 2007, 22, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Bruening, M.; Eisenberg, M.; MacLehose, R.; Nanney, M.S.; Story, M.; Neumark-Sztainer, D. Relationship between Adolescents’ and Their Friends’ Eating Behaviors: Breakfast, Fruit, Vegetable, Whole-Grain, and Dairy Intake. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2012, 112, 1608–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. De la Haye, K.; Robins, G.; Mohr, P.; Wilson, C. Adolescents’ Intake of Junk Food: Processes and Mechanisms Driving Consumption Similarities Among Friends. J. Res. Adolesc. 2013, 23, 524–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Caraher, M.; Landon, J.; Dalmeny, K. Television Advertising and Children: Lessons from Policy Development. Public Health Nutr. 2006, 9, 596–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Radesky, J.; Chassiakos, Y.R.; Ameenuddin, N.; Navsaria, D.; Ameenuddin, N.; Boyd, R.; Selkie, E.; Radesky, J.; Patrick, M.; Friedman, J.; et al. Digital Advertising to Children. Pediatrics 2020, 146, e20201681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kubik, M.Y.; Lytle, L.A.; Hannan, P.J.; Perry, C.L.; Story, M. The Association of the School Food Environment with Dietary Behaviors of Young Adolescents. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1168–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Qutteina, Y.; Hallez, L.; Mennes, N.; De Backer, C.; Smits, T. What Do Adolescents See on Social Media? A Diary Study of Food Marketing Images on Social Media. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Dittmar, H. Consumer Culture, Identity and Well-Being: The Search for the “Good Life” and the “Body Perfect”; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2008; ISBN 9781135420161. [Google Scholar]
  43. Fleming-Milici, F.; Harris, J.L. Adolescents’ Engagement With Unhealthy Food and Beverage Brands on Social Media. Appetite 2020, 146, 104501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Reagan, R.; Filice, S.; Santarossa, S.; Woodruff, S.J. #ad on Instagram: Investigating the Promotion of Food and Beverage Products. J. Soc. Media Soc. 2020, 9, 1–28. [Google Scholar]
  45. Coates, A.E.; Hardman, C.A.; Halford Grovenor Jason, C.; Christiansen, P.; Boyland, E.J. “It’s Just Addictive People That Make Addictive Videos”: Children’s Understanding of and Attitudes Towards Influencer Marketing of Food and Beverages by YouTube Video Bloggers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Kostygina, G.; Tran, H.; Binns, S.; Szczypka, G.; Emery, S.; Vallone, D.; Hair, E. Boosting Health Campaign Reach and Engagement Through Use of Social Media Influencers and Memes. Soc. Media Soc. 2020, 6, 2056305120912475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Spanos, S.; Vartanian, L.R.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. Personality, Perceived Appropriateness, and Acknowledgement of Social Influences on Food Intake. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015, 87, 110–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Cheung, M.L.; Pires, G.D.; Rosenberger, P.J.; Leung, W.K.S.; Salehhuddin Sharipudin, M.N. The Role of Consumer-consumer Interaction and Consumer-brand Interaction in Driving Consumer-brand Engagement and Behavioral Intentions. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2021, 61, 102574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Tajvidi, M.; Wang, Y.; Hajli, N.; Love, P.E.D. Brand value Co-creation in social commerce: The role of interactivity, social support, and relationship quality. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 115, 105238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Folkvord, F.; de Bruijne, M. The Effect of the Promotion of Vegetables by a Social Influencer on Adolescents’ Subsequent Vegetable Intake: A Pilot Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Folkvord, F.; Roes, E.; Bevelander, K. Promoting Healthy Foods in the New Digital Era on Instagram: An Experimental Study on the Effect of a Popular Real Versus Fictitious Fit Influencer on Brand Attitude and Purchase Intentions. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 1677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Harrigan, M.; Feddema, K.; Wang, S.; Harrigan, P.; Diot, E. How Trust Leads to Online Purchase Intention Founded in Perceived Usefulness and Peer Communication. J. Consum. Behav. 2021, 20, 1297–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Schilke, O.; Huang, L. Worthy of Swift Trust? How Brief Interpersonal Contact Affects Trust Accuracy. J. Appl. Psychol. 2018, 103, 1181–1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Oostindjer, M.; Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Wang, Q.; Skuland, S.E.; Egelandsdal, B.; Amdam, G.V.; Schjøll, A.; Pachucki, M.C.; Rozin, P.; Stein, J.; et al. Are School Meals a Viable and Sustainable Tool to Improve the Healthiness and Sustainability of Children’s Diet and Food Consumption? A Cross-national Comparative Perspective. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2017, 57, 3942–3958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Centola, D. How Behaviour Spreads: The Science of Complex Contagions; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA; Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  56. Prentice, D.; Paluck, E.L. Engineering Social Change Using Social Norms: Lessons from the Study of Collective Action. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2020, 35, 138–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Earls, M. Herd. How to Change Mass Behaviour By Harnessing Our True Nature, 2nd ed.; Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  58. Tankard, M.E.; Paluck, E.L. Norm Perception as a Vehicle for Social Change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2016, 10, 181–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kuran, T. Private Truths, Public Lies: The Social Consequences of Preference Falsification; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  60. Nickerson, D.W. Is voting contagious? Evidence from two field experiments. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2008, 102, 49–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Partheymüller, J.; Schmitt-Beck, R. A “Social Logic” of Demobilization: The Influence of Political Discussants on Electoral Participation at the 2009 German Federal Election. J. Elect. Public Opin. Parties 2012, 22, 457–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Schlozman, K.L.; Verba, S.; Brady, H.E. The Unheavenly Chorus: Unequal Political Voice and the Broken Promise of American Democracy; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  63. Zorell, C.V.; Denk, T. Political Consumerism and Interpersonal Discussion Patterns. Scan. Polit. Stud. 2021, 44, 392–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Micheletti, M.; Stolle, D. Vegetarianism—A Lifestyle Politics? In Creative Participation: Responsibility-Taking in the Political World; Micheletti, M., Mcfarland, A.S., Eds.; Paradigm: Boulder, CO, USA, 2010; pp. 125–145. [Google Scholar]
  65. Micheletti, M.; Stolle, D. Sustainable Citizenship and the New Politics of Consumption. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 2012, 644, 88–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Onwezen, M.C.; Reinders, M.J.; Verain, M.C.D.; Snoek, H.M. The development of a single-item Food Choice Questionnaire. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 71, 34–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Lindeman, M.; Väänänen, M. Measurement of Ethical Food Choice Motives. Appetite 2000, 34, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Verain, M.C.D.; Onwezen, M.C.; Sijtsema, S.J.; Dagevos, H. The Added Value of Sustainability Motivations in Understanding Sustainable Food Choices. Appl. Stud. Agribus. Commer. 2017, 10, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Åmna, E.; Ekström, M.; Kerr, M.; Stattin, H. Codebook: The Political Socialization Program; Örebro universitet: Örebro, Sweden, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  70. Zorell, C.V. Varieties of Political Consumerism. From Boycotting to Buycotting; Palgrave Macmillan: Basingstoke, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  71. Spanos, S.; Vartanian, L.R.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. Failure to Report Social Influences on Food Intake: Lack of Awareness or Motivated Denial? Health Psychol. 2014, 33, 1487–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Food properties guiding food choice of youths, mean values.
Figure 1. Food properties guiding food choice of youths, mean values.
Ijerph 19 03139 g001
Figure 2. Most usual sources of information about food (numbers of respondents ranking source 1st to 7th). Note: Ordered from left to right according to source that was most to least often ranked as most important.
Figure 2. Most usual sources of information about food (numbers of respondents ranking source 1st to 7th). Note: Ordered from left to right according to source that was most to least often ranked as most important.
Ijerph 19 03139 g002
Figure 3. Central persons affecting food purchase and choice, frequencies of mentions. Note: Family (total) includes parents plus any other kind of family tie. Therese Lindgren is a Swedish influencer on lifestyle issues in general. The figure reflects the responses provided by 38% of participants, i.e., 166 individuals. In total, 12 persons provided answers that could not be classified. A total of 89 individuals named one central person, the rest named two and a few named a combination of several categories in relation to a single central person, e.g., Therese Lindgren and YouTube and influencer; in the graph, these cases are counted, respectively, with a value for each of the different categories.
Figure 3. Central persons affecting food purchase and choice, frequencies of mentions. Note: Family (total) includes parents plus any other kind of family tie. Therese Lindgren is a Swedish influencer on lifestyle issues in general. The figure reflects the responses provided by 38% of participants, i.e., 166 individuals. In total, 12 persons provided answers that could not be classified. A total of 89 individuals named one central person, the rest named two and a few named a combination of several categories in relation to a single central person, e.g., Therese Lindgren and YouTube and influencer; in the graph, these cases are counted, respectively, with a value for each of the different categories.
Ijerph 19 03139 g003
Figure 4. Central persons and youths eating climate friendly. Lines show 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 4. Central persons and youths eating climate friendly. Lines show 95% confidence intervals.
Ijerph 19 03139 g004
Figure 5. Central persons and youths’ political consumer engagements, (a) buycotting and (b) boycotting. Lines show 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 5. Central persons and youths’ political consumer engagements, (a) buycotting and (b) boycotting. Lines show 95% confidence intervals.
Ijerph 19 03139 g005
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for dependent and independent variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for dependent and independent variables.
VariableObs.MeanStd. Dev.MinMax
Additive index everyday food choices must be sustainable4423.891.4116.78
Boycotting4322.781.7616
Buycotting4393.391.2916
Source of information:
Family4304.151.8017
Friends4293.521.6817
School4294.031.7217
TV4283.661.7317
Newspaper4282.831.7117
Social media4315.161.7717
Other1622.782.2917
Central person(s)4321.380.4912
Climate-friendly eating of central persons1943.331.4015
Gender4360.400.4901
Age44017.920.661622
Table 2. Central persons eating climate friendly.
Table 2. Central persons eating climate friendly.
Central Persons Eating Climate FriendlyFrequenciesPercentage
None of them2713.92
One of them sometimes2311.86
One of them regularly6634.02
Both of them sometimes157.73
Both of them regularly6332.47
Total194100
Note: Next to these 194 individuals and the ones who said they have no central person, another 46 individuals answered with “I do not know”.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zorell, C.V. Central Persons in Sustainable (Food) Consumption. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3139. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19053139

AMA Style

Zorell CV. Central Persons in Sustainable (Food) Consumption. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(5):3139. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19053139

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zorell, Carolin V. 2022. "Central Persons in Sustainable (Food) Consumption" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 5: 3139. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19053139

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop