Next Article in Journal
Requirements of Modern Russian Agricultural Production for Digital Competencies of an Agricultural Specialist
Previous Article in Journal
A Complementary View to Computational Thinking and Its Interplay with Systems Thinking
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Present and Future Undergraduate Students’ Well-Being: Role of Time Perspective, Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation and Intention to Drop-Out

1
Department of Pedagogy, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari, 09123 Cagliari, Italy
2
Faculty of Medicine, University of Cagliari, 09042 Cagliari, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2023, 13(2), 202; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13020202
Submission received: 15 December 2022 / Revised: 2 February 2023 / Accepted: 3 February 2023 / Published: 15 February 2023

Abstract

:
Well-being is a multidimensional construct that affects various areas of a person’s life. In the university context, a student’s well-being can influence not only their academic and professional success but also the future development of society. This study aimed to evaluate how the interactions of time perspective (assessed by the Stanford Time Perspective Inventory—Short Form), self-efficacy (assessed by the General Self-efficacy Scale), self-regulation (assessed by the Self-regulated Knowledge Scale—University), and drop-out intention (assessed by the Intention to Drop-Out Scale) affect students’ perceptions of current and future well-being (assessed by the I COPPE Scale). Using a cross-sectional design, 192 students attending the University of Cagliari (Italy) were evaluated. A partial least squares structural equation modelling (PLS-SEM) analysis was performed to examine the relationships among well-being and all the variables examined herein. The empirical findings highlighted the direct and indirect effects of the studied variables on students’ current and future well-being.

1. Introduction

The existing literature on well-being defines it as a multidimensional and holistic construct comprised of a set of several intersecting factors and dimensions [1,2,3]. The multidimensional aspect of well-being is visible in the models presented in the literature. Researchers agree that a combination of internal and external factors and conditions makes an individual feel good [4]. The World Health Organization considers well-being to be the experience of positive emotions, such as happiness and fulfilment, the development of one’s potential, control over one’s life, a sense of purpose, and the experience of positive relationships [5]. In the last four decades, researchers have focused on specific aspects of well-being, particularly psychological well-being [6] and subjective well-being [7,8]. The entirety of the research in this field presents a conceptualisation of well-being that is focused on the identification of the individual and contextual factors that lead to satisfying lives, favouring the use of models that consider multiple domains. One such multidimensional model of well-being was proposed by Prilleltensky and colleagues [9]. This model, called I COPPE, considers six relevant life domains: Interpersonal, Community, Occupational, Physical, Psychological, and Economic. Interpersonal well-being refers to a person’s degree of satisfaction with their intimate relationships with their family, friends, and colleagues. Community well-being refers to a person’s satisfaction with the place where they live. Occupational well-being refers to a person’s level of satisfaction with their main activity, such as working or caring for their home and family. Physical well-being refers to a person’s general state of health. Psychological well-being refers to a person’s degree of satisfaction with their emotional life. Economic well-being refers to a person’s financial situation [9]. The I COPPE model confirms a systemic vision of well-being that can be achieved by the simultaneous fulfilment of needs at the individual, relational, organisational, and community levels [3,9]. Well-being is a dynamic concept that includes subjective, social, and psychological dimensions as well as health-related behaviours [10]. The theme of well-being is investigated across all age groups; however, the present study focused on university students. The well-being of young people, such as students, is a concern at both the national and the international level; furthermore, it is increasingly at the centre of policies, programmes, and the professional development of teachers in schools. Support for well-being is a growing topic, as evidenced by the increasing amount of literature highlighting the expansion of participation in specific programmes that promote well-being [11]. At university, students encounter a new context that is markedly different from that of high school; thus, higher education often focuses on promoting the positive and holistic development of university students [12]. Moreover, concerns about student well-being became prominent during the COVID-19 pandemic, which highlighted various issues faced by students [13,14,15,16,17].

2. The Present Study

The context of education is an important environment that offers opportunities for personal growth [17] and professional success. Therefore, the present study aimed to develop an explanatory model to determine the factors that may contribute to present and future well-being among university students, with the objective of improving university conditions through the identification of effective interventions or changes, primarily in the Italian context. The need to find reference models has also been emphasised in recent reviews and meta-analyses, questioning what can be considered the antecedents of well-being among university students [18,19]. The factors involved in university well-being are numerous; the problem is highly complex as there are several inter-related factors that may be both positive and negative antecedents of well-being. The present study was intended to investigate the empirical links among the variables identified in the literature as the antecedents of academic success as well as academic and professional well-being.

2.1. Future Time Perspective Related to Self-Regulation, Self-Efficacy, Intention to Dropout, and Well-Being

The theory of Time Perspective (TP) conceptualised by Zimbardo [20], assumes that TP is situationally determined and differs among individuals. TP can be considered an often-unconscious process whereby the continual flows of personal and social experiences are assigned to temporal categories or time frames that help give order, coherence, and meaning to those events. This theory proposes that a person’s motivational, emotional, behavioural, cognitive, and social processes are influenced by the way he/she sees the past, present, and future [20]. Time perspective is learned and determined by multiple factors, such as culture, education, social class, age, and more. According to Zimbardo [20], individuals develop an overreliance on a particular time frame that operates, most of the time, on an unconscious level and influences much of their judgments, decisions, and actions. For example, people who over-rely on the future temporal frame are more likely to be risk-aversive, have higher grades and make healthier choices than those who are more present-oriented because they are thinking about the future consequences of their decisions [20]. Thus, the future time perspective (FTP), the individual’s perception of his/her remaining time to live, has been a focus of growing interest in psychology over the past decade, especially in the fields of aging and health [21]. Time is not just a physical phenomenon; it is open to psychological interpretation, according to James [22]. FTP represents an important psychological variable that can be traced back to Lewin [23], who claimed that a person’s life-space includes not only a geographical and a social environment, but also a temporal dimension. Lewin [23] stated that FTP influences a person’s behaviour and asserted that change within FTP is one of the most fundamental facts of development. Denovan and colleagues [24] emphasised the correlation among FTP, positive emotion and student engagement.
An individual’s TP may impact other constructs, such as achievement, goal setting, addiction, rumination, and well-being [25]. Avci [26] found that FTP and self-regulation strategies have a positive effect on first-grade university students; they could set and attain goals concerning their academic activity more easily. This relationship has been confirmed by other studies and meta-analyses [27,28,29].
Gutiérrez-Braojos [27] presented a notable and complete analysis of the direct and indirect relationship between FTP and self-efficacy in academic achievement. In particular, he reported on the research by Chung et al. [30], which showed that students who linked their future professions to their academic studies exhibited higher self-efficacy beliefs in learning course content and better academic success compared to students without FTP. The relationship between FTP and self-efficacy has been confirmed not only in the university context but also regarding the motivational role of FTP as adaptivity in career construction [31].
Furthermore, FTP appears to be related to motivation and educational drop-out; the latter aspect is associated with people with a short FPT [32].
FTP is related to positive goals and the development of well-being [33]. The relation between FTP and well-being is confirmed in a recent meta-analysis by Kooij and colleagues [34]; however, the ability to foresee, anticipate and plan for future desired outcomes is crucial for present and future well-being. Li Wen Chua and colleagues [35] showed that FTP has important implications for the health and well-being of adolescents. Mascia and colleagues [36] confirmed the positive implications of this related to academic achievement among university students.

2.2. Self-Efficacy Related to Self-Regulation, Intention to Drop-Out, and Well-Being

Self-efficacy is a universal psychological need that controls an individual’s cognition, emotions, and decisions related to psychological well-being [37]. Compared to people with poor self-efficacy, those with high self-efficacy are more likely to use highly adaptive coping mechanisms [38,39]. The relationship between self-efficacy and self-regulation is strongly supported by literature; low self-efficacy and self-regulation can be predictors of drop-out intention [40,41]. Sabouripour and colleagues [42] emphasise a mediation role between optimism, dimensions of psychological well-being, and resilience among Iranian students. Self-efficacy may play a role in how students feel about themselves and whether they accomplish their goals effectively in life. The association between optimism, psychological well-being, and resilience has been confirmed in several studies. Hence, self-efficacy is an essential personal resource for university students to prevent stressors and promote adaptive adjustment [43]. Self-efficacy has been shown to predict students’ scholastic performance and success. As reported by some research studies, students who express high levels of self-efficacy and well-being are motivated to participate in relevant academic activities and to develop positive attitudes that lead to success at school [44,45]. They are also likely to perform well in achieving their academic goals [46]. He and colleagues [47] showed that self-efficacy had a positive effect on psychological well-being among nursing students.

2.3. Self-Regulation Related to Intention to Drop-Out and Well-Being

Research on university norms suggests that personal and environmental characteristics might elicit such fit effects in undergraduate students. Compared to other educational institutions, universities demand increased initiative and self-regulation from students for them to succeed [48]. Many studies confirmed the positive influence of a good level of self-regulation on well-being and academic retention and success [49,50]. The literature shows that highly self-regulated students know what they want to learn; they plan and control their own learning process using the strategies most suitable for this purpose, they monitor their results and, if necessary, they redefine or modify their goals based on what they have experienced [50,51,52]. University students are expected to develop critical thinking skills and strengthen their cognitive skills through behaviours, such as perseverance, self-discipline, self-regulation, and motivation; in order to face the difficulties, they encounter in their academic achievement process [16,53]. High self-regulation levels are positively correlated with lower stress levels among university students [54]; high self-regulation levels are correlated with high well-being levels among the general population [55]. However, self-regulation is considered one of the most important qualities of human beings, as it has allowed us to survive and progress considerably over the ages [56]. Self-regulation skills are significantly related to positive aspects (e.g., lower depression, anxiety, and stress) among the general population and among students [57,58]. In general, self-regulation and self-control influence the well-being of undergraduates [59,60].

2.4. Academic Achievement, Drop-Out, and Well-Being

In the scenario of the university system, students’ well-being can be undermined by specific elements that, if not controlled or managed, can lead to forms of malaise that can affect their current/present and future well-being. It is also important to understand how a student fits into a university context, particularly in terms of his/her goals and academic retention as well as access to individual and contextual resources. A signal of the student’s malaise may be his/her intention to drop-out. In the literature, the term drop-out refers to a student that leaves his/her university studies before having completed the study programme and obtaining a degree [61]. A student drop-out is generated by a long decision-making process and the complex interaction between several factors [62]. The drop-out intention is the result of a complex, dynamic, cumulative, and multifactorial process. This is also emphasised in a review of the literature [63], which highlights the complex interaction among individual, organisational, and social factors that come into play in this process [64,65]. This can be distinguished by the students’ motives to voluntarily abandon their studies [66]. Many studies [36,67] confirmed that the intention to drop-out and dropping out are present in the period of transition from high school to university, particularly in the Italian school system, where the two systems are organised very differently; if not well managed, this transition leads to destabilisation in the student’s life. In empirical research, many of these determinants indicating dropping out voluntarily or involuntarily are analysed. A recent review of 44 empirical studies on student drop-outs from universities in Europe [68] identified nine main factors that influence the decision to drop-out or transfer to a different study programme, subject, or university: study conditions at university; academic integration at university; social integration at university; personal effort and motivation for studying; information and admissions requirements; prior academic achievement in school; personal characteristics of the student; sociodemographic background of the student; external conditions. In this scenario, it is essential to understand the risk and protective factors, the antecedents, and the consequences that these factors have on student well-being. Although some theories support approaching education disruptions as having potentially diverse meanings and effects for some well-being dimensions and facets [69]. In this work, among our hypothesises we look for verifying if the intention to drop-out can have a negative correlation on present and future well-being, starting from the idea that belonging to the university and academic achievement are key variables in promoting students’ well-being [48].

2.5. Aim and Hypotheses

On the basis of the literature taken into account, the following hypotheses have guided our study, in order to identify predictors of students’ psychological present and future well-being (see Figure 1).
The first hypothesis is:
H1. 
Future perspective might affect (a) self-regulation, (b) self-efficacy, (c) intention to drop-out, (d) present well-being, and (e) future well-being.
Thus, the second hypothesis of this study is:
H2. 
Self-efficacy might affect the (a) intention to drop-out, (b) self-regulation, (c) present well-being, and (d) future well-being.
Then, the third hypothesis of this work is:
H3. 
Self-regulation might affect the (a) intention to drop-out, (b) present, and (c) future well-being.
Formerly, the fourth hypothesis of this research is:
H4. 
Intention to drop-out might affect (a) present well-being, and (b) future well-being.
Before, the fifth hypothesis of this study is:
H5. 
The present well-being might affect the (a) future well-being.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Participants Recruitment

The participants were recruited by a non-probabilistic sampling, based on voluntary participation, and they did not receive any incentive for their involvement; the enrollment in the research was publicised and disseminated during the academic activities at the University of Cagliari (Italy). The protocol of research was approved by the local Ethical Committee.

3.2. Procedures and Measures

This study applied a cross-sectional design; quantitative data were collected from an online survey (administered by the LimeSurvey platform) between October 2021 and November 2021. Participants signed an informed consent to participate, in accordance with requirements of the Italian and European norms regarding survey administration and management. To be included in the research, participants must be enrolled at Cagliari University.
The measures administered were articulated in different sections.
The first one included a social, demographic, and academic section (inquiring about age, gender, course, and type of degree attended).
The subsequent sections included different assessment-validated instruments.

3.2.1. Stanford Time Perspective Inventory (STPI) Short Form

The Zimbardo’s Stanford Time Perspective Inventory (STPI) Short Form [70] was also administered in their Italian version [71]. This short version, composed of 22 items, considers the present dimensions and the future dimension. We choose to use in our work only the future dimensions; the items were evaluated by a Likert scale (from 1 to 5). This instrument in the future dimension has good Cronbach’s Alpha reliability (α = 0.67).

3.2.2. General Self-Efficacy Scale

The Italian version of the General Self Efficacy Scale [72] assesses the perceived self-efficacy to predict coping as adaptation after stressful experiences (e.g., “If someone opposes me, I can find the means and ways to get what I want”; “When I am confronted with a problem, I can usually find several solutions”). The questions were evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale extending from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (exactly true); the Cronbach’s Alpha reliability was α = 0.86.

3.2.3. Self-Regulation

The Self-regulated Knowledge Scale University [73] was characterised by 15 items organised into five subscales that evaluate: knowledge networking, knowledge extraction, knowledge practice, knowledge critique, and knowledge monitoring. Each of the subscales consists of three items that define the potential answers to the question “When you study, how often do you do the following things?” The response was rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often).
This instrument has shown good reliability (Cronbach’s alphas of the subscales extended from 0.73 to 0.80).

3.2.4. Intention to Drop-Out

This scale measures the intentions to persist in, versus drop-out of school. It is composed of three items. Two items are the same used by Vallerand et al. [74], which were “I sometimes consider dropping out of school” and “I intend to drop-out of school.” The third item, added by Hardre and Reeve [75] asks about intentions to continue one’s schooling: “I sometimes feel unsure about continuing my studies year after year”. Authors measure correlation among the three items, so they used a three-item scale, because it allowed us to increase both the scope and reliability of our outcome measure (0.79). The questions were evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale, from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much so).

3.2.5. Coppe—I

Prilleltensky and colleagues [9] integrate different models and aspects of subjective well-being into the I COPPE Scale, devised to evaluate individual perceptions of multidimensional well-being. This instrument was validated in this short form in Italy by Esposito et al. [3]. This scale consisted of 14 items and showed good psychometric features and values of reliability (ranging from a minimum of 0.723, to a maximum of 0.935). The items were related to specific dimensions, referring to two different time periods: present (pr) and future (fu). The dimensions considered were overall well-being, interpersonal well-being, community well-being, occupational well-being, physical well-being, psychological well-being, and economic well-being. The responses to each question were assessed by a scale extending from 0 (worst your life can be) to 10 (best your life can be). In our research, we considered the relationships between present and future dimensions.

3.3. Statistics

The descriptive data analyses were carried out to evaluate the distribution of all assessed variables.
In order to explore the relationships among the latent constructs, the Partial Least Squares–Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) was applied. The aim of the Structural Equation Modeling technique is to define the estimations of relationships between unobserved (latent) variables, measured by specific indicators–items. The PLS-SEM aims to maximise the variance explicated between the latent constructs and it is chosen in a special way, contrasting with variance-based SEM, because might deal efficiently with non-normal data, such as the present research [76]. Specifically in this work, the application of PLS-SEM was useful regarding the application of explorative research and targeting test complex models [76,77,78]. Furthermore, the PLS-SEM is advantageous because it might be applied to samples with limited sizes [79,80,81].
In our work, as defined in the literature, a two-stage analysis approach in the PLS-SEM was applied [79].
Indeed, in the first stage of PLS-SEM we evaluated the measurement model by inspecting the reliability and validity of constructs. This step is decisive for the establishment of the measurement integrity of the latent constructs and must be conducted former the assessment of the inner-structural model [76]. To deeply assess the quality of the measurement model, Dijkstra-Henseler’s Rho A, Cronbach’s alpha, average variance extracted (AVE), and Adjusted R2 were considered (the thresholds of the values are reported in the “Results” section) [76]. Moreover, the discriminant validity was evaluated by the computation of the Heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlation. To assess for multicollinearity questions, the variance inflation factors (VIF) for all variables were inspected (the thresholds of the values are described in the “Results” paragraph) [76].
It is useful to consider that in our measurement model, to assess the latent variable of Zimbardo ‘s STPI Time future perspective, the items belonging to this scale were considered as observed variables [70]; to assess the latent variable of self-efficacy the items of this scale were used as observed variables [72]; for the dimension related to the intention to drop-out as observed variables, we used the items belonging to this dimension [75]. The subscales related to the dimensions of self-regulation [73] were used as observed variables. Finally, to assess the latent variable of the present well-being and future well-being, the subscales of the questionnaire regarding each time perspective were considered [9].
Then, the second step of PLS-SEM application was characterised by the evaluation of the structural model, aiming to assess the relationship between the latent constructs. This is defined as inner model, which was studied by the assessment of all identified pathways among the latent constructs. Specifically, we measured the strength of direct and indirect effects, observing the significance of the path coefficients (settling alpha <0.05); in this step a resampling bootstrapping technique (5000 resamples) was applied.
The structural model is presented in Figure 1.
Data were analysed by the open-source software Jasp [82] and the SmartPLS software, version 3.3.9 [78].

4. Results

4.1. Participants Characteristics

Out of 192 participants, n = 33 (17.2%) of the students are male; overall, the participants were aged from 18 to 62 years (age m = 25.70; sd = 9.09).
Of the undergraduates, in the past, 140 (73%) attended high schools having a humanistic curriculum; 38 (20%) attended high schools with scientific curricula; and the remaining 14 (7%) attended technical institutes. The participants at the time of the survey were joining university courses leading to Bachelor’s (79.2%) and Master’s degrees (20.8%) in the Faculty of Humanistic Studies at the University of Cagliari (Italy).
The descriptive statistics regarding all variables were evaluated (see Table 1).

4.2. PLS-SEM Measurement Model

Referring to the measurement model of our variables, the reliability (by Dijkstra-Henseler’s rho A and Cronbach’s Alpha, which should be higher than 0.70) [83], and validity were evaluated. The observed variables loadings and the Average Variance Extracted (AVE) were considered to evaluate the discriminant and convergent validity (a good convergent validity was observed when indicators’ loadings and variables’ AVEs are respectively higher than 0.70 and 0.50) [84] (Table 2).
To evaluate the discriminant validity, we considered also the Heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlation. HTMT values close to 1 represent a deficiency of discriminant validity. Specifically, we assume that if the value of the HTMT is higher than 0.90 [85,86], there is a lack of discriminant validity. Regarding our data, only the index regarding the measurement of well-being in present and in future perspectives showed values of HTMT over the threshold, but this might be specifically related to the features of our assessment (Table 3).
Furthermore, to check for multicollinearity issues, the variance inflation factors (VIF) for all variables were examined (their values should not be overhead the 5.0 threshold) (Table 4).
All results related to the evaluation of the outer model (presented in Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4) suggest that the measurement quality meets the criteria required in the literature.

4.3. PLS-SEM Structural Model

The relationships between latent constructs were evaluated in the structural model by the resampling bootstrapping technique (5000 resamples) of 192 participants, which permitted the evaluation of the significance of path coefficients [77].
The relevant and significant findings of the inner model are shown in Figure 2 and in Table 5 (only the significant results are shown in Table 5; the complete table with all the outcomes obtained in the evaluation of the model can be found in Appendix A).
Specifically, in Figure 2, the paths and the in-line values prove the effects between the variables and their beta coefficients (including their p-values).
The predictive power of the model was evaluated with R Squared (R2) values; these values demonstrate the explained variance of endogenous latent variables for the structural model (these values are displayed regarding the endogenous variables).
The R2 of our variables ranged from a minimum of 0.120 (for the Intention to drop-out) to 0.754 (for the well-being future) (see Table 2) [83].
The effect size F2 was appraised to inspect the impact of an independent latent variable on a dependent latent variable [87,88]. The findings highlighted the significant F2 of: Zimbardo Future perspective on self-efficacy (t = 2.707; p = 0.007); Zimbardo Future perspective on self-regulation (t= 2.916; p = 0.004); well-being present on well-being future (t = 3.922; p < 0.001).
The remaining F2 estimated were not significant.
Concerning the structural model (see Table 5), we evaluate the effects referring to each hypothesis.
Regarding the H1, the findings identify that Zimbardo’s future perspective has a positive significant effect on self-regulation (H1a; β = 0.524, p < 0.001), on self-efficacy (H1b; β = 0.481, p < 0.001), a negative significant effect on the intention to drop-out (H1c; β = −0.302, p < 0.001), a positive significant effect on well-being present (H1d; β = 0.251, p < 0.001); the Zimbardo future perspective did not have a significant effect on well-being future (H1e; β = 0.043, p = 0.373).
Referring to the H1, examination of the indirect effects, highlights the effect of Zimbardo’s future perspective on well-being future (β = 0.323, p ≤ 0.001). We observed significant specific indirect effects from Zimbardo’s future perspective:
  • To self-efficacy, to well-being present (β = 0.093, p = 0.046);
  • To intention to drop-out, to well-being present, to well-being future (β = 0.052, p = 0.028);
  • To well-being present, to well-being future (β = 0.206, p = 0.005);
  • To intention to drop-out, to well-being present (β = 0.064, p = 0.031);
  • To self-efficacy, to well-being present, to well-being future (β = 0.076, p = 0.046).
The total effects from Zimbardo’s future perspective on all latent variables in the model are statistically significant (to Self-regulation β = 0.517, p < 0.001; to Intention to drop-out β = −0.298, p < 0.001; to well-being present β = 0.380, p < 0.001; to well-being future β=0.366, p < 0.001).
Regarding the H2, the findings identify that Self-efficacy did not have a significant effect on the intention to drop-out (H2a; β = −0.107, p = 0.189), on self-regulation (H2b; β = −0.018, p = 0.871), have a positive significant effect on well-being present (H2c; β = 0.193, p = 0.031), did not have a significant effect on well-being future (H2d; β = 0.079, p = 0.095).
Referring to the H2, examination of the indirect effects, highlight the effect of Self-efficacy on well-being future (β = 0.179, p = 0.017).
We observed significant specific indirect effects from self-efficacy to well-being present, to well-being future (β = 0.157, p = 0.031).
The total effects from self-efficacy to Self-regulation (β = -0.018, p = 0.871), to Intention to drop-out (β = −0.107, p = 0.179) are not significant; to well-being present (β = 0.217, p = 0.017); to well-being future (β = 0.258, p = 0.006) are significant.
About the H3, the results identify that Self-regulation did not have a significant effect on the intention to drop-out (H3a; β = 0.103, p = 0.241), on well-being present (H3b; β=−0.056, p = 0.425), on well-being future (H3c; β = −0.057, p = 0.199).
Referring to the H3, also the examination of the indirect effects did not highlight the significant effect of Self-regulation on well-being present (β = −0.021, p = 0.282) and well-being future (β = −0.065, p = 0.285).
Furthermore, we did not observe any significant specific indirect effects or total effects.
Concerning the H4 there is a significant path from intention to drop-out to well-being present (H4a; β = −0.211, p = 0.002); furthermore, there is a non-significant path from intention to drop-out to well-being future (H4b; β = −0.014, p = 0.786).
Similarly, the examination of the indirect effects highlights the significant effect of the intention to drop-out on a well-being future (β = −0.172, p = 0.002). The total effects of the intention to drop-out on well-being present (β = −0.211, p = 0.002) and well-being future are significant (β = −0.186, p = 0.008).
Finally, regarding the H5, we observe a significant path from well-being present to well-being future (H5a; β = 0.817, p < 0.001).

5. Discussion and Conclusions

In the present study, we aimed to identify well-being perceptions among university students to develop a model that could show the relationship among specific variables. We focused on individual factors, but with the awareness that environmental characteristics are fundamental to enhancing academic life and professional success [88,89]. Specifically, we wanted to find negative or positive antecedents of well-being, as defined in Prilleltensky and colleagues [9] model. Particularly in this historical period of great changes and uncertainties related also to the COVID-19 pandemic, it is crucial to identify the factors that can be useful for supporting future perspectives in young people and strengthening their will to persist in achieving their goals [14,90,91].
In the model developed in this study, the interactions among the identified variables could provide teachers, educators, and practitioners with interesting empirical perspectives that can be used to support university students in the pursuit of their goals. Our results are in line with the literature in identifying the correlations and causal relationships between the variables.
The data show that the dimension related to the future perspective has a statistically significant, positive, and direct effect on self-regulation and self-efficacy, a negative and direct effect on the intention to drop-out and a positive and direct effect on present well-being. The effect of future attitude on future well-being is positive, but indirect, as it is mediated by the other variables included in the model (especially by present well-being) [92,93].
Furthermore, concerning the second hypothesis, the results highlight the significant positive effects, direct and indirect, exerted by self-efficacy on present and future well-being [42,46].
The fourth hypothesis was confirmed by our data, highlighting the significant and negative effects of drop-out intention on present and future well-being [69].
The fifth hypothesis appears to be confirmed, consistent with what has been established in the literature [9].
Only the effects of the self-regulation variable on the remaining variables (intention to drop-out, present well-being, and future well-being) were not supported by our data (the third hypothesis). These data are not in line with the literature [49,50].
In general, our data show an important analysis of which elements should be considered to foster students’ well-being. The aim of maintaining positive well-being is fundamental to ensuring that students can have a future in terms of their overall maturation.
Although the subject of well-being is often discussed in the literature [6,7,8,9,10], few models have attempted to study the relationships among the variables that lead to it among university students. To the best of our knowledge, few studies have investigated the measures that universities could take to promote protective factors in the university environment. It would be interesting to focus on whether universities promote well-being programmes to help students increase their awareness of their path, above all in terms of time perspective, self-regulation, and self-efficacy. Institutions should organise more activities to address students’ well-being and to develop models that ensure student well-being on multiple levels.
Despite the innovative perspective adopted in this study, with the assessment of the direct and indirect effects of the variables examined, it is necessary to recognise the limitations of the research. These limitations are related to the non-probabilistic sampling applied (related to the availability of participants) and the limited number of students involved. It would have been useful to involve a larger number of students and to expand the number of dimensions investigated to prepare a model that could consider more interactions with direct and indirect effects. Another limitation due to the chosen study design does not allow causal inferences to be made. The sample is almost exclusively composed of females and is derived from a single field of study (humanistic): hence, this does not support the generalisation of results. There are other demographic variables that may have an impact on students’ well-being (e.g., living conditions, international students during the COVID-19 pandemic, students with learning disabilities, etc.) and that are not considered in the present research.
These findings might be related to specific empirical suggestions that are useful for enhancing students’ well-being and professional success. It would be interesting to promote technology solutions, such as smartphone apps, to make advisors more accessible to support students in reaching out and seeking assistance [94]. Well-being support should also be empowered with platforms to conduct online well-being checks [95,96]. Research on student well-being can help administrators/educators at colleges and universities to understand the degree to which their students are self-accepting, are pursuing meaningful goals with a sense of purpose in life, have established quality ties with others, are autonomous in thought and action, can manage complex environments to suit personal needs and values and continue to grow and develop.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L.M., M.A., C.C., M.P.P.; methodology, M.L.M., M.A., C.C. and M.P.P.; formal analysis, M.A.; investigation, M.L.M., M.A., C.C., D.B., R.R., M.P.P.; data curation, M.L.M. and M.A.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L.M., MA, C.C., D.B., R.R., M.P.P.; writing—review and editing, M.L.M., M.A., C.C., D.B., R.R., M.P.P.; supervision, M.P.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of University of Cagliari (protocol code 33643_20220214, date of approval 13 February 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets for this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the students participating in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. PLS-SEM: Inner model.
Table A1. PLS-SEM: Inner model.
YpothesisRelationshipStandardized betaStandard deviationT-ValuepDecision
Path coefficient
H1ASTPI →Self regulation0.5240.0657.980<0.001Supported
H1BSTPI →Self efficacy0.4810.0637.461<0.001Supported
H1CSTPI →Intention drop out−0.3020.0823.592<0.001Supported
H1DSTPI →Well being present0.2520.0892.8420.004Supported
H1ESTPI → Well being future0.0430.0490.8910.373Not supported
H2ASelf efficacy → Intention drop out−0.1070.0791.3130.189Not supported
H2BSelf efficacy → Self regulation−0.0180.0980.1630.871Not supported
H2CSelf efficacy →Well being present0.1930.0902.1630.031Supported
H2DSelf efficacy → Well being future0.0790.0481.6700.095Not supported
H3ASelf regulation → Intention drop out0.1030.0931.1720.241Not supported
H3BSelf regulation → Well being present−0.0560.0770.7980.425Not supported
H3CSelf regulation → Well being future−0.0570.0471.2850.199Not supported
H4AIntention drop outWell being present−0.2110.0683.0610.002Supported
H4BIntention drop out → Well being future−0.0140.0480.2720.786Not supported
H5AWell being present →Well being future0.8170.0402.475<0.001Supported
Total indirect effects
H1STPI →Well being future0.3230.0654.895<0.001Supported
H1STPI → Well being present0.1280.0751.6050.109Not supported
H1STPI → Intention drop out0.0030.0580.1120.911Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation−0.0070.0480.1590.873Not supported
H2Self efficacy →Well being future0.1790.0752.3970.017Supported
H2Self efficacy → Well being present0.0240.0211.0860.278Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Intention drop out0.0000.0130.1360.892Not supported
H3Self regulation → Well being future−0.0650.0651.0690.285Not supported
H3Self regulation → Well being present−0.0210.0211.0760.282Not supported
H4Intention drop out →Well being future−0.1720.0553.0980.002Supported
Specific indirect effects
H1STPI → Intention drop out → Well being future0.0040.0150.2530.800Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Well being present → Well being future−0.0240.0340.7610.446Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being future0.0000.0030.1330.894Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being present0.0110.0101.0450.296Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Well being future0.0380.0251.5470.122Not supported
H1STPI →Self efficacy →Well being present0.0930.0461.9940.046Supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation−0.0070.0480.1590.873Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being future0.0000.0000.0320.974Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being future−0.0010.0040.1970.844Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future0.0000.0010.1240.901Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being present → Well being future0.0000.0030.1080.914Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being future0.0010.0030.2010.841Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future0.0090.0081.0530.292Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being present0.0000.0040.1090.913Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out0.0000.0060.1320.895Not supported
H1STPI →Intention drop out →Well being present →Well being future0.0520.0232.1920.028Supported
H1STPI →Well being present →Well being future0.2060.0742.8100.005Supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future−0.0090.0091.0350.301Not supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present0.0000.0010.1230.902Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present−0.0110.0111.0240.306Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Intention drop out0.0550.0511.1080.268Not supported
H1STPI →Intention drop out →Well being present0.0640.0282.1640.031Supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Well being future−0.0300.0251.2270.220Not supported
H1STPI → Self regulation → Well being present−0.0300.0410.7690.442Not supported
H1STPI →Self efficacy →Well being present →Well being future0.0760.0381.9990.046Supported
H1STPI → Self efficacy → Intention drop out−0.0510.0391.2550.210Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present0.0000.0030.1270.899Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future0.0190.0161.1100.267Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out0.0000.0130.1360.892Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being future0.0000.0010.0330.973Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being present0.0230.0191.1040.270Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Intention drop out → Well being future0.0010.0070.2070.836Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being present0.0010.0090.1090.913Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being present → Well being future0.0010.0070.1090.913Not supported
H2Self efficacy →Well being present →Well being future0.1570.0732.1600.031Supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Well being future0.0010.0070.1350.893Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future0.0000.0020.1290.897Not supported
Not supported
H3Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present → Well being future−0.0170.0171.0860.277Not supported
H3Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being present−0.0210.0211.0760.282Not supported
H3Self regulation → Well being present → Well being future−0.0460.0630.7900.430Not supported
H3Self regulation → Intention drop out → Well being future−0.0010.0070.2070.836Not supported
H4Intention drop out →Well being present →Well being future−0.1720.0553.0980.002Supported
Total effects
H1STPISelf efficacy0.4810.0637.461<0.001Supported
H1STPI →Well being future0.3660.0725.036<0.001Supported
H1STPI →Well being present0.3800.0665.700<0.001Supported
H1STPI →Intention drop out−0.2980.0644.487<0.001Supported
H1STPI →Self regulation0.5170.0549.358<0.001Supported
H2Self efficacy →Well being future0.2580.0952.7350.006Supported
H2Self efficacy →Well being present0.2170.0902.3970.017Supported
H2Self efficacy → Intention drop out−0.1070.0781.3430.179Not supported
H2Self efficacy → Self regulation−0.0180.098.1630.871Not supported
H3Self regulation → Well being future−0.1210.0801.6210.105Not supported
H3Self regulation → Well being present−0.0770.0791.0610.289Not supported
H3Self regulation → Intention drop out0.1030.0931.1720.241Not supported
H4Intention drop out →Well being future−0.1860.0682.6680.008Supported
H4Intention drop out →Well being present−0.2110.0683.0610.002Supported
H5Well being present →Well being future0.8170.0402.475<0.001Supported
Note: H1a, hypothesis 1a; H1b, hypothesis 1b; H2a, hypothesis 2a; H2b, hypothesis 2b; H3a, hypothesis 3a, H3b: hypothesis 3b; p, probability. The bold type highlights the significant effects.

References

  1. Seligman, M. PERMA and the Building Blocks of Well-Being. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018, 13, 333–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Arcidiacono, C.; di Martino, S. A Critical Analysis of Happiness and Well-Being. Where We Stand Now, Where We Need to Go. Community Psychol. Glob. Perspect. 2016, 2, 6–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Esposito, C.; di Napoli, I.; di Martino, S.; Prilleltensky, I.; Arcidiacono, C. The I COPPE Scale Short Form for Measuring Mul-tidimensional Well-Being: Construct Validity and Reliability from US, Argentinian, and Italian Large Samples. J. Community Psychol. 2022, 50, 696–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Michalos, A.C. Education, happiness and wellbeing. In Connecting the Quality of Life Theory to Health, Well-Being and Education; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 277–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ruggeri, K.; Garcia-Garzon, E.; Maguire, Á.; Matz, S.; Huppert, F.A. Well-Being is More than Happiness and Life Satisfaction: A Multidimensional Analysis of 21 Countries. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2020, 18, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ryff, C.D.; Keyes, C.L.M. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 719–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Diener, E. Subjective Well-Being. Psychol. Bulletin. 1984, 95, 542–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Diener, E.; Oishi, S.; Lucas, R.E. Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annu. Rev. Psychology. 2003, 54, 403–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Prilleltensky, I.; Dietz, S.; Prilleltensky, O.; Myers, N.D.; Rubenstein, C.L.; Jin, Y.; Mcmahon, A. Assessing Multidimensional Well-Being: Development and Validation of the i Coppe Scale. J. Community Psychol. 2015, 43, 199–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Abbas, A.; Ekowati, D.; Suhariadi, F.; Raza Hamid, S.A. Negative vs. Positive Psychology: A Review of Science of Well-Being. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Petrillo, G.; Capone, V.; Caso, D.; Keyes, C.L.M. The Mental Health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF) as a Measure of Well-Being in the Italian Context. Soc. Indic. Res. 2015, 121, 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kimbark, K.; Peters, M.L.; Richardson, T. Effectiveness of the student success course on persistence, retention, academic achievement, and student engagement. Com. College J. Res. Prac. 2017, 41, 124–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zhu, X.; Chai, W.; Shek, D.T.; Lin, L. Promotion of meaning in life and wellbeing among university students during the COVID-19 pandemic via a service-learning subject. Front. Public Health. 2020, 10, 924711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Riboldi, I.; Cavaleri, D.; Calabrese, A.; Capogrosso, C.A.; Piacenti, S.; Bartoli, F.; Carrà, G. Digital mental health interventions for anxiety and depressive symptoms in university students during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Rev. Psiquiatr. Y Salud Ment. 2020, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Groarke, J.M.; Berry, E.; Graham-Wisener, L.; McKenna-Plumley, P.E.; McGlinchey, E.; Armour, C. Loneliness in the UK during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Cross-Sectional Results from the COVID-19 Psychological Wellbeing Study. PLoS ONE. 2020, 15, e0239698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Burns, D.; Dagnall, N.; Holt, M. Assessing the Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Student Wellbeing at Universities in the United Kingdom: A Conceptual Analysis. Front. Educ. 2020, 5, 204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Holzer, J.; Lüftenegger, M.; Korlat, S.; Pelikan, E.; Salmela-Aro, K.; Spiel, C.; Schober, B. Higher Education in Times of COVID-19: University Students’ Basic Need Satisfaction, Self-Regulated Learning, and Well-Being. AERA Open 2021, 7, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Worsley, J.D.; Pennington, A.; Corcoran, R. Supporting mental health and wellbeing of university and college students: A systematic review of review-level evidence of interventions. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0266725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. du Toit, A.; Thomson, R.; Page, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of the antecedents and consequences of wellbeing among university students. Int. J. Wellbeing 2022, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zimbardo, P.G.; Boyd, J.N. Putting Time in Perspective: A Valid, Reliable Individual-Differences Metric. In Time Perspective Theory: Review, Research and Application; Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., van Beek, W., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Coudin, G.; Lima, M.L. Being Well as Time Goes by: Future Time Perspective and Well-being. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 2015, 11, 219–232. [Google Scholar]
  22. Drake, L.; Duncan, E.; Sutherland, F.; Abernethy, C.; Henry, C. Time perspective and correlates of wellbeing. Time Soc. 2008, 17, 47–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Lewin, K. Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers; Cartwright, D., Ed.; Harpers: New York, NY, USA, 1951. [Google Scholar]
  24. Denovan, A.; Dagnall, N.; Macaskill, A.; Papageorgiou, K. Future Time Perspective, Positive Emotions and StudentEngagement: A Longitudinal Study. Stud. High Educ. 2019, 45, 1533–1546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Pethtel, O.L.; Moist, M.; Baker, S. Time perspective and psychological well-being in younger and older adults. J. Posit. Psychol. Wellbeing. 2018, 2, 45–63. [Google Scholar]
  26. Avci, S. Relations between self regulation, future time perspective and the delay of gratification in university students. Education 2013, 133, 525–537. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gutiérrez-Braojos, C. Future time orientation and learning conceptions: Effects on metacognitive strategies, self-efficacy beliefs, study effort and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 35, 192–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Baird, H.M.; Webb, T.L.; Sirois, F.M.; Gibson-Miller, J. Understanding the effects of time perspective: A meta-analysis testing a self-regulatory framework. Psychol. Bulletin. 2021, 147, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Zheng, X.; Wang, W. Time perspective in the self-regulatory mechanism of meaning in life. J. Happiness Stud. 2022, 23, 747–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chung, W.T.; Lee, J.; Husman, J.; Stump, G.; Maez, C.; Done, A. Connecting to the future: How the perception of future impacts engineering undergraduate students’ learning and performance. In Proceedings of the 39th IEEE International Conference on Frontiers in Education Conference, San Antonio, TX, USA, 18–21 October 2009. [Google Scholar]
  31. Jia, Y.; Hou, Z.J.; Shen, J. Adolescents’ future time perspective and career construction: Career adaptability as mediator and hope as moderator. J. Career Dev. 2022, 49, 202–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lens, W.; Paixao, M.P.; Herrera, D.; Grobler, A. Future time perspective as a motivational variable: Content and extension of future goals affect the quantity and quality of motivation. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 2012, 54, 321–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Johnson, S.R.L.; Blum, R.W.; Cheng, T.L. Future orientation: A construct with implications for adolescent health and wellbeing. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 2014, 26, 459–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kooij, D.T.; Kanfer, R.; Betts, M.; Rudolph, C.W. Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 2018, 103, 867–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Wen Chua, L.; Milfont, T.L.; Jose, P.E. Coping Skills Help Explain How Future-Oriented Adolescents Accrue Greater Well-Being Over Time. J. Youth Adolesc. 2015, 44, 2028–2041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Mascia, M.L.; Agus, M.; Zanetti, M.A.; Pessa, E.; Penna, M.P. Academic Retention: Results from a Study in an Italian University Course; International Association for Development of the Information Society: Lisbon, Portugal, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  37. Komarraju, M.; Nadler, D. Self-Efficacy and Academic Achievement: Why Do Implicit Beliefs, Goals, and Effort Regulation Matter? Learn Individ. Differ. 2013, 25, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Chang, Y.; Edwards, J.K. Examining the Relationships Among Self-Efficacy, Coping, and Job Satisfaction Using Social Career Cognitive Theory: An SEM Analysis. J. Career Assess. 2015, 23, 35–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Zhao, F.-F.; Lei, X.-L.; He, W.; Gu, Y.-H.; Li, D.-W. The Study of Perceived Stress, Coping Strategy and Self-Efficacy of Chinese Undergraduate Nursing Students in Clinical Practice. Int. J. Nurs. Pract. 2015, 21, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Alivernini, F.; Lucidi, F. Relationship between social context, self-efficacy, motivation, academic achievement, and intention to drop out of high school: A longitudinal study. J. Educ. Res. 2011, 104, 241–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Morelli, M.; Chirumbolo, A.; Baiocco, R.; Cattelino, E. Self-regulated learning self-efficacy, motivation, and intention to drop-out: The moderating role of friendships at University. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Sabouripour, F.; Roslan, S.; Ghiami, Z.; Memon, M.A. Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy in the Relationship Between Optimism, Psychological Well-Being, and Resilience among Iranian Students. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 1663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Denovan, A.; Macaskill, A.; Uk, M.A. Stress and Subjective Well-Being among First Year UK Undergraduate Students. J. Hap-piness Stud. 2017, 18, 505–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ozer, E.M.; Bandura, A. Mechanisms Governing Empowerment Effects: A Self-Efficacy Analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 472–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Priesack, A.; Alcock, J. Well-Being and Self-Efficacy in a Sample of Undergraduate Nurse Students: A Small Survey Study. Nurse. Educ. Today 2015, 35, e16–e20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Siddiqui, S. Impact of Self-Efficacy on Psychological Well-Being among Undergraduate Students. Int. J. Indian Psychol. 2015, 2, 5–16. [Google Scholar]
  47. He, F.X.; Turnbull, B.; Kirshbaum, M.N.; Phillips, B.; Klainin-Yobas, P. Assessing Stress, Protective Factors and Psychological Well-Being among Undergraduate Nursing Students. Nurse Educ. Today 2018, 68, 4–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Suhlmann, M.; Sassenberg, K.; Nagengast, B.; Trautwein, U. Belonging mediates effects of student-university fit on well-being, motivation, and dropout intention. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 49, 16–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Wang, H.; Yang, J.; Li, P. How and When Goal-Oriented Self-Regulation Improves College Students’ Well-Being: A Weekly Diary Study. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 41, 7532–7543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Schunk, D.H.; Zimmerman, B.J. Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning: Theory, Research, and Applications. In Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning: Theory, Research, and Applications; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 1–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Zeidner, M.; Stoeger, H. Self-Regulated Learning (SRL): A Guide for the Perplexed. High Abil. Stud. 2019, 30, 9–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Schumacher, C.; Ifenthaler, D. Investigating Prompts for Supporting Students’ Self-Regulation—A Remaining Challenge for Learning Analytics Approaches? Internet High Educ. 2021, 49, 100791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Machado, C.T.; Carvalho, A.A. Concept Mapping: Benefits and Challenges in Higher Education. J. Contin. High. Educ. 2020, 68, 38–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Ramli, N.H.H.; Alavi, M.; Mehrinezhad, S.A.; Ahmadi, A. Academic Stress and Self-Regulation among University Students in Malaysia: Mediator Role of Mindfulness. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Tangney, J.P.; Boone, A.L.; Baumeister, R.F. High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success. Self-Regul. Self-Control 2018, 173–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Zimmerman, B.J. Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory Into Pract. 2002, 41, 64–70. [Google Scholar]
  57. Park, C.L.; Edmondson, D.; Lee, J. Development of self-regulation abilities as predic-tors of psychological adjustment across the first year of college. J. Adult Dev. 2012, 19, 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Durand-Bush, N.; McNeill, K.; Harding, M.; Dobransky, J. Investigating Stress, Psychological Well-Being, Mental Health Functioning, and Self-Regulation Capacity Among University Undergraduate Students: Is This Population Optimally Functioning? Can. J. Couns. Psychother. 2015, 49. Available online: https://cjc-rcc.ucalgary.ca/article/view/61066 (accessed on 20 November 2022).
  59. Wayment, H.A.; Cavolo, K. Quiet ego, self-regulatory skills, and perceived stress in college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2019, 67, 92–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Ramos Salazar, L.; Meador, A. College students’ grit, autonomous learning, and well-being: Self-control as a mediator. Psychol. Sch. 2023, 60, 53–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Tinto, V. Dropout from Higher Education: A Theoretical Synthesis of Recent Research. Rev. Educ. Res. 1975, 45, 89–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Behr, A.; Giese, M.; Teguim Kamdjou, H.D.; Theune, K. Dropping out of University: A Literature Review. Rev. Educ. 2020, 8, 614–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. De Witte, K.; Cabus, S.; Thyssen, G.; Groot, W.; van Den Brink, H.M. A critical review of the literature on school dropout. Educ. Res. Rev. 2013, 10, 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Morelli, M.; Chirumbolo, A.; Baiocco, R.; Cattelino, E. Academic failure: Individual, organizational, and social factors. Psicología Educativa. Rev. los Psicólogos la Educ. 2021, 27, 167–175. [Google Scholar]
  65. Cattelino, E.; Morelli, M.; Baiocco, R.; Chirumbolo, A. From external regulation to school achievement: The mediation of self-efficacy at school. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 60, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Larsen, M.R.; Bjørnøy, H.; Michael, S.; Larsen, S. Evidence on Dropout Phenomena at Universities; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  67. Bargmann, C.; Thiele, L.; Kauffeld, S. Motivation Matters: Predicting Students’ Career Decidedness and Intention to Drop out after the First Year in Higher Education. High. Educ. 2022, 83, 845–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kiadó, A.; Kehm, B.M.; Larsen, M.R.; Sommersel, H.B.; Larsen, S.; Kasper, P.; Kornbeck, R.M.; Kristensen, M.R.; Larsen, H.; Bjørnøy, S. Student Dropout from Universities in Europe: A Review of Empirical Literature. Hung. Educ. Res. J. 2019, 9, 147–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Faas, C.; Benson, M.J.; Kaestle, C.E.; Savla, J. Socioeconomic Success and Mental Health Profiles of Young Adults Who Drop out of College. J. Youth Stud. 2017, 21, 669–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zimbardo, P.G.; Keough, K.A.; Boyd, J.N. Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 23, 1007–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. D’Alessio, M.; Guarino, A.; De Pascalis, V.; Zimbardo, P.G. Testing Zimbardo’s Stanford Time Perspective Inventory (STPI)-Short Form. Time Soc. 2003, 12, 333–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Sibilia, L.; Schwarzer, R.; Jerusalem, M. Italian Adaptation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale. 2015. Available online: http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/italian.htm (accessed on 28 December 2022).
  73. Manganelli, S.; Alivernini, F.; Mallia, L.; Biasi, V. The Development and Psychometric Properties of the «Self-Regulated Knowledge Scale-University» (SRKS-U). ECPS-Educ. Cult. Psychol. Stud. 2015, 1, 235–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Vallerand, R.J.; Fortier, M.S.; Guay, F. Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: Toward a motivational model of high school dropout. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 72, 1161–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Hardre, P.L.; Reeve, J. A motivational model of rural students’ intentions to persist in, versus drop out of, high school. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Hair, J.F., Jr.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); Sage Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  77. Henseler, J.; Ringle, C.M.; Sinkovics, R.R. The use of partial least squares path modeling in international marketing. In New Challenges to International Marketing; Sinkovics, R.R., Ghauri, P.N., Eds.; Emerald Group Publishing Limited: Bingley, UK, 2009; pp. 277–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M.; Mitchell, R.; Gudergan, S.P. Partial least squares structural equation modeling in HRM research. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 2020, 31, 1617–1643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Chin, W.W. Commentary: Issues and Opinion on Structural Equation Modeling. MIS Q. 1998, 22, vii–xvi. Available online: http://www.jstor.org/stable/249674 (accessed on 6 October 2022).
  80. Reinartz, W.; Haenlein, M.; Henseler, J. An empirical comparison of the efficacy of covariance-based and variance-based SEM. Int. J. Res. Mark. 2009, 26, 332–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Hair, J.F., Jr.; Sarstedt, M.; Ringle, C.M.; Gudergan, S.P. Advanced Issues in Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling; SAGE Publications: Sauzendeoaks, CA, USA, 2017; 273 p. [Google Scholar]
  82. Jasp:JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.16.3) [Computer Software]; JASP Team: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  83. Hair, J.F.; Sarstedt, M.; Pieper, T.M.; Ringle, C.M. The Use of Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling in Strategic Management Research: A Review of Past Practices and Recommendations for Future Applications. Long Range Plan. 2012, 45, 320–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Schmiedel, T.; Vom Brocke, J.; Recker, J. Development and validation of an instrument to measure organizational cultures’ support of Business Process Management. Inf. Manag. 2014, 51, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Gold, A.H.; Malhotra, A.; Segars, A.H. Knowledge management: An organizational capabilities perspective. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 2001, 18, 185–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Klein, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  87. Chin, W.W. How to Write Up and Report PLS Analyses. In Handbook of Partial Least Squares; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 655–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Bellini, D.; Barbieri, B.; Barattucci, M.; Mascia, M.L.; Ramaci, T. The Role of a Restorative Resource in the Academic Context in Improving Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Flow within the Job Demands–Resources Model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2022, 19, 15263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Bellini, D.; Barbieri, B.; Mondo, M.; Cubico, S.; Ramaci, T. An Environmental Resource within the Job Demands-Resources Model: The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy between Properties of the Learning Environment and Academic Engagement. Soc. Sci. 2022, 11, 548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Riboldi, I.; Crocamo, C.; Callovini, T.; Capogrosso, C.A.; Piacenti, S.; Calabrese, A.; Lucini Paioni, S.; Moretti, F.; Bartoli, F.; Carrà, G. Testing the Impact of Depressive and Anxiety Features on the Association between Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms and Academic Performance among University Students: A Mediation Analysis. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Cataudella, S.; Carta, S.; Mascia, M.L.; Masala, C.; Petretto, D.R.; Penna, M.P. Psychological aspects of students with learning disabilities in e-environments: A mini review and future research directions. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 611818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. De Bilde, J.; Vansteenkiste, M.; Lens, W. Understanding the association between future time perspective and self-regulated learning through the lens of self-determination theory. Learn. Instr. 2011, 21, 332–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Phan, H.P.; Ngu, B.H.; McQueen, K. Future time perspective and the achievement of optimal best: Reflections, conceptualizations, and future directions for development. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ponzo, S.; Morelli, D.; Kawadler, J.M.; Hemmings, N.R.; Bird, G.; Plans, D. Efficacy of the Digital Therapeutic Mobile App BioBase to Reduce Stress and Improve Mental Well-Being among University Students: Randomized Controlled Trial. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2020, 8, e17767. Available online: https://mhealth.jmir.org/2020/4/e17767 (accessed on 7 December 2022). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Eisenstadt, M.; Liverpool, S.; Infanti, E.; Ciuvat, R.M.; Carlsson, C. Mobile Apps That Promote Emotion Regulation, Positive Mental Health, and Well-Being in the General Population: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JMIR Ment. Health 2021, 8, e31170. Available online: https://mental.jmir.org/2021/11/e31170 (accessed on 20 January 2023). [CrossRef]
  96. Ferrari, M.; Allan, S.; Arnold, C.; Eleftheriadis, D.; Alvarez-Jimenez, M.; Gumley, A.; Gleeson, J.F. Digital Interventions for Psychological Well-being in University Students: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J. Med. Internet Res. 2022, 24, e39686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual framework and hypothesis.
Figure 1. Conceptual framework and hypothesis.
Education 13 00202 g001
Figure 2. SEM-PLS application (Bootstrap method). Note: Non-significant paths (p > 0.05) are represented with dotted lines.
Figure 2. SEM-PLS application (Bootstrap method). Note: Non-significant paths (p > 0.05) are represented with dotted lines.
Education 13 00202 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of evaluated variables (n = 192).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of evaluated variables (n = 192).
VariablesMean (SD)
How true do you consider the following statements to be from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (extremely true)?
I sometimes think about dropping out of university2.67 (1.93)
I intend to drop-out of university1.40 (0.98)
Every year I consider dropping out of university1.77 (1.54)
STPI Future perspective30.67 (6.32)
Self-efficacy Scale36.13 (7.62)
Self-regulation—knowledge extraction3.87 (0.96)
Self-regulation—knowledge networking 3.53 (1.06)
Self-regulation—knowledge practice4.10 (0.90)
Self-regulation—knowledge critique 3.29 (1.00)
Self-regulation—knowledge monitoring 4.30 (0.71)
Coppe_Present well-being40.36 (12.80)
Coppe_Future well-being46.23 (12.79)
Coppe_Overall well-being13.03 (4.08)
Coppe_Interpersonal well-being14.43 (4.44)
Coppe_Community well-being11.04 (4.39)
Coppe_Occupation well-being12.79 (4.67)
Coppe_Phisical well-being13.02 (4.67)
Coppe_Psychological well-being11.56 (4.46)
Coppe_Economical well-being12.23 (4.29)
Table 2. PLS-SEM: Measurement—Outer model.
Table 2. PLS-SEM: Measurement—Outer model.
ConstructLatent Variable LoadingsDijkstra-Henseler’s Rho ACronbach’s Alpha Average Variance Extracted (AVE)Adjusted R2
Self regulationFrom 0.549 to 0.7080.6300.6040.3890.278
Intention to drop-outFrom 0.746 to 0.9260.9030.8220.7330.120
Self-efficacyFrom 0.756 to 0.8380.9330.9250.6020.235
STPI Future Temporal perspectiveFrom 0.408 to 0.7440.8440.8170.419
Well-being presentFrom 0.698 to 0.8650.8820.8770.5820.243
Well-being futureFrom 0.696 to 0.8690.8930.8870.6040.754
Table 3. Heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlation.
Table 3. Heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlation.
Self_EfficacyWell Being FutureWell Being PresentSTPIIntention to Drop-OutSelf Regulation
Well being future0.405
Well being present0.3780.973
STPI0.5060.4160.430
Intention to dropout0.2270.3350.3560.331
Self regulation0.3210.2030.2000.7130.139
Table 4. Variance inflation factors (VIF)—Internal values.
Table 4. Variance inflation factors (VIF)—Internal values.
Well Being FutureWell Being PresentIntention to Drop-OutSelf Regulation
Self_efficacy1.3491.3011.2891.288
Well being present1.282
STPI1.8261.7441.6481.288
Intention to drop-out1.1671.112
Self regulation1.3671.3621.349
Table 5. PLS-SEM: Significant paths in the Inner model.
Table 5. PLS-SEM: Significant paths in the Inner model.
Hypothesis RelationshipStandardised BetaStandard DeviationT-ValuepDecision
Direct path coefficient
H1ASTPI → Self regulation0.5240.0657.980<0.001Supported
H1BSTPI → Self efficacy0.4810.0637.461<0.001Supported
H1CSTPI → Intention drop-out−0.3020.0823.592<0.001Supported
H1DSTPI → Wellb_pres0.2520.0892.8420.004Supported
H2CSelf_efficacy → Wellb_pres0.1930.0902.1630.031Supported
H4AIntention drop-out → Wellb_pres−0.2110.0683.0610.002Supported
H5AWellb_pres → Wellb_fut0.8170.0402.475<0.001Supported
Total indirect effect
H1STPI → Wellb_fut0.3230.0654.895<0.001Supported
H2Self_efficacy → Wellb_fut0.1790.0752.3970.017Supported
H4Intention drop-out → Wellb_fut−0.1720.0553.0980.002Supported
Specific indirect effect
H1STPI → Self_efficacy → Wellb_pres0.0930.0461.9940.046Supported
H1STPI → Intention drop-out → Wellb_pres → Wellb_fut0.0520.0232.1920.028Supported
H1STPI → Wellb_pres → Wellb_fut0.2060.0742.8100.005Supported
H1STPI → Intention drop-out → Wellb_pres0.0640.0282.1640.031Supported
H1STPI → Self_efficacy → Wellb_pres → Wellb_fut0.0760.0381.9990.046Supported
H2Self_efficacy → Wellb_pres → Wellb_fut0.1570.0732.1600.031Supported
H4Intention drop out → Wellb_pres → Wellb_fut−0.1720.0553.0980.002Supported
Total effect
H1STPI → Wellb_fut0.3660.0725.036<0.001Supported
H1STPI → Wellb_pres0.3800.0665.700<0.001Supported
H1STPI → Intention drop-out−0.2980.0644.487<0.001Supported
H1STPI → Self regulation0.5170.0549.358<0.001Supported
H2Self_efficacy → Wellb_fut0.2580.0952.7350.006Supported
H2Self_efficacy → Wellb_pres0.2170.0902.3970.017Supported
H4Intention drop-out → Wellb_fut−0.1860.0682.6680.008Supported
Note: H1a: hypothesis 1a; H1b: hypothesis 1b; H2a: hypothesis 2a; H2b: hypothesis 2b; H3a: hypothesis 3a, H3b: hypothesis 3b; p: probability; STPI: Zimbardo’s Stanford Time Perspective Inventory; Wellb_pres: Well being present; Wellb_fut: Well being future.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mascia, M.L.; Agus, M.; Cabras, C.; Bellini, D.; Renati, R.; Penna, M.P. Present and Future Undergraduate Students’ Well-Being: Role of Time Perspective, Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation and Intention to Drop-Out. Educ. Sci. 2023, 13, 202. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13020202

AMA Style

Mascia ML, Agus M, Cabras C, Bellini D, Renati R, Penna MP. Present and Future Undergraduate Students’ Well-Being: Role of Time Perspective, Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation and Intention to Drop-Out. Education Sciences. 2023; 13(2):202. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13020202

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mascia, Maria Lidia, Mirian Agus, Cristina Cabras, Diego Bellini, Roberta Renati, and Maria Pietronilla Penna. 2023. "Present and Future Undergraduate Students’ Well-Being: Role of Time Perspective, Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation and Intention to Drop-Out" Education Sciences 13, no. 2: 202. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13020202

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop