Next Article in Journal
Geochemical Characteristics and Ecological Risk Assessment of Heavy Metals in Surface Soil of Gaomi City
Next Article in Special Issue
Impact of Safety Culture on Safety Performance; Mediating Role of Psychosocial Hazard: An Integrated Modelling Approach
Previous Article in Journal
An Assessment of Government Capacity Building to Restrict the Marketing of Unhealthy Food and Non-Alcoholic Beverage Products to Children in the Region of the Americas
Previous Article in Special Issue
Post-Pandemic Patient Safety Culture: A Case from a Large Metropolitan Hospital Group in Taiwan
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Demand–Control Model as a Predictor of Depressive Symptoms—Interaction and Differential Subscale Effects: Prospective Analyses of 2212 German Employees

1
Unit 3.2 Psychosocial Factors and Mental Health, Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (BAuA), 13017 Berlin, Germany
2
Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (BAuA), 10317 Berlin, Germany
3
Unit 3.0 Work and Health, Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (BAuA), 10317 Berlin, Germany
4
National Research Centre for the Working Environment, 2100 Copenhagen, Denmark
5
Centre for Work Life and Evaluation Studies (CTA) & the Faculty of Odontology, Malmö University, 211 19 Malmö, Sweden
6
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27701, USA
7
Department of Epidemiology, Local Health Unit ASL TO 3, Piedmont Region, 10095 Turin, Italy
8
Department of Medical Psychology, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, 10317 Berlin, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(16), 8328; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168328
Submission received: 21 June 2021 / Revised: 19 July 2021 / Accepted: 26 July 2021 / Published: 6 August 2021

Abstract

:
Testing assumptions of the widely used demand–control (DC) model in occupational psychosocial epidemiology, we investigated (a) interaction, i.e., whether the combined effect of low job control and high psychological demands on depressive symptoms was stronger than the sum of their single effects (i.e., superadditivity) and (b) whether subscales of psychological demands and job control had similar associations with depressive symptoms. Logistic longitudinal regression analyses of the 5-year cohort of the German Study of Mental Health at Work (S-MGA) 2011/12–2017 of 2212 employees were conducted. The observed combined effect of low job control and high psychological demands on depressive symptoms did not indicate interaction (RERI = −0.26, 95% CI = −0.91; 0.40). When dichotomizing subscales at the median, differential effects of subscales were not found. When dividing subscales into categories based on value ranges, differential effects for job control subscales (namely, decision authority and skill discretion) were found (p = 0.04). This study does not support all assumptions of the DC model: (1) it corroborates previous studies not finding an interaction of psychological demands and job control; and (2) signs of differential subscale effects were found regarding job control. Too few prospective studies have been carried out regarding differential subscale effects.

1. Introduction

A widely used psychosocial construct in occupational social epidemiology [1], the demand–control (DC) model, assumes that occupational health risks are dependent on psychological demands and job control [2,3]. The DC model—being introduced by Robert Karasek in 1979—assumes that high psychological demands, e.g., high work load, and low job control, e.g., low autonomy, in a work context pose a special health risk for workers [2,3]. It supposes that exposure to high demands without opportunity to control the work situation would cause a chronic stress reaction, which again would lead to non-somatic and somatic disease outcomes [2,3].
The DC model combines psychological demands and job control into four categories, or quadrants: (i) the strain quadrant has high demands and low job control; (ii) the low strain category has low demands and high job control; (iii) the active quadrant has high demands and high job control; and (iv) the passive quadrant has low demands and low job control. The strain quadrant is proposed to have the highest risk for poor health and the low strain quadrant is proposed to have the lowest risk, whereas the active and passive quadrants would have intermediate risks [3,4]. The DC model supposes that the observed health effect of the strain quadrant would be higher than the additive effect of low job control and high psychological demands (i.e., superadditivity), that is, the model supposes interaction [3,4].
According to the DC model, psychological demands describe the general workload in an occupational context [3]. Job control denotes to what extent autonomy can be exercised through decision authority, e.g., influence at work, and skill discretion, e.g., opportunities for development. Decision authority denotes the extent to which one can influence decisions at the workplace. Skill discretion deals with the opportunity to learn and develop skills at work. Psychological demands have mainly been measured by items on work pace and amount of work (together often called quantitative demands), whereas job control is defined by the decision authority and skill discretion (e.g., opportunities for development) [4,5]. In this paper, we label work pace and amount of work on the one hand and decision authority and skill discretion on the other hand as subscales of the main scales of psychological demands and job control, respectively. The approach of combining these subscales is justified if these subscales have the same association with outcomes as their main scales [1].
Since the introduction of the DC model [2], research on that model has considered depressive symptoms as an outcome [1,6,7]. Even if ample prospective research has established job strain as a risk factor for depressive symptoms [6,7], two issues are still unresolved: (1) we still do not know if this elevated risk is just the sum of the risk of high psychological demands and low job control or if there is a surplus risk over and above their mere sum of risks [1,6,8,9,10]; and (2) we also do not know if health effects of the subscales work pace and amount of work are similar, and if health effects of the subscales decision authority and skill discretion are similar. A systematic investigation of possible differential subscale effects of psychological demands and job control on health has to our knowledge seldom been carried out [1,11,12].
On the basis of these considerations, we tested the following assumptions of the DC model:
  • Regarding the supposed interaction of psychological demands and job control, the combined effect of high psychological demands and low job control on depressive symptoms is stronger than their mere sum (i.e., superadditivity).
  • Regarding differential effects on depressive symptoms of the subscales of psychological demands and job control: 2.a. the subscales of work pace and amount of work have similar associations with depressive symptoms; 2.b. the subscales of decision authority and skill discretion have similar associations with depressive symptoms.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Overview

In a cohort of employees in Germany with self-reported data from 2011/12 and 2017, we carried out binomial regressions with depressive symptoms as the dependent variable in order to test the interaction between the independent variables, psychological demands and job control, and possible differential effects of subdimensions of the independent variables.

2.2. Population

We used data from the German Study on Mental Health at Work (SMGA), which is a nation-wide representative employee cohort study with a baseline survey in 2011/12 and a follow-up in 2017 [13], mean follow-up time 5.1 year (minimum 4.7 years, maximum 5.4 years). At the baseline, the target population consisted of all employees subjected to compulsory social security in Germany on 31 December 2010 born in 1951–1980 [13]. The study population was enrolled through the register of Integrated Employment Biographies (IEB) of the German Federal Employment Agency at the Institute for Employment Research (IAB). This register does not cover civil servants, self-employed workers and freelancers. A comparison of the analyzed cohort with the sample frame shows that people with a lower age and lower social class had a slightly lower response, whereas people with a higher age and higher social class had a slightly higher response; there were no differences due to gender [13,14]. The analyzed cohort comprised 2212 people being employees at the baseline (Figure 1).
In the cohort analyzed, women constituted half of the sample, most participants were skilled workers (International Standard Classification of Occupations, ISCO, main groups 4–7, regarding ISCO see last paragraph of the independent variable Section 2.3.2), and mean age was 47 years (Table 1). Average age was 46.7 years. The median score for psychological demands was 2.3—for the subscales work pace and amount of work, the scores were 3.0 and 1.9, respectively—and the job control scale median was 2.3, for the subscales of decision authority and skill discretion, the scores were 1.8 and 2.8, respectively (Table 1). Employees were distributed evenly in the four quadrants of strain, no strain, passive, and active, reflecting a very low association between psychological demands and job control (Pearson correlation −0.033).

2.3. Variables

Information at the baseline on gender, age, socioeconomic position, psychological demands, and job control was collected via a computer-assisted personal interview. At the end of the baseline and follow-up interviews, participants answered items on depressive symptoms privately in a separate paper questionnaire, which they gave back to the interviewer in a closed envelope to ensure confidentiality. Regarding the scales described below, quartiles, Cronbach’s alphas, and inter-item correlations are shown in Table 1.

2.3.1. Dependent Variable

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the German version of the 9-item scale of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ) [15,16]. Symptoms were measured with the question: “Over the last 2 weeks, how often have you been bothered by any of the following problems?” The nine items in the scale were: “Little interest or pleasure in doing things”, “Feeling down, depressed or hopeless”, “Difficulty falling asleep or sleeping or increased sleep”, “Tiredness or feeling unable to have energy”, “Decreased appetite or excessive need to eat”, “Bad opinion of yourself”, “Difficulty concentrating on something”, “Slowed speech/movement or restlessness (“fidgety”)”, and “Thoughts that you would rather be dead or want to self-inflict pain”. Response options to these questions were: “Not at all” (0), “Several days” (1), “More than half the days” (2), and “Nearly every day” (3). The scale score for depressive symptoms was computed as the sum of all items [15]. If not all, at least half of the items were answered and an adjusted sum was calculated, taking the number of missing items into account. The scale thus ranged from 0 to 27. The Cronbach’s alpha of the published German version was 0.88, which is on the same level as in the present study (Table 1) [15]. For our analyses, we treated depressive symptoms as a dichotomous variable with a cut-off of 10 or above as the screening threshold for major depressive disorder [17]. Of the participants, 8% experienced depressive symptoms at the baseline and 10% at follow-up (PHQ-9 ≥ 10).

2.3.2. Independent Variables

Psychosocial working conditions were measured with items from the 1st version of the Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire (COPSOQ 1) [18,19]. The COPSOQ instrument enables the measurement of various constructs, including the DC model. A comparison of the original items from the Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ) developed to measure the scales from the DC model and the corresponding items from the COPSOQ instrument used in the present paper can be seen in Table 2.
The COPSOQ items had the following response options: “Always” (4), “Often” (3), “Sometimes” (2), “Seldom” (1), and “Never/hardly ever” (0), apart from two items in the scale regarding skill discretion (see below). For the main analysis of interaction, all scales, ranging from 0 to 4, were dichotomized at their medians (median values can be seen in Table 1). For a sensitivity analysis, the main scales of psychological demands and job control were dichotomized at the value 2; high demands were ≥2–4, low control was 0–≤2. This value was chosen as it was in the middle of the scale range from 0 to 4. For the main analyses of subscale effects, subscales (work pace, amount of work, decision authority, and skill discretion) were also dichotomized at their medians (median values can be seen in Table 1) [2] and divided into three categories based on the value ranges 0–<1.333 for “Low”, 1.333–<2.666 for “Medium” and 2.333–4 for “High”—for a sensitivity analysis, see the analysis subsection below. These ranges were chosen so that each covered one third of the value ranges, which, as indicated above, range from 0 to 4.
Psychological demands: The main scale of psychological demands was based on the overall mean of the subscales work pace and amount of work described below (Table 2), such as the Job Content Questionnaire instrument (JCQ) measuring the DC model [4,5]. Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was on the same level as for a scale with the same items in a German validation study amounting to 0.82 (Table 1) [18]. The subscale of work pace was measured with one item (Table 2). The subscale of amount of work was based on the mean of four items (Table 2).
Job control: The main scale of job control (namely, decision latitude) was calculated as the mean of the subscales of decision authority and skill discretion, as described below, in order to accommodate the JCQ instrument [4]. Items of the two subscales below were given different weights in order to correspond with the JCQ measure (Table 2). The subscale decision authority was based on the mean of four items (Table 2). Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was somewhat higher than a scale with the same items in a German validation study amounting to 0.64 (Table 1) [18,19]. The subscale of skill discretion was based on the mean of three COPSOQ items (Table 2). Two items (“learning new things” and “use skills or expertise”) had the response options and values for the scale: “To a very large extent” (4), “To a large extent” (3), “Somewhat” (2), “To a small extent” (1), “To a very small extent” (0). Cronbach’s alpha for the skill discretion subscale was on the same level as a similar scale of a German validation study, being 0.70 (Table 1) [18,19].
DC quadrants: A categorical variable was created in order to measure exposure to combinations of psychological demands and job control with the following categories defined by median values in the population [2]: “Strain” (i.e., exposed to both high psychological demands and low job control: ≥median psychological demands and <median job control); “Active” (i.e., exposed to high psychological demands and high job control: ≥median psychological demands and ≥median job control); “Passive” (i.e., exposed to low job control and low psychological demands: <median psychological demands and <median job control); and “Low strain” (i.e., not exposed; <median psychological demands and ≥median job control), the latter being the reference group. For a sensitivity analysis, a variable was created where the quadrants were defined by value ranges of the scales, where high demands corresponded to a score of 2–4, low demands 0–≤2, low job control 0–2 and high control >2–4.
Covariates: We included gender, age, and socioeconomic status. Age was treated with a linear and a quadratic term, as it has been shown to have a reversed u-shaped association with depressive symptoms in adult working populations, with the lowest levels for the youngest [20,21]. Socioeconomic status was treated as a categorical variable [22]. Occupations were manually coded according to the International Standard Classification of Occupations (ISCO 08) and categorized into four groups on the basis of skill level, following the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) [23]. There were two exceptions: managers (ISCO main group 1) were put together with professionals (main group 2), and plant and machine operators (main group 8) were put together with elementary occupations (main group 9).

2.4. Analyses

We carried out two types of analyses in order to test assumptions of the DC model:
We performed interaction analyses in order to test if the observed combined effect of high psychological demands and low job control on depressive symptoms was stronger than their expected sum (i.e., superadditivity).
We performed differential subscale effect analyses in order to see if there were effects on depressive symptoms from the subscales of psychological demands (i.e., work pace and amount of work) and job control (i.e., decision authority and skill discretion) were the same.

2.4.1. Interaction Analyses

Prospective multiple binomial regression analyses were conducted, as this approach yields Rate Ratios (RR) needed for assessing interaction [24]. In the main analysis, we tested for an interaction of baseline psychological demands and job control with depressive symptoms at follow-up as the dependent variable, adjusted for the following baseline variables: depressive symptoms, socioeconomic position (treated categorically also in all other regressions described below), gender, and age. We used Rothman’s Relative Excess Risk due to Interaction (RERI) approach [25,26] to test whether strain had a higher risk than the sum of the risks of high psychological demands and low job control (i.e., superadditivity), as the DC model proposes [2,3]. REHRI was calculated according to this approach [27]. A significant positive RERI indicates superadditivity, a negative RERI, subadditivity. In a first sensitivity analysis, the main analysis was repeated, excluding those who had depressive symptoms at the baseline. In a second sensitivity analysis, the main analysis was repeated, with high psychological demands and low job control defined by value ranges (see variable description above) instead of medians, as in the main analysis.

2.4.2. Differential Subscale Effect Analyses

Through a log binomial regression, we tested whether each of the baseline subscales of psychological demands (work pace and amount of work) and job control (decision authority and skill discretion)—adjusted for the same covariates as described above—had similar effects on depressive symptoms at follow-up. More specifically, we tackled the question of whether work pace and amount of work, dichotomized at their median, had similar associations with depressive symptoms and whether decision authority and skill discretion had similar associations with depressive symptoms, respectively. In a sensitivity analysis, these analyses were repeated with subscales divided into three categories based on value ranges (see variable description above), in contrast to the median criterion in the main analysis. Differences between RRs of depressive symptoms for the subscales in each dimension were tested assessing heterogeneity of the RRs through fixed-effect meta-analysis [28].

2.4.3. Significance Level and Software Used

The significance level was 0.05. No Bonferroni adjustment for multiple tests was applied [29]. Data were analyzed by means of SPSS 25 (IBM SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA) using the GENLIN command in order to perform the aforementioned binomial regressions [24,25,26,27].

3. Results

3.1. Interaction Analysis

Employees in the strain quadrant had significantly increased RRs for depressive symptoms at follow-up as compared to the low strain quadrant (RR = 1.51, 95% CI = 1.03; 2.23) (Table 3). Being in the passive or active quadrants was not significantly associated with depressive symptoms. The observed combined effect of low control and high demands did not deviate from the expected effect (RERI = −0.2; 95% CI = −0.85; 0.43), that is, no superadditivity was found (Table 3, last two columns). The sensitivity analysis yielded the same results when excluding employees with baseline depressive symptoms (Appendix, Table A1) or when dichotomizing psychological demands and job control based on scale value ranges (Appendix, Table A2).

3.2. Differential Subscale Effect Analyses

3.2.1. Psychological Demand Subscales

The main analysis of risk for depressive symptoms using a median criterion of categorization of the subscales work pace and amount of work found no differential subscale effects (p-value for heterogeneity: 0.92) (Table 4). The sensitivity analysis using a categorization based on value ranges also did not show clear differential subscale effects; although a significantly increased risk was found among those in the highest exposure group for amount of work, this risk estimate was not significantly different from that of high work pace (p-value for heterogeneity: 0.33) (Appendix, Table A3).

3.2.2. Job Control Subscales

The main analysis using a median criterion of categorization of the subscales decision authority and skill discretion found no differential subscale effects (p-value for heterogeneity: 0.58). The sensitivity analysis using a categorization based on value ranges found differential subscale effects; however, they were not strongly significant (p-value for heterogeneity: 0.04) (Appendix, Table A3).

4. Discussion

The present study adds to a handful of studies investigating the rationale of the DC model when predicting depressive symptoms regarding (a) the interaction of the dimensions of psychological demands and job control, and (b) the differential effects of subscales of these two dimensions.
First, the present study indicates that job control does not interact with psychological demands, contrary to the expectation of the DC model: we did not observe a combined effect on depressive symptoms above the additive effect of high psychological demands and low job control, i.e., superadditivity.
Second, the present study found signs of differential effects of the subscales of job control, contrary to the DC model’s expectation of similar effects. While in the main analysis (when dichotomizing subscales at the median), differential effects of the subscales were not found (Table 4), a sensitivity analysis (when categorizing subscales based on value ranges) suggests differential effects for subscales of job control (decision authority and skill discretion).

4.1. Comparison with Other Studies

Three prospective studies and one meta-analysis have investigated whether the combined effect of high psychological demand and low job control poses an additional risk over and above their main effects on depressive symptoms, i.e., whether the risk factors of job control and psychological demands interact [6,8,9,10]. None of the studies found statistical interaction. However, all four analyses did investigate deviations from multiplicativity, which is a more conservative approach than the present study’s analysis of deviations from additivity [25]. The latter approach fits better with what the JCQ model proposed, namely, superadditivity [2,3]. The three original studies were based on 2821 workers from nine companies in Belgium (Belstress), 3366 workers of a Finnish representative sample (the health 2000 study), and 11,552 workers in an energy production company in France (Gazel), respectively [8,9,10]. The meta-analysis relied on 120,211 workers from 14 cohort studies carried out in the UK, Denmark, Finland, and Sweden, none of them being among the above-mentioned original studies [6]. All three original studies used items from the JCQ instrument on psychological demands and job control [4], while the meta-analysis used a previous harmonization of measures of job strain [30]. For the present study, items from the COPSOQ similar to those from the JCQ were chosen (Table 1). To measure depressive symptoms, Belstress and Gazel used the CES-D instrument, a valid indicator of depressive symptoms, just like the present study’s PHQ-9-instrument. The health 2000 study used prescribed antidepressant medication as an indicator. The meta-analysis relied largely on data on clinical depression. In conclusion, our results regarding the absence of interaction appear to be supported by all four studies. It should, however, be noted that these studies’ approaches were more conservative, as they looked for a multiplicative interaction.
With respect to the differential subscale effects of the DC model on depressive symptoms, we are only aware of two studies that examined this question—and only regarding the job control scale [11,12]. One study was based on a random sample of the adult population in Stockholm County, Sweden, comprising 4710 workers (PART study); the other study was conducted on 10,308 civil servants in London, UK (Whitehall study). Both studies investigated the differential effects of decision authority and skill discretion. In the PART study, risks did not differ from each other as CIs were relatively wide [11]. The larger Whitehall study found borderline different subscale effects; the point estimate of low decision authority (men: RR = 1.29, 95% CI 1.1–1.5; women: 1.37, 95% CI 1.1–1.8) was in the upper end of the CIs of low skill discretion (men: 0.9–1.3; women: 0.8–1.4) [12]. Both studies used the JCQ [4]; the PART study categorized subscales based on value ranges, whereas the Whitehall study categorized them using tertiles. The PART study measured depressive symptoms by means of the Major Depression Inventory, which specifically targets depression, while the Whitehall study used the General Health Questionnaire, which measures common psychological disorders. In summary, one high powered study did not find differential subscale effects; another higher-powered study found only borderline significant effects in the same direction as the present study.

4.2. Strengths and Weaknesses

A strength of the present study is that it is prospective, making it easier to assess causality. Second, in order to reduce bias due to the mode of data collection, questions on depressive symptoms using the Patient Health Questionnaire were filled out by the respondent in a separate room and put into an envelope, so as to minimize an interviewer bias. Third, the study did not comprise workers older than 60 years at the baseline. As health selection occurs more often in higher age groups, this age restriction may have limited healthy worker selection already at the baseline. Fourth, we followed up all participating employees in the study disregarding if they still worked or exited work at follow-up. Depressive symptoms have been associated to exit from work [31,32]. Keeping participants who changed jobs or exited work at follow-up contributes to avoiding healthy worker selection during follow-up in the analyses of the present paper.
The strengths of this study need to be balanced against its weaknesses. First, this study is observational, and thus, as always in such studies, selection bias has to be considered. Second, the participation rate in the study is low. Based on comparisons with the study’s sampling frame, we acknowledge that there may be biases due to regional characteristics, gender, age, education, profession, and income [13,14]. Additionally, we cannot rule out biases due to possible higher attrition among those becoming depressed during follow-up. Third, although the study comprises over 2000 participants, it has limited power to detect smaller effects. A post hoc power analysis shows that the present study has a power of 0.53 to find a significant elevated risk of 1.5 [33]. However, the present study’s results could contribute to future meta-analyses. Fourth, the study did not use the original JCQ items, but used the COPSOQ tool [18,34] (Table 2). Therefore, we weighted items for the scales in the present study in order to reflect the items in the original JCQ instrument (see weights in last column of Table 1). It can be questioned that we categorized the JCQ item “My job requires working very hard” as a measure of work pace, as this item could also be considered to measure high amount of work. Using COPSOQ to measure work pace, we only had one item available, namely “Do you have to work very fast?”. However, the single work pace item has been shown to have a good correlation with the complete COPSOQ work pace scale [35].

5. Conclusions

The findings of the present study have implications for practice and research.
From a practical perspective, the results of the present study indicate that if one aims to prevent work-related depressive symptoms, one should reduce psychological demands and increase job control. Further, there seems to be no extra risk reduction for those exposed to both high psychological demand and low job control over and above their additive effect. Organizations should not only focus on those workers being exposed to both these risk factors, but also on those only exposed to high demands or low control. When addressing the two subdimensions of job control, namely, decision authority and skill discretion, more efforts should focus on increasing decision authority than skill discretion in order to prevent work-related depressive symptoms.
From a research perspective, it should be further investigated if the combination of high psychological demands and low job control poses an additional risk over the sum of their main effects. Apart from the present study, only four previous prospective studies have investigated this issue [6,8,9,10]. It should also be investigated if there are differential effects of the subscales of work pace and amount of work of the job demands scale and of the subscales of decision authority and skill discretion of the job control scale. The present study is the only one to have investigated the subscales of psychological demand with regard to job control; this issue has only been studied in three prospective studies (the present study and two previous studies [11,12]). Therefore, we suggest re-analyzing existing longitudinal studies, preferably in a meta-analytic fashion to overcome the limited power offered by most psychosocial cohorts, containing items covering the DC model to confirm or reject the findings of existing studies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.B. (Hermann Burr), M.F., A.P., A.d., T.C. and H.B. (Hanne Berthelsen); methodology, A.S., U.R., A.d., H.B. (Hermann Burr) and G.M.; statistical analyses, H.B. (Hermann Burr), A.d. and A.P.; literature search and review, H.B. (Hermann Burr), A.d. and A.S.; carrying out the S-MGA, H.B. (Hermann Burr), U.R., A.S. and G.M.; writing—original draft preparation, H.B. (Hermann Burr); writing—review and editing, all authors; project administration, U.R.; funding acquisition, U.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The 2011/12 and 2017 rounds of the S-MGA study were funded internally by the BAuA (project no. F 2250 and F 2384). The paper was written as part of the projects “Interaction effects of psychosocial occupational factors on mental health” and “Longitudinal associations between working and employment conditions, burnout and depressive symptoms” at the BAuA (internally funded project no. F 2407 and F 2460).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (BAuA) (approval number 006_2016_Müller, 6 March 2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. All employees in the sample were contacted by mail and the interviews at the baseline were only conducted after each respondent gave their informed oral consent [13]. A written consent was given for the willingness to participate at follow-up.

Data Availability Statement

A scientific use file (SUF) containing the baseline data is available at the Research Data Centre of IAB; a SUF containing the cohort is underway.

Acknowledgments

The S-MGA was conducted in collaboration with the Institute for Employment Research (IAB). The S-MGA was based on samples from statistics of the Federal Employment Agency (BA), which have been merged with Integrated Employment Biographies by the IAB. The S-MGA data were managed by Dagmar Pattloch from the BAuA.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Associations between the DC measure at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2039 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany without depressive symptoms at baseline. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
Table A1. Associations between the DC measure at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2039 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany without depressive symptoms at baseline. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
DC Quadrants (Based on Medians)NObserved Prevalence of Depressive Symptoms at Follow-Up (%)p1RR 295% CIRERI 2,395% CI
0.179
Low strain5475 1
Passive5309 1.611.01; 2.56
Active4926 1.230.75; 2.02
Strain 4709 1.560.98; 2.51−0.28−1.17; 0.61
1 This p value denotes to what extent the categorical DC measure is associated with depressive symptoms. 2 Adjusted for socioeconomic position, gender and age at baseline. 3 Relative Excess Risk due to Interaction. A positive value (i.e., a value above 0) would express superadditivity, that is, the observed combined effect of low control and high demands would be above the additive effect of these two factors [25,26].
Table A2. Associations between a value-based DC measure at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
Table A2. Associations between a value-based DC measure at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
DC Quadrants (Value Based 1)NObserved Prevalence of Depressive Symptoms at Follow-Up (%)p2RR 395% CIRERI 3,§95% CI
0.087
Low strain4926 1
Passive23810 1.370.84; 2.34
Active9679 1.380.93; 2.03
Strain 51516 1.661.12; 2.48−0.08−0.81; 0.63
1 High demands 2–4, low demands 0–<2; low job control 0–2, high job control >2–4. See variable subsection of method section. 2 This p-value denotes to what extent the categorical DC measure is associated with depressive symptoms at follow-up. 3 Adjusted for depressive symptoms, socioeconomic position, gender and age at baseline. § Relative Excess Risk due to Interaction. A positive value (i.e., a value above 0) would express superadditivity, that is, the observed combined effect of low control and high demands would be above the additive effect of these two factors [25,26].
Table A3. Associations between DC measure subscales at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
Table A3. Associations between DC measure subscales at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
DimensionSubscaleNObserved Prevalence of Depressive Symptoms at Follow-Up (%)p 1RR 295% CI
Psychological demandsWork pace 0.510
Low (≤1) 32438 1
Medium (2) 35799 0.910.57; 1.46
High (≥3) 3139011 1.080.70; 1.65
Amount of work 0.103
Low (<1⅓) 47418 1
Medium (1⅓–<2⅔) 4101510 1.190.88; 1.61
High (≥2⅔) 445615 1.411.02; 1.94
Job controlDecision authority 0.110
Low (<1⅓) 466713 1.591.03; 2.44
Medium (1⅓–<2⅔) 4111110 1.460.97; 2.22
High (≥2⅔) 44346 1
Skill discretion 0.528
Low (<1⅓) 411310 0.810.50; 1.33
Medium (1⅓–<2⅔) 453612 1.060.83; 1.36
High (≥2⅔) 415989 1
1 This p-value denotes to what extent the categorical DC measure is associated with depressive symptoms at follow-up. 2 Adjusted for baseline depressive symptoms, socioeconomic position, gender and age. Not adjusted for the other DC measure subscales. 3 Values for the categories of the item. 4 Value ranges for the scale.

References

  1. Burr, H.; d’Errico, A. Priority, methodological and conceptual issues regarding epidemiological research of occupational psychosocial risk factors for poor mental health and coronary heart disease. Sociol. Del. Lav. 2018, 63, 159–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Karasek, R.A. Job Demands, Job Decision Latitude, and Mental Strain: Implications for Job Redesign. Adm. Sci. Q. 1979, 24, 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Karasek, R.; Theorell, T. Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstruction of Working Life; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  4. Karasek, R. Job Content Questionnaire and User’s Guide, Rev. 1.7; University of Massachusetts-Lowel: Lowell, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  5. Choi, B.; Kawakami, N.; Chang, S.; Koh, S.; Bjorner, J.; Punnett, L.; Karasek, R. A cross-national study on the multidimensional characteristics of the five-item psychological demands scale of the Job Content Questionnaire. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2008, 15, 120–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Madsen, I.E.H.; Nyberg, S.T.; Magnusson Hanson, L.L.; Ferrie, J.E.; Ahola, K.; Alfredsson, L.; Batty, G.D.; Bjorner, J.B.; Borritz, M.; Burr, H.; et al. Job strain as a risk factor for clinical depression: Systematic review and meta-analysis with additional individual participant data. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 1342–1356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Theorell, T.; Hammarstrom, A.; Aronsson, G.; Traskman Bendz, L.; Grape, T.; Hogstedt, C.; Marteinsdottir, I.; Skoog, I.; Hall, C. A systematic review including meta-analysis of work environment and depressive symptoms. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Clays, E.; de Bacquer, D.; Leynen, F.; Kornitzer, M.; Kittel, F.; de Backer, G. Job stress and depression symptoms in middle-aged workers--prospective results from the Belstress study. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2007, 33, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Niedhammer, I.; Goldberg, M.; Leclerc, A.; Bugel, I.; David, S. Psychosocial factors at work and subsequent depressive symptoms in the Gazel cohort. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 1998, 24, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Virtanen, M.; Honkonen, T.; Kivimaki, M.; Ahola, K.; Vahtera, J.; Aromaa, A.; Lonnqvist, J. Work stress, mental health and antidepressant medication findings from the Health 2000 Study. J. Affect. Disord. 2007, 98, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Fandino-Losada, A.; Forsell, Y.; Lundberg, I. Demands, skill discretion, decision authority and social climate at work as determinants of major depression in a 3-year follow-up study. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2013, 86, 591–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Stansfeld, S.A.; Fuhrer, R.; Shipley, M.J.; Marmot, M.G. Work characteristics predict psychiatric disorder: Prospective results from the Whitehall II Study. Occup. Environ. Med. 1999, 56, 302–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Rose, U.; Schiel, S.; Schroder, H.; Kleudgen, M.; Tophoven, S.; Rauch, A.; Freude, G.; Muller, G. The Study on Mental Health at Work: Design and sampling. Scand. J. Public Health 2017, 45, 584–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. D’Errico, A.; Burr, H.; Pattloch, D.; Kersten, N.; Rose, U. Working conditions as risk factors for early exit from work—in a cohort of 2351 employees in Germany. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2021, 94, 117–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Gräfe, K.; Zipfel, S.; Herzog, W.; Löwe, B. Screening psychischer Störungen mit dem “Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten (PHQ-D)”. Ergebnisse der deutschen Validierungsstudie [Screening of mental disorders with the “Patient Health Questionnaire Germany (PHQ-D)”. Results of a German validation study]. Diagnostica 2004, 50, 10. [Google Scholar]
  16. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: The PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA 1999, 282, 1737–1744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Manea, L.; Gilbody, S.; McMillan, D. A diagnostic meta-analysis of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) algorithm scoring method as a screen for depression. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 2015, 37, 67–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Nübling, M.; Stößel, U.; Hasselhorn, H.M.; Michaelis, M.; Hofmann, F. Measuring psychological stress and strain at work—Evaluation of the COPSOQ Questionnaire in Germany. Psychosoc. Med. 2006, 3, Doc05. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  19. Kristensen, T.S.; Hannerz, H.; Hogh, A.; Borg, V. The Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire—A tool for the assessment and improvement of the psychosocial work environment. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2005, 31, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Busch, M.A.; Maske, U.E.; Ryl, L.; Schlack, R.; Hapke, U. Prevalence of depressive symptoms and diagnosed depression among adults in Germany: Results of the German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Adults (DEGS1). Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundh. Gesundh. 2013, 56, 733–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Schmitt, M.; Altstötter-Gleich, C.; Hinz, A.; Maes, J.; Brähler, E. Normwerte für das Vereinfachte Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI-V) in der Allgemeinbevölkerung [Norm values for the simplified Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-V) in the general population]. Diagnostica 2006, 52, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Goldthorpe, J.H. On Sociology: Numbers, Narratives, and the Integration of Research and Theory; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  23. International Labor Office Staff. International Standard Classification of Occupations 2008 (ISCO-08): Structure, Group Definitions and Correspondence Tables; International Labour Office: Geneva, Switzerland, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  24. Skov, T.; Deddens, J.; Petersen, M.R.; Endahl, L. Prevalence proportion ratios: Estimation and hypothesis testing. Int. J. Epidemiol. 1998, 27, 91–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rothman, K. Epidemiology. An Introduction; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  26. Andersson, T.; Alfredsson, L.; Kallberg, H.; Zdravkovic, S.; Ahlbom, A. Calculating measures of biological interaction. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 20, 575–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Andersson, T.; Alfredsson, L.; Källberg, H.; Zdravkovic, S.; Ahlbom, A. Excel Sheet to Calculate Measures of Biological Interaction. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/342077886_August_26_2020_Alternative_working_link_to_referred_excel_file_httpswwwbiostatistikseepinetcalculationxls (accessed on 9 December 2020).
  28. Greenland, S. Quantitative methods in the review of epidemiologic literature. J. Epidemiol. Rev. 1987, 9, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Rothman, K. No Adjustments Are Needed for Multiple Comparisons. Epidemiology 1990, 1, 43–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Fransson, E.I.; Nyberg, S.T.; Heikkila, K.; Alfredsson, L.; de Bacquer, D.; Batty, G.D.; Bonenfant, S.; Casini, A.; Clays, E.; Goldberg, M.; et al. Comparison of alternative versions of the job demand-control scales in 17 European cohort studies: The IPD-Work consortium. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Bultmann, U.; Christensen, K.B.; Burr, H.; Lund, T.; Rugulies, R. Severe depressive symptoms as predictor of disability pension: A 10-year follow-up study in Denmark. Eur. J. Public Health 2008, 18, 232–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Porru, F.; Burdorf, A.; Robroek, S.J.W. The impact of depressive symptoms on exit from paid employment in Europe: A longitudinal study with 4 years follow-up. Eur. J. Public Health 2019, 29, 134–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Bickel, P.J.; Kjell, A.D. Mathematical Statistics: Basic Ideas and Selected Topics; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  34. Pejtersen, J.H.; Kristensen, T.S.; Borg, V.; Bjorner, J.B. The second version of the Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire. Scand. J. Public Health 2010, 38, 8–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Burr, H.; Berthelsen, H.; Moncada, S.; Nübling, M.; Dupret, E.; Demiral, Y.; Oudyk, J.; Kristensen, T.S.; Llorens, C.; Navarro, A.; et al. The Third Version of the Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire. Saf. Health Work 2019, 10, 482–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of participation in SMGA’s 2011/12 baseline and the 2011/12–2017 cohort.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of participation in SMGA’s 2011/12 baseline and the 2011/12–2017 cohort.
Ijerph 18 08328 g001
Table 1. Distribution of covariates, psychosocial working conditions at baseline and depressive symptoms at baseline and follow-up among 2212 employees aged 31–60.
Table 1. Distribution of covariates, psychosocial working conditions at baseline and depressive symptoms at baseline and follow-up among 2212 employees aged 31–60.
VariablesN%1st QuartileMedian3rd QuartileCronbach’s AlphaInter Item Correlation Range
Gender
  Men107949
  Women113351
Age 414753
Socioeconomic position
  Un- and semiskilled workers, category (ISCO 1 main group 8,9)25512
  Skilled workers, category (ISCO 1 main group 4–7)78435
  Semi-professionals, category (ISCO 1 main group 3)62928
  Professionals/managers, category (ISCO 1 main group 1,2)54425
Demand control quadrants 2
  Low strain, category 256326
  Passive, category 256826
  Active, category 253224
  Strain, category 254925
Psychological demands, scale 3 1.82.32.80.820.26–0.67
  Work pace, subscale 3 2.03.03.0n/a 4n/a 4
  Amount of work, subscale 3 1.31.82.50.840.48–0.67
Job control, scale 3 1.82.32.80.750.14–0.45
  Decision authority, subscale 3 1.11.82.40.710.32–0.43
  Skill discretion, subscale 3 2.22.83.30.700.41–0.45
Depressive symptoms at baseline, scale 5 2.04.06.00.830.21–0.51
  No, category (PHQ-9 < 10) 6203992
  Yes, category (PHQ-9 ≥ 10) 61738
Depressive symptoms at follow-up, scale 5 2.04.06.00.830.18–0.55
  No, category (PHQ-9 < 10) 6198990
  Yes, category (PHQ-9 ≥ 10) 622310
1 ISCO, International Standard Classification of Occupations, see last paragraph of the Section 2.3.2 “Independent variables” [23]. 2 Defined by medians of psychological demands and job control (see also variable subsection of the method section). 3 Minimum = 0, Maximum = 4. 4 One item measure. 5 Minimum = 0, Maximum = 27. 6 Values for the scale.
Table 2. DC model items and corresponding COPSOQ items for scales and subscales being used in the present paper.
Table 2. DC model items and corresponding COPSOQ items for scales and subscales being used in the present paper.
Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ) Items, Version R-1.11 [4]Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire (COPSOQ) Items in the German Study on Mental Health at Work (S-MGA) [18]
ScaleSubscaleItemItemItem Weight for Subscale and Scale
Psychological demands 1Work pace 2My job requires working very fastDo you have to work very fast?4
My job requires working very hard
Amount of work 2I am not asked to do an excessive amount of work 3Is your workload unevenly distributed so it piles up?1
Do you fall behind with your work?1
I have enough time to get the job done 3How often do you not have time to complete all your work tasks?1
Do you have enough time for your work tasks?1
Job controlDecision authorityMy job allows me to make a lot of decisions on my ownDo you have a large degree of influence concerning your work?3
I have a lot of say about what happens in my jobDo you have a say in choosing who you work with?2
Do you have any influence on what you do at work?2
In my job, I have very little freedom to decide how I do my work 3
Can you influence the amount of work assigned to you?2
Skill discretionMy job requires that I learn new thingsDo you have the possibility of learning new things through your work?4.5
I have an opportunity to develop my own special abilities
My job requires me to be creative
My job requires a high level of skillCan you use your skills or expertise in your work?1.5
My job involves a lot of repetitive work 3Is your work varied?3
I get to do a variety of different things on my job
The JCQ is the official demand–control (DC) model questionnaire [4]. 1 The scale also has an item on conflicting demands, which has not been considered in the present paper [5]. 2 Subscale identified in factor analyses [5]. 3 Reverse coded.
Table 3. Associations between the Demand–Control (DC) measure at baseline in 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up in 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios (RR’s).
Table 3. Associations between the Demand–Control (DC) measure at baseline in 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up in 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios (RR’s).
DC Quadrants (Based on Medians)NObserved Prevalence of Depressive Symptoms at Follow-Up (%)p 1RR 295% CIRERI 2,395% CI
0.204
Low strain5636 1
Passive56811 1.420.96; 2.10
Active5329 1.310.87; 1.96
Strain 54914 1.511.03; 2.23−0.21−0.85; 0.43
1 This p-value denotes to what extent the categorical DC measure is associated with depressive symptoms at follow-up. 2 Adjusted for depressive symptoms, socioeconomic position, gender and age at baseline. 3 A positive value (i.e., a value above 0) would express superadditivity, that is, the observed combined effect of low control and high demands would be above the additive effect of these two factors [25,26]. CI = Confidence Intervals. RERI = Relative Excess Risk due to Interaction.
Table 4. Associations between the DC measure subscales at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
Table 4. Associations between the DC measure subscales at baseline 2011/12 and depressive symptoms at follow-up 2017 among 2212 employees aged 31 to 60 years in Germany. Multiple binomial regression. Rate Ratios.
DimensionSubscaleNObserved Prevalence of Depressive Symptoms at Follow-Up (%)p 1RR 295% CI
Psychological demandsWork pace 0.267
 <median 38229 1
 ≥median 3139011 1.150.90; 1.48
Amount of work 0.201
 <median 312028 1
 ≥median 3101012 1.170.92; 1.49
Job controlDecision authority 0.037
 <median 3112313 1.301.01; 1.66
 ≥median 310898 1
Skill discretion 0.180
 <median 3102212 1.180.93; 1.51
 ≥median 311908 1
1 This p-value denotes to what extent the categorical DC measure is associated with depressive symptoms at follow-up. 2 Adjusted for baseline depressive symptoms, socioeconomic position, gender and age. Not adjusted for the other DC measure subscales. 3 The median value can be seen in Table 2.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Burr, H.; Müller, G.; Rose, U.; Formazin, M.; Clausen, T.; Schulz, A.; Berthelsen, H.; Potter, G.; d’Errico, A.; Pohrt, A. The Demand–Control Model as a Predictor of Depressive Symptoms—Interaction and Differential Subscale Effects: Prospective Analyses of 2212 German Employees. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8328. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168328

AMA Style

Burr H, Müller G, Rose U, Formazin M, Clausen T, Schulz A, Berthelsen H, Potter G, d’Errico A, Pohrt A. The Demand–Control Model as a Predictor of Depressive Symptoms—Interaction and Differential Subscale Effects: Prospective Analyses of 2212 German Employees. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(16):8328. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168328

Chicago/Turabian Style

Burr, Hermann, Grit Müller, Uwe Rose, Maren Formazin, Thomas Clausen, Anika Schulz, Hanne Berthelsen, Guy Potter, Angelo d’Errico, and Anne Pohrt. 2021. "The Demand–Control Model as a Predictor of Depressive Symptoms—Interaction and Differential Subscale Effects: Prospective Analyses of 2212 German Employees" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 16: 8328. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168328

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop