Next Article in Journal
Effects of an Asthma Education Camp Program on Quality of Life and Asthma Control among Thai Children with Asthma: A Quasi-Experimental Study
Next Article in Special Issue
The Relationship between Physiotherapist and Patient: A Qualitative Study on Physiotherapists’ Representations on This Theme
Previous Article in Journal
Formative Assessment of Diagnostic Testing in Family Medicine with Comprehensive MCQ Followed by Certainty-Based Mark
Previous Article in Special Issue
Giving Meaning to Non-Communicable Illness: Mixed-Method Research on Sense of Grip on Disease (SoGoD)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effectiveness of Expressive Writing in Kidney Transplanted Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial Study

1
Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, Via degli Apuli 1, Sapienza University, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of General Surgery “Paride Stefanini”, Viale del Policlinico 155, Sapienza University, 00161 Rome, Italy
3
Department of Clinical Medicine, Viale dell’Università 37, Sapienza University, 00185 Rome, Italy
4
Department of Anaesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Via della Pineta Sacchetti 217, Fondazione Policlinico Universitario A. Gemelli IRCCS, 00168 Rome, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2022, 10(8), 1559; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10081559
Submission received: 11 May 2022 / Revised: 16 July 2022 / Accepted: 13 August 2022 / Published: 17 August 2022

Abstract

:
The present study aimed to assess the effectiveness of an expressive writing (EW) intervention on psychological and physiological variables after kidney transplant. The final sample of 26 were randomly assigned to an expressive writing group (EWG) and control group (CG). Outcomes were focused on depression, anxiety, alexithymia, empathy, resilience, locus of control, creatinine, CDK-EPI, and azotemia. Depressive symptoms and alexithymia levels decreased in the EWG, with better adherence. Resilience declined over time in both groups. The EWG showed a significantly higher CDK-EPI, indicating better renal functioning. EW seems an effective intervention to improve the psychological health of transplanted patients, with a possible effect on renal functioning. These findings open the possibility of planning brief psychological interventions aimed at processing emotional involvement, in order to increase adherence, the acceptance of the organ, and savings in healthcare costs.

1. Introduction

Kidney transplantation is a replacement procedure for chronic kidney disease that is intended to improve the patient’s quality of life. However, the experience may be a strongly emotional one, accompanied by difficulty in accepting the new organ, lifestyle changes, the side-effects associated with the procedure, anxiety, depression, the redefinition of one’s body, and a change in social and family roles [1,2,3,4,5]. Hence, the associated stress may result in an adverse post-operative course, particularly in terms of compliance to the follow-up process, medical prescriptions, and the consequential risk of rejection [6].
Adherence is described as the correct management, conforming to the prescribed anti-rejection drug (immunosuppressive) therapy, maintaining regularity in visiting the pre-established check-ups at the transplant center, and prompt reporting of any complications [4,7]. In kidney transplanted patients, adherence to immunosuppressive therapy is of importance, to reduce the risk of rejection [8]. Several studies have highlighted the role of adherence to treatment on the postoperative course in different transplantation procedures [9], with special emphasis on the association of lower adherence levels with an increase in healthcare costs [10].
Younger age, depression and anxiety, using avoidance as an adaptation strategy, and the inability to communicate emotions and emotional dysregulation represent some of the factors predisposing to poor medication adherence in transplant patients [3,11,12].
Expressive writing seems to be a suitable intervention in transplanted patients, as previously shown by Possemato et al. [13], with its effects on post-traumatic stress symptoms and quality of life. This technique has also been proven effective in patients with chronic diseases and cancer, to reduce and manage psychophysical symptoms [14,15]. The externalization and processing of stressful events related to the disease probably encourage the ability to express emotions, thereby reducing the associated negative thoughts and feelings [16].
The present study aimed to evaluate the effect of expressive writing on psychological variables and on the biological markers of renal function after transplant. It was hypothesized that the expressive writing intervention would be effective in improving psychological evaluation, enabling better adherence, lowering the risk of organ rejection, and decreasing health expenditure.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

Parallel-group randomized trials were applied. The study complied with the guidelines outlined under the Consolidation Standards of Reporting Trials (Extension of the CONSORT 2010 Statement) checklist. The study involved two parallel groups: experimental and control. The samples were composed using a double-blind randomized controlled trial (RCT). The trial was registered on https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/ Reg. no. NCT04486417 with receipt on 12 November 2019 (first posted 29 October 2019).
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, Sapienza University of Rome (approval number 23/2017). Patients who agreed to participate signed a written consent form obtained for experimentation with human subjects, in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Participants

Fifty-seven renal-transplant patients at the Kidney Transplant Center, Policlinico Umberto I, “Sapienza” University of Rome, were contacted between March 2018 and March 2019. Participants aged above 18 years of age with sufficient ability to read, comprehend, and write were considered for the study. Those with severe psychopathological conditions already encountered during the pre-transplant psychiatric evaluation and those with cognitive impairment were excluded. The first contact was with the clinicians of the hospital, who explained to the consulted patients the aim of the research and the clinical aspects of the study, and then established the inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Out of the 57 patients contacted, 35 were recruited initially, of which 2 had an acute rejection before the randomization, and 9 others dropped out, leaving a final sample of 26. The sample size was based on an a priori power analysis, which considered the effect reported in a previous study on similar outcomes, such us depression [17], with a significance level of α = 0.05, β = 0.02 and power (1-β) = 80%. This suggested a sample size of 10 participants (5 per group), therefore the number of participants tested in the present study can be considered adequate. The participants were blindly randomly assigned to one of the two groups by an independent researcher. All participants were randomized into two groups; the random allocation of the participants was performed in a double-blind manner, the reference researcher generated the random allocation sequence, asking the participants to extract from a box a closed sheet, in which the kind of group was specified. The sheets were filled by a clinician and placed in the box. Neither the researcher nor the participant knew a priori the contents of the extracted sheets. Participants were not informed about the treatment tasks. The size of the randomization block was established as reaching 5 consecutive extractions with the same group; at which point, the group was eliminated from the box, and the extraction continued with only one sheet. The two groups were the expressive writing group (EWG) and the control group (CG).

2.3. The Writing Interventions

The expressive writing process consisted of writing on 3 different topics, 20-min every day for 3 consecutive days. On day 1, the EWG participants were asked to write about their deepest emotions, thoughts, and concerns related to the disease and the transplant itself, followed by an account of the most difficult experience of their life and its association with the onset of the kidney disease on day 2, and finally about their future expectations after the operation on day 3. Whereas the CG participants were asked to write about an assigned neutral theme (describe an object in the room), in the most objective way possible, without mentioning emotions or thoughts related to it. The trial was stopped at the end of the 20 min.

2.4. Procedure

The study comprised 3 different phases:
(1) Pre-operative (T0): corresponding to the hospitalization day, just 2 h before the transplantation.
(2) Psychological intervention (IP): Five days after the transplant, all participants were asked to choose one of the two envelopes, containing the expressive or neutral writing tasks, provided by the researcher, to randomly allocate the writing task. The researcher remained available for clarifications, after which the patient was left alone for 20 min to complete the writing task. The same procedure was applied for the next 2 days for both groups.
(3) Post-operative phase consisting of 2 sub-phases: (a) T1-phase, the day of discharge; (b) T2-phase, the 3rd post-operative month.

2.5. Outcome Measures

All participants were invited to complete the following questionnaires at times T0, T1, and T2:
Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [18]; State Trait Anxiety Inventory I and II (STAI-Y1–STAI-Y2) [19]; Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20) [20]; Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [21]; Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC) [22]; Health Locus of Control scale (HLC) [23].
To evaluate the level of adherence, each participant answered the following question: “Have you ever forgotten to take the medications provided for your treatment plan?” with the dichotomous answer yes/no, in both T0 and T2 phases.
The participants’ renal function at T0, T1, and 3 months after transplant (T2) was evaluated based on creatinine, CDK-EPI (creatinine levels calculated based on age and gender), and azotemia levels, according to the immunosuppressive protocol preferred by the transplant center.
Moreover, during the T0 phase, socio-demographic data and detailed features of the present disease (disease onset, duration and type of dialysis, comorbidities, previous or current psychopathology, schooling, and occupation) for all participants were collected. Afterward, a short interview was conducted related to the medical expenditure in the last year seeking information about the frequency of visits to the general practitioner, hospitalizations and days of hospitalization, admission to the emergency room, drugs stipulated by the therapeutic plan, and consumption of psychotropic drugs, at T0 and T2.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using STATISTICA 8.0 StatSoft, Inc. 1994-2007 Tulsa, OK. To evaluate the effectiveness of the expressive writing intervention, a repeated measures (Fisher’s F) ANOVA and planned comparisons on the psychological variables, adherence evaluation, renal function indices, and health expenditure between the two randomized groups was carried out. A Chi-square test (χ2) was used to test the hypotheses with the dichotomous variable of adherence and with patients’ characteristics.

3. Results

Fifty-seven patients were approached for the study, out of which 11 (19%) refused to participate and 11 (19%) were discounted according to the exclusion criteria, resulting in 35 patients being enrolled in the study. The final sample was composed of 26 participants with a mean age of 48.6 ± 14.4 years (Table 1). Three patients received an organ from living donors, while the remaining (n = 23) received a cadaveric transplant (Figure 1). All the participants were Caucasian and Italian citizens. The mean and standard deviation values for each analyzed variable are presented in Table 2. Significant scheduled comparisons for psychological and biological variables are shown in Table 3. The two groups, the EWG (n = 16) and the CG (n = 10), were statistically similar in terms of age distribution (F (1,22) = 0.6, p = 0.45) and pre-operative comorbidities, such as diabetes, hypertension and heart disease (Type 1 diabetes: χ2 (1) = 0.3, p = 0.61 Type 2 diabetes: χ2 (1) = 0.3, p = 0.61 hypertension: χ2 (1) = 0.1; p = 0.80 heart disease: χ2 (1) = 0.3, p = 0.61) (Table 1). The main results of the study are summarized in Figure 2.

3.1. Psychological Variables

The effect of the time factor was for BDI-II (T1 < T0), STAI-Y1 (T1 < T0), STAI-Y2 (T1 < T0), IRI-PT (T1 < T0), IRI-PD (T1 < T0), and CD-RISC-control (T2 < T1) (Table 3).
At T1, the planned comparisons showed that the EWG had a significant reduction in depressive symptoms on BDI-II and levels of alexithymia, as seen in TAS-20-F2. Likewise, both groups had significantly lower empathy levels in the “Perspective Thinking” subscale and “Personal Distress” subscale. Interestingly, at T2, only the EWG had increased empathy levels on the “Personal Distress” subscale. Lastly, at T2, there was a significant reduction in the resilience levels, as measured on the CD-RISC-control for both groups.

3.2. Adherence Variables

The distribution of the participants who responded to the item “Have you ever forgotten to take the medications provided for your treatment plan in the last 3 months?” for the two groups was as follows: the EWG: (Yes: n = 9 at T0, n = 4 at T2; No: n = 7 at T0, n = 12 at T2); CG (Yes: n = 7 at T0, n = 2 at T2; No: n = 3, n = 7 at T2) (Table 3). The distribution of the EWG at T2 was significantly different compared to an equal-distribution (Yes: n = 4, No: n = 12; χ2 (1) = 4.0, p = 0.045).

3.3. Biological Variables

The time factor was significant for CDK-EPI (T1 > T0; T2 > T1), creatinine (T1 < T0; T2 < T1), and azotemia (T2 < T1) (Table 3). A significant effect of group was seen on azotemia levels, with lower levels in the EWG than the CG. This was evident from the planned comparisons, where the EWG at T1 showed a significant decrease in azotemia, creatinine levels, and an increase in the CDK-EPI levels compared to the T0 phase. Similarly, at T1, the CG showed a decrease in creatinine levels and an increase in CDK-EPI levels compared to at T0.
At T1 the EWG showed a higher level of CDK-EPI levels compared to the control group. Furthermore, at T1, the EWG had lower azotemia levels than the CG. In the T2 vs. T1 comparison, the EWG had significantly reduced azotemia levels, and the CG also showed a decrease in azotemia and creatinine levels with increased CDK-EPI levels.
In addition, the EWG and the CG were equally distributed regarding post-operative complications, such as drug toxicity (at T1: χ2 (1) = 0.0, p = 1.000); at T2: χ2 (1) = 1.7, p = 0.187) and cytomegalovirus (at T1: χ2 (1) = 0.0, p = 1.000; at T2: χ2 (1) = 1.2, p = 0.281).

3.4. Health Expenditure

The planned comparisons at T0 showed a significantly greater number of medical visits for the EWG (p = 0.030). There were no significant planned comparisons regarding the number of hospitalizations, number of recovery days, and number of access to emergency room.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the psychological and biological effects of expressive writing on renal transplant patients, in order to develop methods that can be used to mitigate the anxiety and depression associated with such major surgical procedures and ensure patient’s adherence, to increase the acceptance of transplanted kidney.
A main finding of the present study was that the depressive symptoms and alexithymia (in the subscale “describe your emotions”) decreased only in the expressive writing group at T1. Several previous studies have evaluated the effect of a psychological intervention on depressive symptoms in transplanted patients, with a significant decline in depressive symptoms [24], specifically in those who performed an expressive writing treatment [25,26,27]. Regarding alexithymia, the results are debatable, with studies focusing on the role of alexithymia as a moderator of the effects of expressive writing with positive or negative outcomes [28,29], rather than on the effect of expressive writing on alexithymia levels. However, other studies described forms of secondary alexithymia as a response to a psychological distress of an organic disease, rather than a stable personality trait (primary alexithymia) [30]. In the present study, an effect of expressive writing on the ability to describe emotions was observed, which may be explained by the fact that the expressive writing associated with the expectation related to the kidney transplantation may have offered the patients an opportunity to attenuate a secondary alexithymia that may have arisen during the pre-transplantation hemodialysis period [30].
A second finding of the present study was that expressive writing was associated with an increase of adherence to the pharmacological therapy at 3 months after transplantation and with an improvement of renal function, where the CDK-EPI level on the discharge day (T1) in the expressive writing group was significantly higher compared to the control group. Likewise, the azotemia levels also decreased significantly in the patients who performed the expressive writing intervention at T1. Although both groups had significantly reduced creatinine levels at T1, the azotemia and CDK-EPI concentrations suggest a more pronounced improvement of renal function in the patients with the expressive intervention. These results are also supported by the fact that the two groups were quite homogeneous, both for pre-operative comorbidities (diabetes, hypertension and heart disease) and for post-operative complications (cytomegalovirus and drug toxicity). This finding suggests that the better renal function in the expressive writing group was not due to pre-existing medical conditions or post-operative complications. The results of the present study suggest a possible effect of the expressive writing on the depressive symptoms and emotional state in patients undergoing kidney transplantation, confirming the efficacy of expressive writing on the ability to elaborate potential trauma due to critical events in life, such as cancer or related to pregnancy [25,31,32,33,34]. Moreover, the association between the presence of an expressive writing intervention and a higher adherence to the pharmacological treatment with greater kidney functionality suggests the possibility that the expressive writing could also have had an effect, probably mediated by the psychological state and adherence to the immunosuppressive therapy, on the biological acceptance of the organ.
The association between the presence of an expressive writing intervention and kidney functionality was also suggested by a recent study, which described the effect of expressive writing on the immune system [16]. In a previous study on patients receiving the hepatitis B vaccine, a significant increase in antibodies against hepatitis B was found in patients who underwent a writing expressive intervention compared to the untreated group, as well as an increase in CD-4 type T-lymphocytes the day after the writing task [35]. CD-4 type T-lymphocytes, also known as T-helper cells, seem to have a role in the immune response during the acceptance of a new transplanted organ [36]. Another previous study highlighted the efficacy of a psychological intervention in modifying the body’s inflammatory state, as significantly reduced plasma chemokine concentration in patients with moderate and mild depressive symptoms was found [37]. Chemokines and cytokines act by regulating the function of T lymphocytes, which exert a direct cytotoxic effect through the release of TNF (tumor necrosis factor), which eventually can lead to ischemia in the transplanted tissues and subsequent rejection [38,39]. An intriguing hypothesis to explain these findings could be that expressive writing, by improving psychological state, could indirectly affect the peripheral inflammatory state and consequently increase the probability of the acceptance of the transplanted organ.
The present study also showed that, at T2 (3 months), renal functioning (creatinine and CDK-EPI) remained constant in the EWG and was improved in the CG, while azotemia decreased in both groups. These results indicate that after 3 months from the expressive writing intervention, both groups tended to equivalence in terms of renal functioning. Coherently, a recent meta-analysis that analyzed over 140 studies based on expressive writing found that studies with less than 1-month follow-up had larger psychological health effects than studies with a longer follow-up period (≥1 month) [31].
Another factor, empathy, was significantly reduced in both groups on the “perspective thinking” subscale, which assesses the tendency to spontaneously adopt the other’s psychological point of view, and the “personal distress” subscale level, which assesses the tendency to feel distressed in situations when other people are in trouble. However, at T2 (3 months), the empathy levels in the “personal distress” subscale were increased significantly only in the EWG. It is conceivable that in the immediate post-transplant phase, the patient tends to adopt a more self-centered perspective, as an adaptive strategy for receiving and accepting the organ, focusing mainly on the personal moment of difficulty and change, rather than taking a perspective centered on others’ difficulties. Nevertheless, at a later stage, the writing intervention seems to have a significant influence on facilitating the transition from a self-centered perspective to one more concentrated on others.
An interesting result of the present study concerns the resilience in the “control” subscale (in control of your life), which, in both groups, decreased significantly at T2 (3 months). This result indicates that the fear of potential rejection or death after transplantation could invalidate the perception of having control over one’s life and goals, thus decreasing resilience. A recent review showed how the post-transplant phase is gripped by the fear of rejection, of possible infections, or return to dialysis despite the transplant [40,41].
Besides the above-mentioned results, it was seen that the EWG, at T0, turned to their general practitioner more than the CG. This result may be attributed to a chance allotment of the patients who turned to their general practitioner more in the EWG.
The present study has important limitations. First of all, the analysis was performed on a limited cohort, mainly due to a high rate of drop out. Consequently, the sample size was not sufficient to obtain a relevant effect size, reducing the possibility of generalizing the results to the whole population of patients undergoing kidney transplant. Therefore, it will be necessary to confirm the findings with a greater sample size. Moreover, the measure of adherence used in the present study was very basic. Today, the difficulty in finding instruments adapted to evaluating perceived adherence to a pharmacological treatment in a validated and reliable way remains unsolved [42], confirming the necessity of conducting future studies using more appropriate and validated instruments. The high number of T0 refusals (19%) highlighted the resistance to participating in the study in some patients. Moreover, assigning randomized treatments to the participants did not allow personalization of the psychological treatment.
In conclusion, although the statistical evidence was weak, owing to the small sample size, the findings of the present study suggest the effectiveness of expressive writing on the psychological health, on the adherence to the pharmacological treatment, and on the biological acceptance of the transplanted organ in patients undergoing kidney transplantation. At the same time, this study underlines the need to perform a prolonged or repeated expressive intervention during the three months following kidney transplant.
The findings of the present study have relevant clinical implications for the possibility of planning a brief psychological intervention aimed at processing emotional involvement and increasing adherence in transplanted patients, as well as a consequent acceptance of the organ and decreased healthcare costs.

Author Contributions

C.L. and L.P. (Laura Pierro) conceived and designed the study and wrote the paper. C.L. supervised all the work phases. G.S. and E.F. collected the data. P.A. contributed to data analysis and interpretation. R.P., D.S., F.N., S.L. and L.P. (Luca Poli) provided access to crucial research components (samples, environments for collecting data, and administering questionnaires, general management of the data collection). All authors contributed to manuscript revisions. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, Sapienza University of Rome (approval number 23/2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request, due to privacy and ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Pistorio, M.L.; De Pasquale, C.; Veroux, M.; Martinelli, V.; Giaquinta, A.; Veroux, P. Critical Behavior and Psychopathologic Variables of the Parent-Child Couple in Living Kidney Transplantation. Transplant. Proc. 2020, 52, 1539–1543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Aceto, P.; Perilli, V.; Luca, E.; Salerno, M.P.; Punzo, G.; Ceaichisciuc, I.; Cataldo, A.; Lai, C.; Citterio, F.; Sollazzi, L. Perioperative-, Recipient-, and Donor-Related Factors Affecting Delayed Graft Function in Kidney Transplantation. Exp. Clinic. Transplant. 2019, 17, 575–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Lai, C.; Aceto, P.; Luciani, M.; Fazzari, E.; Cesari, V.; Luciano, S.; Fortini, A.; Berloco, D.; Canulla, F.; Bruzzese, V.; et al. Emotional management and biological markers of dietetic regimen in chronic kidney disease patients. Ren. Fail. 2017, 39, 173–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Calia, R.; Lai, C.; Aceto, P.; Luciani, M.; Saraceni, C.; Lai, S.; Gargiulo, A.; Citterio, F. Preoperative psychological factors predicting graft rejection in patients undergoing kidney transplant: A pilot study. Transplant. Proc. 2011, 43, 1006–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Calia, R.; Lai, C.; Aceto, P.; Pascolo, G.; Lai, S.; Romagnoli, J.; Citterio, F. Emotional management and quality of life in mother living versus multi-organ donor renal transplant recipients. J. Health Psychol. 2017, 22, 475–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Calia, R.; Lai, C.; Aceto, P.; Luciani, M.; Camardese, G.; Lai, S.; Fantozzi, C.; Pietroni, V.; Salerno, M.P.; Spagnoletti, G.; et al. Emotional self-efficacy and alexithymia may affect compliance, renal function and quality of life in kidney transplant recipients: Results from a preliminary cross-sectional study. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 142, 152–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Baker, R.J.; Mark, P.B.; Patel, R.K.; Stevens, K.K.; Palmer, N. Renal association clinical practice guideline in post-operative care in the kidney transplant recipient. BMC Nephrol. 2017, 18, 174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ladin, K.; Daniels, A.; Osani, M.; Bannuru, R.R. Is social support associated with post-transplant medication adherence and outcomes? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Transplant. Rev. 2018, 32, 16–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Stilley, C.S.; Di Martini, A.F.; de Vera, M.E.; Flynn, W.B.; King, J.; Sereika, S.; Tarter, R.E.; Dew, M.A.; Rathnamala, G. Individual and environmental correlates and predictors of early adherence and outcomes after liver transplantation. Prog. Transplant. 2010, 20, 58–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Muduma, G.; Odeyemi, I.; Smith-Palmer, J.; Pollock, R.F. Review of the Clinical and Economic Burden of Antibody-Mediated Rejection in Renal Transplant Recipients. Adv. Ther. 2016, 33, 345–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Calia, R.; Lai, C.; Aceto, P.; Luciani, M.; Camardese, G.; Lai, S.; Amato, G.; Pietroni, V.; Salerno, M.P.; Pedroso, J.A.; et al. Attachment style predict compliance, quality of life and renal function in adult patients after kidney transplant: Preliminary results. Ren. Fail. 2015, 37, 678–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Villeneuve, C.; Rousseau, A.; Rerolle, J.P.; Couzi, L.; Kamar, N.; Essig, M.; Etienne, I.; Westeel, P.F.; Büchler, M.; Esposito, L.; et al. Adherence profiles in kidney transplant patients: Causes and consequences. Patient Educ. Couns. 2020, 103, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Possemato, K.; Ouimette, P.; Geller, P.A. Internet-based expressive writing for kidney transplant recipients: Effects on posttraumatic stress and quality of life. Traumatology 2010, 16, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Narayanan, S.; Milbury, K.; Wagner, R.; Cohen, L. Religious Coping in Cancer: A Quantitative Analysis of Expressive Writing Samples from Patients with Renal Cell Carcinoma. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 2020, 60, 737–745.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Usichenko, T.; Wenzel, A.; Klausenitz, C.; Petersmann, A.; Hesse, T.; Neumann, N.; Hahnenkamp, K. Auricular stimulation vs. expressive writing for exam anxiety in medical students-A randomized crossover investigation. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0238307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zachariae, R.; O’Toole, M.S. The effect of expressive writing intervention on psychological and physical health outcomes in cancer patients--a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psycho-Oncology 2015, 24, 1349–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hedayati, A.; Noorbala, A.A.; Khatami, S.M.R. The impact of expressive emotion brief psychotherapy on psychological health of kidney transplant recipients. Iran. Red Crescent Med. J. 2017, 19, e41048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Beck, A.T.; Epstein, N.; Brown, G.; Steer, R.A. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988, 56, 893–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R.; Vagg, P.R.; Jacobs, G.A. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologists Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  20. Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D.; Taylor, G.J. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale--I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 1994, 38, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Davis, M. Medición de las diferencias individuales en la empatía: Evidencia de un enfoque multidimensional. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 44, 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Connor, K.M.; Davidson, J.R. Development of a new resilience scale: The Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depress. Anxiety 2003, 18, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wallston, B.S.; Wallston, K.A.; Kaplan, G.D.; Maides, S.A. Development and validation of the health locus of control (HLC) scale. J. Consult. Clin. Psych. 1976, 44, 580–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sambucini, D.; Ciacchella, C.; Pellicano, G.R.; Zingaretti, G.; Pierro, L.; Aceto, P.; Lai, C. Psychosocial treatment on psychological symptoms, adherence and physiological function on transplanted patients: A systematic review and metanalysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2022, 154, 110717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Sambucini, D.; Aceto, P.; Begotaraj, E.; Lai, C. Efficacy of Psychological Interventions on Depression Anxiety and Somatization in Migrants: A Meta-analysis. J. Immigr. Minor. Health 2020, 22, 1320–1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bellver-Pérez, A.; Peris-Juan, C.; Santaballa-Beltrán, A. Effectiveness of therapy group in women with localized breast cancer. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2019, 19, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vukčević Marković, M.; Bjekić, J.; Priebe, S. Effectiveness of Expressive Writing in the Reduction of Psychological Distress During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 587282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Zheng, D.J.; Lu, X.; Schore, R.J.; Balsamo, L.; Devidas, M.; Winick, N.J.; Raetz, E.A.; Loh, M.L.; Carroll, W.L.; Sung, L.; et al. Longitudinal analysis of quality-of-life outcomes in children during treatment for acute lymphoblastic leukemia: A report from the Children’s Oncology Group AALL0932 trial. Cancer 2018, 124, 571–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Renzi, A.; Solano, L.; Di Trani, M.; Ginobbi, F.; Minutolo, E.; Tambelli, R. The effects of an expressive writing intervention on pregnancy rates, alexithymia and psychophysical health during an assisted reproductive treatment. Psychol. Health 2020, 35, 718–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Messina, A.; Beadle, J.N.; Paradiso, S. Towards a classification of alexithymia: Primary secondary and organic. J. Psychopathology 2014, 20, 38–49. [Google Scholar]
  31. Qian, J.; Zhou, X.; Sun, X.; Wu, M.; Sun, S.; Yu, X. Effects of expressive writing intervention for women’s PTSD, depression, anxiety and stress related to pregnancy: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 288, 112933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Glass, O.; Dreusicke, M.; Evans, J.; Bechard, E.; Wolever, R.Q. Expressive writing to improve resilience to trauma: A clinical feasibility trial. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 2018, 34, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Lai, S.; Mecarelli, O.; Pulitano, P.; Romanello, R.; Davi, L.; Zarabla, A.; Mariotti, A.; Carta, M.; Tasso, G.; Poli, L.; et al. Neurological, psychological, and cognitive disorders in patients with chronic kidney disease on conservative and replacement therapy. Medicine 2016, 95, e5191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Merz, E.L.; Fox, R.S.; Malcarne, V.L. Expressive writing interventions in cancer patients: A systematic review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2014, 8, 339–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Petrie, K.J.; Booth, R.J.; Pennebaker, J.W.; Davison, K.P.; Thomas, M.G. Disclosure of trauma and immune response to a hepatitis B vaccination program. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol 1995, 63, 787–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hall, B.M. CD4+CD25+ T Regulatory Cells in Transplantation Tolerance: 25 Years on. Transplantation 2016, 100, 2533–2547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Romero-Sanchiz, P.; Nogueira-Arjona, R.; Araos, P.; Serrano, A.; Barrios, V.; Argente, J.; Garcia-Marchena, N.; Lopez-Tellez, A.; Rodriguez-Moreno, S.; Mayoral, F.; et al. Variation in chemokines plasma concentrations in primary care depressed patients associated with Internet-based cognitive-behavioral therapy. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hancock, W.W. Chemokine receptor-dependent alloresponses. Immunol. Rev. 2003, 196, 37–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Viau, A.; Bienaimé, F.; Lukas, K.; Todkar, A.P.; Knoll, M.; Yakulov, T.A.; Hofherr, A.; Kretz, O.; Helmstädter, M.; Reichardt, W.; et al. Cilia-localized LKB1 regulates chemokine signaling, macrophage recruitment, and tissue homeostasis in the kidney. EMBO Rep. 2018, 37, e98615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kuba, K.; Weißflog, G.; Götze, H.; García-Torres, F.; Mehnert, A.; Esser, P. The relationship between acceptance, fatigue, and subjective cognitive impairment in hematologic cancer survivors. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2019, 19, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Su, G.; Liu, Z.; Qin, X.; Hong, X.; Liu, X.; Wen, Z.; Lindholm, B.; Carrero, J.J.; Johnson, D.W.; Brusselaers, N.; et al. Vitamin D deficiency and treatment versus risk of infection in end-stage renal disease patients under dialysis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nephrol. Dial. Transplant. 2019, 34, 146–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Zhang, N.J.; Terry, A.; McHorney, C.A. Impact of health literacy on medication adherence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann. Pharmacother. 2014, 48, 741–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Consort 2010 Flow Diagram of the study.
Figure 1. Consort 2010 Flow Diagram of the study.
Healthcare 10 01559 g001
Figure 2. Significant differences between groups (*) and within groups on the investigated variables along the time frame.
Figure 2. Significant differences between groups (*) and within groups on the investigated variables along the time frame.
Healthcare 10 01559 g002
Table 1. Characteristics of the sample.
Table 1. Characteristics of the sample.
Socio-Demographic and Clinical ParametersExpressive Writing Group (n = 16)
M ± SD or n (%)
Control Group (n = 10)
M ± SD or n (%)
Age47.25 ± 16.4750.80 ± 10.59p = 0.45
Gender (Male vs. Female)11 vs. 5 (69% vs. 31%)3 vs. 7 (30% vs. 70%)p = 0.13
Marital StatusMarried 4 (25%)
Unmarried 8 (50%)
Divorced 1 (6%)
Widowed 3 (19%)
Married 6 (60%)
Unmarried 1 (10%)
Divorced 3 (30%)
Widowed 0 (0%)
p = 0.07
SchoolingPrimary school 0 (0%)
Secondary school 9 (57%)
High School 5 (31%)
Degree 2 (12%)
Primary school 1 (10%)
Secondary school 3 (30%)
High School 5 (50%)
Degree 1 (10%)
p = 0.07
EmploymentHousewife 1 (6%)
Unemployed 4 (25%)
Employed 7 (44%)
Pensioned 4 (25%)
Housewife 3 (30%)
Unemployed 2 (20%)
Employed 4 (40%)
Pensioned 1 (10%)
p = 0.38
PsychopathologyPrevious 2 (12.5%)
Actual 2 (12.5%)
Previous 3 (30%)
Actual 1 (10%)
p = 0.62
Dialysis Peritoneal 2 (12.5%)
Hemodialysis 12 (75%), of which 1 (8%) had previously received peritoneal dialysis
Never received 2 (12.5%)
Peritoneal 1 (10%)
Hemodialysis 8 (80%), of which 3 (37%) had previously received peritoneal dialysis
Never received 1 (10%)
p = 0.95
ComorbiditiesType I Diabetes 1 (6.2%)
Type 2 Diabetes 1 (6.2%)
Hypertension 4 (25%)
Heart disease 1 (6.2%)
Type I Diabetes 0 (0%)
Type 2 Diabetes 0 (0%)
Hypertension 2 (20%)
Heart disease 0 (0%)
p = 0.61
p = 0.61
p = 0.80
p = 0.61
Donors’ age52.12 ± 13.7555.0 ± 17.57p = 0.64
Operation time218.12 ± 74.61213.0 ± 77.21p = 0.87
Intraoperative complicationsAbsentAbsent-
Table 2. Averages and standard deviations of psychological variables, adherence, biological variables, and health expenditure variables.
Table 2. Averages and standard deviations of psychological variables, adherence, biological variables, and health expenditure variables.
MeasuresExpressive Writing Group (EWG)Control Group (CG)
T0 (n = 16)T1 (n = 16)T2 (n = 16)T0 (n = 10)T1 (n = 10)T2 (n = 9)
BDI-II5.2 ± 4.72.9 ± 3.13.8 ± 5.84.9 ± 2.332.6 ± 2.82.8 ± 2.9
STAI-Y143.8 ± 12.737.3 ± 13.536.6 ± 13.341.3 ± 8.836.5 ± 8.532.8 ± 7.8
STAI-Y238.6 ± 9.036.1 ± 12.032.2 ± 10.839.2 ± 8.333.7 ± 9.628.7 ± 9.0
TAS-20-F114.7 ± 6.516.1 ± 7.314.8 ± 8.56.4 ± 2.013.9 ± 5.212.5 ± 5.1
TAS-20-F215.3 ± 5.712.9 ± 4.112.4 ± 4.911.2 ± 6.39.9 ± 4.910.1 ± 4.2
TAS-20-F320.6 ± 4.218.8 ± 4.317.2 ± 5.121.8 ± 4.620.2 ± 4.419.5 ± 6.2
TAS-20-TOT50.6 ± 13.347.8 ± 18.945.1 ± 16.948.2 ± 11.844.0 ± 11.342.2 ± 12.6
IRI-PT17 ± 3.414.8 ± 3.613.9 ± 4.118.9 ± 3.516.8 ± 3.616.7 ± 4.1
IRI-FS14.5 ± 3.614.6 ± 4.214.2 ± 4.916.9 ± 3.516 ± 3.817.2 ± 2.7
IRI-EC19.5 ± 4.919.2 ± 2.719.2 ± 4.121 ± 4.720.4 ± 4.422.5 ± 4.3
IRI-PD8.9 ± 4.410.6 ± 4.812 ± 6.49.3 ± 4.7112.8 ± 4.810.4 ± 5.9
CD-RISC competence25.1 ± 4.924.6 ± 5.024.3 ± 6.126.3 ± 4.727.8 ± 4.426.7 ± 3.8
CD-RISC emotion managment19.9 ± 4.119.5 ± 3.519.1 ± 5.821.1 ± 4.621.5 ± 4.322.4 ± 3.4
CD-RISC secure relationship13.9 ± 3.714.2 ± 2.714.6 ± 4.015.7 ± 2.514.9 ± 2.915.5 ± 2.9
CD-RISC control9.1 ± 1.99.3 ± 1.86.0 ± 1.98.8 ± 1.58.8 ± 2.06.0 ± 1.3
CD-RISC spiritual5.6 ± 1.75.6 ± 1.65.4 ± 1.66.5 ± 1.35.9 ± 2.16.7 ± 1.9
HCLI tot15.7 ± 5.914.6 ± 5.714.7 ± 5.014.6 ± 4.116.3 ± 4.416.7 ± 3.2
HLCE tot12.7 ± 5.513.2 ± 6.016.8 ± 5.616.2 ± 6.516.9 ± 8.216.8 ± 5.6
Azotemia97.2 ± 47.179.2 ± 36.449.8 ± 10.5102.7 ± 47.2110.9 ± 54.660.5 ± 19.6
Creatinine7.7 ± 3.21.7 ± 0.71.5 ± 0.77.1 ± 3.62.5 ± 1.81.4± 0.4
CDK-EPI8.3 ± 3.749.7 ± 17.656.6 ± 17.28.3 ± 3.436.8 ± 22.353.5 ± 13.3
Drug toxicity (n^)/0/163/16/0/100/9
Cytomegalovirus (n^)/0/161/16/0/102/9
Adherence9 yes/7 no/4 yes/12 no7 yes/3 no/2 yes/7 no
General pratictioner visits (n^)2.8 ± 3.3/0.7 ± 0.41.0 ± 1.0/0.8 ± 0.4
Hospitalization (n^)0.6 ± 0.5/0.5 ± 0.50.4 ± 0.5/0.4 ± 0.5
Recovery’s (n^/days)11.2 ± 18.313.7 ± 9.77.7 ± 12.212.7 ± 20.613.7 ± 10.76.7 ± 10.0
Access to the Emergency Room (n^)0.3 ± 0.5/0.3 ± 0.50.3 ± 0.5/0.1 ± 0.3
Notes: BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II, STAI-Y1 e STAI-Y2 = State Trait Anxiety Inventory 1 (trait) e 2 (state), TAS-20 = Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20-F1 = Difficulty Identifying Feelings, TAS-20-F2 = Difficulty Describing Feelings, TAS-20-F3 = Externally-Oriented Thinking), IRI = Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-PT = Perspective-Taking IRI-FS = Fantasy Scale, IRI-EC = Empathic Concern, IRI-PD = Personal Distress), CD-RISC = Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (personal competence, emotion’s management, secure relationships, Control, Spirituality), HLC = Health Locus of control scales (HCLE = External Locus of Control, HCLI= Internal Locus of Control), CDK-EPI = creatinine levels calculated based on age and gender.
Table 3. Repeated Measure ANOVA Expressive Writing Group vs. Control Group per 3 times (T1 vs. T0 and T2 vs. T1) on significant psychological variables (BDI-II, STAI-Y1, STAI-Y2, TAS-20, IRI, CD-RISC) and on significant biological variables (Azotemia, Creatinine, CDK-EPI). In (a) are reported between group and within group planned comparisons related to the two at T1 vs. T0 time; in (b) are reported between group and within group planned comparisons related to the two at T2 vs. T1 time.
Table 3. Repeated Measure ANOVA Expressive Writing Group vs. Control Group per 3 times (T1 vs. T0 and T2 vs. T1) on significant psychological variables (BDI-II, STAI-Y1, STAI-Y2, TAS-20, IRI, CD-RISC) and on significant biological variables (Azotemia, Creatinine, CDK-EPI). In (a) are reported between group and within group planned comparisons related to the two at T1 vs. T0 time; in (b) are reported between group and within group planned comparisons related to the two at T2 vs. T1 time.
(a)
T1 vs. T0
(Expressive Writing Group (EWG) N = 16 vs. Control Group (CG) N = 10)
EffectsPlanned Comparison
Between Group
Planned Comparison
Within Group
AzotemiaF Group (1, 24) = 1.13; p = 0.298
F Time (1, 24) = 0.60; p = 0.446
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 4.00; p = 0.057
EWGT0 < CGT0 p = 0.798
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.095
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.035
CGT0 < GCT1 p = 0.442
CreatinineF Group (1, 24) = 0.02; p = 0.876
F Time (1, 24) = 65.09; p = 0.000000
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 1.25; p = 0.274
EWGT0 > CGT0 p = 0.555
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.407
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p < 0.001
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.00017
CDK-EPIF Group (1, 24) = 2.58; p = 0.121
F Time (1, 24) = 77.08; p = 0.000000
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 2.61; p = 0.119
EWGT0 > CGT0 p = 0.995
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.027
EWGT0 < EWGT1 p < 0.001
CGT0 < CGT1 p = 0.027
BDI IIF Group (1, 24) = 0.08; p = 0.771
F Time (1, 24) = 7.71; p = 0.010
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 0.00006; p = 0.994
EWGT0 > CGT0 p = 0.808
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.815
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.034
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.090
STAI Y1F Group (1, 24) = 0.109; p = 0.743
F Time (1, 24) = 5.29; p = 0.030
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 0.05; p = 0.818
EWGT0 > CGT0 p = 0.690
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.863
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.523
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.199
STAI Y2F Group (1, 24) = 0.06; p = 0.800
F Time (1, 24) = 4.81; p = 0.038
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 0.67; p = 0.419
EWGT0 < CGT0 p = 0.888
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.553
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.280
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.067
TAS-20 F2F Group (1, 24) = 3.56; p = 0.071
F Time (1, 24) = 4.02; p = 0.056
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 0.344; p = 0.562
EWGT0 > CGT0 p = 0.588
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.158
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.047
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.374
IRI PTF Group (1, 24) = 2.39; p = 0.135
F Time (1, 24) = 11.63; p = 0.002
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 0.05; p = 0.945
EWGT0 < CGT0 p = 0.184
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.165
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.0097
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.043
IRI PDF Group (1, 24) = 0.52; p = 0.477
F Time (1, 24) = 25.20; p = 0.00004
F Int GxT (1, 24) = 2.80; p = 0.107
EWGT0 < CGT0 p = 0.255
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.822
EWGT0 > EWGT1 p = 0.012
CGT0 > CGT1 p = 0.00003
(b)
T2 vs. T1
(Expressive Writing Group (EWG) N = 16 vs. Control Group (CG) N = 9)
EffectsPlanned Comparison
Between Group
Planned Comparison
Within Group
AzotemiaF Group (1, 23) = 4.60; p = 0.043
F Time (1, 23) = 18.05; p = 0.0003
F Int GxT (1, 23) = 1.33; p = 0.260
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.023
EWGT2 < CGT2 p = 0.443
EWGT1 > EWGT2 p = 0.017
CGT1 > CGT2 p = 0.003
CreatinineF Group (1, 23) = 0.641; p = 0.431
F Time (1, 23) = 6.14; p = 0.021
F Int GxT (1, 23) = 3.20; p = 0.087
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.101
EWGT2 > CGT2 p = 0.070
EWGT1 < EWGT2 p = 0.572
CGT1 > CGT2 p = 0.014
CDK-EPIF Group (1, 23) = 1.10; p = 0.304
F Time (1, 23) = 7.19; p = 0.013
F Int GxT (1, 23) = 0.803; p = 0.379
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.171
EWGT2 > CGT2 p = 0.677
EWGT1 < EWGT2 p = 0.150
CGT1 < CGT2 p = 0.035
IRI PDF Group (1, 23) = 0.28; p = 0.603
F Time (1, 23) = 3.36; p = 0.079
F Int GxT (1, 23) = 2.96; p = 0.098
EWGT1 < CGT1 p = 0.851
EWGT2 > CGT2 p = 0.255
EWGT1 < EWGT2 p = 0.0085
CGT1 < CGT2 p = 0.942
CDRISC controlF Group (1, 23) = 0.77; p = 0.388
F Time (1, 23) = 48.28; p = 0.000000
F Int GxT (1, 23) = 0.008; p = 0.928
EWGT1 > CGT1 p = 0.523
EWGT2 > CGT2 p = 0.455
EWGT1 > EWGT2 p < 0.001
CGT1 > CGT2 p = 0.00015
Notes: F-Group = Group Effect (Expressive Writing Group vs. Control Group), F-Time= Time Effect (T0 vs. T1 vs. T2), F-Int GxT= Interaction Group per Time Effect. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II, STAI-Y1 e STAI-Y2 = State Trait Anxiety Inventory 1 (trait) e 2 (state), TAS-20 = Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20-F2 = Difficulty Describing Feelings), IRI = Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-PT = Perspective-Taking IRI-PD = Personal Distress), CD-RISC = Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (Control), CDK-EPI = creatinine levels calculated based on age and gender.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pierro, L.; Servidei, G.; Pretagostini, R.; Stabile, D.; Nudo, F.; Lai, S.; Aceto, P.; Poli, L.; Fazzari, E.; Lai, C. Effectiveness of Expressive Writing in Kidney Transplanted Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial Study. Healthcare 2022, 10, 1559. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10081559

AMA Style

Pierro L, Servidei G, Pretagostini R, Stabile D, Nudo F, Lai S, Aceto P, Poli L, Fazzari E, Lai C. Effectiveness of Expressive Writing in Kidney Transplanted Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial Study. Healthcare. 2022; 10(8):1559. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10081559

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pierro, Laura, Giulia Servidei, Renzo Pretagostini, Davide Stabile, Francesco Nudo, Silvia Lai, Paola Aceto, Luca Poli, Erika Fazzari, and Carlo Lai. 2022. "Effectiveness of Expressive Writing in Kidney Transplanted Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial Study" Healthcare 10, no. 8: 1559. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10081559

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop