Next Article in Journal
How Does Gender Moderate Customer Intention of Shopping via Live-Streaming Apps during the COVID-19 Pandemic Lockdown Period?
Next Article in Special Issue
Disease Progression Detection via Deep Sequence Learning of Successive Radiographic Scans
Previous Article in Journal
Point of Sale Advertising and Promotion of Cigarettes, Electronic Cigarettes, and Heated Tobacco Products in Warsaw, Poland—A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Validation of an Arabic Version of the Self-Efficacy for Appropriate Medication Use Scale
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Scientometric Analysis of Disaster Risk Perception: 2000–2020

1
School of Engineering and Technology, China University of Geosciences (Beijing), Beijing 100083, China
2
Department of Architecture and Civil Engineering, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong 999077, China
3
China Electric Power Research Institute, Beijing 100192, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(24), 13003; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182413003
Submission received: 16 November 2021 / Revised: 6 December 2021 / Accepted: 7 December 2021 / Published: 9 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Data Science for Environment and Health Applications)

Abstract

:
This paper used 1526 works from the literature on disaster risk perception from 2000 to 2020 in the Web of Science core collection database as the research subject. The CiteSpace knowledge graph analysis tool was used to visual analyze the country, author, institution, discipline distribution, keywords, and keyword clustering mapping. The paper drew the following conclusions. Firstly, disaster risk perception research has experienced three stages of steady development, undulating growth, and rapid growth. Secondly, the field of disaster risk perception was mainly concentrated in the disciplines of engineering, natural science, and management science. Thirdly, meteorological disasters, earthquakes, nuclear radiation, and epidemics were the main disasters in the field of disaster risk perception. Residents and adolescents were the main subjects of research in the field of disaster risk perception. Fourthly, research on human risk behavior and risk psychology and research on disaster risk control and emergency management were two major research hotspots in the field of disaster risk perception. Finally, the research field of disaster risk perception is constantly expanding. There is a trend from theory to application and multi-perspective combination, and future research on disaster risk perception will be presented more systematically. The conclusion can provide a reference for disaster risk perception research, as well as directions for future research.

1. Introduction

Risk perception is a concept used to describe people’s attitudes and intuitive judgments about risk. In a broad sense, it can also refer to people’s general assessment and reflection of risk [1]. According to Slovic, risk perception is an individual’s perception of the possible objective risks in his or her intuition about the environment, and his or her behavior of assessing the risks. Slovic argued that an individual’s subjective judgment and experience gained would affect an individual’s risk perception [2]. Sitkin, on the other hand, defined risk perception as an individual’s assessment of environmental risk, including the probability of assessment of environmental uncertainty, controllable probability, and assessment confidence [3]. He pointed out that risk perception is a decisive element of risk-taking behavior [4].
As for disaster, Fritz, a famous American disaster social scientist, has the most recognized definition by researchers. He believed that a disaster is an event with time-space characteristics that cause shocks to society or other branches of society, leading to disruptions in the social structure and disruptions in the functioning of the survival support systems of its members [5]. Subsequently, Quarantelli made a detailed discussion on the disaster in “What Is a Disaster” by combining the viewpoints of various scholars [6,7] and further refined the definition of disaster. To sum up, disaster refers to the phenomenon of loss and harm to human social life, production, life, health, and property caused by irresistible factors; it mainly refers to natural disasters (such as earthquakes, typhoons, flood, debris flow, epidemic, etc.) and man-made disasters (such as radiation, terrorism, man-made fire, traffic disasters, etc.) [6,7]. Correspondingly, disaster risk perception means the process by which the public learns the information about disaster risk and makes choices, attitudes, and behaviors to avoid disasters or reduce disaster losses according to their situation [8]. The research on disaster risk perception aims to explore how disaster risk perception will affect people’s choices, attitudes, and behaviors towards disaster prevention, so as to enable people at risk to avoid risks or accept lower risks [9].
Urbanization has accelerated in countries worldwide in the past 20 years, and the negative effects of global warming and sea-level rise have become increasingly apparent [10]. Natural disasters, such as floods, mudslides, typhoons, and earthquakes, have occurred frequently, causing huge casualties and property losses and seriously hindering the sustainable development of human society [11]. In addition, the Fukushima nuclear leak, COVID-19, and other comprehensive disasters have also aroused widespread attention worldwide, affecting people’s work and life to a certain extent [12,13]. How to systematically study disaster risk perception in order to control disaster risk has become a hot topic for scholars worldwide. In order to summarize the research status and development prospects of disaster risk perception in recent decades, Wachinger et al. reviewed the main insights related to natural disaster risk perception [14] and Ho et al. explored the impact of different disaster characteristics on risk perception [15]. Although these papers have analyzed the research characteristics of disaster risk perception, they are all based on qualitative analysis rather than quantitative analysis, and the analysis content is not comprehensive enough. Therefore, in this paper, we decided to use the scientometric method to quantitatively explore the research characteristics and development trend of disaster risk perception.
Scientometric analysis approach created by Price provided researchers with a new statistical analysis method for mass data [16]. It is widely used in the world. De Masi et al. applied it to study the connections between natural disasters and cultural heritage [17]. Olawumi et al. did a scientometric review of global research on sustainability and sustainable development [18]. Scientometric analysis explains how to uniquely search for research frontiers, that is, to study trends in related fields by using citation rules that exist in various works of the literature. This method is based on scientific methods, such as co-author analysis, journal analysis, institution analysis, co-citation analysis, and keyword analysis, and uses visual analysis technology to explore the characteristics of the development trend and research hotspots of the discipline [19]. Using scientometrics to explore the research of disaster risk perception can help to improve scholars’ understanding of the research status and significance of disaster risk perception, reveal its internal development law, and promote the sustainable development of disaster risk perception research. Therefore, we tended to use the CiteSpace visualization tool designed by Dr. Chen and his team [20] to conduct a scientometric analysis on the disaster-risk-perception literature from 2000 to 2020 included in the Web of Science Core Collection. Based on the analysis results, this paper systematically provides the overall situation of disaster risk perception research, finds the hotspots of disaster risk perception research, and puts forward constructive suggestions for the future development of disaster risk perception. The main objectives of this study were as follows: (1) to analyze the distribution and cooperation of disaster risk perception researchers and institutions, (2) to analyze the research status of disaster risk perception, and (3) to explore the research topics and hotspots of disaster risk perception.

2. Methods

2.1. Research Methods

Knowledge graph refers to an econometric method that visualizes the internal structure, disciplinary characteristics, research frontiers, and other information of a certain amount of scientific-literature information utilizing computer technology, statistics, graph theory, and other means [20,21]. A wide range of knowledge graph and science mapping tools are available for scientometric analysis, such as VOSviewer, BibExcel, CiteSpace, CoPalRed, and so on. The different tools for scientometric analysis have different capabilities and strengths [22]. Among them, CiteSpace has an outstanding effect in analyzing the trend and hotspots of discipline research [23], and this is in line with the research objective of this paper. Thus, CiteSpace was chosen as the tool for scientometric analysis in this paper.
In 2006, Dr. Chen developed the visual literature-analysis software CiteSpace and widely promoted it [24,25]. With this as the node, many scholars began to use the CiteSpace knowledge graph to analyze research hotspots in disciplines. CiteSpace generates a series of visual knowledge maps to explore the research status, research hotspots, evolution process and discipline structure of a scientific field. Based on the above analysis, researchers can master the research direction of institutions and authors, and judge the classical literature and related auxiliary research. In the field of international scientific research, CiteSpace software is widely used in computer science, information science, medicine and more than 60 fields [24]. As for the field of disaster risk perception, in 2016, Fei et al. conducted a systematic analysis on the research status of international flood risk perception by using CiteSpace software and methods, such as literature co-citation analysis, keyword co-occurrence analysis, and emergent word analysis [26]. However, the research on overall disaster risk perception in the world is still relatively blank.
In this paper, based on the Web of Science (WoS) Core Collection, the visual literature-analysis software CiteSpace (Citespace 5.7R5: Philadelphia, PA, USA) was used to analyze, excavate, and visualize data from the scientific research literature [24]. The potential knowledge and trend in the field of disaster risk perception were scientifically analyzed and revealed by the method of bibliometrics.

2.2. Data Source

Bibliographic databases commonly used by scholars include Google Scholar, Scopus, PubMed, and Web of Science. Among them, Web of Science (WoS) is a large multi-disciplinary core journal citation database covering engineering, natural science, social science, and other fields; the database includes many authoritative and influential academic journals in the world. At the same time, relevant studies show that the WoS database shows a better knowledge map effect when CiteSpace is used for visual analysis [27,28]. The WoS Core Collection is a multidisciplinary large-scale comprehensive citation index database based on WoS. It is a dynamic and updated digital research environment integrated by core academic information resources. It integrates high-quality information resources, analysis tools and professional software seamlessly through powerful retrieval technology and content-based connectivity, with multiple functions of knowledge retrieval, extraction, analysis, evaluation, management, and publication. Therefore, in order to effectively analyze the status and development trend of disaster risk perception, this paper chooses Web of Science Core Collection as the sample database.
The data-acquisition process was as follows. First, the Web of Science Core Collection database was selected for a basic search. We took into consideration that, in some years before 2000, such as 1998 and 1999, no articles related to disaster risk perception were published, and 2021 has not passed yet. The search time was selected between 2000 and 2020. Dr. Chen believes that when investigating a rapidly growing field, topic searches can obtain a more comprehensive dataset than keyword searches [20]. Therefore, this article chose the topic search. To ensure the comprehensiveness of the data, the search words must be chosen with great care [29]. We chose “disaster”, “risk perception”, “risk psychology”, “risk behavior”, and other searching words and tried to combine them. Finally, two retrieval words, “disaster” and “risk perception”, were selected. The word “risk perception” was used to restrict the research content, and the word “disaster” was used to restrict the research field. The search of the literature was carried out by searching for two topics at the same time. It was expressed by a search formula: “TS = risk perception * AND TS = disaster *”. The document type was set as “Article”. It means that only journal articles are retained and review articles were excluded. A total of 1568 records were obtained from the WoS database. Before the research, we conducted a detailed study on the titles, abstracts, and keywords of these records. If the literature abstract was related to disaster risk perception and related fields, then the record was retained. However, some articles also use terms related to disaster risk perception in the abstract but mainly focus on other objects, such as Hopf et al.’s study on people with communication disabilities [30]. These records were excluded. Each record needs to be carefully screened. After filtering out a few relevant records and deleting duplicate data samples in CiteSpace, we finally retained 1526 records of the literature as research samples in this paper.

3. Results

3.1. Time Distribution of Output

The time distribution of the number of published papers in the field of disaster risk perception from 2000 to 2020 is shown in Figure 1. The number of published papers on disaster risk perception research has gone through three stages: steady development, undulating growth, and rapid growth. From 2000 to 2007, the number of published papers on disaster risk perception developed steadily. In this period, the average number of published papers was 9.75, and the total number of published papers was relatively small. Disaster risk perception research was in a preliminary stage of development. From 2008 to 2010, the number of published papers on disaster risk perception fluctuated and increased, with an average annual number of 33.33. From 2011 to 2020, the number of papers published on disaster risk perception increased rapidly, with an average annual number of 134.8 and an average annual growth rate of 22.61%. On the whole, the number of research papers on disaster risk perception shows a steady growth trend and a high growth rate, indicating that disaster risk perception is gradually becoming a hot research issue in the world.

3.2. Characteristics of National or Regional Cooperation

The national or regional cooperation graph reveals the distribution and intensity of cooperation between countries or regions [24]. By analyzing 1526 works of the literature, this paper obtained the national or regional cooperation graph in disaster risk perception, as shown in Figure 2.
The number of nodes in the figure is 103 and the number of links is 473, which indicates that scholars from 103 countries and regions have conducted researches on disaster risk perception and that there is a certain degree of cooperation among these countries and regions. The three countries and regions with the largest nodes are the USA, China, and England, indicating that these three countries and regions have published the most research on disaster risk perception. Table 1 shows the top 15 countries and regions by the number of publications. The results show that the top five countries and regions in terms of published papers are the USA, China, England, Australia, and Japan. Moreover, the total number of published papers by the five countries and regions accounts for 72.8% of the total number of published papers. We believe that the large number of articles published in the field of disaster risk in China and the United States is mainly due to their vast territories and frequent disasters. England, Australia, and Japan rank high mainly because of their strong awareness of disaster management. From the perspective of betweenness centrality, the betweenness centrality of the USA, England, and Germany is higher than 0.2, indicating that these three countries and regions have close cooperation with other countries and regions and have a great influence in the world. Although China, Japan, the Netherlands, and other countries have a high number of published papers, their national cooperation intensity is lower than the USA, England, and Germany. For these countries and regions, the degree of foreign exchange and international influence still needs to be strengthened.
In addition, we can see from the figure that a large part of the connecting line from Germany is wide. It shows that Germany might play a leading role in the cooperation with other countries and regions, and some European countries, such as the Netherlands and Spain, are greatly influenced by Germany in the research field of disaster risk perception. Moreover, we can see that most of the connecting lines from China are fine. It indicates that Chinese scholars prefer independent research; and, on the other hand, it indicates that China has less influence on other countries in collaborative research. The reasons for these phenomena above are probably due to the influence of culture and international relations.

3.3. Characteristics of Author Cooperation

Author collaboration networks can show the key figures in a field and the cooperative relationship between researchers [24]. As shown in Figure 3, there are a total of 594 nodes and 628 links in the figure, indicating that 594 scholars have conducted research on disaster risk perception from 2000 to 2020, and there is a certain degree of cooperation between them. In general, the research in the field of disaster risk perception was scattered as a whole but concentrated in a small part. The research team formed by Dinde Xu, Xin Deng, et al. and the research team formed by Michio Murakami, Seiji Yasumura, et al. were the most important research teams. Based on Table 2, the five authors with the most publications are Dingde Xu, Michio Murakami, Seiji Yasumura, Ziqiang Han, and Michael K. Lindell. Except for Michael K. Lindell, the starting year of these scholars was late, indicating that these scholars still entered the field of disaster risk perception in a relatively short time. At the same time, the lines in the author collaboration network are scattered, and the betweenness centrality of each scholar is low, indicating that each scholar and research team prefer independent research, and the communication and cooperation between scholars are not rich enough.

3.4. Characteristics of Research Institution Cooperation

The graph of institutional cooperation network can show the research centers and institutions in a field, as well as the cooperative relationships among institutions [32]. Figure 4 is the network graph of institutions obtained from this analysis, from which we can see that a total of 465 institutions have conducted researches on disaster risk perception. Texas A&M University has the largest number of published papers, with 27. Among the top 10 institutions in terms of publication volume shown in Table 3, four are in China, three are in the United States, two are in Japan, and one is in New Zealand. Institutions in China, Japan, and the United States have achieved important positions in the field of disaster risk perception research. From the perspective of betweenness centrality, Texas A&M University, Massey University, and Colorado State University have the highest betweenness centrality, indicating that these three universities have richer exchanges with other institutions and have greater influence. Fukushima Medical University mainly studied the disaster risk perception related to nuclear radiation, so it had less cooperation with institutions in different regions and its betweenness centrality was low.

3.5. Characteristics of Discipline Distribution

As shown in Figure 5, research on disaster risk perception mainly focuses on environmental science and ecology, water resources, meteorology and atmosphere science, geology, geoscience multidisciplinary, and so on. Table 4 shows the betweenness centrality and category of Top 10 disciplines ranked by subject co-occurrence frequency. The results showed that engineering accounted for 40%, natural science 30%, management science 20%, economics 10%. Environmental science and ecology, as well as public environment and occupational health, have the most intersections. These two disciplines have the highest betweenness centrality and are closely linked with other disciplines. The betweenness centrality of meteorology and atmospheric science, geology, and geoscience multidisciplinary is 0, indicating that these disciplines are more independent and suitable for independent research. In general, the research of disaster risk perception involves a wide range of disciplines, thus requiring joint exploration by scholars from all walks of life.

3.6. Keyword Analysis

3.6.1. Keywords Graph Analysis

Keywords are the subject and content of highly refined papers. Comprehensive analysis of keywords in the paper shows that, the higher the co-occurrence frequency, the more related topics and contents are involved. Thus, keywords can be used to determine hot issues in a research field [34]. In Figure 6, there are 672 nodes and 2862 connections. The five nodes with the highest frequency are “perception” (462 times), “disaster” (453 times), “risk perception” (431 times), “risk” (365 times), and “climate change” (258 times). The five nodes with the highest betweenness centrality are “perception” (0.14), “disaster” (0.08), “risk” (0.06), “earthquake” (0.06), and “communication” (0.06). After a comprehensive analysis, “perception”, “disaster”, “risk”, “climate change”, and “earthquake” were the top five hot keywords in the research field.
In order to further summarize hot keywords, the keyword co-occurrence network was displayed in the time-zone view in Figure 7. It shows that the number of topics has increased significantly from 2004 to 2014. The hot keywords mentioned above all appeared in an earlier time period. Among the new keywords appearing in the last five years, “flood risk” has a high-risk frequency, which may be closely related to the practical factors of the frequent storm and flood disasters around the world in recent years. In addition, keywords around risk control and emergency management, such as “disaster risk reduction”, and “emergency preparedness”, have also appeared more frequently in recent years.

3.6.2. Keywords Cluster Analysis

To find the relationship between high-frequency keywords and further reveal the research hotspots in the field of disaster risk perception, this study classified high-frequency keywords through high-citation cluster analysis of disaster risk perception [35]. It drew the correlation clustering map in Figure 8. The 13 clusters in the figure have mean silhouette values greater than 0.8, indicating good homogeneity. The analysis was reliable.
As shown in Table 5, the 13 representative clusters are #0Climate Change, #1Resident, #2Radiation, #3Uncertainty, #4Mental Health, and #5Risk Management, #6Evacuation, #7Earthquake, #8Disaster Response, #9Disaster Risk Reduction, #10Pandemic, #11Adolescents, and #12Communication. The 13 clusters are mainly divided into four themes, which are research disaster categories (including tags #0, #2, #7, and #10), risk perception survey object (including tags #1 and #11), risk behavior and risk psychology research (including tags #3, #4, #8, and #12), and risk control and emergency management research for disasters (including tags #5, #6, and #9).

3.6.3. Emergent Keyword Analysis

Emergent keywords refer to the keywords that appear many times in a certain period. Based on the time trend of keywords, we can predict the dynamic research hotspots in a certain field [36]. The map of emergent keywords obtained in this analysis is shown in Figure 9. A total of 30 emergent keywords are obtained. According to the emergence time of keywords, we can divide the period from 2000 to 2020 into three stages. The period from 2000 to 2009 was the first stage, in which keywords such as “radiation”, “stress”, “Chernobyl disaster”, and “natural disaster” appeared, indicating that the focus of attention in this period was on the relationship between disaster itself and risk perception. Moreover, nuclear disaster as a hot disaster has been widely studied. The second stage was from 2010 to 2014, with emergent keywords such as “fire”, “hurricane”, “terrorism”, “adaptive capacity”, and “social amplification”. The types of disasters in this period tended to be diversified. The focus shifted to the relationship between social factors, individual factors, and disaster risk perception. The third stage was from 2015 to 2020, and the emergent keywords were “governance”, “warning”, “social science”, “self-efficacy”, “disaster resilience”, etc. It indicated that the social and individual factors of disaster risk perception in this period were further expanded and deepened, and the focus gradually shifted to risk control and emergency management.
In general, emergent keywords can reveal social reality, as well as research trends, in disaster risk perception to a certain extent. For example, the word “terrorism” came up frequently from 2011 to 2014. The main reason is that there were a number of serious terrorist attacks around the world during this period. In terms of research trends, the frequent emergence of words such as “disaster resilience”, “disaster risk reduction”, “social science”, and “self-efficacy” after 2017 may also indicate that the research in the field of disaster risk perception will become practical and comprehensive.

4. Discussion

4.1. Research Hotspots of Disaster Risk Perception

Through the analysis of keywords, research hotspots in the field of disaster risk perception are summarized in Figure 10.
According to keyword graph analysis and keyword cluster analysis, we can get research hotspots in the field of disaster risk perception. First of all, in terms of disaster categories, “climate change” and “earthquake” were the top five hot keywords obtained from the comprehensive analysis of the graph. It indicated that meteorological disasters and earthquake disasters, as two traditional natural disasters, have always been the hot issues studied by scholars in the field of disaster risk perception. Taking meteorological disasters as an example, the global climate pattern has changed dramatically in the past 20 years, and the impact of extreme weather events, such as droughts, heavy rains, and hurricanes, on global ecosystems has received much attention from scholars worldwide. Grothmann’s and Botzen’s research among them is the most classic. Grothmann et al. proposed a social psychological model for flood disasters based on the protection motivation theory. They used residents’ views on previous flood experiences and future flood risks to infer the reliability of social public flood control, the effectiveness and cost of residents’ self-protection behavior, and residents’ perception of flood risk. This model effectively guides public risk communication and is of great significance to flood risk management [37]. Botzen et al. investigated about 1000 homeowners in the Netherlands. They used a variety of models to estimate the impact of socioeconomic and geographical characteristics, personal flood experience, flood threat knowledge, and personal risk attitude on the formation of risk perception. They pointed out that the difference in expected flood risk is always related to the actual risk level [38]. Due to scholars’ emphasis on meteorological disasters and earthquake disasters, the research methods of risk perception in these areas were rich, and the research content was relatively mature and specific.
In addition, besides traditional natural disasters, “#2 radiation” and “#10 epidemic” also appeared in the keyword clustering map as comprehensive disasters. These disasters have a wider impact. They are more likely to trigger social panic, and often attract the attention of a large number of scholars in a certain period. Since the Fukushima nuclear accident in 2011, relevant scholars have performed many studies on the perception of nuclear radiation risk. For example, Kunii et al. measured the mental health status of 73,569 people aged 15 and above living in the evacuation area of Fukushima Prefecture. They found that the nuclear accident had a serious impact on the mental health of residents, and the excessive perception of radiation risk greatly aggravates people’s psychological pressure [39]. Yoshida et al. conducted a questionnaire survey on 287 students from the department of nursing at a National University in Japan to study the knowledge and risk perception of radiation among nurses and doctors. The survey results showed that Japanese nursing students had little knowledge of radiation and had a great fear of X-rays. They believed that receiving appropriate radiation knowledge education can effectively reduce risk perception and thus reduce fear [40]. Following the outbreak of COVID-19 in late 2019, papers on COVID-19-related risk perception also exploded. Cvetković et al. conducted a study on the risk perception of 975 Serbian citizens during the outbreak. The survey results showed significant differences in public perceptions of the risks posed by the threat of infectious diseases, such as COVID-19. Cvetković et al. argued that emergency management agencies should use these differences to develop targeted strategies, and then strengthen community and national preparedness by promoting behavioral change and improving risk management decisions [41]. Lee et al. explored the effectiveness of obtaining information through emergency warning SMS in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in South Korea and its impact on individual prevention behavior. They believe that the government should actively consider sending emergency warning messages to provide accurate and reliable information to the public, which is conducive to reducing the impact of negative news [42].
The types of disasters studied in the field of disaster risk perception are closely related to the types of disasters occurring in the current reality. Generally speaking, when a new disaster appears, scholars will conduct a lot of research on it, in order to reduce disaster losses in a short time and meet social needs. The research on risk perception of these disaster areas will also become a hot issue during this period. In addition, regional disasters, such as Fukushima nuclear radiation in Japan and hurricane disasters in the United States, are often studied in depth by local scholars. Moreover, global disasters, such as global warming and COVID-19, are often studied by scholars from all over the world. Especially for COVID-19, its emergence and rapid spread at the end of 2019 led to a proliferation of articles about it in 2020. The annual number of articles in disaster risk perception reached a new peak in 2020.
From the perspective of risk perception survey objects, “#1 Resident” and “#11 Adolescents” were the two main survey objects for disaster risk perception. Risk-perception questionnaires were usually targeted at residents or adolescents in disaster-affected areas. For example, Martins studied household disaster preparedness before Storm Sandy among 2001 residents of all five boroughs of New York [43]. When studying the role of behavioral experience in risk judgment, Halpern investigated 577 adolescents and young adults aged from 10 to 30 years old [44]. The main reason was that residents, as direct audiences of disasters, were more representative. Meanwhile, teenagers were affected by many factors such as age and education level, and the survey results varied greatly. These survey objects will be helpful for scholars to study the impact of risk perception from different perspectives.
The clusters of “#3 uncertainty”, “#4 mental health”, ”#8 disaster response”, and “#12 communication” indicated that the study of human risk behavior and risk psychology was a hot research topic in the field of disaster risk perception. In 1960, Bauer applied risk perception to consumer research and proposed the theory of uncertain consequences [45]. The word “uncertainty” firstly appeared in the field of risk perception. Later, the psychometric approach proposed by Slovic [46], the sociocultural theory proposed by Douglas and Wildavsky [47], and other theoretical paradigms had been widely used by many scholars around the world. These theoretical paradigms interpret risk behavior and risk psychology from many different perspectives, such as psychology, social science, and geography. They also interpreted and measured the public’s perception of risk under disasters. For example, McDermott used the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to identify children and adolescents who might need psychological intervention after exposure to wildfire. He pointed out that younger individuals are more likely to appear in developmental and psychological disorders if exposed to threats [48]. This is a typical research process of the psychometric approach; that is, questionnaire data are used as the framework, and influencing factors are quantitatively analyzed by the algorithm. Jennifer et al. surveyed residents of communities downstream from glacial lakes. They found that the persistent attachment of community residents to their valleys and their desire for cultural continuity in the face of social, economic, and environmental changes significantly influenced their perception of risk [49]. This study further demonstrated the profound influence of sociocultural factors on risk perception.
The clusters of “#5 risk management”, “#6 evacuation”, and “#9 disaster risk reduction” indicated that risk control and emergency management for disasters were also hotspots in the field of disaster risk perception. The core of this part was to apply theoretical analysis and results into practice, in order to reduce disaster risk, casualties, and property losses. For example, Wallace interviewed 205 households in North Carolina to assess the connection between perceived risk and actual risk between flood and evacuation. He believed that actual flood risk was an important environmental clue for evaluating risk perception and evacuation decision-making [50]. Chatfield investigated disaster-risk communications during the eruption of Mount Sinabung in Indonesia. Based on his analysis of the Sinabung hashtag on Twitter, he found a lack of engagement in risk perception communication and leadership by the government to respond to the Indonesian public’s concerns about the Mount Sinabung disaster. In addition, Chatfield proposed that Twitter can be effectively used as a multidirectional risk communication tool to share risk perception and disaster information with the public quickly and effectively [51]. In general, this part of disaster risk perception research was more realistic and provided practical guidance in disaster risk reduction

4.2. Research Trends of Disaster Risk Perception

Through the analysis of keywords, research trends in the field of disaster risk perception are summarized in Figure 11.
According to the emergent keywords map, we can summarize the frontier trends of disaster risk perception research. On the whole, the research fields of disaster risk perception were constantly expanding. For example, in terms of the definition of disaster, the main research contents before 2009 were natural disasters and nuclear accidents, while the “terrorism” occurring from 2010 to 2014 and the “wildfire” occurring from 2015 to 2020 have expanded the disaster fields of risk perception.
On the other hand, the research on disaster risk perception tends to shift from theory to application. From the analysis results, we can see that the keywords before 2014 mostly focus on the public’s risk perception and mental health under the background of disasters. However, keywords after 2014, such as “governance” and “disaster resilience”, focus on studies of disaster risk management and emergency management, which can provide more practical guidance for public management and have more practical significance.
In addition, there is a trend of keyword development from multiple perspectives. Many factors are affecting the human cognitive process, and the factors of social and cultural background cannot be ignored [52]. Keywords such as “social science”, “health”, and “self-efficacy” indicate that future studies on disaster risk perception will comprehensively analyze the public’s risk perception of disasters from multiple perspectives, such as society, individual, and culture. The research content of disaster risk perception will be more detailed and scientific.
In general, the field of disaster risk perception in the future will be standardized and systematic. Scholars will conduct research on disaster risk perception from many different perspectives, such as psychology, behavioral science, and geology. They will also innovate research paradigms in the field of disaster risk perception. The two current research hotspots, the research on human risk behavior and risk psychology, as well as the research on disaster risk control and emergency management, will continue to be deepened. In addition, the research content in the field of future disaster risk perception will also be affected by the types of disasters that occur in the future.

4.3. Knowledge Gaps

Even though our understanding of disaster risk perception deepens over time, it is still difficult to apply disaster risk perception to disaster risk reduction in reality. Bubeck mentioned in his paper that the current focus on risk perceptions as a means to explain and promote private flood mitigation behavior is not supported on either theoretical or empirical grounds [53]. How to use risk perception to effectively control disaster risk is not only the current research hotspot but also the current knowledge gap. We believe that some advanced countries or institutions will devote themselves to the practical application of disaster risk perception, while some backward countries or institutions will continue to improve their theoretical systems. The continuous exploration of theory and experience can make this knowledge gap fully filled, thereby making a substantial contribution to disaster risk reduction.

4.4. Limitation of Research

This study used only data from the WoS Core Collection as the data source. Moreover, the research results were limited by the influence of the database. It was not convincing enough to predict the future development trend of disaster risk perception. On the other hand, this paper used CiteSpace to study disaster risk perception from multiple perspectives, including author, region, discipline, key words, and other aspects, but the pertinence was somewhat weak.

5. Conclusions

The in-depth study of disaster risk perception is an important measure to control disaster risk. Disaster risk can be reduced by adjusting public risk psychology and risk behavior. Therefore, using the scientometrics analysis method to explore the research work of disaster risk perception will be conducive to the development of current research, and it will effectively promote the sustainable development of disaster risk perception and related research.
This paper indicated that the research on human risk behavior and risk psychology and the research on disaster risk control and emergency management are two hotspots that have appeared in recent years. It can provide research directions for potential readers, which will push the development of disaster risk perception research so as to adjust public risk psychology and risk behavior and improve public safety.
In addition, the research field of disaster risk perception is constantly expanding. There is the trend of shifting from theory to application and the trend of combining multiple perspectives. This paper predicts that, in the future, the research on disaster risk perception will be more detailed, more specific, and more comprehensive. Scholars should focus on research hotspots such as risk behavior, risk psychology, and risk management and control. They must strengthen theory, focus on practice, and establish a perfect research system and paradigm of disaster risk perception to solve the situation of frequent disasters around the world in recent years.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.Y. and X.W.; methodology, T.Y.; software, T.Y.; validation, T.Y., H.Y. and J.G.; formal analysis, T.Y.; investigation, T.Y.; resources, T.Y.; data curation, T.Y. and H.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, T.Y.; writing—review and editing, T.Y., X.L. and X.W.; visualization, T.Y., X.L. and Y.J.; supervision, H.Y.; project administration, X.W.; funding acquisition, X.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the MOE (Ministry of Education in China) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences (18YJCZH191) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of China (2652019073).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available in the Web of Science core database.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sjoberg, L. Factors in risk perception. Risk Anal. 2000, 20, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Slovic, P. Perception of risk. Science 1987, 236, 280–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sitkin, S.B.; Weingart, L.R. Determinants of Risky Decision-Making Behavior: A Test of the Mediating Role of Risk Perceptions and Propensity. Acad. Manag. J. 1995, 38, 1573–1592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Sitkin, S.B.; Pablo, A.L. Reconceptualizing the Determinants of Risk Behavior. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1992, 17, 9–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Fritz, C.E. Disaster. In Contemporary Social Problems; Merton, R.K., Nisbet, R.A., Eds.; Harcourt: New York, NY, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
  6. Quarantelli, E.L. What is a Disaster? Perspectives on the Question, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  7. Perry, R.W.; Quarantelli, E.L. What is a Disaster? New Answers to Old Questions, 1st ed.; Xlibris: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  8. Hesam, S.; Ezat, S.; Ibrahim, S. Intervention strategies for improvement of disasters risk perception: Family-centered approach. J. Educ. Health Promot. 2019, 8, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Xue, K.J.; Guo, S.L.; Liu, Y.; Liu, S.Q.; Xu, D.D. Social Networks, Trust, and Disaster-Risk Perceptions of Rural Residents in a Multi-Disaster Environment: Evidence from Sichuan, China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Trenberth, K.E. Changes in precipitation with climate change. Clim. Res. 2011, 47, 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Tarshall, T.M. Risk perception and safety culture: Tools for improving the implementation of disaster risk reduction strategies. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020, 47, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gteinhauser, G.; Brandl, A.; Johnson, T.E. Comparison of the Chernobyl and Fukushima nuclear accidents: A review of the environmental impacts. Sci. Total Environ. 2014, 470, 800–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Huang, C.L.; Wang, Y.M.; Li, X.W. Clinical features of patients infected with 2019 novel coronavirus in Wuhan, China. Lancet 2020, 395, 497–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Wachinger, G.; Renn, O.; Begg, C.; Kuhlicke, C. The Risk Perception Paradox—Implications for Governance and Communication of Natural Hazards. Risk Anal. 2013, 33, 1049–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Ho, M.C.; Shaw, D.; Lin, S.; Chiu, Y.C. How Do Disaster Characteristics Influence Risk Perception? Risk Anal. 2008, 28, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Garfield, E. From the science of science to Scientometrics visualizing the history of science with HistCite software. J. Inf. 2009, 3, 173–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. De Masi, F.; Larosa, F.; Porrini, D.; Mysiak, J. Cultural heritage and disasters risk: A machine-human coupled analysis. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021, 59, 102251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Olawumi, T.O.; Chan, D.W.M. A scientometric review of global research on sustainability and sustainable development. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 183, 231–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gao, J.; Wu, X.; Luo, X.; Guan, S. Scientometric Analysis of Safety Sign Research: 1990–2019. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Chen, C.; Chen, Y. Searching for clinical evidence in CiteSpace. AMIA Annu. Symp. Proc. 2005, 2005, 121–125. [Google Scholar]
  21. Allendoerfer, K.; Aluker, S.; Panjwani, G.; Proctor, J.; Sturtz, D.; Vukovic, M.; Chen, C. Adapting the cognitive walkthrough method to assess the usability of a knowledge domain visualization. In Proceedings of the IEEE Symposium on Information Visualization (InfoVis 05), Minneapolis, MN, USA, 23–25 October 2005; pp. 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Cobo, M.J.; López-Herrera, A.G.; Herrera-Viedma, E.; Herrera, F. Science mapping software tools: Review, analysis, and cooperative study among tools. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2014, 62, 1382–1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hosseini, M.R.; Martek, I.; Zavadskas, E.K.; Aibinu, A.A.; Arashpour, M.; Chileshe, N. Critical evaluation of off-site construction research: A Scientometric analysis. Automat. Constr. 2018, 87, 235–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Li, J.; Chen, C. CiteSpace: Text Mining and Visualization in Scientific Literature, 2nd ed.; Capital University of Economics and Business Press: Beijing, China, 2017; pp. 1–301. [Google Scholar]
  25. Synnestvedt, M.B.; Chen, C.; Holmes, J.H. CiteSpace II: Visualization and knowledge discovery in bibliographic databases. AMIA Annu. Symp. Proc. 2005, 2005, 724–728. [Google Scholar]
  26. Fu, F.; Yin, J.; He, C.; Huang, J. Visualizing the Knowledge Domain of Flood Risk Perception Research: A Scietometric Review. J. Catastrophol. 2016, 31, 145–149. [Google Scholar]
  27. Martin-Martin, A.; Orduna-Malea, E.; Thelwall, M.; Delgado Lopez-Cozar, E. Google Scholar, Web of Science, and Scopus: A systematic comparison of citations in 252 subject categories. J. Informetr. 2018, 12, 1160–1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Falagas, M.; Pitsouni, E.; Malietzis, G.; Pappas, G. Comparison of PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, and Google Scholar: Strengths and weaknesses. FASEB J. 2008, 22, 338–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hou, L.; Tan, Y.; Luo, W.; Xu, S.; Moon, S. Towards a more extensive application of off-site construction: A technological review. Int. J. Constr. Manag. 2020, 4, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hopf, S.C.; McLeod, S. Services for people with communication disability in Fiji: Barriers and drivers of change. Rural Remote Health 2015, 15, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zhang, M.; Zhang, R. The Hotspots and Trends of Adaptive Learning: A Visualized Analysis Based on CiteSpace. Int. J. Inf. Edu. Technol. 2020, 10, 394–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gandia, R.M.; Antonialli, F.; Cavazza, B.H.; Neto, A.M.; de Lima, D.A.; Sugano, J.Y.; Nicolai, I.; Zambalde, A.L. Autonomous vehicles: Scientometric and bibliometric review. Transport. Rev. 2019, 39, 9–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Costa, D.F.; Carvalho, F.D.; Moreira, B.C.D.; do Prado, J.W. Bibliometric analysis on the association between behavioral finance and decision making with cognitive biases such as overconfidence, anchoring effect and confirmation bias. Scientometrics 2017, 111, 1775–1799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Railon-Moreno, R.; Jurado-Alameda, E.; Ruiz-Banos, R.; Courtial, J.P. Analysis of the field of physical chemistry of surfactants with the Unified Scienctometric Model. Fit of relational and activity indicators. Scientometrics 2005, 63, 259–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Callon, M.; Courtial, J.P.; Laville, F. Co-word analysis as a tool for describing the network of interactions between basic and technological research: The case of polymer chemsitry. Scientometrics 1991, 22, 155–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Yang, H.; Shao, X.X.; Wu, M. A Review on Ecosystem Health Research: A Visualization Based on CiteSpace. Sustainability 2019, 11, 4908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Grothmann, T.; Reusswig, F. People at risk of flooding: Why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Nat. Hazards 2006, 38, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Botzen, W.J.W.; Aerts, J.; van den Bergh, J. Dependence of flood risk perceptions on socioeconomic and objective risk factors. Water Resour. Res. 2009, 45, 455–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Kunii, Y.; Suzuki, Y.; Shiga, T.; Yabe, H.; Yasumura, S.; Maeda, M.; Niwa, S.; Otsuru, A.; Mashiko, H.; Abe, M.; et al. Severe Psychological Distress of Evacuees in Evacuation Zone Caused by the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Plant Accident: The Fukushima Health Management Survey. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0158821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Yoshida, M.; Iwamoto, S.; Okahisa, R.; Kishida, S.; Sakama, M.; Honda, E. Knowledge and risk perception of radiation for Japanese nursing students after the Fukushima Nuclear Power Plant disaster. Nurs. Educ. Today 2020, 94, 104552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Cvetkovic, V.M.; Nikolic, N.; Nenadic, U.R.; Ocal, A.; Noji, E.K.; Zecevic, M. Preparedness and Preventive Behaviors for a Pandemic Disaster Caused by COVID-19 in Serbia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. You, M.; Lee, M. Effects of COVID-19 Emergency Alert Text Messages on Practicing Preventive Behaviors: Cross-sectional Web-Based Survey in South Korea. J. Med. Internet. Res. 2021, 23, e24165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Martins, V.N.; Louis-Charles, H.M.; Nigg, J.; Kendra, J.; Sisco, S. Household Disaster Preparedness in New York City before Superstorm Sandy: Findings and Recommendations. J. Homel. Secur. Emerg. Manag. 2018, 15, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Halpern-Felsher, B.L.; Millstein, S.G.; Ellen, J.M.; Adler, N.E.; Tschann, J.M.; Biehl, M. The role of behavioral experience in judging risks. Health Psychol. 2001, 20, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Bauer, R.A. Consumer Behavior as Risk Taking. In Risk Taking and Information Handling in Consumer Behavior; Cox, D., Ed.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, CA, USA, 1961; pp. 389–398. [Google Scholar]
  46. Starr, C. Social benefit versus technological risk. Science 1969, 165, 1232–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Weinstein, G. Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers. Am. Polit Sci. Rev. 1983, 77, 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. McDermott, B.M.; Lee, E.M.; Judd, M.; Gibbon, P. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and General Psychopathology in Children and Adolescents Following a Wildfire Disaster. Can. J. Psychiatry 2005, 50, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Sherry, J.; Curtis, A.; Mendham, E.; Toman, E. Cultural landscapes at risk: Exploring the meaning of place in a sacred valley of Nepal. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2018, 52, 190–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wallace, J.W.; Poole, C.; Horney, J.A. The association between actual and perceived flood risk and evacuation from Hurricane Irene, Beaufort County, North Carolina. J. Flood Risk Manag. 2016, 9, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Chatfield, A.T.; Reddick, C.G. Understanding Risk Communication Gaps through E-Government Website and Twitter Hashtag Content Analyses: The Case of Indonesia’s Mt. Sinabung Eruption. J. Homel. Secur. Emerg. Manag. 2015, 12, 351–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Gibson, S.; Lemyre, L.; Lee, J.E.C. Predicting Emergency Response Intentions among the Canadian Public in the Context of Terrorism Threats: Examining Sociodemographics and the Mediating Role of Risk Perception. Hum. Ecol. Risk Assess 2015, 21, 205–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Bubeck, P.; Botzen, W.J.W.; Aerts, J.C. A Review of Risk Perceptions and Other Factors that Influence Flood Mitigation Behavior. Risk Anal. 2012, 32, 1481–1495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Time distribution diagram of publication volume in the field of disaster risk perception.
Figure 1. Time distribution diagram of publication volume in the field of disaster risk perception.
Ijerph 18 13003 g001
Figure 2. Network graph of national or regional cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph is proportional to the number of published papers, and the thickness of the purple circle is the size of betweenness centrality. The line between each node in the graph means that two countries or regions appear together in the literature; that is, two countries or regions are considered to have a cooperative relationship [31], and the thickness of the line reflects the strength of the relationship).
Figure 2. Network graph of national or regional cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph is proportional to the number of published papers, and the thickness of the purple circle is the size of betweenness centrality. The line between each node in the graph means that two countries or regions appear together in the literature; that is, two countries or regions are considered to have a cooperative relationship [31], and the thickness of the line reflects the strength of the relationship).
Ijerph 18 13003 g002
Figure 3. Network graph of author cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph reflects the number of papers published by the current author. The connection between nodes means that there are different authors in a paper simultaneously, so it is considered that there is a cooperative relationship between these authors, and the thickness of the connection indicates the strength of cooperation [24]. Set citation Counts to 5. That is, only authors with 5 or more publications are displayed).
Figure 3. Network graph of author cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph reflects the number of papers published by the current author. The connection between nodes means that there are different authors in a paper simultaneously, so it is considered that there is a cooperative relationship between these authors, and the thickness of the connection indicates the strength of cooperation [24]. Set citation Counts to 5. That is, only authors with 5 or more publications are displayed).
Ijerph 18 13003 g003
Figure 4. Network graph of institution cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph reflects the number of papers published by the institution. The connection between nodes means that different institutions appear simultaneously in a paper, so it is considered that there is a cooperative relationship between these institutions [24]. Set citation Counts to 8. That is, only organizations with eight or more publications are displayed).
Figure 4. Network graph of institution cooperation. (Note: The size of circular nodes in the graph reflects the number of papers published by the institution. The connection between nodes means that different institutions appear simultaneously in a paper, so it is considered that there is a cooperative relationship between these institutions [24]. Set citation Counts to 8. That is, only organizations with eight or more publications are displayed).
Ijerph 18 13003 g004
Figure 5. Discipline co-occurrence graph. (Note: The size of the circular node reflects the co-occurrence frequency of disciplines, and the thickness of the purple circle represents the size of betweenness centrality. The connection between nodes means that a paper belongs to different disciplines simultaneously, so these disciplines are considered to be related [33]. Set citation Counts to 54. That is, only names of disciplines with co-occurrence frequencies of 54 and above are displayed.).
Figure 5. Discipline co-occurrence graph. (Note: The size of the circular node reflects the co-occurrence frequency of disciplines, and the thickness of the purple circle represents the size of betweenness centrality. The connection between nodes means that a paper belongs to different disciplines simultaneously, so these disciplines are considered to be related [33]. Set citation Counts to 54. That is, only names of disciplines with co-occurrence frequencies of 54 and above are displayed.).
Ijerph 18 13003 g005
Figure 6. Keyword co-occurrence graph. (Note: The size of the circular node in the graph reflects the co-occurrence frequency of keywords. The connection between nodes means that different keywords appear in a paper simultaneously, so these keywords are considered to be related [30]. Set citation Counts to 60. That is, only keywords with co-occurrence frequencies of 60 or more are displayed).
Figure 6. Keyword co-occurrence graph. (Note: The size of the circular node in the graph reflects the co-occurrence frequency of keywords. The connection between nodes means that different keywords appear in a paper simultaneously, so these keywords are considered to be related [30]. Set citation Counts to 60. That is, only keywords with co-occurrence frequencies of 60 or more are displayed).
Ijerph 18 13003 g006
Figure 7. Time-zone view of keyword co-occurrence network.
Figure 7. Time-zone view of keyword co-occurrence network.
Ijerph 18 13003 g007
Figure 8. Keyword clustering map.
Figure 8. Keyword clustering map.
Ijerph 18 13003 g008
Figure 9. Emergent keyword map.
Figure 9. Emergent keyword map.
Ijerph 18 13003 g009
Figure 10. Research hotspots of disaster risk perception.
Figure 10. Research hotspots of disaster risk perception.
Ijerph 18 13003 g010
Figure 11. Research trends of disaster risk perception.
Figure 11. Research trends of disaster risk perception.
Ijerph 18 13003 g011
Table 1. Country or region cooperation features and number of published papers from 2000 to 2020.
Table 1. Country or region cooperation features and number of published papers from 2000 to 2020.
Number of PapersBetweenness
Centrality
Country or RegionStarting Year
5210.38USA2000
1760.07Peoples R China2011
1500.34England2002
1380.17Australia2001
1260.14Japan2004
790.14Netherland2000
750.30Germany2005
560.13Italy2003
520.07Canada2003
380.04New Zealand2008
380.01Taiwan2008
340.01France2003
320.04Spain2006
300.00Pakistan2012
280.01Sweden2006
Table 2. Top 5 authors in the number of articles published.
Table 2. Top 5 authors in the number of articles published.
NameNumber of Published PapersStarting Year
Dingde Xu132017
Michio Murakami132017
Seiji Yasumura112016
Ziqiang Han92017
Michael K. Lindell92008
Table 3. Top 10 research institutions in the number of articles published.
Table 3. Top 10 research institutions in the number of articles published.
Published InstitutionNumber of Published PaperBetweenness CentralityCountryStarting Year
Texas A&M University270.09USA2005
Fukushima Med University260.02Japan2012
Kyoto University230.08Japan2004
Colorado State University220.10USA2004
Chinese Academy of Sciences170.02China2011
Tsinghua University150.06China2017
Massey University140.09New Zealand2014
Beijing Normal University130.02China2011
University of Florida130.01USA2017
The Chinese University of Hong Kong120.07China2014
Table 4. Statistics of top 10 disciplines with co-occurrence frequency.
Table 4. Statistics of top 10 disciplines with co-occurrence frequency.
Discipline AreaFrequencyBetweenness CentralityDiscipline Category
Environmental Science and Ecology4910.34Engineering
Water Resources3870.03Engineering
Meteorology and Atmosphere Science3800Natural Science
Geology3700Natural Science
Geoscience Multidisciplinary3150Natural Science
Environmental Studies2710.13Engineering
Public Environmental and Occupational Health2480.42Management Science
Environmental Science1460.08Engineering
Business and Economics1190.17Economics
Social Science/Other Topics1210.17Management Science
Table 5. Statistics of top 10 disciplines with co-occurrence frequency.
Table 5. Statistics of top 10 disciplines with co-occurrence frequency.
Cluster IDCluster NameSilhouetteContain the Keywords
#0Climate Change0.933Household Risk *, Adaptation *, Storm, Flooding *, etc.
#1Resident0.983Environmental Concerns, Emergency Management *, Natural Disasters *, etc.
#2Radiation0.856Fukushima, Causal Attribution, Government Control Level *, etc.
#3Uncertainty1.000Subjectivity, Decision Making, Near Misses, etc.
#4Mental Health0.932Spatial Isolation, Mobile Phone Data *, Knowledge Gap *, etc.
#5Risk Management0.935Doctors And Nurses, Humanitarian Crises, Media Exposure *, etc.
#6Evacuation0.823Conversation *, Risk Assessment *, Earthquake Vulnerability *, etc.
#7Earthquake0.877Disaster Prevention Education *, Public Risk Perception *, Vulnerability *, etc.
#8Disaster Response0.823Humanitarian Assistance *, Critical Infrastructure, Institutions, etc.
#9Disaster Risk Reduction0.956Bioterrorism, Cold Weather Warnings, Income Inequality, etc.
#10Pandemic0.811COVID-19 *, International Public Health Emergencies, Government Assistance, etc.
#11Adolescents0.896SDQ *, Health Self-Assessment, Performance Experience *, etc.
#12Communication0.902Regression Analysis, Tokai floods *, Hispanics *, etc.
*: It means that the keyword appears in multiple clusters and is shared by multiple clusters.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yu, T.; Yang, H.; Luo, X.; Jiang, Y.; Wu, X.; Gao, J. Scientometric Analysis of Disaster Risk Perception: 2000–2020. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 13003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182413003

AMA Style

Yu T, Yang H, Luo X, Jiang Y, Wu X, Gao J. Scientometric Analysis of Disaster Risk Perception: 2000–2020. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(24):13003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182413003

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yu, Tianlong, Hao Yang, Xiaowei Luo, Yifeng Jiang, Xiang Wu, and Jingqi Gao. 2021. "Scientometric Analysis of Disaster Risk Perception: 2000–2020" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 24: 13003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182413003

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop