Next Article in Journal
Real-Time Online Goal Recognition in Continuous Domains via Deep Reinforcement Learning
Previous Article in Journal
Estimating Mutual Information for Spike Trains: A Bird Song Example
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dynamical Analyses Show That Professional Archers Exhibit Tighter, Finer and More Fluid Dynamical Control Than Neophytes

by
Hesam Azadjou
1,
Michalina Błażkiewicz
2,
Andrew Erwin
1,3 and
Francisco J. Valero-Cuevas
1,3,*
1
Alfred E. Mann Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA
2
AWF · Department of Physiotherapy, Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw, 00-968 Warsaw, Poland
3
Division of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Entropy 2023, 25(10), 1414; https://doi.org/10.3390/e25101414
Submission received: 24 August 2023 / Revised: 23 September 2023 / Accepted: 29 September 2023 / Published: 4 October 2023

Abstract

:
Quantifying the dynamical features of discrete tasks is essential to understanding athletic performance for many sports that are not repetitive or cyclical. We compared three dynamical features of the (i) bow hand, (ii) drawing hand, and (iii) center of mass during a single bow-draw movement between professional and neophyte archers: dispersion (convex hull volume of their phase portraits), persistence (tendency to continue a trend as per Hurst exponents), and regularity (sample entropy). Although differences in the two groups are expected due to their differences in skill, our results demonstrate we can quantify these differences. The center of mass of professional athletes exhibits tighter movements compared to neophyte archers (6.3 < 11.2 convex hull volume), which are nevertheless less persistent (0.82 < 0.86 Hurst exponent) and less regular (0.035 > 0.025 sample entropy). In particular, the movements of the bow hand and center of mass differed more between groups in Hurst exponent analysis, and the drawing hand and center of mass were more different in sample entropy analysis. This suggests tighter neuromuscular control over the more fluid dynamics of the movement that exhibits more active corrections that are more individualized. Our work, therefore, provides proof of principle of how well-established dynamical analysis techniques can be used to quantify the nature and features of neuromuscular expertise for discrete movements in elite athletes.

1. Introduction

Dynamical systems theory has provided multiple tools and techniques to investigate athletic performance. By dynamics, we mean the time-varying interactions between the person and environment while executing a task to different levels of performance [1]. Three useful methods to quantify dynamical performance are its ‘phase portrait’ (a geometric description of how system variables interact with each other over time), the ‘persistence’ of the dynamics (the tendency to continue a current kinematic trend), and the ‘irregularity’ of the dynamics (how fluid the dynamical control is).
Although dynamical techniques are common in applied mathematics and in biological time series [2] and biomechanics [3,4], they have had more limited use in sports as they are applied mostly to cyclical behavior like walking and running [5,6,7,8]. Here, we extend the application of nonlinear dynamics analysis to the domain of discrete movements, focusing on the intricate bow-draw movement.
Archery is an exemplary subject for our investigation due to its unique blend of physicality, mental focus, and historical significance as the bow and arrow could be among the earliest examples of complex projectile weaponry [9,10] and shooting arrows demand a greater level of advanced executive functions within the brain when compared to tasks involving spear-throwing [10]. The archer’s quest for mastery transcends the boundaries of mere physical performance; it delves deep into the realms of cognition and motor control [11,12], and it demands athletes to gain focus and consistency in their movement [13].
In addition, prior studies of archery focus on electromyographic signals [14,15,16,17], posture and stability [18,19], reaction times [20], or effects of eye dominance [21]. However, they do not take a truly dynamical approach.
Here, we specifically study the dynamical features of bow-draw motions to quantify how professional archers attain improved performance. By comparing the dynamics of performance between professional and neophyte archers in the context of the bow-draw movement, we aim to study the differences in motor control strategies and provide a proof of principle for the application of nonlinear dynamics analysis in archery sports. Through this interdisciplinary approach, we gain insights into archery mechanics and a broader understanding of human motor control, focus, and cognitive processes during discrete movements in sports.
We find professional archers exhibit movements with less dispersion in their motion dynamics, likely due to more active corrections and more fluid dynamical control compared to neophytes.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The research involved 14 professional archers and 14 neophytes (seven men and women in each) (Table 1). Professional archers played for Poland’s national team, while the neophytes were senior physical education students who had never shot a sports bow or any other type of bow. The participants reported having no existing upper limb injuries or balance disorders and had not undergone any major upper limb surgery. The study was conducted according to the ethical guidelines and principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the University Research Ethics Committee (SEK 01-09/2020). In accordance with the emphasis on safety by the IRB, neophytes were particularly fit individuals for whom the recommended neophyte draw weight of 30 lbs. was not difficult. Also, the spirit of the project was to allow neophytes to, primarily, safely perform the task. Therefore, we asked them to perform the bow-draw motion as they felt most comfortable. Thus, for example, we did not control for eye dominance or ask them to perform the task with full stabilization or viewfinder hardware, which would have proven to be a distraction. Similarly, to further emphasize safety, they did not release the bow string to prevent the string from stinging their bow forearm.

2.2. Experimental Procedure

This research was conducted by utilizing a 9-camera system (Vicon Motion Systems Ltd, Yarnton, UK) operating at 100 Hz. A total of 34 markers were placed on the subjects according to the full body Plug-In-Gait scheme (Figure 1). From these markers, we extracted the 3D location of the Center of Mass (CoM), the bow hand (left arm), and the drawing hand (right arm). The CoM was calculated as the centroid of all body parts, including both hands. Each hand position was measured via markers placed on the dorsum, just below the head of the second metacarpal. After a warm-up, professionals performed two successful shots, and neophytes performed one shot (two shots if the first one was unsuccessful) at the target located 5 m from the platform. The shot cycle (Figure 1) consisted of the following phases: A. Set-up, B. Draw, C. Aim, D. Release, and follow-through [22]. The set-up phase (stage A) finishes when the bow hand is raised to the highest point before the drawing hand moves backward. The draw phase (stage B) finishes when the string touches the face of the archer. The Aim phase (stage C) finishes when the string moves forward from the fingers (the release, stage D). The follow-through time stops when the archer first moves either arm downwards from their end position. The starting point of recording is when the drawing hand and the LASI (Left Anterior Superior Iliac Spine) are at the same height (Figure 1).
The professional archers performed well-aimed shots at the center of the target (A, B, C, and D of the shot cycle, Figure 1), while the neophytes drew the string without taking the arrow down from the bow (for safety reasons, they only performed phases A, B, and C of the shot cycle before reversing the movement back to A, Figure 1). The professional archers had their bow with a draw weight set between 36 and 40 lb. For neophytes, the draw weight was set to 30 lb as our consultant coaches recommended using bows with a draw weight of no more than 30 pounds for neophytes. Before the recorded trials, the neophytes did not learn the technique themselves, but instead watched a professional archer perform the bow draw. Our analysis for each participant was carried out on the first attempt performed without marker occlusion.

2.3. Data Preprocessing

2.3.1. Kinematics Resampling and Normalization

Using phase space reconstruction and Hurst exponent analysis requires the signal to have a large enough number of points relative to their sampling frequency. Given our sampling frequency of 100 Hz, the lengths of the bow-draw movements lasting at most 4.2 s (for professionals) and 9.7 s (for neophytes) did not provide the same number of points per movement. Therefore, we preprocessed and concatenated the signals as follows: (I) The signals were resampled to obtain 1000 samples for the representative movement by each participant. This avoided undersampling (removing information from) the longest (i.e., slowest) movement, which had 970 samples. (II) Then, the resampled signals were normalized by the maximum magnitude value and demeaned. (III) To make the movements continuous, every other time series was reversed; therefore, the time series could be seen as a cyclical movement for the phase space reconstruction and Hurst exponent analysis. (IV) Finally, a low-pass filter was applied to prevent impulsive jumps between concatenated time series ( f c = 0.66 normalized frequency).

2.3.2. Creation of a 1D Time Series for the CoM, Drawing Hand, and Bow Hand

Phase space reconstruction and Hurst exponent analysis both require a 1D time series. Thus, we applied principal component analysis (PCA) to each body part’s 3D time series to reduce the dimensionality (See Figure 2). PCA is a widely employed technique in the fields of biomechanics [23,24] and sports [25,26] to project data into lower dimensions efficiently. The variance explained by the first PC of all body parts is shown in Table 2. In Appendix A, we further analyze the first PC for each body part to determine if they exhibited nonstationary, nonlinear, or chaotic behavior.

2.4. Phase Space Reconstruction

2.4.1. Time Delay and Embedding Dimension

The modeling and visualizing of time-series dynamics require proper state space reconstruction from available data to successfully estimate the invariant properties of the embedded attractor. Therefore, an appropriate time delay ( τ ) and embedding dimension (D) should be selected for the phase space reconstruction [27]. In the case of this paper, time-delayed embedding has been used to embed a signal into higher-dimensional space. To estimate the optimal time delay value, the Average Mutual Information (AMI) method was used for the first PC of each body part to implement the uniform multivariate method [28]. The criterion used for choosing the time delay was to find the first local minima of the AMI function for all body parts in both groups, and the maximum time lag ( τ m a x ) was set to be 1000 samples (the length of one participant’s time series). The dimension was found using the False Nearest Neighbor (FNN) method, which computes the percentage of false nearest neighbors for multidimensional input time series as a function of the embedding dimension [29]. All three first PCs showed a dimension of three.

2.4.2. Convex Hull of Phase Portrait

For the prepared data, by applying the optimal time delay ( τ ) and the embedding dimension ( D = 3 ), a convex hull was calculated for each PC in both groups (Figure 3). The convex hull’s volume was used to compare the phase space domains. Phase space volumes were compared within the first PC of the three body parts and between groups (at the level of the same body part).

2.5. Hurst Exponent Analysis

The Hurst exponent (H) quantifies the time series’ long-term memory by examining the auto-correlations within the series and how these correlations diminish as the time lag between data points increases [30,31]. H represents the relative tendency of a time series to have a mean-reverting or trending pattern. The Hurst exponent can be between 0 and 1, categorizing the series into three types: (1) H > 0.5 , which shows a persistent series. The closer the H value to 1, the stronger the persistence. (2) H = 0.5 , which shows a Brownian motion (i.e., random motion). (3) H < 0.5 , which shows an anti-persistent series. The closer the H value is to 0, the stronger the anti-persistence is. Taken together, H > 0.5 or H < 0.5 shows that a higher value comes after a high value, and vice versa [32]. To calculate the Hurst exponent, we used the Rescaled Range (R/S) Analysis [30]. We define X ( t ) as the first P C with the length of N, and X ( i ) is the ith observation in the P C . R ( n ) is the range of the series over a segment of length n, defined as (Equation (1))
R ( n ) = max i = 1 , , N n + 1 X ( i + n 1 ) min i = 1 , , N n + 1 X ( i )
S ( n ) is the standard deviation of the P C over the same segment, defined as: Equation (2):
S ( n ) = 1 n 1 i = 1 i = n ( X ( i ) μ n ) 2
where μ n is the mean of the series over the segment of length n. The R / S statistic is defined as Equation (3):
R / S ( n ) = R ( n ) S ( n )
The Hurst exponent H is then estimated using the log ( R / S ) versus log ( n ) plot as follows: Equation (4):
log ( R / S ( n ) ) = C + H log ( n ) .
where C is a constant and H is the Hurst exponent. All calculations were performed using MatLab software, version MATLAB R2023a.

2.6. Sample Entropy

Sample entropy ( S a m p E n ) is a modified version of Approximate Entropy ( A p p E n ) that measures complexity or irregularity in time-series data as A p p E n does, but it does not consider self-similarity. For a given embedding dimension (m), tolerance (r), and data length ( l e n g t h ( x ) = N ), we define a temple vector of length m to be X m ( i ) = x i , x i + 1 , , x i + m 1 and a Euclidian distance function d i j = k = 1 m X m ( i ) X m ( j ) 2 , then the S a m p E n is as in Equation (5)
S a m p E n = log C ( m , r ) C ( m + 1 , r )
In which C is a count function as in Equation (6)
C ( i , r ) = j = 1 , j i N ( m 1 ) Θ ( r d i j ) ,
and Θ is the Heaviside step function.
Since C ( m , r ) is always smaller than or equal to C ( m + 1 , r ) , the S a m p E n will always be non-negative. A smaller value of S a m p E n indicates more regularity (less complexity) in the data.

3. Results

The mean trajectories of all body parts were plotted, and the area of ±standard deviations was shaded (Figure 4). Trajectories generally reveal a difference between the two groups. To investigate dynamical differences between professionals and neophytes, phase portraits were plotted for the first PC of the concatenated time series (Figure 5).
All body parts show less dispersed dynamic states (i.e., tighter control) in professionals compared to neophytes. We used the volumes of the convex hulls to measure the dispersion for dynamic states of participants’ control during the bow-draw motion (Table 3). Based on the volumes, all three body parts have smaller dispersion in professionals, and the CoM has the greatest difference.
Hurst exponent values indicate a more persistent drawing behavior in the bow hand and CoM in professionals than neophytes (see Figure 6). A time series with 0.5 < H < 1 is considered persistent, and the closer to one the H value is, the more persistent the time series is. All of the series in this paper were persistent (Table 4), and the H values for the professionals were smaller in all body parts for the professionals compared to the neophytes, but bigger differences were for the bow hand and the CoM, which shows a less persistent control (i.e., more active correction) for the professionals that makes their bow draw more distinctive.
SampEn values were smaller for all body parts in the professionals compared to neophytes, with bigger differences in the drawing hand and center of the mass, indicating that professionals have a less regular drawing style.
Each population (professional or neophyte) is characterized by a single concatenated time series of its participants. To validate our results, we employed bootstrapping to generate 1000 resampled datasets from each population to estimate the underlying distribution of the analyzed signals. Bootstrapping is, therefore, a computational means to enhance the power of statistical tests by simulating data collection in many more participants with similar simulated behavior as the actual participants. We subsequently repeated our analyses and used t-tests to estimate the statistical robustness of differences in test statistics among the resampled groups. Remarkably, except for the bow hand’s sample entropy (i.e., 1 out of 9 test statistics), all other comparisons yielded statistically significant differences (p-value < 0.01), strongly suggesting that the differences in the features from the actual professional and neophyte participants are indeed real, despite the limited number of subjects.

4. Discussion

As expected from their skill levels, there were differences in how professional and neophyte archers executed the bow-draw motion. However, our contribution is to be able to quantify the dynamical features that can capture those differences for this discrete (i.e., non-cyclical) bow-draw task. We show this from a variety of perspectives that include qualitative between-groups differences in the normalized 3D trajectories (Figure 4), and quantitative differences illustrated by dynamical time-series analyses: phase portraits (Figure 5) and Hurst exponents (Figure 6 and Table 4) and sample entropy (Table 5).
It is known that postural stability is a trainable attribute that can be enhanced through consistent practice, as demonstrated in studies by Jagdhane et al. (2016) [33] and Paillard (2017) [34]. In archery, specifically, an examination of postural balance during the aiming phase has revealed that better performance in professional archers, compared to their less-skilled counterparts, can be attributed to reduced postural sway characteristics. Our findings of lower dispersion (i.e., less variability and thus reduced sway) in the movement of the center of mass and hands for the professionals compared to the neophytes agree with those findings [18,19,20,35,36]. Our results critically extend those statistically based studies by revealing that differences in the dynamics of bow-draw motion between professional and neophyte archers can be attributed to different motor control strategies. This is because dynamical features such as smaller state space dispersion, more active corrections (less persistence in Hurst exponent analysis), and less regularity (in sample entropy) also have control of theoretical implications and interpretations.
Moreover, this work also serves as proof of principle that such dynamical time-series analyses can be applied to discrete athletic movements. Historically, dynamical systems analysis has been applied mostly to continuous motions such as quiet stance [37], gait [38,39], postural control [40] and running [41]. This is particularly useful in sports because many sports center around discrete actions. We note this because, before this work, most of these analyses were applied to inherently cyclical sports such as walking, running, and swimming.
Our preprocessing of their time series made applying these methods to the discrete bow draw possible. In particular, we first resampled, normalized, and demeaned the signals to prevent bias due to participant anatomy and movement duration differences. We then concatenated each instance of a movement from each participant group (reversing every other movement). This created a continuous time series amenable to these analyses that represented each participant while producing a balanced and long-enough time series representative of each group. This preprocessing of the time series from individuals’ motion into a single 1D time series provides only one value for the convex hull’s volume and Hurst exponent for each group. Nevertheless, this single number represents each group and lets us compare them as a group. Comparisons of individual subjects were not our goal.
Before interpreting our results, it is important to state some particular limitations of our project. As shown in Figure 1, only the experts completed the task by releasing the arrow (stage D of the shot cycle). This was imposed for the sake of the safety of the neophytes, as the release phase can be dangerous. Therefore, neophytes only reached the Aim phase (stage C) before relaxing. However, this does not affect our analysis of the bow-draw movement as the Aim phase is the end of the same. In addition, before performing the bow draw, the neophytes’ training consisted only of watching a professional archer perform the bow draw and release in silence. Moreover, they did not receive additional instruction, and we only analyzed their first motion for which markers were least occluded. The first trial analyzed only a truly naïve shot. Offering instruction would have added to the confound of learning, which was not the goal of our study. Although we expected this lack of formal instruction could result in greater inconsistency in the neophytes than in the professionals, this did not wash out-group differences, as demonstrated by Table 3 and Table 4. In addition, the purely visual exposure to the task before their bow-draw attempts emphasized learning by demonstration in the neophytes. The consistency we found in their movements (see Table 4) may come from their perception of the most salient visual features of the bow draw, as opposed to the details of motor performance of the task. Regardless of whether the task was being imitated at a perceptual or motor level by the neophytes (i.e., we cannot espouse either at this point), we find differences in the motor performance of the task across groups as described below. Lastly, we did not study, and therefore did not test for, the difference in dynamic performance between the sexes. All participants could easily draw the 30 lb. draw weight, and the average and standard deviation in weight and height between the two groups were similar (except for a greater standard deviation in the neophyte height). Future work that is properly powered for this comparison can assess differences in motor control between the sexes in this sport.
Phase portraits are geometrical representations of the transitions in the dynamical states of a system. Therefore, the volume of their convex hull represents the breadth (dispersion) of its dynamics. Professionals exhibited phase portraits with smaller volumes, indicating that the underlying dynamics of their motion were controlled to have less dispersion—which can be interpreted as having tighter control over the range of positions, velocities, and accelerations of their body parts.
A Hurst exponent of 0.5 represents a truly random Brownian motion. A value closer to 1.0 then quantifies ‘persistence’ in a time series (i.e., the tendency to continue the current trend of a movement). The Hurst exponents we found show that professionals are less persistent as a group (closer to 0.5) compared to the neophytes (closer to 1.0). This can be interpreted as professionals implementing more frequent and minute corrections, resulting in shorter movements.
This study utilizes Sample Entropy (SampEn), a modified adaptation of Approximate Entropy, as a quantitative metric for gauging complexity and irregularity within time series while circumventing the influence of self-similarity inherent to Approximate Entropy. Higher SampEn values indicate more complexity, signifying lower regularity within the time series. Our comprehensive analysis consistently reveals that professional archers exhibit higher sample entropy values across all three body parts under scrutiny, indicating more complexity than neophyte archers. The higher sample entropy values observed for the professional archers can manifest from a heightened adeptness in orchestrating a fluidic and precisely directed bow-draw motion. We conclude that professional archers exhibit tighter and finer control over their discrete bow-draw movements’ more fluid (i.e., less regular) dynamics. Although differences between these groups were expected, our work provides proof of principle of how well-established dynamical analyses can be used to quantify and compare the dynamics of discrete movements in sports.

Author Contributions

M.B. and F.J.V.-C. contributed to the conception and design of the study. H.A. preprocessed the data and performed the signal analysis. H.A., M.B., A.E. and F.J.V.-C. interpreted the results. H.A., M.B. and F.J.V.-C. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. H.A. and A.E. wrote sections of the manuscript, and H.A. created the figures. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

NSF CRCNS Japan-US 2113096; DARPA L2M Program W911NF1820264; NIH R21-NS113613 to FVC.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw (protocol code SEK 01-09/2020, date of approval 1 September 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The measurement data used to support the findings of this study are available from the second author upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Appendix A.1. Further Analysis of First PC

For completeness and to inform our interpretation of the results, we also tested the first PC of the time-series kinematics for nonstationary, nonlinear, and chaotic behavior [42,43,44,45].

Appendix A.1.1. Nonstationarity Test

A time series is nonstationary if its distribution changes across time. In other words, the time series’ mean, variance, and autocovariance must be time-independent to mark a signal as nonstationary. We used the nonstationary test method in [46]. Figure A1 shows a quantile-quantile plot of the quantiles of the first PCs versus the theoretical quantile values from a normal distribution for nonstationary tests. When the signal is nonstationary, the Q-Q plot will deviate from a straight line (normal distribution), and the points will not be distributed symmetrically around it, indicating that the data distribution changes over time. All first PCs were nonstationary.
Figure A1. The quantile-quantile plots for the professionals (ac) and the neophytes (df).
Figure A1. The quantile-quantile plots for the professionals (ac) and the neophytes (df).
Entropy 25 01414 g0a1

Appendix A.1.2. Nonlinearity Test

Surrogate data of an observed signal is a time series with the same linear statistical properties as in the observed signal [47]. The surrogate data test method is based on statistical hypothesis testing [48] to evaluate if it is nonlinear.
First, since all the series were nonstationary, we generated surrogate data using the Amplitude Adjusted Truncated Fourier Transform (AATFT) method [49]. Next, we selected the nonlinear version of autocorrelation statistics, AMI with m = 1 , as discriminate statistics. Finally, we considered the null hypothesis ( H 0 ) as A M I ( m = 1 ) = A M I S U R ( m = 1 ) , which checks if the observed time series is generated from a linear stochastic process possibly undergoing a nonlinear static transform [50]. Here, AMI [51,52] played a role as the discriminating statistic, which is a number that quantifies some aspect of the time series. If this number differs from the observed data, then the null hypothesis can be rejected. All first PCs were nonlinear with a confidence level of 99%.

Appendix A.1.3. Detection of Chaos

The largest Lyapunov exponent was computed using an algorithm from Wolf, Swift, Swinney, and Vastano [53]. The idea of using Lyapunov exponents to identify chaos in a system is based on the assumption that if the average distance between two points increases at an exponential rate, then the system is sensitive to a change in initial conditions, and the value of the Lyapunov exponent is greater than zero. Lyapunov exponents ( λ ) can be defined using Equation (A1),
d ( t ) = C e λ t .
where d ( t ) is the average divergence at time t and C is a constant that normalizes the initial separation. Therefore, the existence of a positive Lyapunov exponent is often considered a necessary and sufficient condition for the presence of chaos in the system. The CoM’s first PC had a positive Lyapunov exponent, and both hands had zero Lyapunov exponents (see Table A1). Therefore, only the CoM in both groups had chaotic behavior. All values are calculated for d(t) = 0.1.
Table A1. Lyapunov exponent values for each body part.
Table A1. Lyapunov exponent values for each body part.
Body PartProfessionalsNeophytes
Bow Hand00
Drawing Hand00
Center of Mass22.120.2

References

  1. Sigmundsson, H.; Trana, L.; Polman, R.; Haga, M. What is trained develops! theoretical perspective on skill learning. Sports 2017, 5, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. de Pedro-Carracedo, J.; Fuentes-Jimenez, D.; Ugena, A.M.; Gonzalez-Marcos, A.P. Phase space reconstruction from a biological time series: A photoplethysmographic signal case study. Appl. Sci. 2020, 10, 1430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Peppoloni, L.; Lawrence, E.L.; Ruffaldi, E.; Valero-Cuevas, F.J. Characterization of the disruption of neural control strategies for dynamic fingertip forces from attractor reconstruction. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0172025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Paterno, M.V.; Kiefer, A.W.; Bonnette, S.; Riley, M.A.; Schmitt, L.C.; Ford, K.R.; Myer, G.D.; Shockley, K.; Hewett, T.E. Prospectively identified deficits in sagittal plane hip–ankle coordination in female athletes who sustain a second anterior cruciate ligament injury after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction and return to sport. Clin. Biomech. 2015, 30, 1094–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Kurz, M.J.; Stergiou, N.; Buzzi, U.H.; Georgoulis, A.D. The effect of anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction on lower extremity relative phase dynamics during walking and running. Knee Surg. Sports Traumatol. Arthrosc. 2005, 13, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Quintana-Duque, J.C. Non-linear dynamic invariants based on embedding reconstruction of systems for pedaling motion. In Proceedings of the Sportinformatik 2012, Konstanz, Germany, 12–14 September 2012. [Google Scholar]
  7. Wu, W.; Zeng, W.; Ma, L.; Yuan, C.; Zhang, Y. Modeling and classification of gait patterns between anterior cruciate ligament deficient and intact knees based on phase space reconstruction, Euclidean distance and neural networks. Biomed. Eng. Online 2018, 17, 165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Zeng, W.; Ismail, S.A.; Pappas, E. Classification of gait patterns in patients with unilateral anterior cruciate ligament deficiency based on phase space reconstruction, Euclidean distance and neural networks. Soft Comput. 2020, 24, 1851–1868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Shea, J.J. The origins of lithic projectile point technology: Evidence from Africa, the Levant, and Europe. J. Archaeol. Sci. 2006, 33, 823–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Williams, V.M.; Burke, A.; Lombard, M. Throwing spears and shooting arrows: Preliminary results of a pilot neuroarchaeological study. S. Afr. Archaeol. Bull. 2014, 69, 199–207. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hatfield, B.D.; Haufler, A.J.; Hung, T.M.; Spalding, T.W. Electroencephalographic studies of skilled psychomotor performance. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004, 21, 144–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Nishizono, H.; Shibayama, H.; Izuta, T.; Saito, K. Analysis of archery shooting techniques by means of electromyography. In Proceedings of the 5 International Symposium on Biomechanics in Sports, Athens, Greece, 12–17 July 1987; pp. 364–372. [Google Scholar]
  13. Wang, D.; Hu, T.; Luo, R.; Shen, Q.; Wang, Y.; Li, X.; Qiao, J.; Zhu, L.; Cui, L.; Yin, H. Effect of cognitive reappraisal on archery performance of elite athletes: The mediating effects of sport-confidence and attention. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 860817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Dorshorst, T.; Weir, G.; Hamill, J.; Holt, B. Archery’s signature: An electromyographic analysis of the upper limb. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2022, 4, e25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Baifa, Z.; Xinglong, Z.; Dongmei, L. Muscle coordination during archery shooting: A comparison of archers with different skill levels. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2023, 23, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Shinohara, H.; Urabe, Y. Analysis of muscular activity in archery: A comparison of skill level. J. Sport. Med. Phys. Fit. 2017, 58, 1752–1758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Ertan, H.; Kentel, B.; Tümer, S.; Korkusuz, F. Activation patterns in forearm muscles during archery shooting. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2003, 22, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kuch, A.; Tisserand, R.; Durand, F.; Monnet, T.; Debril, J.F. Postural adjustments preceding string release in trained archers. J. Sport. Sci. 2023, 41, 677–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Sarro, K.J.; Viana, T.D.C.; De Barros, R.M.L. Relationship between bow stability and postural control in recurve archery. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2021, 21, 515–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Spratford, W.; Campbell, R. Postural stability, clicker reaction time and bow draw force predict performance in elite recurve archery. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2017, 17, 539–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Laborde, S.; Dosseville, F.E.; Leconte, P.; Margas, N. Interaction of hand preference with eye dominance on accuracy in archery. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2009, 108, 558–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Callaway, A.J.; Wiedlack, J.; Heller, M. Identification of temporal factors related to shot performance for indoor recurve archery. J. Sport. Sci. 2017, 35, 1142–1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bartsch-Jimenez, A.; Błażkiewicz, M.; Azadjou, H.; Novotny, R.; Valero-Cuevas, F.J. Fine synergies” describe motor adaptation in people with drop foot in a way that supplements traditional “coarse synergies. Front. Sport. Act. Living 2023, 5, 1080170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Warmenhoven, J.; Cobley, S.; Draper, C.; Harrison, A.; Bargary, N.; Smith, R. Considerations for the use of functional principal components analysis in sports biomechanics: Examples from on-water rowing. Sport. Biomech. 2019, 18, 317–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Federolf, P.; Reid, R.; Gilgien, M.; Haugen, P.; Smith, G. The application of principal component analysis to quantify technique in sports. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2014, 24, 491–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Witte, K.; Ganter, N.; Baumgart, C.; Peham, C. Applying a principal component analysis to movement coordination in sport. Math. Comput. Model. Dyn. Syst. 2010, 16, 477–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Matilla-García, M.; Morales, I.; Rodríguez, J.M.; Ruiz Marín, M. Selection of embedding dimension and delay time in phase space reconstruction via symbolic dynamics. Entropy 2021, 23, 221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wang, H.Y.; Sigerson, L.; Jiang, H.; Cheng, C. Psychometric properties and factor structures of Chinese smartphone addiction inventory: Test of two models. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kennel, M.B.; Brown, R.; Abarbanel, H.D. Determining embedding dimension for phase-space reconstruction using a geometrical construction. Phys. Rev. A 1992, 45, 3403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Hurst, H.E. Long-term storage capacity of reservoirs. Trans. Am. Soc. Civ. Eng. 1951, 116, 770–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hurst, H.E.; Black, R.P.; Simaika, Y.M. Long term storage. Exp. Study. 1965. Available online: https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1573387449265485184 (accessed on 23 August 2023).
  32. Kroha, P.; Skoula, M. Hurst Exponent and Trading Signals Derived from Market Time Series. In Proceedings of the ICEIS (1), Funchal, Madeira, 21–24 March 2018; pp. 371–378. [Google Scholar]
  33. Jagdhane, S.; Kanekar, N.; S Aruin, A. The effect of a four-week balance training program on anticipatory postural adjustments in older adults: A pilot feasibility study. Curr. Aging Sci. 2016, 9, 295–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Paillard, T. Plasticity of the postural function to sport and/or motor experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 72, 129–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Mason, B.R.; Pelgrim, P.P. Body stability and performance in archery. Excel 1986, 3, 17–20. [Google Scholar]
  36. Simsek, D.; Cerrah, A.; Ertan, H.; Soylu, A. A comparison of the ground reaction forces of archers with different levels of expertise during the arrow shooting. Sci. Sports 2019, 34, e137–e145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Mégrot, F.; Bardy, B.G. Changes in phase space during learning an unstable balance. Neurosci. Lett. 2006, 402, 17–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. van Schooten, K.S.; Rispens, S.M.; Pijnappels, M.; Daffertshofer, A.; van Dieen, J.H. Assessing gait stability: The influence of state space reconstruction on inter-and intra-day reliability of local dynamic stability during over-ground walking. J. Biomech. 2013, 46, 137–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Sessa, S.; Saito, K.; Zecca, M.; Bartolomeo, L.; Lin, Z.; Cosentino, S.; Ishii, H.; Ikai, T.; Takanishi, A. Walking assessment in the phase space by using ultra-miniaturized Inertial Measurement Units. In Proceedings of the 2013 IEEE International Conference on Mechatronics and Automation, Takamatsu, Japan, 4–7 August 2013; pp. 902–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Saraiva, M.; Vilas-Boas, J.P.; Fernandes, O.J.; Castro, M.A. Effects of motor task difficulty on postural control complexity during dual tasks in young adults: A nonlinear approach. Sensors 2023, 23, 628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. López-Méndez, A.; Casas, J.R. Model-based recognition of human actions by trajectory matching in phase spaces. Image Vis. Comput. 2012, 30, 808–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Takens, F. Detecting strange attractors in turbulence. In Dynamical Systems and Turbulence, Warwick 1980; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 366–381. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ottino, J.M.; Shinbrot, T. Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos (with Applications to Physics, Biology Chemistry, and Engineering); Strogatz, S.H., Ed.; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1994; 498p. [Google Scholar]
  44. Kaplan, D.; Glass, L. Time-series analysis. In Understanding Nonlinear Dynamics; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 278–358. [Google Scholar]
  45. Ott, E. Chaos in Dynamical Systems; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  46. Zhivomirov, H.; Nedelchev, I. A Method for Signal Stationarity Estimation. Rom. J. Acoust. Vib. 2020, 17, 149–155. [Google Scholar]
  47. Prichard, D.; Theiler, J. Generating surrogate data for time series with several simultaneously measured variables. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1994, 73, 951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Martinez, W.L.; Martinez, A.R. Computational Statistics Handbook with MATLAB; Chapman and Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kantz, H.; Schreiber, T. Nonlinear Time Series Analysis; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004; Volume 7. [Google Scholar]
  50. Theiler, J.; Eubank, S.; Longtin, A.; Galdrikian, B.; Farmer, J.D. Testing for nonlinearity in time series: The method of surrogate data. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 1992, 58, 77–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fraser, A.; Swinney, H. Independent coordinates for strange attractors from mutual information. Phys. Rev. A 1986, 33, 1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Liebert, W.; Schuster, H. Proper choice of the time delay for the analysis of chaotic time series. Phys. Lett. A 1989, 142, 107–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Wolf, A.; Swift, J.B.; Swinney, H.L.; Vastano, J.A. Determining Lyapunov exponents from a time series. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 1985, 16, 285–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Motion capture stick figure of a professional participant during the shot cycle: (A) Set-up phase, (B) Draw phase, (C) Aim phase, (D) Release phase (not included in the analysis and not done by neophytes).
Figure 1. Motion capture stick figure of a professional participant during the shot cycle: (A) Set-up phase, (B) Draw phase, (C) Aim phase, (D) Release phase (not included in the analysis and not done by neophytes).
Entropy 25 01414 g001
Figure 2. Preprocessed and normalized time series (of each group) of the Cartesian coordinates of the center of mass (CoM) and their projection onto their first principal component (PC). We resampled the bow-draw movement from all participants (which could have taken a different amount of time) to have 1000 samples each and then concatenated them to create the time series for each group. Considering the number of participants in each group (N = 14), the concatenated time series has 14,000 samples for neophytes and professionals.
Figure 2. Preprocessed and normalized time series (of each group) of the Cartesian coordinates of the center of mass (CoM) and their projection onto their first principal component (PC). We resampled the bow-draw movement from all participants (which could have taken a different amount of time) to have 1000 samples each and then concatenated them to create the time series for each group. Considering the number of participants in each group (N = 14), the concatenated time series has 14,000 samples for neophytes and professionals.
Entropy 25 01414 g002
Figure 3. Steps for: (A) Plotting the phase portrait for the 1D time series of the CoM using the time-lag method, (B) Fitting the convex hull to the phase space trajectories, and (C) Removing the trajectories from the convex hulls to generate Figure 5c.
Figure 3. Steps for: (A) Plotting the phase portrait for the 1D time series of the CoM using the time-lag method, (B) Fitting the convex hull to the phase space trajectories, and (C) Removing the trajectories from the convex hulls to generate Figure 5c.
Entropy 25 01414 g003
Figure 4. Normalized Cartesian coordinates of the hands (a,b) and CoM (c) for the single bow-draw motion for all participants. The asterisks indicate the start of the motion, and the shaded areas represent standard deviations from the mean presented by the solid line.
Figure 4. Normalized Cartesian coordinates of the hands (a,b) and CoM (c) for the single bow-draw motion for all participants. The asterisks indicate the start of the motion, and the shaded areas represent standard deviations from the mean presented by the solid line.
Entropy 25 01414 g004
Figure 5. Convex-hulls of the phase portraits for the bow hand (a), the drawing hand (b), and CoM (c) of the professionals and neophytes. There is one convex hull per group, as each phase portrait was obtained from the concatenated time series from all subjects in that group. The volume of each convex hull is shown in Table 3.
Figure 5. Convex-hulls of the phase portraits for the bow hand (a), the drawing hand (b), and CoM (c) of the professionals and neophytes. There is one convex hull per group, as each phase portrait was obtained from the concatenated time series from all subjects in that group. The volume of each convex hull is shown in Table 3.
Entropy 25 01414 g005
Figure 6. The slope of the fitted line on the calculated data points from the logarithm of rescaled range ( R / S ) vs. the logarithm of time lags is equal to the Hurst exponent.
Figure 6. The slope of the fitted line on the calculated data points from the logarithm of rescaled range ( R / S ) vs. the logarithm of time lags is equal to the Hurst exponent.
Entropy 25 01414 g006
Table 1. Participant characteristics (mean ± SD).
Table 1. Participant characteristics (mean ± SD).
GroupAge [Years]Body Mass [kg]Height [cm]Training History [Years]Draw Weight [lb]
Professionals (N = 14)23.7 ± 9.973.2 ± 16.5175.7 ± 9.711.1 ± 7.936–40
Neophytes (N = 14)23.5 ± 1.373.3 ± 16.7176 ± 13.5030
Table 2. Percentage of variance explained by the first PC for each body part.
Table 2. Percentage of variance explained by the first PC for each body part.
Body PartProfessionalsNeophytes
Bow Hand64.368.6
Drawing Hand76.174.9
Center of Mass56.562.5
Table 3. Convex hull volumes (normalized units) for the phase portraits of each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Table 3. Convex hull volumes (normalized units) for the phase portraits of each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Body PartProfessionals Neophytes
Bow Hand7.0< *8.3
Drawing Hand7.3< *8.0
Center of Mass6.3< *11.2
Table 4. Hurst exponent (H) values for each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Table 4. Hurst exponent (H) values for each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Body PartProfessionals Neophytes
Bow Hand0.71< *0.79
Drawing Hand0.85< *0.86
Center of Mass0.82< *0.86
Table 5. Sample Entropy values for each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Table 5. Sample Entropy values for each body part. * denotes statistical significance with p < 0.01 .
Body PartProfessionals Neophytes
Bow Hand0.024>0.023
Drawing Hand0.020> *0.014
Center of Mass0.035> *0.025
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Azadjou, H.; Błażkiewicz, M.; Erwin, A.; Valero-Cuevas, F.J. Dynamical Analyses Show That Professional Archers Exhibit Tighter, Finer and More Fluid Dynamical Control Than Neophytes. Entropy 2023, 25, 1414. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25101414

AMA Style

Azadjou H, Błażkiewicz M, Erwin A, Valero-Cuevas FJ. Dynamical Analyses Show That Professional Archers Exhibit Tighter, Finer and More Fluid Dynamical Control Than Neophytes. Entropy. 2023; 25(10):1414. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25101414

Chicago/Turabian Style

Azadjou, Hesam, Michalina Błażkiewicz, Andrew Erwin, and Francisco J. Valero-Cuevas. 2023. "Dynamical Analyses Show That Professional Archers Exhibit Tighter, Finer and More Fluid Dynamical Control Than Neophytes" Entropy 25, no. 10: 1414. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25101414

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop