Next Article in Journal
Objective Analysis of Movement in Subjects with ADHD. Multidisciplinary Control Tool for Students in the Classroom
Next Article in Special Issue
Investigating the Relationship between Work-To-Family Conflict, Job Burnout, Job Outcomes, and Affective Commitment in the Construction Industry
Previous Article in Journal
Changes in the Static Balance of Older Women Participating in Regular Nordic Walking Sessions and Nordic Walking Combined with Cognitive Training
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Predicting Errors, Violations, and Safety Participation Behavior at Nuclear Power Plants

1
State Key Laboratory of Nuclear Power Safety Monitoring Technology and Equipment, China Nuclear Power Engineering Co., Ltd., Shenzhen 518172, China
2
Institute of Human Factors and Ergonomics, College of Mechatronics and Control Engineering, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(15), 5613; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155613
Submission received: 11 July 2020 / Revised: 29 July 2020 / Accepted: 30 July 2020 / Published: 4 August 2020

Abstract

:
Commissioning workers at nuclear power plants have long been ignored in previous studies, although their performance is closely related to the overall safety of plants. This study aimed to explain and predict three types of behavior, i.e., errors, violations, and safety participation, of commissioning workers, under the general framework of the theory of planned behavior (TPB) and by considering organization and planning factors. The validity of the model was evaluated with a sample of 167 commissioning workers who completed a self-reported questionnaire. The results showed that perceived behavioral control, along with organization and planning, significantly affected all types of behavior. It was also found that violations and errors were a direct result of attitude. Besides, errors were predicted by subjective norm; unexpectedly, this occurred in a positive way. These findings revealed the underlying mechanisms for the development of errors, violations, and safety participation among commissioning workers and provided practical implications for safety improvement at the commissioning workplace.

1. Introduction

The burning of fossil fuels emits large quantities of air pollutants that are harmful to the environment and public health [1,2]. To cope with these adverse impacts, many countries have tried to reduce their reliance on fossil fuels by developing clean and renewable energy, such as nuclear, wind, solar, and geothermal energy. Among these emerging energy sources, nuclear energy has gained much attention for its relatively low cost and stable production rate [3]. By 2017, there were about 450 nuclear power reactors worldwide, providing over 10% of the world’s electricity [4]. These numbers will keep increasing, as many new nuclear power plants are on order or planned [4], notably in Asian countries such as China. China recently has restarted the construction of nuclear power plants after a two-year hiatus [5], with about 15 under construction and more about to start construction. With the intense development, commissioning, one of the key phases prior to the start of nuclear power plants’ commercial operation, has raised much attention recently.
According to the International Atomic Energy Agency [6], commissioning is “the process by means of which systems and components of facilities and activities, having been constructed, are made operational and verified to be in accordance with the design and to have met the required performance criteria”. Commissioning is a safety-critical phase in the nuclear power plant lifecycle because it aims at noticing and fixing all deficiencies and possible errors before the plant commences operation. Great challenges lie in commissioning phase, such as the pressure of completing work within a limited time, the need to test new features of the plants, and the possible unexpected emergencies with regard to the fuel loading stage [7]. Incidents or accidents during commissioning can lead to consequences ranging from minor (e.g., malfunction of devices) to severe (e.g., prolonged completion time and even excessive radiation leakage). Much evidence in occupational safety literature showed that the aberrant behavior of workers, such as errors and violations, was one important cause of accidents [8,9], while workers’ safety participation behavior significantly reduced accident risk [10,11]. These findings indicate that the behavioral performance of the commissioning workers is of great importance to the safety and efficiency of commissioning phase and to the future safety of nuclear power plants when they are in operation.
Numerous studies have been conducted and many theories have been proposed to explain individuals’ aberrant and safety participation behavior [12]. The investigated worker groups include construction workers [13], aviation maintainers [12], and oilrig workers [14]. Workers at nuclear power plants have also attracted some research attention, but the focus has been mostly on control room operators [15,16]. No research has focused on the behavior of commissioning workers. There is a great difference between commissioning workers and control room operators in terms of core tasks and ways of organization. For instance, the main tasks at control room are to monitor the status of nuclear power units and handle abnormities based on specific regulations. The main commissioning tasks, in contrast, are to start up the nuclear power units and conduct experiments to examine if the units can run normally, and such tasks are filled with uncertainties [7]. Another significant difference is that the control room usually has stable team members, while commissioning tends to build flexible teams based on specific tasks [17]. The complex and challenging features of commissioning may make the behavioral characteristics of commissioning workers quite different from those of control room operators. Therefore, studies focusing on commissioning workers are needed to understand the mechanisms behind behavior during commissioning.
Another research gap is that most of the available studies mentioned above emphasized only the aberrant behaviors and ignored workers’ intent and enthusiasm for proactively engaging in safety-promoting behavior. Such voluntary behavior, often referred to as safety participation, extends beyond compliance with safety regulations and is not formally rewarded by the organization but can be beneficial to the overall safety of the organization [18,19]. To fill these gaps, this study aimed to propose models to predict and explain errors, violations, and safety participation behavior of commissioning workers. Related findings not only shed theoretical light on understanding antecedents of commissioning workers’ behavior, but also provide guidelines to policymakers when designing countermeasures to advocate or reduce certain types of behavior. The remainder of the introduction describes the theoretical foundation underlying this study and the model and hypotheses proposed.

1.1. Errors, Violations, and Safety Participation Behavior

Aberrant behavior, which refers to a straying from the path [20], could be classified into two types based on whether the behavior is intentional or not. Instances of intentional aberrant behavior are traditionally referred to as violations, which involve deliberate deviation from rules or practices that are important in maintaining the safety of a particular task or job [12]. This is opposed to errors, which refer to unintended outcomes caused by slips, lapses, and mistakes made by individuals [12,20]. Both types of behavior are shaped by cognitive, psychological, and social factors. However, the effect sizes of these factors on violations and errors are different; the former are more closely associated with social and psychological factors such as attitude, while the latter are more strongly affected by deficiencies in cognitive abilities such as information processing efficiency and organization skills [21]. They also differ in their associations with demographic variables such as age and gender [22] and in their contributions to accidents [23,24].
While compliance with safety rules and regulations is important in lowering the risk of accidents, organizations also need individuals’ proactive participation in safety [18]. Safety participation refers to an employee’s voluntary participation in safety activities that is beyond the employee’s formal role but does contribute to the development of a supportive safety environment [25]. Examples of safety participation include promoting safety programs within the workplace, helping coworkers, and raising safety concerns [26,27]. Safety participation has been reported to be a significant predictor of occupational accidents [10,28], the effect size of which is even greater than that of aberrant behavior [27].

1.2. Theory of Planned Behavior

Many theories have been proposed and applied to explain human behavior [29]. For instance, theory of self-efficacy posits that two types of expectancies, i.e., outcome expectancy and self-efficacy expectancy, exert powerful influence on individual behavior [30]. Self-determination theory, by contrast, suggests that motivations are the strongest determinants in shaping who we are and how we behave. In particular, individuals are motivated to engage in certain activities when their needs for competence, connection, and autonomy are fulfilled [31]. Another well-recognized theory that tries to explain human behavior from a social cognitive perspective is the theory of planned behavior (TPB) proposed by Ajzen [32]. It is derived from the original theory of reasoned action (TRA) [33]. TRA was developed based on the premise that individuals make reasoned decisions to engage in specific behavior by evaluating the information available to them. It proposes that two factors, attitude and subjective norm, directly determine an individual’s behavioral intention. Attitude refers to the degree to which a person has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation or appraisal of the behavior in question, while subjective norm refers to the perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform the behavior [33]. The formed intention further decides whether the individual would actually do that behavior.
Although intention is a strong predictor of actual behavior, there are times when individuals do not execute an action, despite having the intention to act in a certain manner, because of external factors that fall outside their control. As a result, TRA is limited to only predicting behavior of people who have complete volitional control [34]. To extend the explanatory scope, TPB was proposed; this theory incorporates perceived behavioral control, defined as the perceived ease or difficulty of performing the behavior [32], into the model. TPB posits that attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control together shape one’s intention to engage in certain behavior, and this intention, together with perceived behavioral control, determine the probability that an actual action will be taken. As a general rule, the more favorable the attitude and subjective norm with respect to the behavior are, the greater the perceived behavioral control is, and the stronger an individual’s intention to perform the behavior should be [32]. Recent evidence suggested that perceived behavioral control and subjective norm could also impact behavior indirectly by influencing attitude [12,35,36].
As a type of intentional or planned behavior, violations at workplace should follow the framework in TPB. This has been confirmed by some empirical studies that reported the effectiveness of TPB in explaining violations in aviation maintenance [12], construction worksites [37], and road transportation [38,39,40]. For instance, Fogarty and Shaw [12] reported that a positive attitude towards violations and the perception that other people in work commit violations (i.e., subjective norm) significantly increased workers’ intention to violate and the actual occurrence of violations. Similarly, Wang et al. [40] found that all factors of TPB showed significant effects on violations in lane changing behavior.
The other type of aberrant behavior, errors, seem to fall outside of TPB’s explanatory scope given that they are unintentional or unplanned behavior. However, some factors of TPB have been reported as significant predictors of errors in safety literature. For instance, Victoir et al. [41] found that perceived behavioral control was a dominant determinant that explained 33% of variance in driving errors. Lucidi et al. [42] and Mallia et al. [43] found that drivers possessing more a positive safety attitude reported fewer errors, suggesting that attitude towards traffic safety rules was a significant predictor of driving errors. Paletz et al. [44] proposed taking subjective norm into consideration when analyzing errors. Based on the above studies, it was hypothesized that TPB should have some explanatory power in regard to errors.
Finally, although the motivating mechanism of safety participation has not been intensively investigated, there has been evidence suggesting that it falls under the explanatory power of TPB. Fugas et al. [39] applied TPB as the framework to predict safety participation behavior at a transportation organization. They found that subjective norm was the most significant predictor of safety participation behavior of transportation organization workers, followed by safety attitude. However, in their study, perceived behavioral control was not identified as a significant determinant. A more recent study [45] investigating safety participation behavior at construction sites showed that all three factors of TPB were significant predictors.
Taking all evidence together, this study applied TPB as the theoretical framework to predict errors, violations, and safety participation behavior of commissioning workers. It should be noted though, that when predicting a specific type of behavior, most TPB-related studies have measured the perception or attitude towards this specific behavior. For instance, attitudes towards violations were measured when using TPB to predict violations [12]. However, instead of measuring the specific perception towards each type of behavior, this study used a general perception towards safety regulations to predict the three types of behavior. That is, we have measured the attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control towards safety regulations.
Based on the above evidence, it was hypothesized that:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Attitude would have a direct negative effect on errors and violations and a direct positive effect on safety participation.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Subjective norm would have a direct negative effect on errors and violations and a direct positive effect on safety participation.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Perceived behavioral control would have a direct negative effect on errors and violations and a direct positive effect on safety participation.
With regard to the relations among the three predictors, TPB assumes that they are correlated. However, many empirical studies have demonstrated that subjective norm and perceived behavioral control are predictors of attitude [12,35]; therefore, it was further hypothesized that:
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Subjective norm would have a direct positive effect on attitude.
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
Perceived behavioral control would have a positive effect attitude.

1.3. Executive Function

TPB offers theoretical explanations in terms of why certain behavior would be planned and whether the planned behavior would be carried out or not. However, whether a behavior can be executed the way it is planned also depends on individuals’ executive ability. For instance, there are situations where individuals intend to comply with rules but end up failing to do so due to a low level of executive function [34,46,47,48]. Executive function includes many aspects relevant to successful task completion, such as organization and planning, inhibiting responses, thinking abstractly, and reallocating mental resources [47]. In the context of commissioning, the aspect of executive function most relevant to task completion is probably organization and planning. Organization and planning refer to the ability to think ahead and to carry out organized behavior through functions like multitasking, sequencing, and holding information in mind to make decisions [49]. As mentioned above, commissioning is characterized by collaborations within and across teams, in addition to a tight schedule, all of which require organization and planning ability to coordinate different parties and manage the projects [7]. Some empirical studies found that organization and planning ability could reduce errors associated with ineffective communication or planning and reduce regulation violations due to inappropriate organization of tasks [46]. Although little attention has been paid to the effect of organization and planning ability on safety participation, it was expected that better organization and planning ability could help workers improve work efficiency, therefore giving them more time and energy to engage in voluntary safety promotion programs. Based on the above evidence, it is therefore hypothesized that:
Hypothesis 6 (H6).
Organization and planning would have a positive effect on errors and violations and a negative effect on safety participation.

1.4. Proposed Model

The proposed model is shown in Figure 1. Utilizing the TPB framework, it is hypothesized that attitude, perceived behavioral control, and subjective norm would directly impact the occurrence of errors, violations, and safety participation behavior. Besides, subjective norm and perceived behavioral control could indirectly affect workers’ behavior through the attitude factor. Moreover, organization and planning ability is a direct determinant of commissioning workers’ behavior.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Questionnaire

Data used in this study were extracted from a large questionnaire survey investigating the relationship between personality, cognitive abilities, and performance of commissioning workers. The questionnaire was designed after an extensive review of the literature and revised based on the feedback from three commissioning experts to improve its clarity and readability. Three experts on human factors were also consulted to guarantee the quality of the questionnaire. Questions related to this study are introduced below and listed in detail in Table A1 in Appendix A.
Basic information inventory: This part was designed to collect demographic information including age, gender, educational level, marriage status, working experience, and working division (i.e., technique management, electrical commissioning, conventional island commissioning, instrumentation and control commissioning, nuclear island commissioning, or other).
Behavior: This part required respondents to rate their frequency of errors, violations, and active safety participation behavior at work on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “never” to 5 = “always”. Seven items from Rao et al. [50] were adopted to measure errors (e.g., “I promised to return to someone with information but forgot to do so.”), and three items from the same source as above were used to measure violations (e.g., “sometimes I do not use the correct safety procedures for carrying out my job”). Based on its definition and discussion with commissioning experts, three items were developed to measure safety participation (e.g., “I would alert my co-workers if potential safety concerns exist”).
TPB factors: This section was used to collect respondents’ perceptions and attitudes towards safety regulations at work. In particular, subjective norm was measured by adjusting the three items from Venkatesh and Davis [51] (e.g., “people who influence me a lot think I should follow rules and procedures at work”); perceived behavioral control was measured by adapting the five items from Taylor and Todd [52] (e.g., “I am able to follow rules and procedures at work”); and attitude was measured using three items from Iversen [53] (e.g., “many rules must be ignored to ensure work flow”). Items were also rated with a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “totally disagree” to 5 = “totally agree”.
Organization and planning: This factor was measured using the twelve items from the Executive Function Index (EFI) developed by Spinella [49]. Respondents were required to indicate the extent to which they agreed with descriptions of themselves in daily life (e.g., “organized person”). Similarly, a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “totally disagree” to 5 = “totally agree” was used by respondents to rate their answers.

2.2. Participants

Face-to-face invitation to participate in the survey was given by visiting the commissioning offices at Yangjiang and Taishan nuclear power plants in July of 2019. These two nuclear power plants are located in Guangdong Province in China, and both of them belong to the General Nuclear Power Corporation. Therefore, they are comparable in organizational structures, work regulations, and work environments. The ethics approval is included as parts of the official contract of the collaboration project between Shenzhen University and the State Key Laboratory of Nuclear Power Safety Monitoring Technology and Equipment of China. The ethic approval code is 007-EC-B-2019-C83-P.S.20-01122. Participants were informed that they could quit the survey at any time and all data collected would be handled anonymously and confidentially and only be used for research purposes. A total of 187 questionnaires were distributed, and 179 were returned within the required time period. Of the returned questionnaires, 12 were excluded from further analysis because of the high proportion of unanswered questions (>20%). Data from the remaining 167 questionnaires were used in this study.
Of the valid responses, 130 (77.8%) were from Yangjiang nuclear power plant, and 37 (22.2%) were from Taishan nuclear power plant. Almost all respondents (n = 165, 98.8%) were males. On average, they were 32.5 years old (SD = 4.8), and they had an average working experience of 7.7 years (SD = 2.4). The majority of the respondents were married (n = 123, 73.6%). With regard to educational level, the majority (n = 146, 88.0%) had a bachelor’s degree, a few had a master’s degree (n = 13, 7.8%), and the rest had a college degree or below (n = 7, 4.2%). Given that the distribution of gender, marriage status, and educational level variables were highly skewed, their possible confounding effects on the proposed model were not investigated. The workers were distributed in different divisions: technique management (n = 21, 12.5%), electrical commissioning (n = 30, 18.0%), conventional island commissioning (n = 39, 23.3%), instrumentation and control commissioning (n = 36, 21.6%), nuclear island commissioning (n = 34, 20.4%), and other (n = 7, 4.2%).

2.3. Data Analysis

A two-step procedure for conducting structural equation modelling (SEM), as proposed by Anderson and Gerbing [54], was adopted. In the first step, a series of tests were carried out to evaluate the reliability and validity of the measurement model. In particular, internal consistency was evaluated with Cronbach’s α. A value of Cronbach’s α greater than 0.6 indicates acceptable internal consistency [55]. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to examine whether multiple items of the same factor were in agreement, i.e., the convergent validity. Convergent validity is achieved when the model shows a good fit and the factor loading of an item on its posted underlying factor is significant and larger than 0.5 [56]. The model was considered to have a good fit when the following criteria were fulfilled: ratio of chi-square value to degree of freedom (χ2/df) < 3, comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ 0.90, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) ≥ 0.90, incremental fit index (IFI) ≥ 0.90, standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) < 0.08, and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.06 [57,58,59,60]. Discriminant validity reflects the extent to which the factors differ from one another empirically [56]. According to the Fornell and Larcker criterion, discriminant validity is achieved if the square root of average variance extracted (SAVE) for each of the factors is greater than any of the bivariate correlations involving the factor in the model [61].
In the second step, SEM was used to assess the goodness of fit of the proposed model and to investigate the strength of the relationships among factors in the model (i.e., hypothesis testing). The same goodness-of-fit criteria (i.e., χ2/df < 3, CFI ≥ 0.90, TLI ≥ 0.90, IFI ≥ 0.90, SRMR < 0.08, and RMSEA < 0.06) were applied to evaluate the fit of the proposed model. All analyses were performed in R software (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) (R Core Team. (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/).

3. Results

3.1. Reliability and Validity of the Measures

The CFA results showed that the factor loadings of five items from the organization and planning factors (marked in the Table A1) were smaller than the required minimum value of 0.5 and therefore were removed from further analysis. The CFA was then reconducted. The revised model showed a satisfactory goodness of fit (χ2/df = 1.6, CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.90, IFI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.072, and RMSEA = 0.059; see Table 1). All items in the revised model had factor loadings larger than 0.5, suggesting an acceptable convergent validity of the factors. All Cronbach’s α values were greater than 0.6 (Table 1), suggesting a good internal consistency of these factors. The SAVE (shown on the diagonal in Table 1) of each factor was greater than the associated interfactor correlations (shown off the diagonal in Table 2), indicating acceptable discriminant validity. In addition, all the interfactor correlations are smaller than 0.85, suggesting that multicollinearity is not a problem [62]. In summary, the measurement model showed a satisfactory reliability and validity and was appropriate for the analysis of the structure model. Furthermore, given that age and working experience showed no significant effects on variables in the proposed model, their effects were not controlled in the following SEM analysis.

3.2. Structure Model

The SEM results suggested that our model had a satisfactory goodness of fit, with χ2/df = 1.6, CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.90, IFI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.072, and RMSEA = 0.059. The estimated standardized path coefficients of the significant relationships in the model and the proportion of explained variance (R2) are shown in Figure 2. For clarity, models of the three kinds of behavior (i.e., errors, violations, and safety participation) are presented separately. For relationships within the three factors from TPB, attitude was significantly predicted by perceived behavioral control (β = 0.338, p < 0.001), with a higher level of perceived behavioral control over following safety rules leading to better attitude. The effect of subjective norm on attitude did not reach a significant level.
Of the three types of behavior, errors were directly determined by attitude (β = −0.203, p = 0.036), subjective norm (β = 0.227, p = 0.009), and organization and planning (β = −0.368, p = 0.012). As hypothesized, workers with better attitude and a higher organization and planning ability had fewer errors. Perceived behavioral control did not have a significant direct effect on errors (β = 0.042, p = 0.736). However, it showed a significant effect on attitude, which further significantly predicted errors. Therefore, perceived behavioral control could indirectly influence the occurrence of errors by affecting attitude. Unexpectedly, workers who perceived a higher norm that they should follow rules and procedures were more likely to make errors. A total of 20.5% of the variance in errors was explained by the proposed model.
With regard to violations, the path coefficients of attitude (β = −0.191, p = 0.036) and organization and planning (β = −0.503, p = 0.001) were significant. Again, perceived behavioral control did not show a direct effect on violations (β = −0.049, p = 0.694) and could only indirectly influence it through attitude. Subjective norm was not a significant predictor of violations either (β = −0.005, p = 0.953). A total of 38.0% of the variance in violations was explained.
For safety participation, the most significant predictor was perceived behavioral control (β = 0.407, p = 0.002), followed by organization and planning (β = 0.352, p = 0.012). These two factors explained as high as 49.1% of the variance in safety participation. Attitude (β = 0.055, p = 0.549) and subjective norm (β = −0.093, p = 0.252) did not show any significant effects in shaping workers’ safety participation behavior. Table 3 summarizes the results of the hypothesis tests.

4. Discussion

Commissioning workers have long been ignored in previous studies although their performance is closely related to the overall safety of nuclear power plants. This study aimed to investigate the determinants of three types of behavior (i.e., errors, violations, and safety participation) of commissioning workers, under the general framework of TPB and by considering organization and planning factors. Our work demonstrated the usefulness of TPB in explaining commissioning workers’ behavior and revealed the working mechanisms under the three types of investigated behavior.
All three types of behavior were significantly predicted by workers’ organization and planning ability. First, commissioning workers with a better organization and planning ability reported fewer errors. This is probably because those good at multitasking, sequencing, and planning are better prepared to deal with the pressure of time and the challenge of technological complexity during commissioning work, which have been reported in literature as major contributors to errors [12,63]. Another possible explanation is that those with a better organization and planning ability are less likely to engage in distracting activities [64] and therefore may commit fewer errors associated with distraction. Second, consistent with findings in the context of driving [46,48], our results show that violations were negatively associated with organization and planning. According to Lund and Rundmo [65], some violations are a result of underestimation of possible harmful consequences associated with such behavior. Organized persons, who carry out careful and comprehensive evaluation before taking action, are more likely to realize the negative consequences of violations and therefore less likely to violate rules. Finally, active safety participation was positively associated with organization and planning ability, probably because those with clear plans for the future would devote greater efforts to activities that are beyond their formal role but can contribute to realizing their future goals, like safety participation activities. Together, our results demonstrate that organization and planning affect all kinds of behavior and are critical to the overall safety of commissioning work.
A favorable attitude towards rules and procedures resulted in fewer errors and violations but did not promote voluntary safety participation behavior. This result agrees with previous evidence that violations are deliberate and planned behavior and such behavior is a direct result of attitude [12,37,40]. Errors, which are supposed to be unintended or unplanned behavior, were also partially determined by attitude. This is probably because those showing a positive attitude towards safety rules and procedures pay more attention to work, which helps reduce the probabilities of temporary failures of concentration, memory, or judgement that may cause errors. Surprisingly, contrary to previous results [39,66], better attitude did not promote safety participation, but better perceived behavioral control and organization and planning did. These results in combination suggest that active involvement in safety promotion depends on organizational and individual support, not on individual preference.
The three types of behavior showed different relationships with subjective norm. On one hand, behavior with clear intention, i.e., violation and safety participation, was not predicted by subjective norm in this study. This finding is inconsistent with previous studies which have found that behavior at workplaces like those of transportation [39,40] and construction [67,68] was related to subjective norm. This unexpected finding is probably a result of the specialization of commissioning work. Commissioning work requires specified knowledge and professional training, and workers might believe that others, especially those not working in commissioning, know very little about their work. Therefore, commissioning workers may not take his family members’ or friends’ opinions into consideration when planning intended behavior, i.e., violations and safety participation. On the other hand, subjective norm positively impacted the occurrence of errors. A possible explanation is that while perceived norm did not impact intended decisions, it is a kind of pressure that might lead to unintentional lapses and slips [69].
Finally, perceived behavioral control was a critical factor of commissioning safety, as it affected all three types of behavior. Consistent with much evidence in literature [70], those who perceived themselves as more capable of following rules and procedures at work had a more positive attitude towards rules and further reported fewer errors and violations. More importantly, our results showed that commissioning workers who had higher perceived behavioral control were more likely to actively participate in safety promotion activities. This suggests that, besides the widely recognized factors such as supervisor leadership [71,72] and safety climate [73,74], perceived behavioral control should also be considered when developing interventions to promote workers’ safety participation.

5. Conclusions

This study is one of the first studies to investigate the behavior of commissioning workers at nuclear power plants. The main findings include that (1) all three types of investigated behavior (i.e., errors, violations, and safety participation) are significantly predicted by perceived behavioral control and organization and planning; (2) violations and errors are a direct result of attitude towards safety rules and procedures, while safety participation is not related to such attitude; and (3) errors are further positively predicted by subjective norm. These findings revealed the underlying mechanisms for the development of errors, violations, and safety participation among commissioning workers and provided practical implications for safety improvement at the commissioning workplace.
Findings of this study have several practical implications for safety improvement at the commissioning workplace. Given the important and positive role of organization and planning in shaping commissioning workers’ behavior, it is recommended that organization and planning ability could be used as a reference when hiring commissioning workers or be considered as content for training in worker improvement programs. In addition, ways to improve workers’ feelings of being in control of their behavior, such as making safety equipment easier to use or offering enough personnel trainings, are of great importance for reducing violations and errors and promoting safety participation. Another remediation of errors is to reduce the level of perceived subjective norm, given that this factor was identified as a significant positive predictor of errors. Practically, this can be achieved by avoiding the emphasis of work regulations by friends and family members.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.Z., D.T., X.Q., and Z.L.; Formal analysis, T.Z. and S.Z.; Funding acquisition, X.Q. and Z.L.; Investigation, T.Z., S.Z., D.T., and X.Q.; Methodology, T.Z. and D.T.; Project administration, T.Z. and D.T.; Supervision, X.Q. and D.T; Validation, D.T. and T.Z.; Writing—original draft, T.Z. and S.Z.; Writing—review & editing, T.Z., D.T., and X.Q. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 71801156), the Startup Foundation of SZU (Grant No. 000002110284) and the Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province, China (Grant No. 2019A1515010863).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. List of items used in this study.
Table A1. List of items used in this study.
FactorsItemsReferences
Subjective normPeople who influence me a lot think I should follow rules and procedures at work.[51]
People who are important to me think I should follow rules and procedures at work.
My colleagues think I should follow rules and procedures at work.
Perceived behavioral controlI am able to follow rules and procedures at work.
Following rules and procedures at work is under my control.
I am capable of following rules and procedures at work.
I have resources to follow rules and procedures at work.
I have the knowledge to follow rules and procedures at work.
AttitudeMany rules must be ignored to ensure work flow. (*) [53]
Some procedures can be ignored because they are too restrictive. (*)
Work rules are often too complicated to be carried out in practice. (*)
Organization and planningOrganized person [49]
Save money regularly (#)
Self-monitor for mistakes
Plan for the future (#)
Use of memory strategies (#)
Anticipate consequences of actions (*)
Learn from mistakes
Trouble summing information for decisions (#)(*)
Distractibility (*)
Lost track of what I am doing (*)
Mix up the sequences of actions (*)
Trouble doing two things at once (#)(*)
ErrorPromised to return to someone with information but forgot to do so[50]
Forgot to perform an operation in a sequence of operations that I planned to carry through
Was forced to interrupt a surveillance and control task because I was disturbed by someone and forgot to return to what I was busy doing
I misinterpreted a situation and therefore made an error.
I was not attentive enough and therefore I missed important information.
I could not remember essential information while performing an operation and therefore I had to ask for information again.
ViolationSometimes I do not use all the necessary safety equipment to do my job.[50]
Sometimes I do not use the correct safety procedures for carrying out my job.
Sometimes I do not ensure the highest levels of safety when I carry out my job.
Safety participation behaviorI would alert my co-workers if potential safety concerns exist.Self-developed
I would alert my co-workers if they do not follow rules and procedures at work.
I would propose suggestions to improve work safety to my supervisor.
*: Items reverse coded; #: Items excluded from the final model.

References

  1. Nicoletti, G.; Arcuri, N.; Nicoletti, G.; Bruno, R. A technical and environmental comparison between hydrogen and some fossil fuels. Energy Convers. Manag. 2015, 89, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Mukhopadhyay, K.; Forssell, O. An empirical investigation of air pollution from fossil fuel combustion and its impact on health in India during 1973–1974 to 1996–1997. Ecol. Econ. 2005, 55, 235–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Petrescu, F.I.; Apicella, A.; Petrescu, R.V.; Kozaitis, S.; Bucinell, R.; Aversa, R.; Abu-Lebdeh, T. Environmental protection through nuclear energy. Am. J. Appl. Sci. 2016, 13, 941–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. World Nuclear Association. Plans for New Reactors Worldwide. 2019. Available online: https://www.world-nuclear.org/information-library/current-and-future-generation/plans-for-new-reactors-worldwide.aspx (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  5. Fairley, P. China’s Nuclear Hiatus May be Coming to an End. 2019. Available online: https://www.technologyreview.com/s/612872/chinas-nuclear-hiatus-may-be-coming-to-an-end (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  6. International Atomic Energy Agency. IAEA Safety Glossary: Terminology Used in Nuclear Safety and Radiation Protection; Internat, Atomic Energy Agency: Vienna, Austria, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  7. Gotcheva, N.; Oedewald, P. SafePhase: Safety Culture Challenges in Design, Construction, Installation and Commissioning Phases of Large Nuclear Power Projects; Swedish Radiation Safety Authority: Stockholm, Sweden, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  8. Zhang, T.; Chan, A.H.S.; Xue, H.; Zhang, X.; Tao, D. Driving anger, aberrant driving behaviors, and road crash risk: Testing of a mediated model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Patterson, J.M.; Shappell, S.A. Operator error and system deficiencies: Analysis of 508 mining incidents and accidents from Queensland, Australia using HFACS. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010, 42, 1379–1385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lyu, S.; Hon, C.K.H.; Chan, A.P.C.; Wong, F.K.W.; Javed, A.A. Relationships among safety climate, safety behavior, and safety outcomes for ethnic minority construction workers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Martínez-Córcoles, M.; Schöbel, M.; Gracia, F.J.; Tomás, I.; Peiró, J.M. Linking empowering leadership to safety participation in nuclear power plants: A structural equation model. J. Saf. Res. 2012, 43, 215–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fogarty, G.J.; Shaw, A. Safety climate and the theory of planned behavior: Towards the prediction of unsafe behavior. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010, 42, 1455–1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Choudhry, R.M.; Fang, D. Why operatives engage in unsafe work behavior: Investigating factors on construction sites. Saf. Sci. 2008, 46, 566–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mathisen, G.E.; Bergh, L.I.V. Action errors and rule violations at offshore oil rigs: The role of engagement, emotional exhaustion and health complaints. Saf. Sci. 2016, 85, 130–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Morrow, S.L.; Koves, G.K.; Barnes, V.E. Exploring the relationship between safety culture and safety performance in US nuclear power operations. Saf. Sci. 2014, 69, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Martínez-Córcoles, M.; Gracia, F.; Tomás, I.; Peiró, M.J. Leadership and employees’ perceived safety behaviours in a nuclear power plant: A structural equation model. Saf. Sci. 2011, 49, 1118–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Grauf, E. Commissioning of Nuclear Power Plants (NPPs). In Infrastructure and Methodologies for the Justification of Nuclear Power Programmes; Woodhead Publishing Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2012; pp. 741–772. [Google Scholar]
  18. Didla, S.; Mearns, K.; Flin, R. Safety citizenship behaviour: A proactive approach to risk management. J. Risk Res. 2009, 12, 475–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Curcuruto, M.; Conchie, S.M.; Mariani, M.G.; Violante, F.S. The role of prosocial and proactive safety behaviors in predicting safety performance. Saf. Sci. 2015, 80, 317–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Reason, J.; Manstead, A.; Stardling, S.; Baxter, J.; Campbell, K. Errors and violations on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics 1990, 33, 1315–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Lawton, R.; Parker, D. Individual differences in accident liability: A review and integrative approach. Hum. Factors 1998, 40, 655–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Zhang, T.; Chan, A.H.; Zhang, W. Dimensions of driving anger and their relationships with aberrant driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2015, 81, 124–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Parker, D.; Reason, J.T.; Manstead, A.S.R.; Stradling, S.G. Driving errors, driving violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics 1995, 38, 1036–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Ancel, E.; Shih, A.T.; Jones, S.M.; Reveley, M.S.; Luxhøj, J.T.; Evans, J.K. Predictive safety analytics: Inferring aviation accident shaping factors and causation. J. Risk Res. 2015, 18, 428–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Borman, W.C.; Motowidlo, S. Expanding the Criterion Domain to Include Elements of Contextual Performance. In Personnel Selection in Organizations; Schmitt, N., Borman, W.C., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1993; pp. 71–98. [Google Scholar]
  26. Griffin, M.A.; Neal, A. Perceptions of safety at work: A framework for linking safety climate to safety performance, knowledge, and motivation. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2000, 5, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Clarke, S. The relationship between safety climate and safety performance: A meta-analytic review. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2006, 11, 315–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Neal, A.; Griffin, M.A. A study of the lagged relationships among safety climate, safety motivation, safety behavior, and accidents at the individual and group levels. J. Appl. Psychol. 2006, 91, 946–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Tao, D.; Wang, T.; Wang, T.; Zhang, T.; Zhang, X.; Qu, X. A systematic review and meta-analysis of user acceptance of consumer-oriented health information technologies. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 104, 106147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Maddux, J.E.; Sherer, M.; Rogers, R.W. Self-efficacy expectancy and outcome expectancy: Their relationship and their effects on behavioral intentions. Cognit. Ther. Res. 1982, 6, 207–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Motivation, Personality, and Development Within Embedded Social Contexts: An Overview of Self-Determination Theory. In the Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; pp. 85–107. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hall, P.A.; Fong, G.T. Temporal self-regulation theory: A model for individual health behavior. Health Psychol. Rev. 2007, 1, 6–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kim, E.; Ham, S.; Yang, I.S.; Choi, J.G. The roles of attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control in the formation of consumers’ behavioral intentions to read menu labels in the restaurant industry. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2013, 35, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Chan, D.C.; Wu, A.M.; Hung, E.P. Invulnerability and the intention to drink and drive: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010, 42, 1549–1555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Goh, Y.M.; Binte Sa’adon, N.F. Cognitive factors influencing safety behavior at height: A multimethod exploratory study. J. Constr. Eng. Manag. 2015, 141, 04015003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Forward, S.E. The theory of planned behaviour: The role of descriptive norms and past behaviour in the prediction of drivers’ intentions to violate. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2009, 12, 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fugas, C.S.; Silva, S.A.; Melia, J.L. Another look at safety climate and safety behavior: Deepening the cognitive and social mediator mechanisms. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012, 45, 468–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Wang, X.; Xu, L.; Hao, Y. What factors predict drivers’ self-reported lane change violation behavior at urban intersections? A study in China. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0216751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Victoir, A.; Eertmans, A.; Van den Bergh, O.; Van den Broucke, S. Learning to drive safely: Social-cognitive responses are predictive of performance rated by novice drivers and their instructors. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2005, 8, 59–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lucidi, F.; Giannini, A.M.; Sgalla, R.; Mallia, L.; Devoto, A.; Reichmann, S. Young novice driver subtypes: Relationship to driving violations, errors and lapses. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010, 42, 1689–1696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mallia, L.; Lazuras, L.; Violani, C.; Lucidi, F. Crash risk and aberrant driving behaviors among bus drivers: The role of personality and attitudes towards traffic safety. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2015, 79, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Paletz, S.B.F.; Bearman, C.; Orasanu, J.; Holbrook, J. Socializing the human factors analysis and classification system: Incorporating social psychological phenomena into a human factors error classification system. Hum. Factors 2009, 51, 435–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Liu, Q.; Xu, N.; Jiang, H.; Wang, S.; Wang, W.; Wang, J. Psychological Driving Mechanism of Safety Citizenship Behaviors of Construction Workers: Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior and Norm Activation Model. J. Constr. Eng. Manag. 2020, 146, 04020027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tabibi, Z.; Borzabadi, H.H.; Stavrinos, D.; Mashhadi, A. Predicting aberrant driving behaviour: The role of executive function. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2015, 34, 18–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Starkey, N.J.; Isler, R.B. The role of executive function, personality and attitudes to risks in explaining self-reported driving behaviour in adolescent and adult male drivers. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016, 38, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hayashi, Y.; Foreman, A.M.; Friedel, J.E.; Wirth, O. Executive function and dangerous driving behaviors in young drivers. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2018, 52, 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Spinella, M. Self-rated executive function: Development of the executive function index. Int. J. Neurosci. 2005, 115, 649–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Rao, L.-L.; Xu, Y.; Li, S.; Li, Y.; Zheng, R. Effect of perceived risk on nuclear power plant operators’ safety behavior and errors. J. Risk Res. 2017, 20, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Venkatesh, V.; Davis, F.D. A Theoretical Extension of the Technology Acceptance Model: Four Longitudinal Field Studies. Manag. Sci. 2000, 46, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Taylor, S.; Todd, P.A. Understanding Information Technology Usage: A Test of Competing Models. Inf. Syst. Res. 1995, 6, 144–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Iversen, H. Risk-taking attitudes and risky driving behaviour. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2004, 7, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Anderson, J.C.; Gerbing, D.W. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 103, 411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Taber, K.S. The Use of Cronbach’s Alpha When Developing and Reporting Research Instruments in Science Education. Res. Sci. Educ. 2017, 48, 1273–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hamid, M.R.A.; Sami, W.; Mohmad Sidek, M.H. Discriminant Validity Assessment: Use of Fornell & Larcker Criterion Versus HTMT Criterion. In Journal of Physics Conference Series; IOP Publishing: Bristol, UK, 2017; Volume 890. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hu, L.t.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Anaysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  59. Zhang, T.; Tao, D.; Qu, X.; Zhang, X.; Lin, R.; Zhang, W. The roles of initial trust and perceived risk in public’s acceptance of automated vehicles. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2019, 98, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Tao, D.; Zhang, R.; Qu, X. The role of personality traits and driving experience in self-reported risky driving behaviors and accident risk among Chinese drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 99, 228–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kenny, D. Measuring Model Fit. 2011. Available online: http://davidakenny.net/cm/fit.htm (accessed on 14 June 2020).
  63. Rasmussen, M.; Standal, M.I.; Laumann, K. Task complexity as a performance shaping factor: A review and recommendations in Standardized Plant Analysis Risk-Human Reliability Analysis (SPAR-H) adaption. Saf. Sci. 2015, 76, 228–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pope, C.N.; Bell, T.R.; Stavrinos, D. Mechanisms behind distracted driving behavior: The role of age and executive function in the engagement of distracted driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 98, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Lund, I.O.; Rundmo, T. Cross-cultural comparisons of traffic safety, risk perception, attitudes and behaviour. Saf. Sci. 2009, 47, 547–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Clarke, S. An integrative model of safety climate: Linking psychological climate and work attitudes to individual safety outcomes using meta-analysis. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2010, 83, 553–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Jitwasinkul, B.; Hadikusumo, B.H.W.; Memon, A.Q. A Bayesian Belief Network model of organizational factors for improving safe work behaviors in Thai construction industry. Saf. Sci. 2016, 82, 264–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Zhang, M.; Fang, D. A cognitive analysis of why Chinese scaffolders do not use safety harnesses in construction. Constr. Manag. Econ. 2013, 31, 207–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Read, G.J.; Lenne, M.G.; Moss, S.A. Associations between task, training and social environmental factors and error types involved in rail incidents and accidents. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012, 48, 416–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Topa, G.; Moriano, J.A. Theory of planned behavior and smoking: Meta-analysis and SEM model. Subst. Abuse Rehabil. 2010, 1, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Clarke, S. Safety leadership: A meta-analytic review of transformational and transactional leadership styles as antecedents of safety behaviours. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2013, 86, 22–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Griffin, M.A.; Hu, X. How leaders differentially motivate safety compliance and safety participation: The role of monitoring, inspiring, and learning. Saf. Sci. 2013, 60, 196–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Brondino, M.; Silva, S.A.; Pasini, M. Multilevel approach to organizational and group safety climate and safety performance: Co-workers as the missing link. Saf. Sci. 2012, 50, 1847–1856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lu, C.-S.; Yang, C.-S. Safety climate and safety behavior in the passenger ferry context. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2011, 43, 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Proposed model for explaining errors, violations, and safety participation behavior of commissioning workers.
Figure 1. Proposed model for explaining errors, violations, and safety participation behavior of commissioning workers.
Ijerph 17 05613 g001
Figure 2. The final model and the significant standardized path coefficients, (a) result of SEM analysis for errors, (b) result of SEM analysis for violations, (c) result of SEM analysis for safety participation. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; dotted line represents a nonsignificant path parameter.
Figure 2. The final model and the significant standardized path coefficients, (a) result of SEM analysis for errors, (b) result of SEM analysis for violations, (c) result of SEM analysis for safety participation. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; dotted line represents a nonsignificant path parameter.
Ijerph 17 05613 g002
Table 1. Fit indices for the tested models.
Table 1. Fit indices for the tested models.
Fit IndicesRecommended ValuesCFASEM
χ2/df<31.61.6
CFI≥0.900.910.91
TLI≥0.900.900.90
IFI≥0.900.910.91
SRMR<0.080.720.072
RMSEA<0.060.0590.059
Table 2. Mean, standard deviation (SD), and zero-order correlation between factors.
Table 2. Mean, standard deviation (SD), and zero-order correlation between factors.
Variables123456789
1. Age
2. Working Experience0.58 ***
3. Organization and Planning0.080.12(0.71)
4. Subjective Norm−0.24 **−0.110.04(0.92)
5. Perceived Behavioral Control0.090.200.42 ***0.06(0.80)
6. Attitude0.120.120.36 ***0.140.62 ***(0.61)
7. Errors−0.080.03−0.41 ***0.13−0.25 ***−0.31 ***(0.68)
8. Violations−0.10−0.13−0.38 ***−0.10−0.37 **−0.42 ***0.26 **(0.71)
9. Safety Participation0.070.130.36 ***−0.010.45 ***0.58 ***−0.31 ***−0.42 ***(0.61)
Mean32.507.733.643.494.124.032.221.953.76
SD4.752.360.390.890.450.470.490.600.54
Cronbach’s α0.780.940.900.800.830.760.67
Note: The numbers in parentheses are the SAVEs. The off-diagonal elements are the correlations between the factors. ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Summary of the hypothesis test results.
Table 3. Summary of the hypothesis test results.
HypothesesPathsSupported?
H1Attitude → BehaviorSupported in Errors and Violations;
Rejected in Safety Participation;
H2Subjective norm → BehaviorSupported in Errors;
Rejected in Violations and Safety Participation;
H3Perceived Behavioral Control → AttitudeSupported in Safety Participation;
Rejected in Errors and Violations;
H4Subjective Norm → BehaviorRejected
H5Perceived Behavioral Control → AttitudeSupported
H6Organization and Planning → BehaviorSupported

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhang, T.; Liu, Z.; Zheng, S.; Qu, X.; Tao, D. Predicting Errors, Violations, and Safety Participation Behavior at Nuclear Power Plants. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5613. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155613

AMA Style

Zhang T, Liu Z, Zheng S, Qu X, Tao D. Predicting Errors, Violations, and Safety Participation Behavior at Nuclear Power Plants. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(15):5613. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155613

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhang, Tingru, Zhaopeng Liu, Shiwen Zheng, Xingda Qu, and Da Tao. 2020. "Predicting Errors, Violations, and Safety Participation Behavior at Nuclear Power Plants" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 15: 5613. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155613

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop