Next Article in Journal
The Effectiveness of a Four-Week Digital Physiotherapy Intervention to Improve Functional Capacity and Adherence to Intervention in Patients with Long COVID-19
Next Article in Special Issue
Different Exercise Types Produce the Same Acute Inhibitory Control Improvements When the Subjective Intensity Is Equal
Previous Article in Journal
Age and Sex Differences in the State and Relationships between Process and Product Assessments of Fundamental-Motor Skills in Five to Eight-Year-Olds: The ExAMIN Youth SA Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

Physical Exercise and Cognitive Function

by
Diego Pastor
*,
Juan Arturo Ballester-Ferrer
,
Laura Carbonell-Hernández
,
Sabina Baladzhaeva
and
Eduardo Cervello
Sports Research Centre, Department of Sport Sciences, Miguel Hernández University of Elche, 03202 Elche, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(15), 9564; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159564
Submission received: 28 July 2022 / Accepted: 29 July 2022 / Published: 3 August 2022

1. Livelong Cognitive Function

Cognitive skills are relevant predictors of academic achievement, employability, socioeconomic success, health, and longevity [1]. It has been shown that cognitive skills are consolidated during adolescence and achieve maximum efficiency during youth [2]. However, during the aging process, there is a decrease in cognitive function with the years [3]. The cognitive function decline can be explained by the deterioration of the central nervous system during aging. Moreover, brain volume is reduced in regions including the frontal, parietal and temporal lobes and is possibly linked to the observed reductions in brain blood flow [4]. In addition, the decline in the hippocampus volume has been related to cognitive decline during aging [5]. Hippocampus is a cerebral structure that plays a central role in processes associated with declarative and visuospatial memory [6].
Moreover, some molecular changes can accelerate cognitive decline. These alternations include decreased levels of neurotrophic factors, such as BDNF (Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor) and IGF-1 (Insulin-like Growth Factor-1), both leading to impaired neuronal survival and synaptic decline [7]. In addition, at a vascular level, reduced production of VEGF (Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor), a potent angiogenic factor, disrupts the creation of new blood vessels [8].
Cognitive decline affects activities of daily living in older people. However, it is essential to know that the decline rate is different between individuals and is influenced by many factors [1]. Consequently, it is crucial to identify the factors that can explain the slower rate of cognitive decline. For example, both physical activity and exercise have been shown to attenuate it [9]. Therefore, understanding the mechanisms responsible for this cognitive protection with physical exercise is essential.

2. Physical Inactivity, Physical Activity, and Physical Exercise Relationship with Cognitive Function

The study of physical inactivity’s effects on cognitive decline is an exciting way to understand the relevance of physical activity. Furthermore, physical inactivity has been considered the most significant health problem in the XXI century [10]. Physical inactivity leads to a range of adverse health consequences, including cognitive decline [11] and an increased risk of neurodegenerative diseases in older adults [12].
Physical activity and physical exercise are erroneously equated in some texts. It is important to differentiate both terms to understand the mechanisms regarding cognitive function [13]. Physical activity is any muscle movement that increases energy expenditure above the resting metabolic rate [14]. In contrast, physical exercise is a subset of physical activity that is planned, structured, repetitive and performed to improve or maintain one or more dimensions of fitness [14].
The term “physical exercise” is more common in the literature, as experimental research uses planned, structured and repetitive activities to study the relationship between physical exercise and cognition. A large body of research demonstrates the benefits of both acute [13,15,16,17,18,19] and chronic physical exercise [13,17,18,19,20,21] in improving cognitive function.
However, there are still many questions about the optimal type or dose of physical exercise to improve cognition efficiently. Moreover, we still do not know the influence of major moderators in this relationship. In the next section, we will discuss the role of some of the common moderators and highlight important questions that the research must still resolve to understand the relationship between physical exercise and cognitive function.

3. Physical Exercise and Cognitive Function. An Actual Point of View of Its Relationship

Many factors can moderate the physical exercise and cognitive response relationship [18,19]. Stillman, Cohen, Lehman and Erickson [18] classified the mechanisms moderating the exercise–cognition relationship in three levels. Thus, their classification explains cognitive improvements through (a) molecular and cellular modifications (level 1), (b) changes in brain structure and functional changes (level 2), and (c) behavioral and socioemotional changes (level 3). However, recent research has focused on the analysis of the first two levels of the exercise–cognition relationship; meanwhile, the behavioral influences remain unexplored [18]. As such, level one changes are most evident from the animal models, where observed benefits of physical exercise on the hippocampus imply enhanced spatial learning, memory, and exploration, all of which could be moderated by increased neurogenesis. [22]. In turn, the structural changes of level two have been reported in older adults, where physical exercise programs promoted increased hippocampal volume [23].
However, other moderators such as sleep, mood, or psychological well-being have been studied less extensively, despite some promising research [18]. For example, exercise was shown to improve psychological well-being [24] and modulate learning and academic achievement in young students [25,26]. Consequently, more research on level three should be interesting to improve our understanding of the exercise–cognition relationship.
Beyond these three levels, other moderators must be considered to increase the efficiency of the exercise–cognition relationship. We commonly use the acronym FITT to speak about distinct variables of the physical exercise prescription: Frequency, Intensity, Time and Type. There is much research on exercise intensity, which can significantly moderate the cognitive response after an acute exercise [27], with the most improvements attributable to high-intensity exercise. In terms of the time variable, there are some indications that low-volume sessions are conducive to the most benefits [27].
Regarding exercise type, most research articles analyze the effects of aerobic exercise on cognition. However, there is a growing research interest on the impact of resistance training on cognitive response, with some evidence that suggests positive improvements after both acute and chronic resistance exercise [17,20,28]. On the other hand, there is still a high heterogeneity in both the effects of resistance training and design protocols [20], and further research is needed on the dose–response effects for cognitive enhancement [17]. Moreover, we have an absolute lack of knowledge about frequency.
Individualized protocols should be considered to improve the efficiency of the exercise prescription for cognitive benefits. However, we still lack knowledge about specific populations. For example, most research has focused on older people and children [19], but adolescents and young people have been studied less [19]. Moreover, we already know that sex can be a powerful moderator in the exercise–cognition relationship [21,29], but there are questions remaining about the influence of sex. For example, some studies have observed higher levels of BDNF in men than in women, both after acute and chronic physical exercise [30]. In contrast, other studies have found better cognitive response after aerobic exercise in women rather than in men [29].
In conclusion, future research should aim at resolving these questions to promote evidence-based exercise prescription to improve cognitive function.

Author Contributions

Writing—original draft preparation, J.A.B.-F. and D.P.; writing—review and editing, L.C.-H., E.C. and S.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Grant RTI2018-098335-B-I00 funded by Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación: MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lovden, M.; Fratiglioni, L.; Glymour, M.M.; Lindenberger, U.; Tucker-Drob, E.M. Education and Cognitive Functioning Across the Life Span. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest A J. Am. Psychol. Soc. 2020, 21, 6–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Luna, B.; Marek, S.; Larsen, B.; Tervo-Clemmens, B.; Chahal, R. An integrative model of the maturation of cognitive control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 38, 151–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Bherer, L.; Erickson, K.I.; Liu-Ambrose, T. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J. Aging Res. 2013, 2013, 657508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Kirk-Sanchez, N.J.; McGough, E.L. Physical exercise and cognitive performance in the elderly: Current perspectives. Clin. Interv. Aging 2014, 9, 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Bettio, L.E.B.; Rajendran, L.; Gil-Mohapel, J. The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 79, 66–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Squire, L.R. Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev. 1992, 99, 195–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kowianski, P.; Lietzau, G.; Czuba, E.; Waskow, M.; Steliga, A.; Morys, J. BDNF: A Key Factor with Multipotent Impact on Brain Signaling and Synaptic Plasticity. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2017, 38, 579–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Ladoux, A.; Frelin, C. Expression of vascular endothelial growth factor by cultured endothelial cells from brain microvessels. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 1993, 194, 799–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blondell, S.J.; Hammersley-Mather, R.; Veerman, J.L. Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Blair, S.N.; Sallis, R.E.; Hutber, A.; Archer, E. Exercise therapy—The public health message. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2012, 22, e24–e28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Falck, R.S.; Davis, J.C.; Milosevic, E.; Liu-Ambrose, T. How much will older adults exercise? A feasibility study of aerobic training combined with resistance training. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2017, 3, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Paillard, T. Preventive effects of regular physical exercise against cognitive decline and the risk of dementia with age advancement. Sports Med. Open 2015, 1, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Herold, F.; Muller, P.; Gronwald, T.; Muller, N.G. Dose-Response Matters!—A Perspective on the Exercise Prescription in Exercise-Cognition Research. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Caspersen, C.J.; Powell, K.E.; Christenson, G.M. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 1985, 100, 126–131. [Google Scholar]
  15. Chang, Y.K.; Labban, J.D.; Gapin, J.I.; Etnier, J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Pontifex, M.B.; McGowan, A.L.; Chandler, M.C.; Gwizdala, K.L.; Parks, A.C.; Fenn, K.; Kamijo, K. A primer on investigating the after effects of acute bouts of physical activity on cognition. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019, 40, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Soga, K.; Masaki, H.; Gerber, M.; Ludyga, S. Acute and Long-term Effects of Resistance Training on Executive Function. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2018, 2, 200–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stillman, C.M.; Cohen, J.; Lehman, M.E.; Erickson, K.I. Mediators of Physical Activity on Neurocognitive Function: A Review at Multiple Levels of Analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Stillman, C.M.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Brown, B.; Bender, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. Effects of Exercise on Brain and Cognition Across Age Groups and Health States. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Landrigan, J.F.; Bell, T.; Crowe, M.; Clay, O.J.; Mirman, D. Lifting cognition: A meta-analysis of effects of resistance exercise on cognition. Psychol. Res. 2020, 84, 1167–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Pühse, U.; Looser, V.N.; Kamijo, K. Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 603–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. van Praag, H. Neurogenesis and exercise: Past and future directions. Neuromol. Med. 2008, 10, 128–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Erickson, K.I.; Voss, M.W.; Prakash, R.S.; Basak, C.; Szabo, A.; Chaddock, L.; Kim, J.S.; Heo, S.; Alves, H.; White, S.M.; et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 3017–3022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Cervelló, E.; Peruyero, F.; Montero, C.; González-Cutre, D.; Beltrán-Carrillo, V.J.; Moreno-Murcia, J.A. Exercise, psychological well-being, sleep quality and situational motivation in physical education students. Cuad. Psicol. 2014, 14, 31–38. [Google Scholar]
  25. Garcia, D.; Jimmefors, A.; Mousavi, F.; Adrianson, L.; Rosenberg, P.; Archer, T. Self-regulatory mode (locomotion and assessment), well-being (subjective and psychological), and exercise behavior (frequency and intensity) in relation to high school pupils’ academic achievement. PeerJ 2015, 3, e847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Yu, L.; Shek, D.T.L.; Zhu, X. The Influence of Personal Well-Being on Learning Achievement in University Students Over Time: Mediating or Moderating Effects of Internal and External University Engagement. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Oberste, M.; Javelle, F.; Sharma, S.; Joisten, N.; Walzik, D.; Bloch, W.; Zimmer, P. Effects and Moderators of Acute Aerobic Exercise on Subsequent Interference Control: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Herold, F.; Torpel, A.; Schega, L.; Muller, N.G. Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements—A systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. Off. J. Eur. Group Res. Into Elder. Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Barha, C.K.; Davis, J.C.; Falck, R.S.; Nagamatsu, L.S.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2017, 46, 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Szuhany, K.L.; Bugatti, M.; Otto, M.W. A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2015, 60, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pastor, D.; Ballester-Ferrer, J.A.; Carbonell-Hernández, L.; Baladzhaeva, S.; Cervello, E. Physical Exercise and Cognitive Function. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 9564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159564

AMA Style

Pastor D, Ballester-Ferrer JA, Carbonell-Hernández L, Baladzhaeva S, Cervello E. Physical Exercise and Cognitive Function. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(15):9564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159564

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pastor, Diego, Juan Arturo Ballester-Ferrer, Laura Carbonell-Hernández, Sabina Baladzhaeva, and Eduardo Cervello. 2022. "Physical Exercise and Cognitive Function" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 15: 9564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159564

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop