Next Article in Journal
Body Composition and Its Perception among Professional Female Volleyball Players and Fitness Athletes (Silesia, Poland)
Previous Article in Journal
The Association between COVID-19 Pandemic and the Quality of Life of Medical Students in Silesian Voivodeship, Poland
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Hope for the Future and Academic Performance in Adolescents: Results from the K-CHILD Study

1
Department of Global Health Promotion, Tokyo Medical and Dental University (TMDU), Tokyo 113-8510, Japan
2
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(19), 11890; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191911890
Submission received: 27 July 2022 / Revised: 10 September 2022 / Accepted: 14 September 2022 / Published: 20 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Adolescents)

Abstract

:
In Japan, having hope for the future is emphasized in school. This study aimed to examine the association between hope for the future and academic performance among Japanese adolescents. Data were taken from the population-based Kochi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (K-CHILD) study conducted in 2016. Participants included 3477 adolescents in the eighth grade (i.e., 13–14 years old) in Kochi Prefecture. Information on hope for the future, self-rated academic performance, and time used for studying or playing was provided by the adolescents via a questionnaire. The question on resilience was answered by their caregivers. Propensity-score matching was applied for the allocation of hope for the future. Overall, 2283 adolescents (65.6%) had some form of hope for the future. Adolescents having hope for the future showed a higher self-rated academic performance (β = 0.21, 95% confidence interval (Confidence Interval (CI) = 0.10 to 0.32)), spent more time studying except in class (Odds Ratio (OR) = 1.89, 95% CI = 1.37 to 2.61), read more books (OR = 1.45, 95% CI = 1.19 to 1.75), and had a higher score of resilience (β = 1.48, 95% CI = 0.98 to 1.98), while the time to watch TV or DVDs was not different (p = 0.61). Our results highlight the importance of encouraging adolescents to have hope for the future to promote academic performance.

1. Introduction

Academic performance among adolescents is highly related to their health status. Adolescents who have poor academic performance have a higher risk of negative lifetime outcomes, such as mental health problems [1,2], substance use [3], or delinquency [4]. Hence, it is important to show the modifiable determinants of academic performance, such as having hope for the future. The reason why we chose hope for the future among many modifiable determinants is that having hope for the future is emphasized in school, especially in Japan [5]. Children are often asked “What do you want to become in the future?” by teachers, parents, or relatives.
Hope for the future was used in a study of self-esteem and academic performance among early Japanese adolescents [5]. In the study, hope for the future was measured by four items: (a) I have already decided what I want to become in the future; (b) I have a thing that I try to do in the future; (c) I think my future life is bright; (d) I do not think about how my future will be (reverse item). The authors found that changing the process of hope for the future was not associated with academic performance [5]. However, as the previous study’s population had a small sample size (n = 755) and was not population-based, a further study that addresses the limitations is needed.
In other countries, the association between hope for the future and academic performance has not been revealed. As a similar concept, future orientation or planning is only used in some studies. For example, in China and Iran, a significant association between future orientation or planning and academic achievement was demonstrated [6,7]. Hence, the main purpose of this study is to investigate the association between hope for the future and academic performance. Future orientation or planning may be associated with higher academic performance because an academic score is needed to obtain the occupation that he or she desires. However, it is not clear whether future orientation is determined by himself or herself; that is, hope for the future can have a more internal motive basis, whereas future orientation or planning has a more instrumental motive basis. As previous psychological studies revealed, internal motives would be more important for achievement in later life [8].
Moreover, other behaviors that are proven to be associated with academic performance, such as the amount of time spent on doing their homework and schoolwork [9], and the amount of time spent on playing video games [10], need to be assessed. The previous study about the future time perspective shows that time management disposition has a significant positive association with future positiveness and future plans, which can be seen as a similar concept of hope for the future. The previous study also showed that time planning has a significant indirect effect on academic achievement through time efficacy [11]. We postulate that this association may be adaptable to hope for the future. However, to our knowledge, there is a dearth of literature examining hope for the future and time used for studying or playing in adolescents.
Resilience was also noted as a predictor of academic performance [12]. In addition, future orientation, which is a similar concept to hope for the future, has been shown to greatly impact the development of resilience among maltreated youth [13]. We hypothesize that this association may also be adaptable to hope for the future, but resilience has never been addressed as a mediator between hope for the future and academic achievement.
Hence, another purpose of this study is to investigate the association of hope for the future with academic performance, time usage, and resilience. However, it is of note that hope for the future can vary by demographics, socioeconomic status, and daily activities, which can be a confounder of these associations [14,15]. Propensity score matching (PSM) is a method of balancing covariates between the exposure and non-exposure groups using propensity scores in a real-life situation where a randomized experimental design is not possible. As multiple factors can be confounded simultaneously, propensity score matching will enable us to conduct a pseudo-randomized controlled trial, which might estimate the causal relationship better than a cross-sectional study. In addition, this method is more robust in handling endogeneity bias [16].
In this study, using propensity score matching, we expected to find a positive association between hope for the future and academic performance among Japanese adolescents. In addition, we also hypothesized that hope for the future is associated with adolescents’ time usage and resilience. Speaking of time usage, it was anticipated that adolescents having hope for the future would spend less time playing games, watching TV or DVDs, and spend more time reading books or studying except when in class.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sampling Method

We used data from the Kochi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (K-CHILD) study, a population-based cross-sectional study conducted in Kochi Prefecture, Japan, in 2016. The K-CHILD study aimed to examine the living environment and health of children in Kochi Prefecture. Participants included first, fifth, eighth, and eleventh-grade students and their caregivers in all public, private, and special needs schools in the prefecture, except for correspondence course high schools and a special needs school. Further details are described elsewhere [17].
The current study targeted students in the second grade in junior high schools (i.e., 13–14 years old in the eighth grade) and their caregivers. The self-administered anonymous questionnaires for the children and their caregivers were distributed to a total of 6192 eighth graders, and 3634 questionnaires were returned in anonymous envelopes to the Office of Children and Family Services in Kochi City by mail or to the schools in person (response rate: 54.7%). Of the returned questionnaires, 3624 were valid except for those completely blank (n = 10). We also excluded the ones which did not report on hope for the future (n = 147). Finally, 3477 subjects were used as the analytical sample in this study (Figure 1).

2.2. Measurements

2.2.1. Hope for the Future

Hope for the future was assessed using the question, “Have you already decided what you want to become in the future?” The children responded with either “1” (yes) or “0” (no). The same question was used to measure hope for the future in a previous study on Japanese adolescents [5].

2.2.2. Self-Related Academic Performance

Self-rated academic performance was assessed using the question, “How do you rate your academic performance in a class?” The children responded with “1” (higher than average) to “5” (lower than average), or “6” (I do not know). In this study, scores were used in reverse order so that a higher score indicates a higher level of academic performance. When we analyzed the relationship between hope for the future and self-related academic performance, we excluded “6” (I do not know) from the analysis as it was done in a previous study [17].

2.2.3. Time Usage

This survey asked about the time spent on four activities such as playing games, watching TV or DVDs, reading books, and studying outside of class. Children were asked, “How often do you play games in a week?”, “How often do you watch TV or DVD in a week?” and “How often do you read books in a week?” Their responses ranged from “more than 2 h in a day”, “1 to 2 h in a day”, “less than 1 h in a day”, “4 to 5 days in a week”, “2 to 3 days in a week”, and “1 day in a week”, to “almost never”. For the statistical analysis, this categorical variable was changed into dichotomous variables. As for the time playing games and watching TV or DVDs, following the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline on screen time for children [18], “More than 2 h in a day”, and “1 to 2 h in a day” were categorized as “1” and “less than 1 h in a day”, “4 to 5 days in a week”, “2 to 3 days in a week”, “1 day in a week”, and “almost never” were categorized as “0”. As for the time spent reading and studying, reading and studying outside of class are recommended in school in Japan [19]. “More than 2 h in a day”, “1 to 2 h in a day”, “4 to 5 days in a week”, “2 to 3 days in a week”, “1 day in a week”, and “less than 1 h in a day” were categorized as “1” and “almost never” was categorized as “0”.

2.2.4. Resilience

Children’s resilience was assessed using the Children’s Resilient Coping Scale (CRCS) developed for use in the Japanese context regarding resilience and coping skills [11]. CRCS consists of eight items: (1) speaks positively about their future; (2) tries to do their best; (3) able to take teasing or mean comments well; (4) knows how to properly greet others; (5) able to get ready for school, study, and do his/her chores without directions; (6) seeks appropriate advice when necessary; (7) able to give up on things they want or do things that they do not like to do for better future outcomes; and (8) able to ask questions to learn about what they do not understand. Caregivers answered each item from “0” (never) to “4” (very frequently). This scale showed high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.80) and sufficient validity in the literature [20]. A higher score indicates a higher level of resilience (range: 0–32).

2.2.5. Covariates

In the questionnaire for caregivers, the demographic variables, socio-economic status, and physical and mental health of caregivers were assessed. Demographic variables included child’s sex (male or female), child’s height (<150, 150–159.9, 160–169.9, ≥170 cm), child’s weight (<40, 40–49.9, 50–59.9, ≥60 kg), maternal and paternal age (<40, 40–49, ≥50 years old), maternal and paternal education level (high school or less, some college education, college education or higher, other/unknown), maternal and paternal occupation (full-time job, part-time job, self-employed or others, not working), marital status (married or unmarried), whether the child is living with maternal and paternal grandparents (yes or no), and whether the child is living with siblings (yes or no). Socioeconomic status was assessed by annual household income (<JPY 3,000,000 (≒USD 27,777 in 2016), JPY 3,000,000–5,999,999, ≥JPY 6,000,000, or unknown), whether the household is receiving public assistance (yes or no), whether they are experiencing economic difficulties (yes or no), and whether they have experienced lack of daily necessities (yes or no).
Caregivers also answered self-rated physical health (good, average, poor). The caregiver’s mental health was measured using the Japanese version of the Kessler 6 (K6) [21], which comprises six items on a scale of “0” (all of the time) to “4” (none of the time). The total scores range from 0 to 24 and were divided into three levels using a score of 4/5 and 12/13 as cut-off points. Higher scores indicate a higher risk of having psychological distress. Caregivers also answered maternal and paternal smoking habits with either of the three responses (“I currently smoke”, “I smoked in the past”, or “I have never smoked”).
Lastly, the child’s mental health was measured using the Japanese version of the Depression Self-Rating Scale (DSRS) [22] in the questionnaire for children. In this study, children answered 15 items on a scale of “0” (never) to “2” (most of the time). The total scores range from 0 to 30 and were divided into two levels using a score of 16 as a cut-off point [23].

2.2.6. Statistical Analysis

First, univariate linear regression analyses (crude model) and multivariate linear regression analyses (adjusted model) were conducted to examine the association between hope for the future and academic performance and the child’s resilience score. The adjusted model included the covariates listed above, such as demographic variables, socioeconomic status, self-rated academic performance in class, physical and mental health of mothers and fathers, and mental health of children. The rationale for these covariates is as follows: According to previous studies on future orientation, characteristics of parents, such as socioeconomic status or health status of parents were known to have an impact on the future orientation of children [24,25,26,27,28,29]. In addition, as children were less likely to have future-related thoughts if they had experienced childhood adversity like parental separation and divorce [30], we included marital status as covariates. In addition, as an association between a supportive home environment and adolescent future orientation was reported [31,32], covariates of siblings or grandparents were included. Further, previous studies revealed a significant association of future orientation with the child’s sex [33,34] and eating behavior, which is related to height and weight [35]. Adolescents’ mental health is associated with and conceptually close to future orientation [36,37,38,39,40]. We also conducted the crude and multivariate logistic regression analysis for the time used for studying or playing. Further, multicollinearity was checked by the variance inflation factor (VIF) and confirmed all VIFs < 10.
Second, propensity-score (PS) matching was used to compare adolescents who have hope for the future with those without, which can remove bias due to all observed covariates and make the observational study pseudo-experimental [16]. We included the same covariates used in the multivariate model. Missing data were categorized into one group of each covariate. PS matching was performed using the command “psmatch2” with a logistic regression model in STATA. We conducted 1 to 1 optimal matching, with a caliper width equal to 0.01, and no replacement was applied for PS matching. Standardized bias was used to assess the balance of potential confounders within the matched pairs. Then, conditional regression analysis was performed to examine the association between hope for the future and academic performance, and between hope for the future and CRCS total score, using the matched pairs. Similarly, the analyses for time spent playing games, watching TV or DVD, reading books, and studying outside of class were conducted using conditional logistic regression analysis.
All analyses were performed using STATA 14.2.3. To address multiple comparisons in an outcome-wide study [41], as we have six outcomes (academic performance, time to play games, time to watch TV or DVDs, reading books, studying except class, and resilience), we considered alpha = 0.05/6 = 0.0083 as a significant level, applying the Bonferroni correction.

3. Results

3.1. Distribution of Characteristics

Table 1 describes the characteristics of adolescents with and without hope for the future before and after propensity score matching. Overall, 2283 adolescents (65.6%) had hope for the future while 1194 adolescents (34.3%) had none. Female students and those having lower depressive symptoms were more likely to have hope for the future before PS matching. After PS matching, the standard bias of almost all covariates was less than 5%, which means that the bias of covariates within matched pairs was significantly lower than that of unmatched samples.
Table 2 shows the distribution of time usage. About 70 percent of adolescents play games for less than 1 h per day. On the other hand, about 70 percent of them watch TV or DVDs for more than 1 h per day. The percentage of adolescents who almost never read books was approximately 30%, while adolescents who almost never study except in class was 10% or less.

3.2. Propensity Score Matching

Table 3 shows the results of academic performance and resilience by univariate linear regression analyses (crude model) and multivariate linear regression analyses (adjusted model) before and after PS matching. All crude models and adjusted models before and after PS matching showed similar statistical differences. In the model before PS matching, adolescents with hope for the future showed higher self-rated academic performance (β = 0.19, 95% confidence interval (CI = 0.10 to 0.28)) and a higher score of resilience (β = 1.51, 95% CI = 1.11 to 1.91). In the model after PS matching, the significant difference in both academic performance and resilience remained (both p < 0.001).
Table 4 describes the results of time usage by logistic regression analyses before and after PS matching. Having hope for the future was also associated with studying except in class (OR = 1.45, 95% CI = 1.19 to 1.75) and reading books (OR = 1.89, 95% CI = 1.37 to 2.61). This result did not change before and after PS matching. There was no association between having hope for the future and time spent watching TV or DVDs. The association between hope for the future and time playing games was marginal: those who have hope for the future were 19% less likely to play games for 1 h or more per day (OR = 0.81, 95% CI = 0.67 to 0.98), which was not statistically significant after the Bonferroni correction.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to describe the association between hope for the future and academic performance, time usage, such as studying time, reading time, gaming time, and time watching TV/DVDs, and resilience among adolescents in Japan.
The results of the current study indicate that hope for the future was significantly associated with subjective academic performance. Though the significant association between future orientation or planning and academic achievement was demonstrated in previous studies in China and Iran [6,7], the association between hope for the future and academic performance has not yet been revealed in other countries. In Japan, the previous study that examined the association between hope for the future and academic performance among early Japanese adolescents showed that changing the process of hope for the future was not associated with academic performance [5].
This study adds to the literature in China and Iran by not showing future orientation but that hope for the future, which is based on an internal motive [8] and can be inspired in school, was associated with academic performance among Japanese adolescents. In addition, this study employed propensity score matching to eliminate measured bias for having hope for the future, to overcome the limitation of the previous study conducted in Japan.
Further, we found that hope for the future was significantly associated with time spent on reading books and studying except in class. That is, these results indicate that students who have hope for the future might be able to manage their time more effectively to achieve the desired outcome. It is also possible that students who have already decided what they want to do in the future tend to consider what they have to do now and manage their time accordingly. Our findings are consistent with previous ones about future time perspective [11], which is a similar concept to hope for the future. In the previous study, it was reported that future positiveness and future plans, which are components of future time perspective, have significant positive correlations with time management disposition. Another important point of this previous study is that hope for the future can be inspired by adults in a comparatively easy way.
In addition, it was previously shown that time planning has a significant indirect effect on academic achievement through time efficacy [11]. Considering this result, the association between having hope for the future and academic performance might be mediated by reading books and studying except in class. A further randomized controlled study of the effectiveness of hope for the future and its association with time management is warranted.
The current study also revealed that hope for the future has a positive association with resilience. This result is also consistent with the previous study, which revealed the association between future time perspective and resilience [42]. The students who have hope for the future might be able to imagine their future vision or future goals more clearly, and their will to realize their dreams might enable them to develop their patience. Further studies using hope for the future as an intervention, especially among those who need to develop resilience, such as those experiencing childhood adversity [43], are warranted.
Several limitations need to be addressed. First, the details of individual hope for the future were not investigated. Hope for the future was determined by the response to one question only. In addition, academic performance was subjectively measured. Future studies need to objectively assess academic performance as well as the types of jobs or social status the child hopes to have in the future. Second, the current study cannot determine the causal association because reverse causation is likely to have occurred due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, even though propensity score matching analysis was used. Those who have better academic performance may be more likely to have hope for the future. Furthermore, unmeasured confounders, such as child temperament, genetic factors, or childhood environment, were not addressed. We need a further longitudinal study to elucidate the mechanisms of whether hope for the future promotes better future academic performance.

5. Conclusions

This study highlighted that having hope for the future was associated with better academic performance among adolescents. This association may be partially explained by reading books, studying except in class, and resilience. A further study investigating the details of individual hope for the future or objective academic performance is warranted to show a more precise inference on the association. Nonetheless, the current findings provide important information on encouraging adolescents to have hope for the future to promote better academic performance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.K. and T.F.; methodology, T.K. and T.F.; formal analysis, T.K. and S.D.; data curation, S.D.; writing—original draft preparation, T.K.; writing—review and editing, S.D., Y.Y., and T.F.; funding acquisition, T.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 16H03276, 17K19794, 19H04879, 21H04848, 21K18294).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tokyo Medical and Dental University (M2017-243).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all caregivers and children involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Jonsson, U.; Goodman, A.; von Knorring, A.L.; von Knorring, L.; Koupil, I. School performance and hospital admission due to unipolar depression: A three-generational study of social causation and social selection. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2012, 47, 1695–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Verboom, C.E.; Sijtsema, J.J.; Verhulst, F.C.; Penninx, B.W.; Ormel, J. Longitudinal associations between depressive problems, academic performance, and social functioning in adolescent boys and girls. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 247–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Cox, R.G.; Zhang, L.; Johnson, W.D.; Bender, D.R. Academic Performance and Substance Use: Findings From a State Survey of Public High School Students. J. Sch. Health 2007, 77, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Maguin, E.; Loeber, R. Academic Performance and Delinquency. Crime Justice 1996, 20, 145–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Tsuzuki, M. Dynamic changing process of hope in early adolescence: Analysis of individual differences during the transition from elementary school to junior high school1. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 2012, 54, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Zhao, J.; Li, R.; Ma, J.; Zhang, W. Longitudinal relations between future planning and adolescents’ academic achievement in China. J. Adolesc. 2019, 75, 73–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Hejazi, E.; Moghadam, A.; Naghsh, Z.; Tarkhan, R.A. The future orientation of Iranian adolescents girl students and their academic achievement. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 15, 2441–2444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wrzesniewski, A.; Schwartz, B.; Cong, X.; Kane, M.; Omar, A.; Kolditz, T. Multiple types of motives don’t multiply the motivation of West Point cadets. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, 10990–10995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Leone, C.M.; Richards, H. Classwork and homework in early adolescence: The ecology of achievement. J. Youth Adolesc. 1989, 18, 531–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Anand, V. A Study of Time Management: The Correlation between Video Game Usage and Academic Performance Markers. CyberPsychol. Behav. 2007, 10, 552–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gang, D.; Hou-Tiao, L. The Relationship of Adolescents’ Future Time Perspective and Academic Achievement:The Mediation Effect of Time Management Disposition. J. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 40, 96–102. [Google Scholar]
  12. Rodríguez-Fernández, A.; Ramos-Díaz, E.; Axpe-Saez, I. The Role of Resilience and Psychological Well-Being in School Engagement and Perceived Academic Performance: An Exploratory Model to Improve Academic Achievement. Health Acad. Achiev. 2018, 18, 159–176. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cui, Z.; Oshri, A.; Liu, S.; Smith, E.P.; Kogan, S.M. Child Maltreatment and Resilience: The Promotive and Protective Role of Future Orientation. J. Youth Adolesc. 2020, 49, 2075–2089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Chen, E.; Miller, G.E.; Lachman, M.E.; Gruenewald, T.L.; Seeman, T.E. Protective factors for adults from low-childhood socioeconomic circumstances: The benefits of shift-and-persist for allostatic load. Psychosom. Med. 2012, 74, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Poole, M.E.; Cooney, G.H. Orientations to the future: A comparison of adolescents in Australia and Singapore. J. Youth Adolesc. 1987, 16, 129–151. [Google Scholar]
  16. Rosenbaum, P.R.; Rubin, D.B. The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika 1983, 70, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Doi, S.; Fujiwara, T.; Isumi, A. Association between maternal adverse childhood experiences and child’s self-rated academic performance: Results from the K-CHILD study. Child Abus. Negl. 2020, 104, 104478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Council on Communications and Media; Strasburger, V.C.; Hogan, M.J.; Mulligan, D.A.; Ameenuddin, N.; Christakis, D.A.; Cross, C.; Fagbuyi, D.B.; Hill, D.L.; Levine, A.E.; et al. Children, Adolescents, and the Media. Pediatrics 2013, 132, 958–961. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology Japan. Promotion of Reading Books among Children Tokyo; Ministry of Education Culture, Sports, Science and Technology Japan: Tokyo, Japan, 2022; Available online: https://www.mext.go.jp/a_menu/sports/dokusyo/hourei/cont_001/001.htm (accessed on 13 September 2022).
  20. Doi, S.; Fujiwara, T.; Ochi, M.; Isumi, A.; Kato, T. Association of sleep habits with behavior problems and resilience of 6- to 7-year-old children: Results from the A-CHILD study. Sleep Med. 2018, 45, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Furukawa, T.A.; Kawakami, N.; Saitoh, M.; Ono, Y.; Nakane, Y.; Nakamura, Y.; Tachimori, H.; Iwata, N.; Uda, H.; Nakane, H.; et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2008, 17, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Sato, H.; Arai, K. The investigation of factor structure and normative data for Depression Self-Rating Scale for Children (DSRS) Japanese version. Bull. Tsukuba Dev. Clin. Psychol. 2002, 14, 85–91. [Google Scholar]
  23. Nagai, S. Investigation of the factor structure model and normative data for Depression Self-Rating Scale for Children (DSRS) among junior high-school students. Jpn. J. Res. Emot. 2008, 16, 133–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Webley, P.; Nyhus, E.K. Parents’ influence on children’s future orientation and saving. J. Econ. Psychol. 2006, 27, 140–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hill, N.E.; Castellino, D.R.; Lansford, J.E.; Nowlin, P.; Dodge, K.A.; Bates, J.E.; Pettit, G.S. Parent Academic Involvement as Related to School Behavior, Achievement, and Aspirations: Demographic Variations Across Adolescence. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1491–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Masa, R.; Bates, C.; Chowa, G. Direct and Indirect Associations of Food Insecurity, Adolescent–Parent Relationship, and Adolescent Future Orientation. J. Fam. Issues 2021, 42, 1285–1307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gardner Neblett, N.; Schnabel Cortina, K. Adolescents’ thoughts about parents’ jobs and their importance for adolescents’ future orientation. J. Adolesc. 2006, 29, 795–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ragozin, A.S.; Basham, R.B.; Crnic, K.A.; Greenberg, M.T.; Robinson, N.M. Effects of maternal age on parenting role. Dev. Psychol. 1982, 18, 627–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Finley, G.E. Parental Age and Parenting Quality as Perceived by Late Adolescents. J. Genet. Psychol. 1998, 159, 505–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chainey, C.; Burke, K.; Haynes, M. Does Parenting Moderate the Association between Adverse Childhood Experiences and Adolescents’ Future Orientation? J. Child Fam. Stud. 2022, 31, 2359–2375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pulkkinen, L. Home atmosphere and adolescent future orientation. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1990, 5, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Broomfield, K.A. Mechanisms through Which Supportive Adult Relationships and Future Orientation Contribute to Positive Outcomes in Low-Income African-American Adolescents. Ph.D. Thesis, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA, 2007; p. 33. [Google Scholar]
  33. Nurmi, J.-E. How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Dev. Rev. 1991, 11, 1–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Seginer, R.; Mahajna, S. Future orientation links perceived parenting and academic achievement: Gender differences among Muslim adolescents in Israel. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018, 67, 197–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Dassen, F.C.M.; Houben, K.; Jansen, A. Time orientation and eating behavior: Unhealthy eaters consider immediate consequences, while healthy eaters focus on future health. Appetite 2015, 91, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kim, D.H.; Bassett, S.M.; So, S.; Voisin, D.R. Family stress and youth mental health problems: Self-efficacy and future orientation mediation. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2019, 89, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zhang, J.; Zhao, G.; Li, X.; Hong, Y.; Fang, X.; Barnett, D.; Lin, X.; Zhao, J.; Zhang, L. Positive future orientation as a mediator between traumatic events and mental health among children affected by HIV/AIDS in rural China. AIDS Care 2009, 21, 1508–1516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Threlfall, J.M.; Auslander, W.; Gerke, D.; McGinnis, H.; Myers Tlapek, S. Mental Health and School Functioning for Girls in the Child Welfare System: The Mediating Role of Future Orientation and School Engagement. Sch. Ment. Health 2017, 9, 194–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gerson, A.C.; Perlman, D. Loneliness and expressive communication. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1979, 88, 258–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Seginer, R.; Lilach, E. How adolescents construct their future: The effect of loneliness on future orientation. J. Adolesc. 2004, 27, 625–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. VanderWeele, T.J.; Mathur, M.B.; Chen, Y. Outcome-Wide Longitudinal Designs for Causal Inference: A New Template for Empirical Studies. Stat. Sci. 2020, 35, 437–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. O’Neill, E.; Clarke, P.; Fido, D.; Vione, K.C. The Role of Future Time Perspective, Body Awareness, and Social Connectedness in the Relationship Between Self-efficacy and Resilience. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2022, 20, 1171–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Fujiwara, T. Impact of Adverse Childhood Experience on Physical and Mental Health: A life-course epidemiology perspective. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Requirement flow chart.
Figure 1. Requirement flow chart.
Ijerph 19 11890 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of covariates before and after propensity score matching.
Table 1. Characteristics of covariates before and after propensity score matching.
Before PS Matching After PS Matching
Hope for the Future Hope for Future
Yes (n = 2283; 65.7%)No (n = 1194; 34.3%)p ValueBias
(%)
Yes
(n = 1105)
No
(n = 1105)
p ValueBias
(%)
n%n% n%n%
Child’s sex 0.003 0.625
boy100944.259249.6 55750.453848.7
girl122053.456647.4 12.152147.253548.4 −2.5
Missing 542.4363.0 −4.0272.4322.9 −2.8
Child’s height 0.137 0.882
<1502169.5907.5 726.5837.5
150–<16093541.047239.5 2.943639.544039.8 −0.7
160–<17075232.942235.3 −5.140937.039435.7 2.9
170+2199.61109.2 1.3999.01029.2 −0.9
Missing 1617.11008.4 −5.0898.1867.8 1.0
Child’s weight 0.267 0.996
<401787.81008.4 867.8918.2
40–<5098443.150542.3 1.647142.647342.8 −0.4
50–<6061727.034028.5 −3.232029.031628.6 0.8
60+25211.01068.9 7.21009.1999.0 0.3
Missing 25211.014312.0 −2.912811.612611.4 0.6
Maternal age 0.518 0.918
<4037916.618815.8 17515.817515.8
40–<50152666.878565.8 2.373566.573366.3 0.4
50+22910.013211.1 −3.311210.112010.9 −2.4
Missing1496.5897.5 −3.6837.5777.0 2.1
Paternal age 0.239 0.994
<402159.41099.1 1019.1999.0
40–<50122453.664053.6 060054.359653.9 0.7
50+40517.724020.1 −6.021419.421819.7 −0.9
Missing43919.220517.2 5.319017.219217.4 −0.5
Maternal education 0.385 0.943
High school or less82636.240233.7 37533.937233.7
Some college97242.652443.9 −2.647543.049044.3 −2.7
College or more33514.717814.9 −0.716514.916214.7 0.8
Other/Unknown40.250.4 −4.530.330.3 0
Missing1466.4857.1 −2.9877.9787.1 3.2
Paternal education 0.300 0.904
High school or less92140.348640.7 42538.544139.9
Some college36015.819716.5 −2.019717.818316.6 3.4
College or more55924.531126.1 −3.628826.128826.1 0
Other/Unknown120.530.3 4.420.230.3 −1.5
Missing43118.919716.5 6.219317.519017.2 0.7
Maternal job 0.259 0.951
Full-time83736.741835.0 40135.638734.4
Part-time80535.341634.8 0.939635.240435.9 −2.3
Others26611.712810.7 3.011810.511510.2 2.0
Not working24310.615012.6 −6.013812.314012.4 0.8
Missing1325.8826.9 −4.5736.5807.1 3.7
Paternal job 0.065 0.998
Full-time139561.173061.1 70062.270262.3
Part-time743.2433.6 −2.0403.6373.3 −1.0
Others35815.720717.3 −4.518616.518616.5 2.7
Not working241.1221.8 −6.6141.2141.2 −3.0
Missing43218.919216.1 7.518616.518716.6 0.7
Marital states 0.089 0.941
Married182980.198882.8 90581.991082.4
Unmarried42918.819916.7 5.619217.418817.0 0.9
Missing251.170.6 5.680.770.6 1.0
Annual household income (JPY) 0.340 0.926
<3 million44819.620917.5 17916.219017.2
3 million–<6 million70130.736430.5 0.533830.633330.1 1.0
6 million+71631.438532.2 −1.936132.736232.8 −0.2
Unknown994.3463.9 2.4403.6444.0 −1.8
Missing 31914.019015.9 −5.418716.917615.9 2.8
Public assistance 0.207 0.926
No138860.876163.7 71263.271763.7
Yes160.760.5 2.650.460.5 −1.2
Missing87938.542735.8 5.740936.340335.8 −2.1
Economic difficulties 0.387 0.843
No190883.699082.9 92884.091883.1
Yes28612.516513.8 −3.814413.015313.9 −2.4
Missing893.9393.3 3.4333.0343.1 −0.5
Lack of daily necessities for children 0.214 0.979
No134158.769858.5 65559.365158.9
Yes51522.629524.7 −5.126023.527323.9 −0.9
Missing 42718.720116.8 4.919017.218717.2 0.0
Lack of daily necessities 0.313 0.925
No141962.276063.7 70764.070263.5
Yes39617.421518.0 −1.718716.919417.6 −1.7
Missing 46820.521918.3 5.521119.120918.9 0.5
Caregiver’s physical health 0.120 0.947
Good129956.969358.0 63757.764458.3
Average59025.831526.4 −1.229927.128826.1 2.3
Poor2189.612010.1 −1.71059.511010.0 −1.5
Missing1767.7665.5 8.8645.8635.7 0.4
Caregiver’s mental health (K6) 0.506 0.935
<5153867.483469.9 77670.276769.4
5–<1354824.0265265.0 4.324221.925122.7 −1.9
13+1165.15454.0 2.6464.2494.4 −1.3
Missing813.64141.0 0.6413.7383.4 1.5
Maternal smoking habit 0.476 0.951
Smoking now33714.815513.0 15514.015213.8
Smoking past33214.518715.7 −3.116114.618015.4 −2.3
No history147064.477464.8 −0.972065.271864.4 1.5
Missing1446.3786.5 −0.9696.2716.4 −0.7
Paternal smoking habit 0.103 0.960
Smoking now74232.538532.2 35332.036332.9
Smoking past61326.936130.2 −7.533029.932029.0 2.0
No history50522.125721.5 1.423921.624021.7 −0.2
Missing42318.519116.0 6.718316.618216.5 0.2
Living with maternal grandmother 0.112 0.686
No208591.3110992.9 102092.3102592.8
Yes1988.7857.1 5.8857.7807.2 1.7
Living with maternal grandfather 0.131 0.607
No215094.2113995.4 105895.8105395.3
Yes1335.8554.6 5.5474.3524.7 −2.0
Living with paternal grandmother 0.095 0.606
No202988.9108390.7 100791.1100090.5
Yes25411.11119.3 6.0988.91059.5 −2.1
Living with paternal grandfather 0.535 0.931
No212793.2111993.7 103493.6103593.7
Yes1566.8756.3 2.2716.4706.3 0.4
Older brother 0.242 0.847
No170974.987273.0 81073.381473.7
Yes57425.132227.0 −4.229526.729126.3 0.8
Older sister 0.102 0.731
No176477.389374.8 82674.883375.4
Yes51922.730125.2 −5.827925.327224.6 1.5
Younger brother 0.103 0.962
No161570.787673.4 80773.080672.9
Yes66829.331826.6 5.929827.029927.1 −0.2
Younger sister 0.758 0.962
No166773.086672.5 80172.580072.4
Yes61627.032827.5 −1.130427.530527.6 −0.2
Child’s depression (DSRS) <0.001 0.868
No (0–15)201888.497381.5 91983.292884.0
Yes (16+)23010.120917.5 −21.717615.916815.2 2.1
Missing351.5121.0 4.7100.990.8 0.8
Table 2. Distribution of answers about time usage before and after propensity score matching.
Table 2. Distribution of answers about time usage before and after propensity score matching.
Before PS MatchingAfter PS Matching
Hope for the Future n (%)n (%)
Time of playing gamesYesLess than 1 h/day (=0)1602 (70.2)767 (69.4)
More than 1 h/day (=1)619 (27.1)315 (28.5)
Missing62 (2.7)23 (2.1)
NoLess than 1 h/day (=0)766 (64.1)720 (65.2)
More than 1 h/day (=1)396 (33.2)357 (32.3)
Missing32 (2.7)28 (2.5)
Time to watch TV or DVDYesLess than 1 h/day (=0)679 (29.7)334 (30.2)
More than 1 h/day (=1)1538 (67.4)742 (67.1)
Missing66 (2.9)29 (2.6)
NoLess than 1 h/day (=0)382 (32.0)352 (31.9)
More than 1 h/day (=1)774 (64.8)719 (65.1)
Missing38 (3.2)34 (3.1)
Reading timeYesAlmost never (=0)545 (23.9)266 (24.1)
More than 1day/ week (=1)1660 (72.7)807 (73.0)
Missing78 (3.42)32 (2.9)
NoAlmost never (=0)380 (31.8)348 (31.5)
More than 1day/week (=1)772 (64.7)719 (65.1)
Missing42 (3.5)38 (3.4)
Studying time except in classYesAlmost never (=0)146 (6.4)65 (5.9)
More than 1day/week (=1)2089 (61.5)1034 (93.6)
Missing48 (2.1)6 (0.5)
NoAlmost never (=0)128 (10.7)117 (10.6)
More than 1day/week (=1)1045 (87.5)981 (88.8)
Missing21 (1.8)7 (0.6)
Table 3. Statistical results of academic performance and resilience before and after propensity score matching.
Table 3. Statistical results of academic performance and resilience before and after propensity score matching.
Before PS MatchingAfter PS Matching
Crude ModelAdjusted Model
Mean (SD)β95%CIp Valueβ95%CIp ValueMean (SD)β95%CIp Value
Self-rated academic performance in classYes3.03 (1.27)0.210.12 to 0.30<0.0010.190.10 to 0.28<0.0013.02 (1.28)0.210.10 to 0.32<0.001
No2.82 (1.28) 2.81 (1.28)
Child’s resilience (CRCS total score)Yes18.8 (5.58)1.771.35 to 2.19<0.0011.511.11 to 1.91<0.00119.1 (5.82)1.480.98 to 1.98<0.001
No20.6 (5.82) 20.8 (6.01)
Table 4. Statistical results of time usage before and after propensity score matching.
Table 4. Statistical results of time usage before and after propensity score matching.
Before PS MatchingAfter PS Matching
Crude ModelAdjusted Model
OR95%CIp ValueOR95%CIp ValueOR95%CIp Value
Time of playing games0.750.64 to 0.87<0.0010.810.69 to 0.970.0180.810.67 to 0.980.028
Time to watch TV or DVDs1.120.96 to 1.300.151.060.91 to 1.250.4491.050.87 to 1.260.61
Reading books1.501.28 to 1.75<0.0011.501.27 to 1.77<0.0011.451.19 to 1.75<0.001
Studying except in class11.77.93 to 15.5<0.0011.651.26 to 2.14<0.0011.891.37 to 2.61<0.001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kashiwabara, T.; Fujiwara, T.; Doi, S.; Yamaoka, Y. Association between Hope for the Future and Academic Performance in Adolescents: Results from the K-CHILD Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 11890. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191911890

AMA Style

Kashiwabara T, Fujiwara T, Doi S, Yamaoka Y. Association between Hope for the Future and Academic Performance in Adolescents: Results from the K-CHILD Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(19):11890. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191911890

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kashiwabara, Tomoka, Takeo Fujiwara, Satomi Doi, and Yui Yamaoka. 2022. "Association between Hope for the Future and Academic Performance in Adolescents: Results from the K-CHILD Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 19: 11890. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191911890

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop