Next Article in Journal
Effect Sizes for Estimating Differential Item Functioning Influence at the Test Level
Previous Article in Journal / Special Issue
Measuring the Effectiveness of Career Education: A Kindergarten Intervention
 
 
Please note that, as of 22 March 2024, Psych has been renamed to Psychology International and is now published here.
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Nonverbal Intelligence Does Matter for the Perception of Second Language Sounds

by
Georgios P. Georgiou
Department of Languages and Literature, University of Nicosia, Nicosia 2417, Cyprus
Psych 2023, 5(1), 123-132; https://doi.org/10.3390/psych5010012
Submission received: 29 November 2022 / Revised: 19 January 2023 / Accepted: 3 February 2023 / Published: 8 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Prominent Papers in Psych  2021–2023!)

Abstract

:
Although there has been considerable research on the interplay between intelligence and second language (L2) learning, research focusing on the intelligence and L2 speech perception link is limited. The present study aims to fill this gap. The native language of the participants was Cypriot Greek and they spoke English as an L2. The participants completed a forced-choice psychoacoustic test in which they discriminated L2 sound contrasts and a nonverbal intelligence test which measured their nonverbal intelligence capacities. They were divided into two groups according to their performance in the intelligence test, namely, a low IQ and a high IQ group. The results showed that the high IQ group discriminated the majority of the L2 contrasts better than the low IQ group. In addition, the degree of perceived difficulty for most L2 contrasts differed between the two groups. It is concluded that nonverbal intelligence is associated with the discrimination of L2 sounds. This can be explained by the possibility that either intelligence triggers the more efficient functioning of other domains, such as information processing and attention, leading to increased speech perception skills, or that it directly affects the categorization of speech sounds resulting in the development of more robust L2 categories.

1. Introduction

The perception of a large number of speech sounds across world languages is an easy task during early infancy, but this ability gradually declines after the first few months of life [1,2]. As a consequence, adults cannot easily distinguish the sounds of a second language (L2). This is because the learning of all subsequent speech sounds occurs through the lens of the speakers’ first language (L1) (see [3,4]). As speech perception relies on the categorization of L2 sounds to L1 categories [5,6,7], difficulties may arise when two L2 sounds are mapped to the same L1 category. For example, Georgiou [8], who investigated the perception and production of English vowels /ɪ/ and /iː/ by Cypriot Greek speakers of L2 English, reported that the English vowels were mostly classified in terms of Cypriot Greek category /i/, and therefore L2 speakers could not discriminate this contrast well. Thus, single category mapping may deprive listeners of the ability to accurately distinguish non-native sound contrasts since they perceive the two non-native sounds as being acoustically similar to a single L1 sound.
Variation in the acquisition of L2 sounds led researchers to focus on particular characteristics of listeners which may potentially shape their language acquisition skills. They examined the effect of a variety of factors such as linguistic, sociolinguistic, biological, psychological, etc. on L2 speech perception. A series of studies by Flege and colleagues indicated the importance of several sociolinguistic factors such as the age of L2 learning onset, the length of residence in a foreign country, L1–L2 use, the quality and quantity of L2 input, etc. Flege et al. [9] examined two groups of Italian/English bilinguals who shared an identical (low) age of learning in Canada. The first group used their L1 frequently while the second only rarely. The results showed that bilinguals with frequent L1 use had stronger foreign accent than bilinguals with rare L1 use. McKay et al. [10] observed that L1 influence on the perception and production of English stops was stronger for late bilinguals than early bilinguals due to the lack of quality and quantity as regards the input they received. More recent studies investigated other factors such as vocabulary size and proficiency level. Bundgaard-Nielsen et al. [11] and Georgiou et al. [12] highlighted the role of increased vocabulary size in the perception of phonological L2 contrasts. In particular, Georgiou [12] found that adult learners with a larger vocabulary in English could discern L2 contrasts in a more accurate manner, while this ability was reduced for learners with a low vocabulary in their L2. In addition, Georgiou [13] examined the perception of L2 English vowels by Cypriot Greek child learners of English, concluding that the proficiency level of the learners did not affect the discrimination of L2 contrasts. The author attributed this finding to the absence of pronunciation teaching for these populations. Two recent phonetic training paradigms by Georgiou [14,15] have proven successful for the perception of L2 sounds. For example, Cypriot Greek child and adult learners of English improved their discrimination accuracy over L2 contrasts after receiving some sessions of high variability perceptual phonetic training. Similarly, the discrimination of L2 Cypriot Greek vowel contrasts by Egyptian Arabic learners of Greek was improved after the learners received high variability phonetic training.
Less attention has been paid to the effect of cognitive factors, especially intelligence, on L2 acquisition. Most theories recognize intelligence as a higher-order factor of human abilities [16]. Intelligence refers to the set of cognitive abilities used to perform a wide range of tasks [17]. Cattell [18] distinguished between two types of intelligence: the fluid and crystallized. Fluid intelligence is associated with problem-solving skills and the ability to think flexibly and fast. This type of intelligence is independent of individuals’ past experiences. Crystallized intelligence is associated with acculturated knowledge and learnt skills. It is language- and culture-specific and changes over time. Fluid intelligence is assessed through the use of nonverbal measures including visual analogies and matrices based on pattern completion. Among the most popular tests measuring this type of intelligence are the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI [19]) and the Raven Progressive Matrices [20]. Crystallized intelligence is assessed through verbal measures demanding the formation of verbal concepts and information flow [21].
There is a significant body of research focusing on the interface between nonverbal intelligence and L2 learning. Brooks et al. [22] found that nonverbal intelligence predicted the learning of L2 Russian morphology and the generalization of case-marking inflections to new vocabulary. Schmidt and Blumenthal [23] examined the connection between nonverbal intelligence and the lexical abilities of German learners of English. The results showed that learners with above-average intelligence performed better in receptive English lexical knowledge compared to learners with average or below-average intelligence. Similar results were obtained by Woumans et al. [24]. The authors explored the effect of nonverbal cognitive abilities on L2 acquisition, reporting that intelligence was one of the factors that contributed to L2 learning and specifically the acquisition of L2 Dutch vocabulary. Research about the effect of intelligence on L2 speech acquisition is very limited. Rota and Reiterer [25] examined, among other variables, the association between verbal and nonverbal intelligence and phonetic abilities. The findings demonstrated the absence of a connection between verbal and nonverbal intelligence and pronunciation, while a connection was found only between verbal intelligence and phonetic coding ability. Fodor [26] characterized speech perception as a modular system which functions quickly without the need for conscious attention and the direction of higher-order skills such as intelligence and inductive language learning. This is supported by the fact that phonological processing problems can appear in L2 learners with average or above-average intelligence and that the phonetic coding skills of exceptional L2 learners with below-average intelligence can be strong [27]. However, a recent study by Georgiou and Giannakou [28] provides contradictory findings. The authors found that nonverbal intelligence accounted for the discrimination of the majority of non-native contrasts by Standard Modern Greek speakers of L2 English. This shows that intelligence is somehow positively linked to the perception of L2 sounds.
This study aims to investigate the effect of nonverbal intelligence on the discrimination of L2 vowel contrasts. To the best of our knowledge, the link between the perception of L2 speech sounds and intelligence has received only scarce scientific attention. The participants’ native language was Cypriot Greek and they could speak English as an L2. There are considerable differences between the vowel systems of the two languages. Cypriot Greek has a small and simple vowel system consisting of vowels /i e a o u/ [29], while English (Standard Southern British English) has a larger and more complex vowel system consisting of lax vowels /ɪ ʊ e æ ʌ ɒ/ and tense vowels /iː uː ɜː ɔː ɑː/ [30]. Greek speakers encounter difficulties with the discrimination of the majority of English vowel contrasts due to the small size and the limited complexity of their L1 vowel system [31,32] as well as the acoustic-phonetic differences between the L1 and L2 vowel systems [8]. This study followed an experimental protocol using a forced-choice psychoacoustic task for the assessment of speakers’ L2 sound discrimination abilities and a psychometric tool for the assessment of speakers’ nonverbal intelligence. Participants were divided into two groups (a high IQ group and a low IQ group) according to their performance in the nonverbal intelligence test. The ability of speakers with low intelligence to discriminate L2 vowel contrasts was compared with the corresponding ability of speakers with high intelligence. Although some studies in the literature do not favor the positive impact of intelligence on speech perception, it is assumed that at least some contrasts will be discriminated more accurately than others by speakers with high intelligence compared to speakers with low intelligence. This hypothesis is based on the most recent evidence indicating a positive link between L2 contrast discrimination and intelligence [28], and the findings of previous studies supporting a positive effect of nonverbal intelligence on L2 learning [22,23,24].

2. Methodology

2.1. Participants

Sixteen Cypriot Greek speakers (nfemales = 8) with an age range of 20–43 (M = 31.81; SD = 7.94) participated in the study. All participants permanently resided in Cyprus and had a moderate socioeconomic background. They reported knowledge of English at B2/C1 levels and knowledge of some other languages (e.g., French, Italian) at lower levels. In addition, they rated their understanding skills in English as 4.5/5 (SD = 0.63). Their mean English learning onset age was 8.19 years (SD = 1.52), the daily use of English was 1.13 h (SD = 1.31), and the daily input in English was 3.31 h (SD = 2.02) on average. None of them had ever lived for a long time in an English-speaking country. All participants reported healthy vision and hearing and the absence of any cognitive or language disorders. The participants were divided into two groups according to their intelligence capacities (i.e., low IQ/high IQ) after conducting a median split on the raw scores of the nonverbal intelligence test (Mdn = 52.5) (see Section 2.3.2, which describes the procedure of the intelligence test). The low IQ group had an average intelligence raw score of 46.75 (SD = 4.03), while the high IQ group had an average intelligence raw score of 54.88 (SD = 1.25); according to the results of independent t-tests, there were statistically significant differences between the scores of the two groups (t = 5.45, df = 14, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences between the two IQ groups in terms of English learning onset age (t = 0.48, df = 14, p = 0.64), daily use of English (t = 0.37, df = 14, p = 0.72), daily input in English (t = 0.36, df = 14, p = 0.73), or English understanding skills (t = −0.67, df = 14, p = 0.12); this shows that the aforementioned characteristics were similar across the two groups.

2.2. Stimuli

The stimuli of the study consisted of the 11 English monophthongs embedded in monosyllabic /hVd/ words, which were part of the carrier phrase “They say <word> now”. These words were hid, heed, head, herd, had, hard, hud, hod, hoard, who’d, and hood, representing English vowels /ɪ iː e ɜː æ ɑː ʌ ɒ ɔː uː ʊ/ respectively. Two adult female English (Standard Southern British English) native speakers were recruited for the production of the stimuli. The phrases were presented on paper using Standard British English orthography. The speakers were asked to produce the phrases as naturally as possible as if speaking to a friend and their output was recorded at a 44.1 kHz sampling rate using a professional audio recorder. The data were normalized for peak intensity using Praat [33].

2.3. Procedure

2.3.1. Discrimination Test

All participants completed a forced-choice AX discrimination test using Praat. The experiments took place in quiet rooms and participants did not have contact with each other. The stimuli were grouped into six “different” pairs and six “same” pairs. The “different” pairs included the following six English contrasts: /ɪ/–/iː/, /iː/–/e/, /ɑː/–/ʌ/, /æ/–/ɑː/, /ɔː/–/ɒ/, and /uː/–/ʊ/. Each of the six “different” conditions contained eight repetitions of the contrastive vowels (4 AB and 4 BA types). Similarly, each of the six “same” conditions contained eight repetitions of the contrastive vowels (4 AA and 4 BB types). Each participant discriminated a total number of 96 items (6 contrasts × 2 conditions × 8 repetitions), which were presented in random order. The stimulus pairs always included recordings from different speakers to avoid solely auditory decisions. Participants sat in front of a PC monitor, maintaining a consistent distance from it. They listened to the stimuli through a set of headphones connected to the PC and were asked to select whether the pair tokens were acoustically the same or different by clicking on the relevant script label (“same” vs. “different”). The interstimulus interval was set at 300 m.s. There was no option to repeat the stimuli and no feedback was given on the participants’ responses. In addition, there was an optional five-minute break at the midpoint. Prior to the main experiment, participants completed a familiarization test with four items on the script to ensure that they understood the instructions of the test. Moreover, before the experiment, it was ensured that participants knew the target words. The test lasted about 15 min for each participant.

2.3.2. Nonverbal Intelligence Test

Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices test [20] was used to measure participants’ nonverbal intelligence. This test measures abstract reasoning and fluid intelligence and is considered an indicator of general human intelligence [34]. The test was provided in written form and completed individually in quiet rooms. There were 60 black and white items in 5 sets (e.g., A to E) of 12 items (e.g., A1 to A12). The items within a set progressively became more complex, so the information demanded a higher degree of cognitive capacity to encode and analyze. The test was completed within 30–45 min. During the test, there were no breaks and no feedback was given to the participants. The performance of the participants was measured using the raw scores (out of 60).

2.4. Statistical Analysis

A binomial logistic mixed-effects model [35] was fitted in R [36] as the dependent variable, namely, response, was dichotomous (correct/incorrect). Contrast (/ɪ/–/iː/, /iː/–/e/, /ɑː/–/ʌ/, /æ/–/ɑː/, /ɔː/–/ɒ/, /uː/–/ʊ/), groupIQ (low/high), and contrast × groupIQ were the fixed factors, while subject was the random factor. The pairwise comparisons were conducted using the emmeans package [37] and the Tukey method.

3. Results

The results showed that all contrasts but /iː/–/e/ could be discriminated better by the high IQ than the low IQ group. For the low IQ group, all contrasts but /iː/–/e/ were discriminated below chance (i.e., 50%). In addition, /iː/–/e/ was the most accurate contrast, followed by /ɔː/–/ɒ/, /ɑː/–/ʌ/, /uː/–/ʊ/, /ɪ/–/iː/, and /æ/–/ɑː/. For the high IQ group, only one contrast was discriminated below chance, namely, /ɑː/–/ʌ/. Moreover, similarly to the low IQ group, /iː/–/e/ was the most accurate contrast, followed by /ɔː/–/ɒ/, /ɪ/–/iː/, /uː/–/ʊ/, /æ/–/ɑː/, and /ɑː/–/ʌ/, which was the least accurate one. It can be observed that the discrimination of some contrasts differed between the two IQ groups. Figure 1 illustrates the correct discrimination percentages and SDs for all contrasts in the low and high IQ groups. Figure 2 and Figure 3 illustrate the discrimination of contrasts by each subject in the low and high IQ groups.
A binomial mixed-effects model indicated significant differences only between contrast /ɔː/–/ɒ/ and contrast /æ/–/ɑː/ (the Intercept term) (β = 1.16, SE = 0.41, z = 2.85, p = 0.004) for the high IQ group. In addition, a significant effect of group.IQ was observed (β = −1.88, SE = 0.47, z = −4, p < 0.001), showing that the low IQ group discriminated /æ/–/ɑː/ less accurately than the high IQ group. The contrast × group.IQ interaction was significant, but only for /ɑː/–/ʌ/ (β = −1.4, SE = 0.56, z = 2.49, p = 0.013), indicating that the low IQ group discriminated this contrast more accurately than /æ/–/ɑː/ in comparison to the high IQ group. The results of the model are shown in Table 1.
Pairwise comparisons of groupIQ × contrast were used to compare the discrimination of particular vowel contrasts between the low IQ and the high IQ groups. The results revealed that there were significant differences between the low IQ and the high IQ groups for /æ/–/ɑː/ (β = 1.88, SE = 0.47, z = 4, p = 0.004), /ɪ/–/iː/ (β = 2.29, SE = 0.47, z = 4.92, p < 0.001), /ɔː/–/ɒ/ (β = 1.95, SE = 0.46, z = 4.25, p = 0.001), and /uː/–/ʊ/ (β = 1.57, SE = 0.44, z = 3.58, p = 0.02). In sum, the results indicate that the majority of L2 contrasts were discriminated better by the high IQ group compared to the low IQ group. The results of pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 2.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to examine the effect of nonverbal intelligence on the discrimination of L2 vowel contrasts. The participants were Cypriot Greek speakers of L2 English, who completed a forced-choice AX discrimination test using a computer-based Praat script and a nonverbal intelligence test on paper. The analysis of data was conducted with the use of a binomial mixed-effects model and pairwise comparisons in R statistical software. According to the results, the high IQ group demonstrated a better performance than the low IQ group for most L2 contrasts. Specifically, English /ɪ/–/iː/, /æ/–/ɑː/, /ɔː/–/ɒ/, and /uː/–/ʊ/ contrasts were discriminated more accurately by the high IQ group. Therefore, the discrimination of L2 vowel contrasts was affected by the intelligence of the L2 speakers. This corroborates the findings of Georgiou and Giannakou [23] who observed such an effect in the discrimination of L2 English contrasts by Standard Modern Greek speakers. However, the results of this study are inconsistent with other findings, which show no connection between nonverbal intelligence and phonetic abilities (e.g., [25]). In addition, it has been proposed that the speech perception mechanism is not associated with intelligence, since perception is instantaneous and fast and does not need the guidance of a cognitive process [26].
The findings of this study may have emerged from an indirect effect of nonverbal intelligence on L2 speech perception. That is, intelligence may have triggered the more efficient functioning of other domains leading to better speech perception skills. For example, intelligence is related to an increased ability to process and learn new information (see [38]). So, intelligent L2 speakers have more advanced processing and learning skills, which can also be employed in speech perception. In addition, intelligence signals better attention control [39], which is crucial for L2 phonological learning. Individuals with advanced attention control skills can extract relevant acoustic information more easily during speech processing, thereby acquiring L2 speech sounds more accurately [40]. A more direct effect of intelligence on L2 speech perception is not precluded. It might be that speech perception is something more than just a simple realization of acoustic events. It has been supported that humans develop speech categories in their minds after extracting acoustic information found in the speech signal [8,41]. Thus, perceived speech features are categorized to particular speech categories. Perhaps, intelligence aids the connection between perceived acoustic information and the development of speech categories resulting in the activation of more robust L2 phonetic categories.
The results indicated that the majority of L2 contrasts (four out of six) were discriminated better by the high IQ group compared to the low IQ group. Nevertheless, the discrimination of two contrasts, that is /iː/–/e/ and /ɑː/–/ʌ/, did not differ between the two groups. The nonsignificant differences in the discrimination of /iː/–/e/ can be explained by the fact that this contrast is an easy distinction for the L2 speakers since both sounds do not acoustically overlap (i.e., they are associated with two different categories of their L1). In terms of the Universal Perceptual Model [7,8], this contrast would be considered nonoverlapping and therefore speakers would not have difficulties in discriminating between the contrast members. It seems that the between-group differences mostly appear in L2 contrasts considered difficult for these speakers. Another important finding is that most contrasts (except /iː/–/e/ and /ɔː/–/ɒ/) exerted a different degree of difficulty for the individuals in each group. For example, /æ/–/ɑː/ was the most difficult contrast for the low IQ group, while /ɑː/–/ʌ/was the most difficult contrast for the high IQ group. The perceived difficulty of particular speech contrasts by the groups differing in intelligence capacities is a matter which requires further investigation.
The study revealed that intelligence is somehow linked to L2 speech perception. However, this is a preliminary finding considering that research on this topic is very limited in the literature. Future studies can employ larger samples and populations with different L1s to examine how L2 speech perception relates to intelligence. It is also of paramount importance to use different tools to measure participants’ intelligence. Although there was an attempt to control several factors in this study such as proficiency level, age of L2 English learning onset, L2 use, etc. there might be other factors that potentially affect the results such as phonological short-term memory, attention, motivation toward the learning of the L2, lexical access time, etc. These factors can be considered in future studies.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Department of Languages and Literature at the University of Nicosia.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants were informed about the goals of the study and gave their written consent for their participation in the experiments.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The study was conducted with the support of the Cyprus Linguistics and Humanities Research group, an official research center at the University of Nicosia. We would like to thank all the participants for their willingness to participate in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kuhl, P.K.; Stevens, E.; Hayashi, A.; Deguchi, T.; Kiritani, S.; Iverson, P. Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, F13–F21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Werker, J.F.; Tees, R.C. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behav. Dev. 1984, 7, 49–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Georgiou, G.P.; Perfilieva, N.; Denisenko, V.; Novospasskaya, N. Perceptual realization of Greek consonants by Russian monolingual speakers. Speech Commun. 2020, 125, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Melnik-Leroy, G.A.; Turnbull, R.; Peperkamp, S. On the relationship between perception and production of L2 sounds: Evidence from Anglophones’ processing of the French/u/–/y/contrast. Second Lang. Res. 2022, 38, 581–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Best, C.T.; Tyler, M. Non-native and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege; Bohn, O.-S., Munro, M.J., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 13–34. [Google Scholar]
  6. Flege, J.E. Assessing constraints on second-language segmental production and perception. In Phonetics and Phonology in Language Comprehension and Production; Schiller, O., Meyer, A.S., Eds.; Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2003; pp. 319–355. [Google Scholar]
  7. Georgiou, G.P. Toward a new model for speech perception: The Universal Perceptual Model (UPM) of second language. Cogn. Process. 2021, 22, 277–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Georgiou, G.P. The Acquisition of /ɪ/–/iː/ is challenging: Perceptual and production evidence from Cypriot Greek speakers of English. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Flege, J.E.; Frieda, E.M.; Nozawa, T. Amount of native-language (L1) use affects the pronunciation of an L2. J. Phon. 1997, 25, 169–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. MacKay, I.R.A.; Meador, D.; Flege, J.E. The identification of English consonants by native speakers of Italian. Phonetica 2001, 58, 103–125. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bundgaard-Nielsen, R.L.; Best, C.T.; Tyler, M.D. Vocabulary size is associated with second-language vowel perception performance in adult learners. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2011, 33, 433–461. [Google Scholar]
  12. Georgiou, G.P.; Perfilieva, N.; Tenizi, M. Vocabulary size leads to a better attunement to L2 phonetic differences: Clues from Russian learners of English. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2020, 16, 382–398. [Google Scholar]
  13. Georgiou, G.P. ‘Bit’ and ‘beat’ are heard as the same: Mapping the vowel perceptual patterns of Greek-English bilingual children. Lang. Sci. 2019, 72, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  14. Georgiou, G.P. Effects of Phonetic Training on the Discrimination of Second Language Sounds by Learners with Naturalistic Access to the Second Language. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2021, 50, 707–721. [Google Scholar]
  15. Georgiou, G.P. The Impact of Auditory Perceptual Training on the Perception and Production of English Vowels by Cypriot Greek Children and Adults. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2022, 18, 379–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carroll, J.B. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies (No. 1); Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ellis, R. The Study of Second Language Acquisition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University: Oxford, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cattell, R.B. Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  19. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI); Pearson Assessment: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  20. Raven, J.; Raven, J.C.; Court, H.H. Raven Manual: Section 3. Standard Progressive Matrices; Oxford Psychologists Press, Ltd.: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  21. García-Navarro, C.; de Ory, S.J.; Higueras, C.V.; Zamora, B.; Prieto, L.; Ramos, J.T.; Navarro, M.L.; Escosa-García, L.; Jurado-Barba, R.; Falcón, D.; et al. Significant differences between verbal and non-verbal intellectual scales on a perinatally HIV-infected cohort: From pediatrics to young adults. Heliyon 2020, 6, e03600. [Google Scholar]
  22. Brooks, P.J.; Kwoka, N.; Kempe, V. Distributional effects and individual differences in L2 morphology learning. Lang. Learn. 2017, 67, 171–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schmidt, K.; Blumenthal, Y. Early immersion education: L2 vocabulary acquisition and the role of non-verbal intelligence. In Cognition and Second Language Acquisition: Studies on Pre-School, Primary School and Secondary School Children; Piske, T., Steinlen, A., Eds.; Narr Francke Attempto: Tübingen, Germany, 2022; pp. 37–58. [Google Scholar]
  24. Woumans, E.; Ameloot, S.; Keuleers, E.; Van Assche, E. The relationship between second language acquisition and nonverbal cognitive abilities. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2019, 148, 1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Rota, G.; Reiterer, S. Cognitive aspects of pronunciation talent. Lang. Talent. Brain Act. 2009, 1, 67–96. [Google Scholar]
  26. Fodor, J. Modularity of Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  27. Sparks, R.L.; Humbach, N.; Patton, J.O.N.; Ganschow, L. Subcomponents of second-language aptitude and second-language proficiency. Mod. Lang. J. 2011, 95, 253–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Georgiou, G.P.; Giannakou, A. Discrimination of L2 vowel contrasts and the role of phonological short-term memory and nonverbal intelligence. 2023; submitted. [Google Scholar]
  29. Georgiou, G.P.; Themistocleous, C. Vowel Learning in Diglossic Settings: Evidence from Arabic-Greek Learners. Int. J. Biling. 2021, 25, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Deterding, D. The formants of monophthong vowels in Standard Southern British English pronunciation. J. Int. Phon. Assoc. 1997, 27, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hacquard, V.; Walter, M.A.; Marantz, A. The effects of inventory on vowel perception in French and Spanish: An MEG study. Brain Lang. 2007, 100, 295–300. [Google Scholar]
  32. Iverson, P.; Evans, B.G. Learning English vowels with different first-language vowel systems: Perception of formant targets, formant movement, and duration. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007, 122, 2842–2854. [Google Scholar]
  33. Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. 2022. Available online: http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ (accessed on 31 October 2022).
  34. Bilker, W.B.; Hansen, J.A.; Brensinger, C.M.; Richard, J.; Gur, R.E.; Gur, R.C. Development of abbreviated nine-item forms of the Raven’s standard progressive matrices test. Assess 2012, 19, 354–369. [Google Scholar]
  35. Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S.; Christensen, R.H.B.; Singmann, H.; Dai, B.; Scheipl, F.; Grothendieck, G. Package ‘lme4’. Cran R. 2022. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html (accessed on 31 October 2022).
  36. Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 31 October 2022).
  37. Lenth, R.; Singmann, H.; Love, J.; Buerkner, P.; Herve, M. Package ‘emmeans’. Cran R. 2022. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.html (accessed on 31 October 2022).
  38. Sternberg, R.J. Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1985, 49, 607–627. [Google Scholar]
  39. Sweller, J. Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cogn. Sci. 1988, 12, 257–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Darcy, I.; Park, H.; Yang, C.-L. Individual differences in L2 acquisition of English phonology: The relation between cognitive abilities and phonological processing. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 40, 63–72. [Google Scholar]
  41. Flege, J.E.; Bohn, O.S. The revised speech learning model (SLM-r). In Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021; pp. 3–83. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Discrimination of the English vowel contrasts by the low and the high IQ groups.
Figure 1. Discrimination of the English vowel contrasts by the low and the high IQ groups.
Psych 05 00012 g001
Figure 2. Discrimination of contrasts by each subject in the low IQ group.
Figure 2. Discrimination of contrasts by each subject in the low IQ group.
Psych 05 00012 g002
Figure 3. Discrimination of contrasts by each subject in the high IQ group.
Figure 3. Discrimination of contrasts by each subject in the high IQ group.
Psych 05 00012 g003
Table 1. Results of the binomial mixed-effects model (Intercept: groupIQ high, contrast /æ/–/ɑː/).
Table 1. Results of the binomial mixed-effects model (Intercept: groupIQ high, contrast /æ/–/ɑː/).
Fixed Effects:
EstimateStd. Errorz-Valuep-Value
(Intercept)0.262860.295790.8890.37419
contrast /iː/–/e/18.34617170.669710.1070.91440
contrast /ɪ/–/iː/0.719230.383781.8740.06092
contrast /ɔː/–/ɒ/1.164100.408582.8490.00438 **
contrast /ɑː/–/ʌ/−0.527650.36523−1.4450.14855
contrast /uː/–/ʊ/0.342870.370960.9240.35535
groupIQ low−1.876320.46935−3.9986.40E-05 ***
contrast /iː/–/e/:groupIQ low−13.24089170.67594−0.0780.93816
contrast /ɪ/–/iː/:groupIQ low−0.414150.59285−0.6990.48482
contrast /ɔː/–/ɒ/:groupIQ low−0.076690.58861−0.1300.89633
contrast /ɑː/–/ʌ/:groupIQ low1.403240.563542.4900.01277 *
contrast /uː/–/ʊ/:groupIQ low0.305170.572570.5330.59405
Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘’ 1.
Table 2. Pairwise comparisons for groupIQ:contrast.
Table 2. Pairwise comparisons for groupIQ:contrast.
groupIQ:contrastEstimateStd. Errorz-Valuep-Value
high /æ/–/ɑː/- low /æ/–/ɑː/1.87600.4693.9980.0037 **
high /iː/–/e/- low /iː/–/e/15.117170.6750.0891.0000
high /ɪ/–/iː/- low /ɪ/–/iː/2.29000.4654.9210.0001 ***
high /ɔː/–/ɒ/- low /ɔː/–/ɒ/1.95300.4604.2480.0013 **
high /ɑː/–/ʌ/- low /ɑː/–/ʌ/0.47300.4261.1120.9943
high /uː/–/ʊ/- low /uː/–/ʊ/1.57100.4393.5820.0178 *
Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘’ 1.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Georgiou, G.P. Nonverbal Intelligence Does Matter for the Perception of Second Language Sounds. Psych 2023, 5, 123-132. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych5010012

AMA Style

Georgiou GP. Nonverbal Intelligence Does Matter for the Perception of Second Language Sounds. Psych. 2023; 5(1):123-132. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych5010012

Chicago/Turabian Style

Georgiou, Georgios P. 2023. "Nonverbal Intelligence Does Matter for the Perception of Second Language Sounds" Psych 5, no. 1: 123-132. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych5010012

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop