Next Article in Journal
Work Ability after Breast Cancer: Study of Healthcare Personnel Operating in a Hospital of South Italy
Next Article in Special Issue
Climbing as an Add-On Treatment Option for Patients with Severe Anxiety Disorders and PTSD: Feasibility Analysis and First Results of a Randomized Controlled Longitudinal Clinical Pilot Trial
Previous Article in Journal
The Impact of COVID-19 on Physical (In)Activity Behavior in 10 Arab Countries
Previous Article in Special Issue
Psychopathological Impact in Patients with History of Rheumatic Fever with or without Sydenham’s Chorea: A Multicenter Prospective Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parental Practices and Environmental Differences among Infants Living in Upper-Middle and High-Income Countries: A Cross-Sectional Study

by
Carolina Fioroni Ribeiro da Silva
1,†,
Valentina Menici
2,3,†,
Eloisa Tudella
1,*,
Elena Beani
2,
Veronica Barzacchi
2,4,
Roberta Battini
2,5,
Alessandro Orsini
6,
Giovanni Cioni
2 and
Giuseppina Sgandurra
2,5
1
Postgraduate Program in Physiotherapy (PPG-Ft), Department of Physiotherapy, Nucleus of Studies in Neuropediatrics and Motricity (NENEM), Federal University of São Carlos (UFSCar), São Carlos 13565-905, Brazil
2
Department of Developmental Neuroscience, IRCCS Fondazione Stella Maris, Viale del Tirreno 331, 56128 Pisa, Italy
3
Ph.D. Programme in Clinical and Translational Sciences, University of Pisa, Via Roma 67, 56126 Pisa, Italy
4
Tuscan Ph.D. Programme of Neuroscience, University of Florence, 50121 Florence, Italy
5
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, Via Roma 67, 56126 Pisa, Italy
6
Pediatric Neurology, Pediatric Department, Santa Chiara University Hospital, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria Pisana, 56126 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this article and share first authorship.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(17), 10833; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710833
Submission received: 31 July 2022 / Revised: 24 August 2022 / Accepted: 26 August 2022 / Published: 31 August 2022

Abstract

:
Parental practices and environmental factors can impact a child’s development and, consequently, functionality. The objective is to assess the parental practices and environmental differences in healthy and at-risk infants at 3–6 months of age living in upper-middle (Brazil) and high-income (Italy) countries. A total group of 115 infants was identified and classified into four groups: healthy Italian infants (H_IT); Italian infants exposed to biological risk factors (R_IT); healthy Brazilian infants (H_BR); and Brazilian infants exposed to environmental risk factors (L_BR). The dependent variables were parental practices and environmental factors, which were assessed through a semi-structured interview and the “variety of stimulation dimension” from the Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development—Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS) questionnaire. Descriptive analyses, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), and correlation tests were applied. Regarding the environment and parental practices, the mother’s age, maternal and paternal education, civil status, and variety of stimulation showed significant differences among the infants living in Brazil or in Italy. There were strong dissimilarities in parental practices and environmental factors among infants living in low/upper-middle and high-income countries. Since the home environment is the main stimulus for infant growth and development, our results are meaningful for providing knowledge about these two different cultures.

1. Introduction

An infant’s nervous system is not a simple collection of reflexes, but a complex system, characterized by high brain plasticity, which can manage its motor behavior according to its adaptive needs and response to its interaction with the environment [1,2]. Infants are more susceptible to receive, interpret, and respond to sensory inputs and environmental demands during the first 1000 days of life, leading to the development of body structures, functions, and new skills [3]. This period is named the sensitive period because, at this time, a high level of neuroplasticity is present and, consequently, the infants are in an intensive process of neurogenesis, migration, synaptogenesis, axonal and dendritic growth, [4] and therefore able to receive, interpret, and respond to extrinsic stimuli [5,6]. In this way, parental practices, maternal and paternal education, socioeconomic status (SES), and the variety of stimulation offered in the home environment, i.e., extrinsic factors, can impact the child’s development, affecting motor and cognitive skills, learning, behavior, and, consequently, functionality [7,8].
When it comes to extrinsic factors, low SES is an alarming environmental factor worldwide. In Italy, a high-income country, 1.9 million people have been reported as living in poverty, representing about 7.5% of the total population [9]. Regarding the number of children, approximately 1.337 million children are harmed by poverty [10]. In upper-middle-income countries (UMIC), the picture is more complex, since 1.3 billion people live in multidimensional poverty [11], lacking healthcare, education, housing, political participation, gender equality, and economic and basic elements [12]. Furthermore, UMICs have about 644 million multidimensional poor children, higher than in high-income countries [11]. In Brazil, until the beginning of 2020, 40% of children and adolescents lived in poverty, and about 12% in extreme poverty. According to the United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund (UNICEF), and the United Nations (UN), extreme poverty proportionally doubles the impact on children and adolescents, compared to adults [13].
Low SES is associated with home chaos and the impaired socio-emotional, cognitive, and behavioral development of infants [14]. Home chaos is a disorganized and noisy environment that lacks a family routine, planning, and structure to perform daily activities [15]. In addition, it was observed that children from families in poverty can present alterations structural brain patterns, such as a reduction of the gray matter in the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes, and in the hippocampus [16]. This finding occurs, therefore, due to the events related to a low socioeconomic status, such as continuous noise, a low educational level, and the caregivers’ stress problems; in this way, these factors can interfere with attention and emotional levels [17].
The researchers show that the improvement of family stress and, consequently, parental care practices can produce an environment that promotes healthy development, even though infants are exposed to socioeconomic risk factors [18,19]. However, different cultural practices can interfere with motor development [20,21], and the countries present different ways to stimulate motor development and interact with infants (Karasik et al. 2015 [22]). It was observed, in a multicentric study, that infants who were born in Ghana and infants who were born in Norway presented the emergence of independent sitting at different ages, being at 5 and 7 months of age, respectively. The explanation for this fact may be that, in Ghana, mothers spontaneously provided a variety of stimuli in the sitting position, which helped the infant to acquire this skill. Conversely, Norwegian mothers did not encourage their children to perform the sitting posture as soon as possible [23].
The environment at home is crucial, and essential for the first months of infancy, due to the presence of stimuli for learning, responsive care, and organization in the home environment. Positive correlations were observed between parental practices, an adequate environment, and child development; infants and children aged 4 to 26 months succeeded in the domains of language, and in gross motor and socio-emotional development [24].
A variety of stimulation through parenting practices such as infant physical activity (e.g., tummy time) was longitudinally associated with advanced child development [25]. However, a study carried out in the United Kingdom, a high-income country, showed that just over 30% of families place their infants in the tummy time position [26]. Therefore, an Australian research group is studying strategies to make families aware and increase their adherence to adequate parental practices [27].
However, in UMICs such as Brazil, the mothers may have difficulties attending prenatal visits and maintaining a healthy pregnancy, probably because most are teenagers facing an unplanned pregnancy or lacking proper feeding, due to reduced economic resources [28]. Conversely, it is estimated that, in Italy, 94.3% of the mothers attend prenatal visits and receive health orientation from the third month of pregnancy [29]. In addition, it is known that biological risk factors, such as gestational age, increase the chances of the infant being at risk for neuromotor disorders. Approximately every year, 15 million babies are born preterm (before 37 completed weeks of gestation), and this number is rising. Preterm infants, even in high-income countries, are exposed to an increased risk of adverse neonatal outcomes and long-term neurodevelopmental and behavioral sequelae, lower cognitive functioning, and ongoing respiratory and other morbidities [30]. However, a suitable home environment shows benefits in cognitive ability, even for very preterm infants [31].
The environment plays an important role in neuromotor development, e.g., the earlier you optimize the home environment, the better the results in neuromotor development [32,33]. An assessment in order to create tools to improve the parental practices and environment is essential for providing the facilities that children need to fully develop during the first months of life, the sensitive period [34]. During this period, infants are in a constant process of neuroplasticity, that is, intense neurogenesis, migrations, neuronal, synaptogenesis, myelination, and axonal and dendritic growth, that is, able to receive, interpret and respond to extrinsic stimuli [6].
Thus, seeking more knowledge regarding parental practices and environmental factors in different cultures, SES, and populations, this study aimed to assess the parental practices and environmental differences in healthy and at-risk infants at 3–6 months of age living in upper-middle (Brazil) and high-income (Italy) countries.
Based on the research reported, it was hypothesized that Brazilian infants exposed to a lower socioeconomic level would show significantly different environment and parenting practices when compared to Italian infants.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

This is a retrospective, cross-sectional study, which follows the STROBE statement guidelines for observational studies [35].

2.2. Participants

A multicenter, retrospective study was designed involving the Department of Physiotherapy, Nucleus of Studies in Neuropediatrics and Motricity (NENEM) in Brazil, and the Department of Developmental Neuroscience of Stella Maris Foundation Italian Research and Clinical Scientific Institute (IRCCS) (Pisa) in Italy.
For the present retrospective study, infants and their caregivers were selected from medical records of basic health units, patient database collection, and from the maternity hospital of a medium-sized city. All the infants’ caregivers who attended the inclusion criteria were invited by phone or message to participate in the study.
The inclusion criteria were: (i) the gestational age of infants between 28 + 0 weeks and 42 + 0 weeks, and (ii) a clinical evaluation available at the infants’ corrected age between 3 and 6 months. The exclusion criteria defined were: (i) the presence of a brain injury, (ii) infants born small for their gestational age, (iii) a history of seizures, and (iv) a known genetic or metabolic disease.
The caregivers of these infants who agreed to answer the semi-structured interview were included in the study, and all of them agreed to participate. The study does not present missing data and dropouts. The infants were divided into four groups according to their characteristics, as reported by the caregiver or present in their medical records:
-
Healthy Italian infants (H_IT): Italian infants who were not exposed to environmental risk (i.e., low SES), and were not diagnosed with any health conditions when they were evaluated. The infants were not exposed to biological risks (i.e., without prenatal (small for their gestational age, suggesting intrauterine growth restriction), and perinatal (anoxia and hypoxia) or postnatal (neurological, hearing, visual, or sensory deficits, genetic syndromes, and musculoskeletal and cardiac abnormalities) impairments.
-
Italian infants exposed to biological risk factors (R_IT): Italian infants prematurely born (≤37 weeks) [8].
-
Healthy Brazilian infants (H_BR): composed of Brazilian infants who were not exposed to environmental risk, as written for the H_IT.
-
Brazilian infants exposed to environmental risk factors (L_BR): Brazilian infants exposed to low SES.

2.3. Procedures and Dependent Variables

The caregivers of infants were recruited by voluntary participation. The semi-structured interview was carried out face-to-face or remotely, according to the caregivers’ preference. It is divided into two parts:
  • Environmental factors: information regarding the infant’s and caregiver’s data, selected by the authors of this study. The dependent variables were: maternal age, number of children at home, number of adults at home, SES, maternal and paternal education, and civil status. The variables have been made dichotomous and qualitative (yes/no), such as the SES and civil status. Specifically, the SES for Brazilian families was determined by considering the income-to-poverty ratio (PIR) [36] and, for Italian families, according to the calculation of the poverty threshold as defined by the National Institute of Statistics [37]. In order to equalize the groups according to the countries’ income, both tools take into account the poverty level specific to the area of residence. The level of maternal and paternal education was classified according to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) [38] and categorized as low (ISCED levels < 3), intermediate (ISCED levels 3–4), or high (tertiary education, ISCED levels 5–8).
  • Parental practices: the area of “variety of stimulation dimension”, from the Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development—Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS) questionnaire [39]. It includes questions about the amount of time that the infant is awake and performing certain activities or postures, for instance, playing with other children; participating in games with practice-learning about body parts (for example, “where is your hand?”); being carried in an adult’s arms; being attached to the caregiver’s body or in some carrying device (baby bag, sling, cradleboard, etc.); in a seating device (high chair, stroller, car seat, or any other type of seating device); in a walking device (walker, exersaucer, or any other type of device that provides help for the child to walk and/or support themself while standing up); in a playpen, or other similar equipment, bed or crib; in tummy time play.
The semi-structured interview used is available in Appendix A. All the data were anonymized and coded in order to protect the data.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The analyses were carried out using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA, version 22). Descriptive analyses among the four groups and for each group were performed. The data were expressed as the mean and standard deviation or the median and confidence interval. The categorical data were presented as absolute and relative frequencies. The data normality distribution was verified using Shapiro–Wilk’s Test.
In order to compare the groups, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted on all the variables which compose the dependent variables, an environmental factor, and the parental practices among the four groups (H_IT, R_IT, H_BR, and L_BR), with age as a covariate. After this step, post hoc comparisons were carried out to explore the pair group differences. Significance values, set up for p-value ≤ 0.05, were adjusted by the Bonferroni test. Effect sizes were calculated to avoid type II errors, the interpretation of which was based on eta-squared (η2): η2 = 0.01 indicates a small effect; η2 = 0.06 indicates a medium effect; η2 = 0.14 indicates a large effect [40].
In addition, correlation tests were performed, whereby the researchers investigate negative or positive associations between the variety of stimulation versus the environmental and parental practice variables. The tests considered not only the variety of stimulation total scores but also the correlation between each question. Coefficients of determination (r2) were also presented as effect sizes for the correlation tests and interpreted as insignificant (<0.10), weak (0.10 to 0.39), moderate (0.40 to 0.69), strong (0.70 to 0.89), and very strong (0.90 to 1.00) (Schober, Boer, Schwarte, 2018).

2.5. Ethical Clearance

The study was approved by the human research ethics committee of the Federal University of São Carlos (CAAE: 04097718.9.0000.5504), and by the Tuscany Region Pediatric Ethics Committee (NCT01990183), following the resolution nº 510/2016 of the National Health Council and Declaration of Helsinki. All the caregivers accepted participating in the study through the consent form and providing written consent at the beginning of the data collection.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

A total of 115 infants (4.46 mean age ± 0.62 months, age range 3.09–5.95) participated in this study. The infants were distributed among the groups: H_IT (n = 34; mean age = 4.44 (±0.71)); R_IT (n = 23; mean age = 4.58 (±0.66)); H_BR (n = 31; mean age = 4.49 (±0.66)), and L_BR (n = 27 mean age = 4.34 (±0.63)) (Table 1).

3.2. Environment and Parental Practices

Regarding the environment and parental practices, the mother’s age (p-value < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.40; large effect size), education level (p-value < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.60; large effect size) and the education level of the father (p-value < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.49; large effect size), civil status (p-value = 0.01; ηp2 = 0.10; medium effect size), and variety of stimulation (p-value < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.27; large effect size) showed significant differences between the infants living in Brazil and those in Italy (Table 2), especially when there was an environmental risk factor, such as in the L_BR group, which obtained the lowest scores.
The post hoc analyses revealed that the mother’s age was statistically significantly different between the pair groups, except between the two healthy groups (H_IT and H-BR), and the variety of stimulation for motor development from AHEMD-IS was statistically significant between the pair groups, except between the two Italian groups (H_IT and R_IT). Moreover, the number of children and adults at home showed a significant difference between the groups, with the highest scores for the L_BR group (Table 3).

3.3. Environmental Factors and Parental Practices versus Variety of Stimulation

Regarding the total score of the variety of stimulation, positive correlations were found for (i) the maternal age (rho = 0.34, r2 = 0.11, p < 0.00); (ii) SES (rho = 0.48, r2 = 0.23, p < 0.00); and (iii) maternal and paternal education (rho = 0.42, r2 = 0.17, p < 0.00; rho = 0.28, r2 = 0.07, p < 0.00, respectively), while negative correlations were found only with the number of children at home (rho = −0.23, r2 = 0.05, p < 0.00). The variety of stimulation questions request information about the infants’ and children’s daily interactions. The variety of stimulation includes games that encourage the infant to learn about the body parts, the time that the infant spends carried in an adult’s arms, seated in a sitting device, standing in a standing device, e.g., walker, exersaucer, or any other type of device that provides help for the child to walk and/or support themself while standing up, in a playpen, playing in the tummy time position, and free to move, e.g., on the floor. Then, regarding these variables, the correlation analyses found positive correlations in the tummy time position, and the time that the infant is placed to move freely, with maternal age, and maternal and paternal education (Table 4).

4. Discussion

The present study verified that there is a significant difference in the parental practices and environments in healthy and at-risk infants at 3–6 months of age, living in upper-middle- (Brazil) and high-income (Italy) countries. The variety of stimulation for motor development was also different between infants living in Italy and Brazil, both in health and risk conditions. In high-income countries, the biological risk factor seems not to impact the variety of stimulation while, in the upper-middle-income countries, it is impacted by the environmental risk factors. It is crucial that, in high-income countries where the infants are well stimulated, they can continue to be stimulated also in risk conditions while, in the upper-middle-income countries, where the infants are already different for lower stimulation, the gap is even higher than when a risk situation is present. To our knowledge, so far there are no published studies that have evaluated these aspects, and this is a very crucial and novel finding of the present study.
Infants classified as having a low socioeconomic status, living in an upper-middle-income country (Brazil), presented low scores in maternal age, maternal and paternal education, and variety of stimulation, which may be explained by different access to quality education, scarce economic resources to survive, and consequently less knowledge about the stimulation of motor development [11,13].
The level of maternal and paternal education is an essential factor, not only for early childhood but also for school-age children, since parents with a high level of education adopt simple and easy parental practices, e.g., a disciplined routine of studies, which stimulates the potential for development and learning [41].
It is observed that there is gender inequality in the care of infants. Mothers are the protagonists of care more than fathers [42]. Consequently, it was observed that maternal education has more impact on child development when compared to paternal education [43].
In this context, in countries where there are education issues, i.e., UMIC, previous family knowledge on how to work horizontally with health professionals may be useful as orientation, and effective learning strategies (workshops, illustrative materials, practices on dolls, active methodology, and problem-based approaches) should be used to teach about a child’s motor skills development in order to improve and stimulate parental knowledge [44].
The World Health Organization and United Nations International Children’s Emergency Foundation encourage the implementation of responsive care and early learning to enhance early childhood development [8] and promote opportunities for full developmental potential, regardless of the risk exposure [34]. UNICEF has created some programs in UMICs in order to promote child development and empower caregivers to practice parenting positively, learning, responsive care, and stimulating the infant’s motor and cognitive abilities through websites and videos [45]. In addition, there are social and health programs especially for supporting growth and feeding [46]. However, in a big and diverse country like Brazil, the population may have not enough information, internet availability, and education [34] to access these programs. For instance, one of every four people in Brazil has no access to the internet. In total terms, this represents about 46 million people who are not able to access online health materials [47]. In this context, the local government needs to embrace the idea and provide the structure to allow the population to take advantage of these resources.
In addition, when it comes to planning public policies for early childhood, the managers should pay attention not only to mortality rates but also to stimulating the potential for child development, which is important for society to have an active and participating population. Overcoming mortality in the first year of life does not guarantee life and functionality, where wealth vs. vulnerability and opportunities vs. unemployment coexist, as in Brazil [48].
The present study observed that low maternal and paternal education is associated with poor parental practices and routines, such as the lack of games that encourage the infant to learn about parts of the body, the practice of the tummy time position, and if the parents leave the infant free to move, e.g., on the floor on a mattress.
The practice of the tummy time position showed important benefits for preventing obesity, a sedentary lifestyle, and plagiocephaly while improving motor performance, and the ability to move in prone, supine, crawling, and rolling positions [49]. However, parents must not only supervise but also interact and play with the infant, so that this is a pleasurable activity [50].
In Brazil, differently from Italy, it was observed that the caregivers never encourage infants in the tummy time position, although this activity is a component of the National Movement Guidelines in Australia, the United Kingdom, Canada, and South Africa [51,52,53,54], in the National Academy of Medicine, the American Academy of Pediatrics and the World Health Organization global guidelines for physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep for children <5 years of age. It is highly recommended that infants practice the tummy time position for at least thirty minutes, spread out over the course of every day [55].
Parental practices that encourage child development, such as face-to-face interaction, and leaving the infant free to move, are beneficial for the infant’s development and autonomy [56]. Another factor that is different between the analyzed countries is the maternal age groups. These data provide evidence that the mother’s age is a predictor of the risks, the older age for biological risk, and the younger age for environmental and parental practices risk.
The age difference between countries can be explained by differences in access to suitable quality education, gender inequality, lack of use or access to contraceptive methods, and participation in the labor market [57,58]. Even though the older mother has more risks of preterm birth, the maternal age showed a positive association with parental practices, e.g., the variety of stimulation, games that encourage the infant to learn about body parts, the practice of the tummy time position, space to move, learning time, and proposing suitable and recycled toys [59].
The present study shows some limitations. Firstly, even though the sample size is quite large, there is some heterogeneity in the different groups, as the different risk factors are environmental for the Brazilian groups, and biological for the Italian ones. Secondly, the semi-structured interview created by the authors was not validated, and other crucial variables were not investigated. In further studies, it might be interesting to consider other variables, such as the different types of parenting practices, the influence of kindergarten or home care, and to follow up by evaluating the motor and cognitive development of infants. It would also be interesting to consider how to implement the recommended parental practices, especially in UMIC.

5. Conclusions

Our results suggest that there are significant differences in the parental practices and environmental factors among infants living in low/middle- and high-income countries. Moreover, we have shown that risk factors affect parental practice only in the UMIC.
Low/middle-income countries presented low scores in the maternal age, maternal and paternal education, and variety of stimulation. The present study observed that low maternal and paternal education is associated with poor parental practices and routines such as, for instance, games that encourage the infant to learn about parts of the body, the practice of the tummy time position, and freedom for the infant to move, e.g., on the floor on a mattress. In low/middle-income countries, differently from in high-income countries, it was observed that the caregivers never encourage infants to practice the tummy time position. Since the home environment is the main stimuli for infant growth and development, our results are important for providing knowledge about the different cultures.
Overall, these findings highlight the importance of addressing new policies to improve awareness around the importance of providing enriched environments at home, especially in at-risk conditions, and also by means of technological tools [60,61].

Author Contributions

G.S. and E.T. conceived the idea for this original research and all other authors contributed to the conception and the design of the study. C.F.R.d.S., V.M. and E.B. carried out the enrolment of all infants and caregivers for the study. C.F.R.d.S. and V.M. performed the semi-structured interviews and data curation. G.S. and C.F.R.d.S. carried out the statistical analysis. C.F.R.d.S., V.M., G.S. and E.T. conceived and prepared the manuscript. V.B., R.B., A.O. and G.C. were responsible for the writing, reviewing and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially supported by the Italian Ministry of Health Grant RF-2013-02358095, Grant RC2019, and 5 X 1000 Health Research 2017, and the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) (process number 2018/24930-0; 2020/11267-1).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the human research ethics committee of the Federal University of São Carlos (CAAE: 04097718.9.0000.5504), and by the Tuscan Region Pediatric Ethics Committee (NCT01990183), following the resolution nº 510/2016 of the National Health Council and Declaration of Helsinki. All the caregivers accepted participating in the study through the consent form and providing written consent at the beginning of the data collection.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank caregivers of infants for their availability, and participation in the research, the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) (process number 2018/24930-0; 2020/11267-1), and the support of the funds from the Italian Ministry of Health and fund 5 × 1000.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

SEMI-STRUCTURED INTERVIEW
Name of the child ___________________________
Date of birth of the child ___________________________
Duration of pregnancy (weeks) ___________________________
Sex of the child
Male
Female
Mother age ___________________________
How many children live in the house or apartment (including the one involved in the study.)?
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
How many adults (>18 years) live in the house or apartment?
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
The civil status between the parents of the child:
  • Single or divorced
  • Married or civil union
Maternal education (ISCED classification):
  • Primary education
  • Lower secondary education
  • Upper secondary education
  • Bachelor’s or equivalent level
  • Master’s or equivalent level
  • Doctoral or equivalent level
Paternal education (ISCED classification):
  • Primary education
  • Lower secondary education
  • Upper secondary education
  • Bachelor’s or equivalent level
  • Master’s or equivalent level
  • Doctoral or equivalent level
Monthly family income
  • <1000
  • 1100–1200
  • 1300–1400
  • 1500–1600
  • 1700–1800
  • 1900–2000
  • >2000
AFFORDANCES IN THE HOME ENVIRONMENT FOR MOTOR DEVELOPMENT—INFANT SCALE (AHEMD-IS)—VARIETY OF STIMULATION DIMENSION
Reference: Caçola PM, Gabbard C, Montebelo MI, Santos DC. Further Development and Validation of the Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development-Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS). Phys Ther. 2015 Jun;95(6):901-23. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20140011. Epub 2014 Dec 18. PMID: 25524875.
During the day, but ONLY referring to the time spent in your home:
My/our child regularly (at least twice a week) plays with other children.
  • yes
  • no
I/we regularly (at least twice a week) play games with my/our child to practice learning about body parts (for example, where is your hand?).
  • yes
  • no
On a typical day, how would you describe the amount of AWAKE time your child spends in each of the situations below?
Carried in an adult’s arms, attached to a caregiver’s body or in some carrying device (baby bag, sling, cradleboard, etc.).
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always
In a seating device (high-chair, stroller, car seat, or any other type of seating device).
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always
In a walking device (walker, exersaucer, or any other type of device that provides help for the child to walk and/or support while standing up).
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always
In a playpen, or other similar equipment, bed or crib.
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always
In tummy time play.
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always
Free to move in any space of the house.
  • never
  • sometimes
  • almost always
  • always

References

  1. Cioni, G.; Sgandurra, G. Normal Psychomotor Development. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2013; Volume 111, pp. 3–15. ISBN 978-0-444-52891-9. [Google Scholar]
  2. Inguaggiato, E.; Sgandurra, G.; Cioni, G. Brain Plasticity and Early Development: Implications for Early Intervention in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Neuropsychiatrie l’Enfance l’Adolescence 2017, 65, 299–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Cusick, S.; Georgieff, M.K. The First 1000 Days of Life: The Brain’s Window of Opportunity. Available online: https://www.unicef-irc.org/article/958-the-first-1000-days-of-life-the-brains-window-of-opportunity.html (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  4. Hadders-Algra, M. Early Diagnosis and Early Intervention in Cerebral Palsy. Front. Neurol. 2014, 5, 185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R. Tactile Stimulation after Frontal or Parietal Cortical Injury in Infant Rats Facilitates Functional Recovery and Produces Synaptic Changes in Adjacent Cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 214, 115–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kolb, B.; Mychasiuk, R.; Williams, P.; Gibb, R. Brain Plasticity and Recovery from Early Cortical Injury. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2011, 53 (Suppl. S4), 4–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Araújo Rohr, L.; Cabral, T.I.; Moraes, M.M.d.; Tudella, E. Reaching Skills in Six-Month-Old Infants at Environmental and Biological Risk. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0254106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. World Health Organization. Improving Early Childhood Development. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/97892400020986 (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  9. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica (ISTAT) Absolute Poverty Stable in 2021. Available online: https://www.istat.it/it/files//2022/04/STAT_TODAY_POVERTA-ASSOLUTA_2021_eng.pdf (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  10. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica (ISTAT) Poverty in Italy, Year 2020. Available online: https://www.istat.it/it/files//2021/07/Poverty_2020_En.pdf (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  11. United Nations Children and Adolescents Were the Most Affected by Poverty in Brazil during the Pandemic. Available online: https://brasil.un.org/pt-br/175893-criancas-e-adolescentes-foram-os-mais-afetados-pela-pobreza-no-brasil-na-pandemia (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  12. Silva, J.J.D.; Bruno, M.A.P.; Silva, D.B.D.N. Pobreza Multidimensional No Brasil: Uma Análise Do Período 2004–2015. Brazil. J. Polit. Econ. 2020, 40, 138–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. United Nations Ending Poverty. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/global-issues/ending-poverty#:~:text=Poverty%20entails%20more%20than%20the,of%20participation%20in%20decision%2Dmaking (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  14. Marsh, S.; Dobson, R.; Maddison, R. The Relationship between Household Chaos and Child, Parent, and Family Outcomes: A Systematic Scoping Review. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Evans, G.W.; Wachs, T.D. Chaos and Its Influence on Children’s Development: An Ecological Perspective; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2010; ISBN 978-1-4338-0565-3. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hair, N.L.; Hanson, J.L.; Wolfe, B.L.; Pollak, S.D. Association of Child Poverty, Brain Development, and Academic Achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 822–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. Poverty, Stress, and Brain Development: New Directions for Prevention and Intervention. Acad. Pediatr. 2016, 16, S30–S36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sechi, C.; Prino, L.E.; Rollé, L.; Lucarelli, L.; Vismara, L. Maternal Attachment Representations during Pregnancy, Perinatal Maternal Depression, and Parenting Stress: Relations to Child’s Attachment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 19, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sgandurra, G.; Beani, E.; Inguaggiato, E.; Lorentzen, J.; Nielsen, J.B.; Cioni, G. Effects on Parental Stress of Early Home-Based CareToy Intervention in Low-Risk Preterm Infants. Neural Plast. 2019, 2019, 7517351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Valentini, N.C.; Nobre, G.C.; Gonçalves Duarte, M. Gross Motor Skills Trajectory Variation between WEIRD and LMIC Countries: A Cross-Cultural Study. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0267665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Karasik, L.B.; Adolph, K.E.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Bornstein, M.H. WEIRD Walking: Cross-Cultural Research on Motor Development. Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 95–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Karasik, L.B.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Adolph, K.E.; Bornstein, M.H. Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2015, 46, 1023–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Who Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group; Onis, M. Assessment of Sex Differences and Heterogeneity in Motor Milestone Attainment among Populations in the WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study: Assessment of Differences in Motor Development. Acta Paediatr. 2007, 95, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kuhn, L.; Liu, C.; Wang, T.; Luo, R. Home Environment and Early Development of Rural Children: Evidence from Guizhou Province in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 6121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Carson, V.; Zhang, Z.; Predy, M.; Pritchard, L.; Hesketh, K.D. Longitudinal Associations between Infant Movement Behaviours and Development. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2022, 19, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hesketh, K.R.; Janssen, X. Movement Behaviours and Adherence to Guidelines: Perceptions of a Sample of UK Parents with Children 0-18 Months. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2022, 19, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hewitt, L.; Frohmuller, C.; Wen, L.M.; Okely, A.D. Effect of a Multicomponent Intervention in Postnatal Mothers’ Groups on Meeting the Australian Physical Activity Guidelines for Infants: Protocol for a Randomised Controlled Trial. BMJ Open 2022, 12, e054183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Esposti, C.D.D.; Santos-Neto, E.T.d.; Oliveira, A.E.; Travassos, C.; Pinheiro, R.S. Desigualdades Sociais e Geográficas No Desempenho Da Assistência Pré-Natal de Uma Região Metropolitana Do Brasil. Ciênc. Saúde Coletiva 2020, 25, 1735–1750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica (ISTAT) Pregnancy, Childbirth and Breastfeeding in Italy. Available online: https://www.istat.it/en/archive/141520 (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  30. World Health Organization Preterm Birth. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  31. Vanes, L.D.; Hadaya, L.; Kanel, D.; Falconer, S.; Ball, G.; Batalle, D.; Counsell, S.J.; Edwards, A.D.; Nosarti, C. Associations Between Neonatal Brain Structure, the Home Environment, and Childhood Outcomes Following Very Preterm Birth. Biol. Psychiatry Glob. Open Sci. 2021, 1, 146–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Dusing, S.C.; Lobo, M.A.; Lee, H.-M.; Galloway, J.C. Intervention in the First Weeks of Life for Infants Born Late Preterm: A Case Series. Pediatric Phys. Ther. 2013, 25, 194–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hadders-Algra, M.; Boxum, A.G.; Hielkema, T.; Hamer, E.G. Effect of Early Intervention in Infants at Very High Risk of Cerebral Palsy: A Systematic Review. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2017, 59, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kohli-Lynch, M.; Ponce Hardy, V.; Bernal Salazar, R.; Bhopal, S.S.; Brentani, A.; Cavallera, V.; Goh, E.; Hamadani, J.D.; Hughes, R.; Manji, K.; et al. Human Resources and Curricula Content for Early Child Development Implementation: Multicountry Mixed Methods Evaluation. BMJ Open 2020, 10, e032134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Von Elm, E.; Altman, D.G.; Egger, M.; Pocock, S.J.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Vandenbroucke, J.P. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: Guidelines for Reporting Observational Studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2008, 61, 344–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Karlamangla, A.S.; Merkin, S.S.; Crimmins, E.M.; Seeman, T.E. Socioeconomic and Ethnic Disparities in Cardiovascular Risk in the United States, 2001–2006. Ann. Epidemiol. 2010, 20, 617–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica (ISTAT) Calcolo Della Soglia Di Povertà Assoluta. Available online: https://www.istat.it/it/dati-analisi-e-prodotti/contenuti-interattivi/soglia-di-poverta (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  38. Eurostat International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED). Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php?title=International_Standard_Classification_of_Education_(ISCED) (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  39. Caçola, P.M.; Gabbard, C.; Montebelo, M.I.L.; Santos, D.C.C. The New Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development—Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS): Versions in English and Portuguese Languages. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 2015, 19, 507–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Hillsdale, N.J., Ed.; L. Erlbaum Associates: Oxfordshire, UK, 1988; ISBN 978-1-134-74270-7. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tamayo Martinez, N.; Xerxa, Y.; Law, J.; Serdarevic, F.; Jansen, P.W.; Tiemeier, H. Double Advantage of Parental Education for Child Educational Achievement: The Role of Parenting and Child Intelligence. Eur. J. Public Health 2022, ckac044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Johnston, R.M.; Mohammed, A.; van der Linden, C. Evidence of Exacerbated Gender Inequality in Child Care Obligations in Canada and Australia during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Pol. Gen. 2020, 16, 1131–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Swaminathan, A.; Lahaie Luna, M.; Rennicks White, R.; Smith, G.; Rodger, M.; Wen, S.W.; Walker, M.; Corsi, D.J. The Influence of Maternal and Paternal Education on Birth Outcomes: An Analysis of the Ottawa and Kingston (OaK) Birth Cohort. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2022, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Torquato, I.M.B.; Collet, N.; Forte, F.D.S.; França, J.R.F.d.S.; Silva, M.d.F.d.O.C.; Reichert, A.P.d.S. Effectiveness of an Intervention with Mothers to Stimulate Children under Two Years. Rev. Latino-Am. Enferm. 2019, 27, e3216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. UNICEF Parenting. Available online: https://www.unicef.org/lac/en/parenting-lac (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  46. UNICEF Mapping of Parenting Programmes for Adolescents in Latin America and the Caribbean. Available online: https://www.unicef.org/lac/media/34526/file/Mapping-of-Parenting-Programmes.pdf (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  47. Tokarnia, M. One of Every Four Brazilians Has No Internet Access. Available online: https://agenciabrasil.ebc.com.br/en/en/economia/noticia/2020-04/one-every-four-brazilians-has-no-internet-access (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  48. Schenkman, S.; Bousquat, A.; Ferreira, M.P. Efficiency Analysis in Brazil’s Sao Paulo State Local Unified Health System (SUS): From Gender-Ethnicity-Power Inequities to the Dissolution of Health Effectiveness. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 2990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Hewitt, L.; Kerr, E.; Stanley, R.M.; Okely, A.D. Tummy Time and Infant Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics 2020, 145, e20192168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Palmer, C.F.; Rindler, D.; Leverone, B. Moving into Tummy Time, Together: Touch and Transitions Aid Parent Confidence and Infant Development. Infant. Ment. Health J. 2019, 40, 277–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Indolfi, G.; Hierro, L.; Dezsofi, A.; Jahnel, J.; Debray, D.; Hadzic, N.; Czubkowski, P.; Gupte, G.; Mozer-Glassberg, Y.; van der Woerd, W.; et al. Treatment of Chronic Hepatitis C Virus Infection in Children: A Position Paper by the Hepatology Committee of European Society of Paediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2018, 66, 505–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Okely, A.D.; Ghersi, D.; Hesketh, K.D.; Santos, R.; Loughran, S.P.; Cliff, D.P.; Shilton, T.; Grant, D.; Jones, R.A.; Stanley, R.M.; et al. A Collaborative Approach to Adopting/Adapting Guidelines—The Australian 24-Hour Movement Guidelines for the Early Years (Birth to 5 Years): An Integration of Physical Activity, Sedentary Behavior, and Sleep. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Tremblay, M.S.; Chaput, J.-P.; Adamo, K.B.; Aubert, S.; Barnes, J.D.; Choquette, L.; Duggan, M.; Faulkner, G.; Goldfield, G.S.; Gray, C.E.; et al. Canadian 24-Hour Movement Guidelines for the Early Years (0–4 Years): An Integration of Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour, and Sleep. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Reilly, J.J.; Hughes, A.; Janssen, X.; Martin, A.; Hesketh, K.; Livingstone, S.; Hill, C.; Kipping, R.; Draper, C.; Okely, A.; et al. Expert Working Group Working Paper: Under 5s. UK Physical Activity Guidelines: Draft Review and Recommendations for the Under 5s 2018. Available online: https://www.bristol.ac.uk/media-library/sites/sps/documents/cmo/scm-slides-under-fives.pdf (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  55. World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 (accessed on 22 August 2022).
  56. Franchak, J.M.; Kretch, K.S.; Adolph, K.E. See and Be Seen: Infant-Caregiver Social Looking during Locomotor Free Play. Dev. Sci. 2018, 21, e12626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Guarga Montori, M.; Álvarez Martínez, A.; Luna Álvarez, C.; Abadía Cuchí, N.; Mateo Alcalá, P.; Ruiz-Martínez, S. Advanced Maternal Age and Adverse Pregnancy Outcomes: A Cohort Study. Taiwan J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2021, 60, 119–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Mills, M.; Rindfuss, R.R.; McDonald, P.; te Velde, E.; ESHRE Reproduction and Society Task Force. Why Do People Postpone Parenthood? Reasons and Social Policy Incentives. Hum. Reprod. Update 2011, 17, 848–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Rohr, L.A.; Cabral, T.I.; Weber, M.D.; Tudella, E. Manual Exploratory Behaviors in Six-Month-Old Infants at Environmental and Biological Risk. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2022, 129, 104317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Sgandurra, G.; Lorentzen, J.; Inguaggiato, E.; Bartalena, L.; Beani, E.; Cecchi, F.; Dario, P.; Giampietri, M.; Greisen, G.; Herskind, A.; et al. A Randomized Clinical Trial in Preterm Infants on the Effects of a Home-Based Early Intervention with the “CareToy System”. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0173521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Sgandurra, G.; Beani, E.; Giampietri, M.; Rizzi, R.; Cioni, G.; CareToy-R Consortium. Early Intervention at Home in Infants with Congenital Brain Lesion with CareToy Revised: A RCT Protocol. BMC Pediatr. 2018, 18, 295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Characteristics of infants.
Table 1. Characteristics of infants.
VariablesH_IT
(n = 34)
R_IT
(n = 23)
H_BR
(n = 31)
L_BR
(n = 27)
Mother of age
mean (SD)
32.47 (3.47)37.39 (5.35)31.87 (7.10)24.30 (5.67)
Number of children at home median (min–max)1.00 (1.00–4.00)2.00 (1.00–3.00)2.00 (1.00–3.00)2.00 (1.00–6.00)
Number of adults at home
median (min–max)
2.00 (1.00–2.00)2.00 (2.00–3.00)2.00 (1.00–6.00)2.00 (1.00–7.00)
Civil status/married people
n (%)
15 (44.11%)5 (21.73%)28 (90.32%)9 (29.03%)
Maternal education
mean (SD)
5.41 (1.78)4.74 (1.66)4.65 (1.31)0.89 (0.80)
Paternal education
mean (SD)
4.85 (1.52)4.78 (1.62)4.26 (1.44)1.41 (1.08)
Variety of stimulation score
mean (SD)
12.79 (2.27)13.30 (1.99)11.68 (2.15)9.74 (2.31)
Legend: healthy Italian infants (H_IT); Italian infants exposed to biological risk factors (R_IT); healthy Brazilian infants (H_BR); Brazilian infants exposed to environmental risk factors (L_BR).
Table 2. Comparison between the groups.
Table 2. Comparison between the groups.
Error dfFp-ValuePartial Eta Squared
Gestational age (weeks)111.002.510.060.06
Age at assessment111.000.650.580.02
M = 1; F = 2111.000.570.630.02
Maternal age111.0024.500.000.40
Number of children at home111.008.120.000.18
Number of the adults in the house111.003.810.010.09
Maternal education: ISCED classification111.0056.440.000.60
Paternal education: ISCED classification111.0035.990.000.49
Civil status (1: married or civil union, 0: no)111.004.150.010.10
SES (not Low: 1, low: 0)111.0057.420.001.00
Total score of Variety of Stimulation111.0013.840.000.27
Interpretation of Variety of Stimulation111.0011.200.000.23
Legend: (M) male; (F) female; (ISCED) International Standard Classification of Education; (SES) socioeconomic status.
Table 3. Post hoc analyses in order to compare the group.
Table 3. Post hoc analyses in order to compare the group.
L_BR vs. R_ITL_BR vs. H_BRL_BR vs. H_ITH_BR vs. R_ITH_BR vs. H_ITH_IT vs. R_IT
Mean Difference [95% CI]p-ValueMean Difference [95% CI]p-ValueMean Difference [95% CI]p-ValueMean Difference [95% CI]p-ValueMean Difference [95% CI]p-ValueMean Difference [95% CI]p-Value
Mother age−13.10 [−17.3/−8.89]0.00−7.57 [−11.47/−3.68]0.00−8.17 [−11.99/−4.36]0.00−5.52 [−9.60/−1.45]0.00−0.60 [−4.28/−3.08]1.00−4.92 [−8.92/−0.92]0.01
Number of children at home1.06 [0.36/1.75]0.000.86 [0.22/1.50]0.001.02 [0.39/1.65]0.000.20 [−0.48/0.87]1.000.16 [−0.45/0.77]1.000.04 [−0.62/0.70]1.00
Number of adults in the house0.47 [−0.14/1.07]0.240.30 [−0.26/0.86]0.940.67 [0.12/1.22]0.010.17 [−0.42/0.76]1.000.38 [−0.15/0.91]0.36−0.20 [0.78/0.37]1.00
Maternal education: ISCED classification−3.85 [−4.95/−2.75]0.00−3.76 [−4.78/−2.73]0.00−4.52 [−5.52/−3.52]0.00−0.09 [−1.16/0.98]1.00−0.77 [−1.73/0.20]0.210.67 [−0.38/1.72]0.53
Paternal education: ISCED classification−3.38 [−4.46/−2.29]0.00−2.85 [−3.86/−1.84]0.00−3.45 [−4.44/−2.46]0.00−0.52 [−1.58/0.53]1.00−0.59 [−1.55/0.36]0.580.07 [−0.97/1.11]1.00
Civil status (1: yes, 0: no)0.03 [−0.28/0.34]1.00−0.12 [−0.41/0.16]1.000.23 [−0.05/0.51]0.190.15 [−0.15/0.45]1.000.35 [0.08/0.62]0.01−0.19 [−0.49/0.10]0.46
SES (not low: 1, low: 0)−1.00 [−1.00/−1.00]0.00−1.00 [−1.00/−1.00]0.00−1.00 [−1.00/−1.00]0.000.00 [0.00/0.00]1.000.00 [0.00/0.00]1.000.00 [0.00/0.00]1.00
Total score of Variety of Stimulation−3.56 [−5.24/−1.89]0.00−1.94 [−3.49/−0.39]0.01−3.05 [−4.47/−1.53]0.00−1.63 [−3.25/0.00]0.05−1.12 [−2.58/0.35]0.26−0.51 [−2.10/1.08]1.00
Interpretation of Variety of Stimulation−1.35 [−2.08/−0.63]0.00−0.71 [−1.38/−0.03]0.04−1.22 [−1.88/−0.56]0.00−0.65 [−1.35/0.06]0.09−0.52 [−1.15/0.12]0.19−0.13 [−0.82/0.56]1.00
Legend: healthy Italian infants (H_IT); Italian infants exposed to biological risk factors (R_IT); healthy Brazilian infants (H_BR); Brazilian infants exposed to environmental risk factors (L_BR); International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED); socioeconomic status (SES).
Table 4. Pearson correlation between environmental factors and parental practices versus questions which compose the variety of stimulation dimension from AHEMD-IS.
Table 4. Pearson correlation between environmental factors and parental practices versus questions which compose the variety of stimulation dimension from AHEMD-IS.
I/We Regularly (at Least Twice a Week) Play Games with my/our Child to Practice Learning about Body Parts. (e.g.,
Where Is Your Hand?)
My/our Baby Plays Regularly (at Least Twice a Week) with Other ChildrenCarried in Adult Arms, Attached to Caregiver’s Body or in some Carrying Device (Baby Bag, Sling, Cradleboard, etc).In a Seating Device (High Chair, Stroller, Car Seat, or any Other Type of Seating Device).In a Walking Device (Walker, Exersaucer, or any Other Type of Device that Provides Help for the Child to Walk and/or Support While Standing
up).
In a Playpen, or Other Similar Equipment, Bed or crib.In the Tummy Time Play.Free to Move in any Space of the HouseTotal Score of Variety of Stimulation
Age at assessment0.0160.0040.0450.0690.043−0.0010.0410.270 **0.203 *
Sex0.019−0.1270.101−0.171−0.009−0.074−0.009−0.008−0.088
maternal age0.1370.0930.095−0.059−0.015−0.0760.317 **0.469 **0.344 **
number of children at home−0.288 **0.211 *0.049−0.1140.044−0.129−0.188 *−0.190 *−0.232 **
number of the adults in the house0.0190.019−0.0580.1370.0820.114−0.176 *−0.225 **−0.071
Maternal schoolarity: ISCED classification0.271 **0.0480.0540.0130.0540.0560.333 **0.455 **0.428 **
Paternal schoolarity:ISCED classification0.205 *0.0860.0060.0170.0630.0200.199 *0.363 **0.287 **
civil status−0.1410.0180.192 *−0.078−0.0240.085−0.096−0.002−0.019
SES0.299 **0.0330.024−0.0040.0480.0030.420 **0.531 **0.484 **
Legend: healthy Italian infants (H_IT); Italian infants exposed to biological risk factors (R_IT); healthy Brazilian infants (H_BR); Brazilian infants exposed to environmental risk factors (L_BR). * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level. ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fioroni Ribeiro da Silva, C.; Menici, V.; Tudella, E.; Beani, E.; Barzacchi, V.; Battini, R.; Orsini, A.; Cioni, G.; Sgandurra, G. Parental Practices and Environmental Differences among Infants Living in Upper-Middle and High-Income Countries: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 10833. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710833

AMA Style

Fioroni Ribeiro da Silva C, Menici V, Tudella E, Beani E, Barzacchi V, Battini R, Orsini A, Cioni G, Sgandurra G. Parental Practices and Environmental Differences among Infants Living in Upper-Middle and High-Income Countries: A Cross-Sectional Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(17):10833. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710833

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fioroni Ribeiro da Silva, Carolina, Valentina Menici, Eloisa Tudella, Elena Beani, Veronica Barzacchi, Roberta Battini, Alessandro Orsini, Giovanni Cioni, and Giuseppina Sgandurra. 2022. "Parental Practices and Environmental Differences among Infants Living in Upper-Middle and High-Income Countries: A Cross-Sectional Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 17: 10833. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710833

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop