Next Article in Journal
Fundamental Knowledge on Forgotten Species: An Exploration of Data from Rarely Studied Captive Animals
Next Article in Special Issue
An Introduction to the Special Issue: “The Animals Will Play While the Visitors Are Away: What Happens When Zoos and Aquariums Are Closed to Visitors?”
Previous Article in Journal
The Impacts of Evening Events in Zoos: A Christmas Event at Knowsley Safari
Previous Article in Special Issue
Behavioural Changes in Zoo Animals during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Long-Term, Multi Species Comparison
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Gorilla Activities and Social Behavior: Assessing Changes Associated with Absence of Zoo Visitors, Zoo Attendance, Time of Day and Increasing Maturity

Research Program, Los Angeles Zoo, Los Angeles, CA 90027, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2023, 4(1), 39-49; https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4010004
Submission received: 11 November 2022 / Revised: 27 December 2022 / Accepted: 29 December 2022 / Published: 5 January 2023

Abstract

:
In this study we report on associations found between several variables and changes in gorilla behavior. Two groups of gorillas were observed, one was a family group and the other was a bachelor group. The zoo was closed to visitors for two periods during the COVID-19 pandemic which made it possible to document any differences associated with the presence and absence of visitors. We also considered possible changes in behavior in relation to daily zoo attendance, time of day, and increasing maturity of group members. Output variables included six activities and one location which were assessed by instantaneous sampling, as well as level of aggressive and affiliative behavior assessed by recording all occurrences. Hour in which observations were done and successive months in the study, which serves as a measure of increasing maturity of younger gorillas, were the best predictors of activity and location, with each showing significant relationships with three output variables. Absence of visitors during zoo closure was also significantly associated with an increase in time the gorillas were proximate to potential visitor viewing areas. Aggression among the bachelors was lower when the zoo was closed and higher with maturation of the youngest male. Reviewing differences in level of aggression during the two periods of closure shows that decreased aggression among the bachelors was not a result of zoo closure.

1. Introduction

While zoo visitors often seek interactions with animals that they come to learn about and enjoy [1], it is not clear if zoo animals seek and/or benefit from the presence of visitors. Zoological institutions strive to improve the well-being of animals in human care and contribute to the growing field of visitor studies. To date positive, neutral, as well as negative visitor effects have been reported [2,3,4,5]. For example, time spent playing in Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana) was found to increase with a greater number of visitors [2] and play is associated with positive welfare states [6]. In contrast, time slender-tailed meerkats (Suricata suricatta) spent vigilant or looking at visitors, as well as distance between meerkats and visitors, did not vary in response to reduced visitor noise and meerkat-directed visitor activity [3] which is interpreted as a neutral effect. Soemmerring’s gazelles (Gazella soemmerringii) kept in enclosures with greater exposure to zoo visitors had more frequent gazelle-gazelle agonistic interactions [7], an undesirable or negative effect. Results of visitor studies clearly vary with the species observed and likely vary as a function of housing provided as well as individual histories and individual characteristics of the particular animals studied [5]. When doing research in the zoo uncontrolled extraneous factors may influence outcomes. Numerous studies have explored the effects of visitor density on animal behavior but in many cases visitor density is associated with time of day and/or time of year. Differences attributed to visitor density may be confounded with these and other variables such as animal care routines, feeding times, and presence of enrichment. Studies of visitor effects are often carried out over relatively brief periods and differences resulting from increases in maturity of animals are rarely considered.
The presence of visitors is one of many variables that may alter the behavior of zoo animals. Additions to animals housed together, whether due to births or introductions of incoming animals, are likely to bring social changes. Observations before, during, and following births and introductions expand our knowledge of behavior and provide information that can assist in animal management. Such events were anticipated during winter of 2019/2020 in each of the two western lowland gorilla groups (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) housed at the Los Angeles Zoo. Studies to document changes in time spent in specific activities, habitat utilization, and frequency of social behavior in relation to zoo attendance, time of day, and increasing gorilla maturity were underway when COVID-19 was declared a pandemic in Los Angeles.
To reduce the spread of the virus, people were advised to restrict their outside activities. In response to pandemic conditions most zoos were closed to visitors for one or more periods of time. Zoo closures made it possible to assess differences in the behavior of zoo animals with and without visitors present. At the Los Angeles Zoo we continued gorilla observations, and, in this report, we include the presence or absence of visitors as one of the input variables to be assessed with respect to changes in gorilla behavior, including frequency of both affiliative and agonistic actions directed to gorillas and to zoo visitors.

2. Materials and Methods

Two groups of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) have been housed at the Los Angeles Zoo since 2007 and changes in the composition of both groups occurred during the winter of 2019/2020. A blackback male was introduced to the bachelor group of two older silverbacks and a female was born in the family group consisting of one older silverback and three adult females. Life-history parameters of gorillas are known and indicated that during the nineteen-month study the eleven-year-old male’s secondary sexual characteristics would continue to develop as he approached the age of a young silverback [8,9]. The infant female would be carried and nursed by her dam, but she would increasingly interact with other members of the family group [10].
A total of eight gorillas were observed who ranged in age from newborn to 43 years at the start of the study; their ID codes, sex and birth years are shown in Table 1. All but the female born in 1976 were parent reared. During the day the gorillas occupy the outdoor multilevel habitat between 0945 and 1600. The bachelor and family groups reside in adjacent areas in the Campo Gorilla Reserve which is diagramed in Figure 1. Usable outdoor gorilla space which can be viewed by visitors totals 14,963 sq. ft. The outdoor habitat utilized by the family group is approximately twice the size of that used by the bachelor group. Behind the indoor night-quarters there is an off-exhibit outdoor annex which provides an additional 858 sq. ft. of usable space for the bachelors who can access it from their outdoor multilevel habitat. At night the bachelors are housed separately; in the family group the oldest female is housed separately, other members of the family group can choose to rest together or apart.
The gorillas are fed three times each day. The morning meal, browse and enrichment items are spread across the multilevel outdoor habitat prior to 0945. The gorillas may be briefly brought into the night-quarters while the mid-day meal is scattered in their outdoor habitat. Alternatively, an extra portion may be included in the morning feed or tossed in while gorillas remain in the habitat. The evening meal is provided at 1630, after the gorillas have entered their night-quarters.
The study reported here covers a 19-month period and includes two periods of zoo closure which were preceded or followed by periods in which zoo visitors were present. In phase 1 (1 November 2019 through 12 March 2020) visitors were present; incoming male, HJ, was introduced to the bachelor group on 12 December and female A was born to N in the family group on 18 January. In phase 2 (13 March through 25 August 2020) visitors were absent. In phase 3 (26 August through 6 December 2020) visitors were present but limited to stay within the attendance range specified by the City of Los Angeles and Los Angeles County Department of Public Health who were closely monitoring the level of COVID-19. Date spans and maximum to be admitted per day in phase 3 follow: 26–30 August/1200, 31 August–31 October/1800, 1 November–6 December/2300. In phase 4 (7 December 2020 through 15 February 2021) visitors were absent. In phase 5 (16 February through 30 June 2021) visitors were present but attendance was limited to stay within the city and county specified ranges. Date spans and maximum to be admitted per day in phase 5 follow: 16 February–11 March/2400, 12 March–15 April/4400, 16 April–26 May/5000, 27 May–14 June/7700, 15–30 June/10,000. The number of focal observations of each gorilla during each of the 5 different visitor phases is shown in Table 1.
All gorilla observations were made from visitor outdoor viewing areas between 0945 and 1545. On study days each gorilla who had access to the multi-level outdoor area was observed as the focal animal for a 15 min period. A focal observation started with sequential description of all social behavior directed to or from the focal gorilla. Potential recipients included neighboring gorillas and zoo visitors as well as other gorillas in the same habitat. Time markers sounded at 60 s intervals and the observer instantaneously recorded focal activity and habitat sector occupied, then continued with sequential recording [11]. Definitions of behaviors recorded are given in Table 2. Members of the bachelor group were observed for a total of 96.25 h and members of the family group for a total of 210.75 h.
Analysis of activity and location input variables included whether visitors could enter the zoo, sequential month of the study, zoo attendance on observation days, and hour at start of focal observation. Social interactions within bachelor groups and family groups are well known to differ in type and in frequency. When housed with other near-adult or adult males, bachelors show substantial non-contact and contact aggression which peaks in young silverbacks between the ages of 14 and 20 years [12]. Occurrences of social behavior were analyzed separately for the two groups. All outcome variables were recorded as counts and analyzed using mixed-effects negative binomial regression (MENBREG). MENBREG in Stata 17.0 was utilized as it is suitable for non-normal distributions of count data, can incorporate repeated observations of the same individuals, and allows multiple input variables to be considered. In the analyses reported below repeated observations were nested in focal gorillas. To account for variation in amount of time a focal animal could be seen, visible activities instantaneously sampled were summed and used as the exposure variable for activity analyses in Stata. Number of minutes in which social interactions between an initiator and recipient could be documented was used as the exposure variable for social interaction analyses. As we would be assessing multiple input variables alpha was set at 0.01.

3. Results

3.1. Activity and Location

Regressions were done for the six instantaneously sampled measures described in Table 2; results are shown in Table 3. The hour at which the observation was started was significantly associated with three output variables: proportion of scans feeding and/or foraging, proportion of scans stationary or inactive, and proportion of scans in proximity to visitor viewing areas. At later start times the proportion of scans in foraging activity, as well as scans in proximity to visitor viewing areas, decreased while the proportion of scans in which the gorillas were stationary increased. In the morning, before gorillas had access to the outdoor areas, keepers scattered food throughout the habitat, including areas near visitor viewing sites. The gorillas spent the most time foraging when they first entered the outdoor habitat. As food was consumed foraging opportunities decreased, including in the areas proximate to visitor viewing. As a result, gorillas foraged less in later hours, were less likely to spend time in areas adjacent to visitor viewing, and tended to be less active.
Study month was also associated with three output variables: exploration/self-play, not visible, and time proximate to visitor viewing areas. In this study successive study months are associated with increasing maturity of the introduced bachelor and of the infant. As months progressed, the gorillas spent a greater proportion of scans in exploration/self-play, and a smaller proportion of scans not visible. They also spent a greater proportion of scans in proximity to visitor viewing areas.
Zoo closure was associated with just one instantaneously sampled outcome variable; the gorillas spent a greater proportion of scans proximate to visitor viewing areas when there were no visitors.
When observing it was clear that female E, who had been nursery-reared, utilized the display habitat differently from others. In contrast to the other family gorillas in the same habitat, she spent a substantially greater proportion of her time in sectors proximate to visitor viewing areas. During the 19-month study, E spent an average of 0.7635 (standard deviation = 0.3585) proportion of her scans in those areas in contrast to the other four gorillas in the same habitat who spent an average of 0.3549 (standard deviation = 0.37696) proportion of their scans there.
None of the input variables, including zoo attendance, showed a significant relationship to proportion of scans in self-directed activity which is often used as an indicator of stress [13,14,15].

3.2. Social Interactions

Non-contact threats including tight lips, stiff stance, chest beat, bluff charge, object display, and chase were summed with contact aggression to obtain total occurrences of aggressive behavior. Brief contact, sustained contact, sexual contact, social examine/groom, and social play were summed to obtain total occurrences of affiliative behavior. Interactions among the three bachelors were more than six times as frequent as those among the family group of five gorillas.
Among the bachelor group an average of 1.3806 occurrences of aggressive behavior (standard deviation of 3.0935) and an average of 0.0382 occurrences of affiliative behaviors (standard deviation of 0.4676) took place during focal observations. Among the family group an average of 0.0241 occurrences of aggressive behavior (standard deviation of 0.1899) and 0.1509 occurrences of affiliative behavior (standard deviation of 0.4676) took place during focal observations.
There were few interactions between the two gorilla groups: During 96.25 bachelor focal observation hours we documented a total of 9 threats from a bachelor towards the family area and all occurred in the last phase of the study when bachelor aggression was markedly increasing (3 from H, the dominant male, and 6 from HJ, the introduced male). During 210.75 family focal observation hours no aggression was directed to the bachelors. No affiliative behavior was seen between the two gorilla groups.
The bachelors directed a total of 13 threats (3 from H, 6 from J, 4 from HJ) and showed no affiliative behavior to visitors. The family group gave 11 threats to visitors (5 from E, 4 from K, 2 from R) and, except for gorilla E, no affiliative behavior to visitors. In contrast to others in the family group, E directed a total of 122 affiliative behaviors to visitors.
MENBREG showed considerable differences between the two groups with respect to associations between input variables and within group social occurrence outcomes that can be viewed in Table 4. For bachelors, progression in study months was positively associated with increased aggression (see Figure 2). As months progressed, introduced male HJ was maturing and becoming more assertive. MENBREG also shows there was significantly more bachelor aggression when the zoo was open to visitors but less aggression when attendance was higher. The family group also showed less aggression with higher attendance and the association between affiliative behavior and attendance approached significance. That is, there was less affiliative behavior among family members when attendance was higher (p = 0.016).
While there is a statistically significant association between zoo closure and lower bachelor aggression, looking at the level of aggression in Figure 3 during each of the two closures separately makes it clear that closure does not cause the reduction in bachelor aggression. Over the course of the 19-month study, the lowest level of bachelor aggression that occurred was during the first closure while the highest level of aggression that occurred was during the second closure.

4. Discussion

In this study we focused on assessing possible effects of the closure on changes in gorilla behavior while looking at other factors that could also be contributing to changes. Just two significant relationships were found with zoo closure: more time was spent in areas proximate to visitor viewing when the zoo was closed to visitors and the level of aggression among the bachelors was lower. In fact, the level of bachelor aggression during the two closures was quite different, with low levels of aggression during the first closure but high levels of aggression during the second closure which was much briefer. Increases in aggression are characteristic of maturing blackbacks [12,16] and consistent with the difference in level of aggression among the bachelors during the successive closures. HJ had been introduced at 11 years of age and by the time of the second closure he had reached 12 years of age. Both Figure 2 and Figure 3 clearly show that aggression is increasing as HJ is maturing, a finding consistent with the literature.
A further indication that lack of visitors is unlikely to result in differences in gorilla social behavior is that, with the exception of nursery-reared gorilla E, few social behaviors were directed to visitors when the zoo was open. In short, we found just one change that is likely to have resulted from zoo closure, increased time in proximity to visitor viewing areas. This does suggest that viewing visitors is of interest to the gorillas and for this reason we have initiated a follow-up study in which visual attention is being recorded. A long-term study exploring the effects of zoo closure on the behavior of gorillas is underway at the Buffalo Zoo where researchers also report minimal effects [17,18].
Effects of zoo attendance or visitor density on behavior of gorillas residing at zoological institutions has been explored in a number of studies. Wells reports high visitor density encouraged more aggression, stereotypies and autogrooming [19]. Lewis et al. report high visitor density resulted in increased locomotion and increased visitor vigilance [20]. Stoinski et al., who assessed possible visitor effects in four gorilla groups, report that with high crowds, two groups showed increased stereotypies while the other two groups expressed fewer stereotypies. The investigators also found that only males showed more contact aggression [21]. Kuhar, who assessed two groups, reports that with high crowds, gorillas were less visible and that the bachelor group showed increased aggression [22]. Hashmi and Sullivan report high visitor density resulted in less time spent inactive, more time feeding, and no change in stereotypy [23]. In this study less aggression among the bachelors as well as among the family group was associated with higher zoo attendance which had not previously been reported. However, a significant association does not imply causality. Prior to the pandemic the Los Angeles Zoo would host as many as 12,000 visitors on a single high attendance Saturday or Sunday; the average number of visitors admitted during phase 1 observation days was 3694. In order to maximize distance between visitors and minimize risk of disease transmission, when the zoo re-opened attendance was intentionally limited to 1200 visitors. Over the next four months the number of visitors who could enter was gradually raised to 2300. Average number of visitors during study days in this period, phase 3, was 1515. In December 2020, COVID-19 surged in Los Angeles and the zoo again closed to visitors. When the zoo reopened following the second closure, admission was initially limited to 2400 visitors. The number admitted was raised in increments to 10,000 by the end of June, 2021, when observations for this study came to a close. Average attendance during study days in phase 5 was 3405 which was still lower than the average attendance during study days in phase 1. Over the course of the study HJ was maturing and becoming more aggressive in the bachelor group. At the same time the infant in the family group was maturing and the adults were becoming less protective, engaging in more aggressive behavior than they had when the infant was younger. However, the number of visitors admitted had been restricted due to COVID-19 policies. Gorilla aggression and zoo attendance are confounded in phases 2–5 and the higher attendance in phase 1 should not be construed as resulting in lower levels of aggression.
The one gorilla, E, who behaved somewhat differently during the study, was the only gorilla in the group who had been nursery-reared subsequent to her 1976 birth. E spent more time in proximity to visitor viewing areas throughout the study and she was the only gorilla to direct affiliative behaviors towards visitors. Her interest in visitors can be attributed to nursery rearing as we know that early care-takers can influence later relationships [24]. This finding serves to illustrate how the same input variables may have different associations with output variables in members of the same species housed at the same institution as has been reported by both Stoinski et al. and Hashmi and Sullivan [21,23].
Despite lack of opportunity to interact with visitors when the zoo was closed, the proportion of time E spent in self-directed activity, a measure of stress, did not vary significantly during the course of the study. Reviewing other studies shows variable results. As mentioned above, Wells did find more self-grooming with higher visitor density [19]. Carder and Semple explored this relationship in two gorilla groups housed at different institutions and found increased scratching with increased visitor density in one of the groups [25]. However, in a well-controlled study, Kuhar reports no relationship was found between large crowds and proportion of scans spent self-grooming in two gorilla groups housed at the same institution [22].
Including multiple input variables in studies is useful in interpreting outcomes and in this study, we found that zoo closure had minimal effects. While a general variable such as the presence or absence of visitors or attendance has the potential to affect the well-being of animals, we need to identify what aspects of visitation are mediating the effects. Is it the behavior of the visitors? Is it exposure to visual and/or auditory and/or olfactory stimuli associated with visitors that influences animals? In the case of the pandemic, could the behavior of animals vary in response to their keepers’ emotions? Such questions should stimulate further research which can be usefully applied to increasing our ability to provide optimal care for the animals we manage.

5. Conclusions

The presence or absence of zoo visitors may be associated with changes in the behavior of animals as may other variables that can be operating at the same time. In this study two groups of gorillas were observed. Time of observation and successive month of the study were associated with more changes in activity and occurrences of social behavior than whether the zoo was open to visitors. The only causal relationship between zoo closure and behavior was more time spent proximate to visitor viewing areas when visitors were not present. Over the course of the 19-month study, with successive months serving as a proxy for increasing maturity, aggression within the bachelor group as well as the family group did rise.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.R.C., J.I.M. and J.S.W.; methodology, C.R.C., J.I.M. and J.S.W.; validation, J.I.M.; formal analysis, C.R.C.; investigation, C.R.C.; resources, C.R.C.; data curation, C.R.C. and J.S.W.; writing—original draft preparation, C.R.C.; writing—review and editing, J.S.W. and J.I.M.; visualization, C.R.C.; supervision, C.R.C.; project administration, C.R.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The project was entirely observational in nature and carried out to provide information that would assist in animal management. As all observations were made from visitor areas and there were no changes in management required, the need for ethical review was waived.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Requests for data should be sent to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We much appreciate the contributions of Jo-Ann Krohn, Amanda Killian, Fielding Etheridge, Deborah Wheeler, and Paula Orlovich who utilized their time, skills, and energy to increase the number of focal gorilla observations made as well as that of Cambria Cox-Bain who drew the diagram illustrating Campo Gorilla Reserve, Los Angeles Zoo’s gorilla facility. We also appreciate the high-quality gorilla care and communication provided by the animal care staff. Many thanks to Christine Wells, UCLA Office of Advanced Research Computing, who advised on appropriate analysis.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Luebke, J.F.; Watters, J.V.; Packer, J.; Miller, L.J.; Powell, D.M. Zoo visitor’s affective responses to observing animal behaviors. Visit. Stud. 2016, 19, 60–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Todd, P.A.; Macdonald, C.; Coleman, D. Visitor-associated variation in captive Diana monkey (Cercopithecus diana diana) behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 107, 162–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sherwin, L.; Magrath, M.J.; Butler, K.L.; Phillips, C.J.; Hemworth, P.A. A multi-enclosure study investigating the behavioural response of meerkats to visitors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 156, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hosey, G. Zoo animals and their human audiences; What is the visitor effect? Anim. Welf. 2000, 9, 343–357. [Google Scholar]
  5. Hosey, G. A preliminary model of human-animal relationships in the zoo. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 109, 105–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Boissy, A.; Manteuffel, G.; Jensen, M.B.; Moe, R.O.; Spruijt, B.; Keeling, L.J.; Winckler, C.; Forkman, B.; Dimitrov, I.; Langbein, J.; et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 92, 375–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Mansour, A.A.H.; Zakaria, A.H.; Fraser, A.F. Effect of enclosure quality on reactivity and welfare of captive Soemmerring’s gazelle (Gazella soemmerringii). J. Appl. Anim. Welfare Sci. 2000, 3, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Breuer, T.; Breuer-Ndoundou Hockemba, M.; Olejniczak, C.; Parnell, R.J.; Stokes, E.J. Physical maturation, life-history classes and age estimates of free-ranging western gorillas—Insights from Mbeli Bai, Republic of Congo. Amer. J. Primatol. 2009, 71, 106–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Stoinski, T.S.; Kuhar, C.W.; Lucas, K.E.; Maple, T.L. Social dynamics of captive western lowland gorillas living in all-male groups. Behaviour 2004, 141, 169–195. [Google Scholar]
  10. Nowell, A.A.; Fletcher, A.W. Development of independence from the mother in Gorilla gorilla gorilla. Int. J. Primatol. 2007, 28, 441–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Altmann, J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Stoinski, T.S.; Lucas, K.E.; Kuhar, C.W. Effects of age, and group type on social behaviour of male western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in North American Zoos. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 316–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Maestripieri, D.; Schino, G.; Aureli, F.; Troisi, A. A modest proposal: Displacement activities as an indicator of emotion in primates. Anim. Behav. 1992, 44, 967–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Maestripieri, D. Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety-eliciting situations. Ethology 1993, 95, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Schino, G.; Perretta, G.; Taglioni, A.M.; Monaco, V.; Troisi, A. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 1996, 2, 186–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Pullen, P.K. Preliminary comparisons of male/male interactions within bachelor and breeding groups of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 90, 143–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Miller, M.E.; Robinson, C.M.; Margulis, S.W. Behavioral implications of the complete absence of guests on a zoo-housed gorilla troop. Animals 2021, 11, 1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Masman, M.; Scarpace, C.; Liriano, A.; Margolis, S.W. Does the absence of zoo visitors during the COVID-19 pandemic impact gorilla behavior? J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2022, 3, 349–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wells, D.L. A note on the influence of visitors on the behaviour and welfare of zoo-housed gorillas. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 93, 13–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lewis, R.N.; Chang, Y.; Ferguson, A.; Lee, T.; Clifforde, L.; Abeyesinghe, S.M. The effect of visitors on the behavior of zoo-housed western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Zoo Biol. 2020, 39, 283–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Stoinski, T.S.; Jaicks, H.F.; Drayton, L.A. Visitor effects on the behavior of captive western lowland gorillas: The importance of individual differences in examining welfare. Zoo Biol. 2012, 31, 586–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kuhar, C.W. Group differences in captive gorillas’ reaction to large crowds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hashmi, A.; Sullivan, M. The visitor effect in zoo-housed apes: The variable effect on behaviour of visitor number and noise. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2020, 8, 268–281. [Google Scholar]
  24. Lonsdorf, E.V.; Ross, S.R. Socialization and development of behavior. In The Evolution of Primate Societies; Mitani, J.C., Call, J., Kappeler, P.M., Palombit, R.A., Silk, J.B., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2012; pp. 245–268. [Google Scholar]
  25. Carder, G.; Semple, S. Visitor effects on anxiety in two captive groups of western lowland gorillas. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 115, 211–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Gorilla outdoor habitat, night-quarters, and off-exhibit outdoor habitat annex. Dark cross-hatching indicates physical and visual barriers. Yellow areas represent habitat sectors within 40 feet of visitor viewing areas.
Figure 1. Gorilla outdoor habitat, night-quarters, and off-exhibit outdoor habitat annex. Dark cross-hatching indicates physical and visual barriers. Yellow areas represent habitat sectors within 40 feet of visitor viewing areas.
Jzbg 04 00004 g001
Figure 2. The model developed in statistical testing of bachelor aggression predicts a rise in aggression initiated by HJ, the introduced blackback. Adjusted predictions are shown with 95% confidence intervals. The zoo was closed during study months 5 through 10 and during months 15 through 16.
Figure 2. The model developed in statistical testing of bachelor aggression predicts a rise in aggression initiated by HJ, the introduced blackback. Adjusted predictions are shown with 95% confidence intervals. The zoo was closed during study months 5 through 10 and during months 15 through 16.
Jzbg 04 00004 g002
Figure 3. The mean number of occurrences of bachelor aggression during 15 min observation periods in each phase of the visitor study. The zoo was closed to visitors in phases 2 and 4.
Figure 3. The mean number of occurrences of bachelor aggression during 15 min observation periods in each phase of the visitor study. The zoo was closed to visitors in phases 2 and 4.
Jzbg 04 00004 g003
Table 1. Gorilla IDs, sex, birth year, and number of observations in each visitor phase.
Table 1. Gorilla IDs, sex, birth year, and number of observations in each visitor phase.
Bachelor GroupNumber of 15 Minute Focal Observations in Visitor Phases during Which
Visitors are Present (Y) or Absent (0)
IDSexBirth YearY0Y0Y
HM19942927252029
JM19972926252029
HJM20082626252029
Family GroupY0Y0Y
KM19877231251528
EF19767231251528
RF19847231251528
NF19947231251528
AF20206031251528
Table 2. Ethogram of gorilla activities, location, and occurrences of social behavior.
Table 2. Ethogram of gorilla activities, location, and occurrences of social behavior.
Activity/
Behavior
DefinitionCollection
Method
Feed &/or
Forage
Gorilla is engaged in an activity directly related to the acquisition and intake of food or fluid. This category includes gather, carry food items, drag browse, strip bark, ingest, chew, eat and drink. Additionally includes “wearing” browse while in locomotionInstantaneous
Sampling
Explore/
Self-Play
Gorilla physically investigates an area or manipulates an object. Includes digging. Additionally includes “wearing” or carrying burlap when in locomotion. Includes all solitary play activities that are not directed to other gorillas or people such as doing somersaults, turning in circles, patting self, clapping, wading, splashing, object play.Instantaneous
Sampling
LocomoteGorilla moves from one location to another, includes changing stance, walking, crawling, scooting, running, and climbing.Instantaneous
Sampling
Self-DirectGorilla rubs, scratches, picks through fur or examines any part of own body. Includes nose picking as well as poking anus and removing feces.Instantaneous
Sampling
StationaryGorilla is not moving; includes sleep, lie, sit, and stand.Instantaneous
Sampling
Not VisibleGorilla or gorilla’s activity is not visible.Instantaneous
Sampling
Near Visitor ViewingWithin 40 feet of nearest visitor viewing area.Instantaneous
Sampling
Aggressive
Behavior
Includes threats such as tight lips, stiff stance, chest beat, bluff charge, object display and chase as well as contact aggression.All
Occurrences
Affiliative
Behavior
Includes brief contact, sustained contact, sexual contact, social examine/groom and social playAll
Occurrences
Table 3. Mixed-effect negative binomial regression associations between input variables and measures of activity and location close to visitor viewing areas. Variables significantly associated with output measures are shaded in blue.
Table 3. Mixed-effect negative binomial regression associations between input variables and measures of activity and location close to visitor viewing areas. Variables significantly associated with output measures are shaded in blue.
Open to VisitorsStudy MonthAttendanceStart Hour
Activityzp > |z|zp > |z|Zp > |z|zp > |z|
Forage2.320.020−0.250.803−0.340.734−9.390.000
Expl/SlfPlay−0.890.3722.650.0080.530.5941.680.092
Locomote−1.16−0.2460.160.872−0.120.9060.120.906
Self-direct−0.600.550−0.770.442−0.750.4511.890.059
Stationary−2.220.026−1.740.0810.430.6709.260.000
Not Visible−0.120.903−2.660.008−0.690.4932.150.031
Location
Near View−2.660.0082.800.0050.430.668−6.740.000
Table 4. Mixed-effect negative binomial regression of social occurrences. Input variables are shown in column headings. Input variables significantly associated with output measures are shaded in blue.
Table 4. Mixed-effect negative binomial regression of social occurrences. Input variables are shown in column headings. Input variables significantly associated with output measures are shaded in blue.
Open to VisitorsStudy MonthAttendanceStart Hour
Aggression within GroupZp > |z|zp > |z|zp > |z|zp > |z|
Bachelors3.050.0022.580.010−3.210.0011.510.132
Family−0.910.363−1.440.149−2.760.006−0.250.801
Affiliation within Group
Bachelors0.810.4150.460.648−0.260.7931.220.224
Family−2.200.0282.280.028−2.410.0161.900.057
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cox, C.R.; Werner, J.S.; Mead, J.I. Gorilla Activities and Social Behavior: Assessing Changes Associated with Absence of Zoo Visitors, Zoo Attendance, Time of Day and Increasing Maturity. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2023, 4, 39-49. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4010004

AMA Style

Cox CR, Werner JS, Mead JI. Gorilla Activities and Social Behavior: Assessing Changes Associated with Absence of Zoo Visitors, Zoo Attendance, Time of Day and Increasing Maturity. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2023; 4(1):39-49. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4010004

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cox, Cathleen R., Jill S. Werner, and Joan I. Mead. 2023. "Gorilla Activities and Social Behavior: Assessing Changes Associated with Absence of Zoo Visitors, Zoo Attendance, Time of Day and Increasing Maturity" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 4, no. 1: 39-49. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4010004

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop