Next Article in Journal
LIDAR Scan Matching in Off-Road Environments
Previous Article in Journal
Collection and Analysis of Human Upper Limbs Motion Features for Collaborative Robotic Applications
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Use of UTAUT and Post Acceptance Models to Investigate the Attitude towards a Telepresence Robot in an Educational Setting

1
Department of Computer Education, Cheongju National University of Education, Cheongju 28690, Korea
2
Sheffield Robotics and Faculty of Arts, Computing, Engineering and Sciences, Sheffield Hallam University, Sheffield S1 1WB, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Robotics 2020, 9(2), 34; https://doi.org/10.3390/robotics9020034
Submission received: 27 February 2020 / Revised: 3 May 2020 / Accepted: 11 May 2020 / Published: 13 May 2020

Abstract

:
(1) Background: in the last decade, various investigations into the field of robotics have created several opportunities for further innovation to be possible in student education. However, despite scientific evidence, there is still strong scepticism surrounding the use of robots in some social fields, such as personal care and education. (2) Methods: in this research, we present a new tool named the HANCON model, which was developed merging and extending the constructs of two solid and proven models—the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT) model used to examine the factors that may influence the decision to use a telepresence robot as an instrument in educational practice, and the Post Acceptance Model used to evaluate acceptability after the actual use of a telepresence robot. The new tool is implemented and used to study the acceptance of a double telepresence robot by 112 pre-service teachers in an educational setting. (3) Results: the analysis of the experimental results predicts and demonstrate a positive attitude towards the use of telepresence robot in a school setting and confirm the applicability of the model in an educational context. (4) Conclusions: the constructs of the HANCON model could predict and explain the acceptance of social telepresence robots in social contexts.

1. Introduction

A robot is defined by the International Organization of Standardization (ISO) as “a programmable device that can move and perform tasks in its environment” [1]. This meaning includes robotic devices ranging from fully autonomous robots to remote-controlled robots such us telepresence robots. Currently, no consensual definition of robots exists, due to the rapid evolution of this technology.
However, the term “robotics” includes a variety of research sub-areas: social robotics, involving robots that engage in social interaction with humans through speech, gestures, or other means of communication; assistive robotics, which generally involves robots that assist people with physical and neurodevelopmental disabilities. Another sub-area of robotics is Socially Assistive Robotics (SAR), a fast-emerging field that has developed from the intersection of these two and involves robots that are designed to help through advanced interaction which is driven by user needs via multimodal interfaces [2].
Technological advances in the last decades have boosted the area of robotics and resulted in fast growth of possible applications, with a consequent solid impact on people’s daily lives. Thanks to evidence from various studies and the use of new robotic platforms concerning applications in social contexts, education [3] and care [4] have received particular consideration. However, notwithstanding the extensive work done in human-robot interaction and technology acceptance, suggest that advances in robotics require supplemental research [5].
Based on the above, this study was conducted using the Double robot, a telepresence robot in an educational setting. To evaluate the acceptability of the participants we used a questionnaire inspired by the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT) model [6], while the Post acceptance model [7] was used to evaluate attitudes to the continued use of the robot after its initial use. Currently, recent literature in the field of human-robot interaction reports a higher frequency of use of a single questionnaire than two or more, to investigate the participant’s acceptability or aptitude towards robotic technology. Usually, the questionnaire used is based on a single theoretical model (e.g., UTAUT, TAM, etc.), and is filled out in the final part of the experiment, after the interaction with the social robot [8]. In this way, important information about the “before” of the interaction could be inexorably lost.
In this study we used two different models, UTATUT and PAM, highlighting the clear difference between before use and after the actual use of the robot. The innovative aspect of the research is given by the use of a robust model such as the PAM model to evaluate post acceptance in the robotics field, because often the model used “before” and “after” the interaction is the same. The purpose of our research was to confirm the reliability of the proposed model using a questionnaire inspired by two solid models, UTAUT and PAM, and its applicability in an educational context. In this paper, we proposed an analysis of the perception of a telepresence robot as an instrument for their future use in an actual educational setting. However, we would remark that the application to education is offered as a proof-of-concept, whereas the fundamental aim of the research presented in this article was to develop a new acceptance model that could be applied in many other social settings.

2. Related Work

In the last decades, robots are starting to become a part of working life in many sectors including journalism, agriculture, the military, medicine such as surgery, and education [9]. A factor influencing the attitude toward robots may be a concern over the risk of unemployment caused by robots, considering certain occupations are even at risk of being replaced by robots or other technology [10].
Europeans interviewed in a recent Eurobarometer survey (n = 26.751) generally showed a positive view of robots, although they do not feel comfortable with robots in some specific areas, such as the care of children, the elderly, and the disabled. In detail, the survey stated that 60% of Europeans surveyed thought robots should be “banned” from such care activities [11]. In a study conducted by Taipale et al. [12] the participants showed reluctance to use the robot in various areas, including childcare, care for elderly, leisure and education. In another recent European survey [13] only 26% of respondents showed that they were comfortable “with having a robot to provide services and companionship to the infirm or elderly” or “with having a medical operation performed on them by a robot”. This result could be linked to the common perception that people have of robots. Robots are considered as dangerous and technically powerful machines, which could be mainly useful in those activities where humans are not available, for example: in military applications, in space exploration, and industries. For this reason, the purposes of current robotics research focus on adapting the robot’s appearance and behaviour to improve end-user acceptance [14,15]. In another recent study with an Italian sample, the authors compared the acceptance of practitioners and students who would be future practitioners. They reported that as experienced practitioners they felt sceptical and perceived the assistive robot as an expensive and limited tool, although the sample showed an overall positive attitude towards the use of the robot [4].
In recent decades, extensive researches on the factors that can influence the acceptance by possible users and on how such acceptance can be increased have been conducted. Examining technology acceptance is closely related to research fields of social acceptance and attitudes in general. In detail, the deployment of new technology about social and human factors has been studied within the concept of technology acceptance [16], and based on the theory of reasoned action [17]. In general, attitudes refer to fairly constant positive, negative, and neutral evaluations of an object or concept [18]. Some studies have shown that attitudes could be defined as “a type of knowledge structure stored in memory” [19], where other studies have also connected attitudes more tightly to neurological processes [20]. Additionally, the acceptability of robots to people is an important matter which depends on several variables, where the acceptance is described as the “robot being willingly incorporated into the person’s life” and implies long-term usage [21].
The literature suggests that individual users’ psychological variables could influence the person’s acceptance process [22], and their social and physical environment [23]. Heerink [24], suggested that participants with a higher level of education were less open to perceiving the robot as a social entity. The implication that adults can respond to technology differently than young people has been shown by Scopelliti et al. [25]. While the effects of age and anxiety on robots have been studied by Nomura et al. [26]. The results showed that young people who experienced humanoid robots directly or through the media had higher levels of anxiety towards robots than those aged 50 to 60. Women were more sceptical about using robots than men, as also reported by Arras and Cerqui [27]. Gross et al. [28] found that the sample, although initially negative, started to appreciate the benefits and found the robot more acceptable after spending one day using it. The novelty effects may initially improve Perceived Enjoyment (PE) but then decrease over time, potentially resulting in lower acceptance of the robot in the longer term. Specifically, De Graaf’s [29] and Torta et al. [30] suggest that PE reduced over six to eight months. Considering that it is easier to form a clear vision of robots if there are already previous encounters in the individual’s life, the literature suggests that attitudes based on direct experience are more extreme and less ambivalent [31]. In fact, before a subject has his first direct experience with robots, he forms a mental perception that conditions subsequent responses and attitudes towards robots.
The past personal experience and second-hand sources of information external to the individual, such as science fiction and the media, influence these mental models. In a recent study, Savela et al. [32] found that when the participants did not have actual experiences with the robot in question, negative attitudes were more likely to be reported in the studies. For this reason, the lack of first hand or direct experiences forces people to resort on their social representations or mental images of robots. These seem to influence attitudes towards them, as confirmed by attitudes theories [31]. Currently, the research focused on technology that already exists around automated robotic devices and telepresence robots, instead of emerging technology such as autonomous service robots. Telepresence robots were highly approved by patients [33] and workers [34], especially regarding home care.
Recently Benitti [35] examined the scientific literature on the use of robotics in schools, concluding that appropriate use of educational robotics can act as an element that improves learning. In particular, robotic assistants have the potential to overcome concerns about the physical effects of a student’s use of computer-based tools, because they encourage the scholar to be active during a play [36]. Additionally, the robot can be a practical learning partner that motivates students, arousing learning performance naturally [37]. In a recent article, the authors specified that in educational settings robots are accepted in work tasks related to education, and attitudes toward educational robots were neutral and robots could be imagined in subjects such as science, technology, engineering, and mathematics [38]. However, respondents were reluctant to participate in teaching provided by a robot and could not imagine a robot in subjects such as social sciences or art [38].
In recent years, robotics research has shown numerous benefits of using robot in the treatment of children with special needs and neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [39], in particular a new direction is to create partially automatic robots in combination with machine learning strategy [40].

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Technology Acceptance Model (TAM)

The Diffusion of Innovations model (DoI) [41], the Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) [42], and the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) [43] have examined variables that motivate individuals to accept new Information Systems (IS), and how they do it. These attitude theories suggested that “intention” is the strongest and most immediate predictor of individual behaviour [43]. The theoretical association comes from Cognitive Dissonance Theory (CDT), which suggests that users may experience cognitive dissonance or psychological tension if their pre-acceptance usefulness perceptions are disconfirmed during actual use [44]. Rational users may try to remedy this dissonance by distorting or modifying their usefulness perceptions in order to be more consistent with reality. Davis et al. [45], and Taylor and Todd [46], empirically validated a strong correlation between intentions and behaviours in IS usage contexts.
In an empirical analysis conducted by Bhattacherjee [47], attitude theories hold that human behaviours are influenced by subjective perceptions, though such perceptions are biased or inaccurate; consequently, perceived rather than objective assessment (e.g., third party) usefulness is relevant. Specifically, the first studies on technology acceptance modelling can be traced back to Davis with the Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) [42]. This model, used for different types of technology, states that the user’s perception of the usefulness and ease of use of a system determines the intention and subsequently the actual use of the system itself.
Frequently consumers show unrealistically low or high initial expectations of new innovative services because they are unsure what to expect from them. Although low initial expectations are easily confirmed, these expectations themselves may be adjusted upwards as a result of their usage experience, if customers realize that their initial expectations may have been unrealistically low. Similarly, unreasonably high initial expectations may be lowered throughout a service’s initial use, as some of those expectations are unconfirmed [48]. The higher or lower level of expectations obtained may then serve to motivate or demotivate further usage intentions and defined continuance. Results of Bhattacherjee’s study [47] support that satisfaction and Perceived Usefulness (PU) are strong predictors of consumers’ intention to continue IS services. Specifically, PU was identified as a secondary determinant of continuance intention, and loyalty incentives did not have any significant effect on continuance intention. PU refers to users’ subjective probability that IS use will improve their performance [45], and therefore captures the instrumentality or rational component of their usage decision. Satisfaction is conceptually distinct from the attitude in that satisfaction is a transient, experience-specific affect, while attitude is a relatively more enduring affect transcending all prior experiences [49]. Tse and Wilton [50] have shown that satisfaction and attitude differ in their predictive abilities, while Oliver [48] suggested that satisfaction temporally and causally precedes post-purchase attitude in a path-analytic model. Hunt [51] argues that attitude is an emotion, but satisfaction is an evaluation of that emotion. As described earlier, drawing from TAM, PU captures the instrumentality of IS use, while ease of use taps into the self-efficacy dimension. Because PU and FC are the primary motivators of IS acceptance, it is plausible that they can also influence subsequent continuance decisions.
In another research, Venkatesh et al. [6] published an inventory of current models and factors and presented a model called the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT). The UTAUT was developed as a model of general technology acceptance that aims to unify eight existing models of technology acceptance and usage behaviour. In the UTAUT model proposed by Heerink et al. [52], they defined the constructs represented by a few questions and the scores for the constructs can be mapped and interrelated.

3.2. Post Acceptance Model

While existing studies have tended to investigate individuals’ decisions to initially adopt an Information Technology (IT), there is less attention paid to the post-adoption environment where individuals decide on the continued or discontinued use of an IT. Contrarily, in consumer behaviour literature, research into consumers’ satisfaction and re-purchase decisions shows the expectancy–confirmation paradigm as a dominant theme (e.g., [53,54]). The Expectation–Confirmation Theory (ECT) is widely used in consumer behaviour literature to study consumer satisfaction, post-purchase behaviour (e.g., repurchase, complaining), and service marketing in general [50].
Specifically, Oliver’s process [48] where consumers reach repurchase intentions in an ECT is as follows: consumers initially form an expectation of a specific product/service before purchase. They subsequently accept and use that product/service, but only after an initial consumption period they manage to form perceptions about its performance. They assess its perceived performance vis-a-vis their original expectation and determine “Confirmation 2”, namely the extent to which their expectation is confirmed. Finally, consumers form a satisfaction or affect, based on their confirmation level and expectation on which that confirmation was based and form a repurchase intention, while dissatisfied users discontinue subsequent use. Churchill and Surprenant [53], added that the consumer’s expectations are confirmed when the product/service performs as much as expected; negatively disconfirmed when it performs worse than expected, and positively disconfirmed when it performs better than expected.
In the Information Technology (IT) literature, Bhattacherjee [7] proposes an Expectation-Confirmation Model (ECM) of IT continuance based on the congruence between individuals’ continued IT usage decisions and consumers’ repeat purchase decisions. The purpose of Bhattacherjee’s studies [7] was to understand continued use or “continuation”, in contrast to initial use or “acceptance”. Continuance in Information Systems (IS) research has been examined variously as “implementation” [55], “incorporation” [56], and “routinization” [57] in IS literature. IS and IT are often considered synonymous, but IT is a subset of IS. Hence, the ECM suggests that post-adoption expectations are the relevant determinants of a user’s level of satisfaction with an IT, instead of pre-adoption expectations. In the expectancy-confirmation paradigm, the expectation is commonly defined as individual beliefs or a sum of beliefs about the levels of attributes possessed by a product/service (e.g., [49]). Since, among the various beliefs in IT adoption research, Perceived Usefulness (PU) is the most consistent antecedent of a user’s Intention To Use (ITU), and consequently, IT is the logical choice as a surrogate for post-adoption expectations (e.g., [58]). Moreover, the ECM does not include the performance variable, as it presumes that the influence of performance is already accounted for by the confirmation variable [7].
Pioneering studies [59,60] attempted to integrate variables from different adoption perspectives (e.g., TAM, TPB, Innovation Diffusion) into a single framework in order to improve the explanation of the initial adoption behaviour. Consistent with the view in ECM that post-adoption expectations refer to users’ beliefs about the attributes possessed by an IT [7], the post-adoption expectations in the proposed model are represented by PU, perceived Facilitating Conditions in their use (FC) and Perceived Enjoyment (PE). Previous empirical evidence has shown that perceived FC is one of the major cognitive beliefs in determining users’ affect (attitude) towards technology adoption (e.g., [45]). Specifically, in motivation research, there are two types of motivation: intrinsic and extrinsic [61]. PE can be described as an intrinsic motivation, whereas perceived usefulness in TAM is an example of extrinsic motivation [62].
Considering that the process of confrontation in disconfirmation judgments requires the deliberate processing of information, Oliver confirms that the expectancy disconfirmation paradigm is mainly cognitive [48]. The cognitive and affective responses in post-purchase judgments may be seen as distinct components in response to environmental events, and each would appear to introduce its own influence on the consumption process.

3.3. Overview of Construct Interrelations

The model inspired by UTAUT includes the following constructs: Anxiety (ANX), Attitude Towards Technology (ATT), Facilitating Conditions (FC), Intention to Use (ITU), Perceived Adaptiveness (PAD), Perceived Enjoyment (PENJ), Perceived Ease of Use (PEOU), Perceived Sociability (PS), Perceived Usefulness (PU), Social Influence (SI), Social Presence (SP), Trust, and Use [4,52]. Instead, the constructs of the post acceptance model are: IS continuance intention, Satisfaction, Perceived Usefulness, and Confirmation [7].
From the combination of parts of these two models, we identified 15 constructs as potential direct determinants of intention to use and post-acceptance use. We selected only those constructs that were in adherence with the objectives of the present research. Specifically, in the first part of the questionnaire we did not investigate the constructs: PEOU, which is the degree to which one believes that using the system would be free of effort, and USE, which is the actual use of the system over a longer period in time. We decided to insert the PEOU construct in the second part of the questionnaire, after the real use of the telepresence robot. Furthermore, in the second part of the questionnaire we inserted all constructs of the post acceptance model but in some cases we modified the name, but not the meaning, of the construct (e.g., IS continuance intention as Post Intention To Use (PITU); Satisfaction as User Satisfaction (US); and Confirmation as Met expectation (ME).
Of these constructs, we theorize six to play a significant role as direct determinants of intention to use (ITU): perceived usefulness (PU), gender, perceived enjoyment (PENJ), trust technology (TTrust), attitude (ATT), and social influence (SI). Whereas perceived adaptivity (PAD), anxiety (ANX), perceived sociability (PS), social presence (SP), and facilitating conditions (FC) are theorized but not direct determinants of intention to use (ITU).
Furthermore, intention to use (ITU) determines met expectation (ME). We identify perceived ease of use (PEOU) as directly determined by met expectation (ME) but determines for user satisfaction (US) and post intention to use (PITU).
Figure 1 visualizes this model, featuring the following hypothetical construct interrelations that will be tested in our experiments.
The hypotheses considered were:
  • H1—Intention to use (ITU) is determined by (a) perceived usefulness (PU), (b) perceived enjoyment (PENJ), (c) attitude (ATT), (d) trust of technology (TTrust), (e) social influence (SI), and (f) gender.
  • H2—Perceived usefulness (PU) is influenced by (a) perceived adaptability (PAD), (b) anxiety (ANX), and (c) trust of technology (TTrust).
  • H3—Perceived enjoyment (PENJ) is influenced by (a) social presence (SP), (b) perceived sociability (PS), and (c) trust of technology (TTrust).
  • H4—Perceived sociability (PS) is influenced by trust of technology (TTrust).
  • H5—Social influence (SI) is influenced by facilitating conditions (FC).
  • H6—Social presence (SP) is influenced by perceived sociability (PS).
  • H7—Post-intention to use (PITU) is determined by (a) user satisfaction (US) and (b) perceived ease of use (PEOU).
  • H8—User satisfaction (US) is influenced by (a) met expectation (ME) and (b) perceived ease of use (PEOU).
  • H9—Perceived ease of use (PEOU) is influenced by met expectation (ME).
  • H10—Met expectation (ME) is influenced by the intention to use (ITU).
We decide to name the model “HANCON”, as part of the authors’ surnames.

3.4. The Instrument

The questionnaire used includes 45 items, takes from the models UTAUT and PAM models. In details, items 1 to 34 were administered before the actual participants use of the telepresence robot, and items 35 to 45 were administered only after the real interaction.
The questionnaire was completed anonymously by the participants and the answers were given on a Likert five-point scales: (1) Strongly Disagree, (2) Disagree, (3) Neither Agree nor Disagree, (4) Agree and (5) Strongly Agree, taking into account variables that can be influenced after use of the telepresence robots.
The questionnaire was based on studies by Heerink et al. [52] and Conti et al. [4]. Considering that these studies refer to social robots, we have replaced in the questionnaire the term “robot” with “telepresence robots” in order to promote a better understanding of the participants. Furthermore, we have modified and added some elements, to attempt to stay as close to the original form as possible: for the PS, we replaced two items “I feel the robot understands me” and “I think the robot is nice” with three items “I consider the student via the telepresence robot a pleasant conversational as much as the other students in my class”, “I find the student via the telepresence robot pleasant to interact in the classroom with the other students”, and “I feel the tele-operating student via telepresence robot can well interact with me as a teacher”. On behalf of SP, we replaced three items “I can imagine the robot to be a living creature”, “I often think the robot is not a real person”, and “Sometimes the robot seems to have real feelings”, with “I felt like the tele-operating student via the telepresence robot to be in the classroom”. Finally, we decide to rename the “Trust” construct with “TTrust” which indicates the Trust Technology that is the belief that the system performs with personal integrity and reliability. We decided to replace the following two items: “I would trust the robot if it gave me advice” and “I would follow the advice the robot gives me”, with three items “I think that the head image quality and reliability visible on the telepresence robot screen is not good”, “I think that the sound quality and reliability the telepresence robot is good” and “I think that the movement quality and reliability of telepresence robot is not good”. These items TTrust have been verified in a pilot test with nine Korean participants [63], where the clarity of the items was confirmed. See the Appendix A for a complete view of the questionnaire.

3.5. Experimental Setup

3.5.1. The Telepresence Robotic Platforms

The telepresence robot used for the experiment was a Double robot and a remote-control platform with a notebook PC equipped with a video communication camera. This robot had a 9.7-inch tablet PC and a visual effect that realizes an anthropomorphic upright posture. It could move around on two wheels using a gyro sensor and park. It was connected to the school Wi-Fi or LTE network and showed sufficient performance for video call and robot manoeuvring. It had a built-in speaker and could communicate clearly with participants in the classroom. As shown in Figure 2, the Double robot wore a t-shirt to personify it as a student because people are inclined to interact with the robots whose personalities conform to the robot’s occupational role [64], and the LED circle light on the center of the t-shirt was turned on to make it easy for a participant to recognize when his/her voice was transmitted into robot platform. Considering that when people interact with robots, they have impressions of the robots in terms of perceived robot personality [65], we made these changes with the aim of promoting a social aspect in the setting.

3.5.2. Participants

A total of 112 undergraduate students (n = 112, Males = 34, Females = 78, M-age = 21.5 years, range = 20–23, SD = 1.06) were recruited as pre-service teachers with teaching experience in South Korea schools. Specifically, with “pre-service teachers” we mean university students with educational curricula who carry out the apprenticeship in educational settings, while “teachers” are those who work as teachers in schools. The recruitment had been conducted through posting on the university board from October 2018. The participation in the experiment was voluntary, and all personal information obtained was anonymized except for the participant’s gender and age. The participant was free to withdraw the experiment at any time, for whatever reason.
We did not consider the influence of age by adopting the pre-service teachers with an average age of 21.5 years. The female proportion of pre-service teachers was of 69.5%, and this gender imbalance reflects the population ratio, as being a teacher is a very popular occupation for women in South Korea. In order to test the methodology proposed in the experiment, we carried out a pilot experiment with seven pre teacher students (n = 7, Males = 2, Females = 5). Only for this occasion did we use a KUBI telepresence robot. In conclusion, excluding seven participants who had previously participated in the pilot experiment, the participants were a total of 105. All the participants we included had no previous experience of interaction with telepresence robotics platforms, nor had the use of robots been previously presented to them as an instrument of support for teachers and learners.
Ethical approval was obtained by the Ethical Committee of the Cheongju National University of Education. Informed consent to participate and to use data for scientific research was obtained from all participants prior to the study. The methods were carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations for human subjects.

3.5.3. Experimental Procedure

The experiment was conducted at the university building for four weeks, where a total of 16 slots were planned. The participants were assigned to a group with less than six people in a randomized manner.
The Double telepresence robot base could move in forward, backward, right, and left directions, and it was remotely controlled by detecting the gestures of the participants. For example, the telepresence robot could easily look at a student who called his name and move to the corresponding position, thanks to features such as camera image recognition, microphone voice recognition and interaction based on various sensors. The teacher should be close to individual pupils or the classroom as a whole to be more effective [66]. For this reason, robot mobility during the instruction was a learning benefit for the experiment.
The experimentation took place in two different rooms (no. 1 and no. 2) on the same floor and were 50 m apart. Specifically, classroom 1 was used for interaction with the telepresence robot, while classroom no. 2 for remote control. During all the sessions in each class, a research assistant was present. In the experiment procedure, the first part was conducted by the research assistant K, who explained the experiment purpose, and gave an example of robot-assisted learning (Figure 3), and provided a brief description of the robot hardware configuration. Only for the pilot session the KUBI telepresence robot was used, as an example. This is shown in Figure 4.
Simultaneously, in room no. 2 the research assistant B remotely controlled the Double telepresence robot in the classroom. The research assistant B via a telepresence robot was in front of the participants’ desks and blackboard, as shown in Figure 5a,b.
The research assistant K as a teacher, and the robot by research assistant L as a student, showed teaching and learning demonstration of mathematics problem-solving (Figure 6).
After 5 min of class demonstration, one of the participants moves to the remote-control room (Figure 7). In this phase, the participant controls the robot by himself and interacts with the other participants for 5 min (Figure 8). In this stage, all the participants were expecting the direct remote control of the telepresence robot. In the final part of the experiment, and after the administration of the second part of the questionnaire, the debriefing phase took place. At this stage, the experimenter disclosed to the participant the purpose and nature of the experiment and to answer any questions that the participant asked about the experiment.
The total session time was 40 min. One ticket for coffee was provided for each participant at the end of the experiment.

3.6. Data Analyses

The data from questionnaires were analysed using statistical package R. To validate the questionnaire, Cronbach ’s Alpha for each variable was calculated.
The average and standard deviation of each variable were calculated, and Shapiro-Wilk normality test was performed. Since the assumption of normality is rejected, hypotheses were tested with correlations (exploratory analysis) and linear regression analysis (confirmative) by the nonparametric methods. The Mann-Whitney test was conducted for gender, Spearman’s rho analysis was used to examine correlations between variables, and Kendall-Theil Sen Siegel nonparametric regression analysis was used for the correlations.
The Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 24 was used for statistical analyses.

4. Results

The results showed that the use of the HANCON model with telepresence robots reported a Cronbach Alpha of = 0.509 for FC and 0.443 for PAD. This result was lower than in a recent study [4] on the acceptance of robotics were the Cronbach alpha values of ANX, FC, and PAD constructs were below 0.6. The authors [4] imputed the Cronbach’s alpha of FC as a result of limited experience and use of a social humanoid robot. In this study, we found that pre-service Korean teachers considered FC (item 8-network infra, average = 3.21) to be slightly better than the FC (item 9-facility condition, average = 2.88) for telepresence robots. Table 1 shows the questionnaire structure with the number of items and Cronbach’s alpha for each construct. The two items of FC were removed, while PAD was improved from 0.443 to 0.681, removing item no. 14, used in [4]. Cronbach’s Alpha of FC was 0.427. Moreover, Cronbach’s Alpha of PAD with items no.13, no.14, and no.15 was 0.384. For this reason, we removed items no.13 and no.14 of PAD because the Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.526 without item no.13 and 0.594 without items no.13 and no.14.
We conducted the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality, as shown in Table 2. Furthermore, we used the nonparametric statistical analysis since all the variables were not assumed to be normally distributed. In the case of gender construct, Mann-Whitney U = 1024 was not significant by p-value = 0.312 for ITU.
The nonparametric Spearman rho correlation is shown in Table 3. Correlations were not significantly correlated in only a few other cases and were highly relevant. This supported the hypotheses (H1 to H10) for UTAUT and PAM models.
The results of the nonparametric regression analysis in Table 4 show significant results except for hypothesis H6. In other words, the students’ PS connected via a telepresence robot does not affect the SP. Nevertheless, PS and SP have a significant impact on PENJ.
In addition to this, to obtain a further solidity of the study, we compare the results obtained in this study (left), with results of a Korean teacher sample (N = 110) (right), as shown in Table 5. We show the descriptive statistics of the constructs: the minimum (Min) and maximum (Max), standard deviation (SD), and the percentage of positive (POS), and negative (NEG) perception of the participants.
Finally, in Figure 9 we reported the final model, where interrelations were confirmed by regression scores for the experiments, while the dotted line indicated that it is not confirmed by any regression analysis.

5. Discussion and Conclusions

In this article, we presented the development and validation of a new acceptance model, named HANCON, to study robotics applications in social context. In a proof-of-concept study, we used a Double telepresence robot that could move and get closer to the student, to make learning more effective. Considering the comparison between robots and computers with students, the literature showed results more effective with a robot in learning of a second language compared to computer [67]. Additionally, Hyun et al. [68] showed a robot’s media effectiveness compared to computers in word recognition in reading, story building, vocabulary, and understanding activities in a kindergarten setting.
Obviously, there are preservice teachers, teachers or educators who have a negative view of robot-assisted learning, which is currently being studied in various fields. The development of robot technology and robot-assisted learning must be an objective of technology acceptance that may soon be found. Currently, the literature shows the results of the UTAUT model using humanoids robots. In this study, we used a telepresence robot and a new model, named by us as the HANCON model, which integrates two already solid models (UTAUT and PAM). This model was used here for assisted learning from a robot connected remotely based on a video call.
The HANCON model showed a predictive force and solid constructs. These findings suggest that in general this model could be used to predict and explain the acceptance of social telepresence robots in different contexts. Specifically, the variables that significantly influence the intention to use were: perceived usefulness, attitude, social influence and perceived enjoyment. However, social presence is not influenced by perceived sociability although it has an important role for social enjoyment.
Finally, several limitations need to be considered. First, the sample of participants who participated in the research, pre-service teachers and teachers, could be considered small in order to define the solidity of a model. Second, the sample came from the same university. We cannot know if this could influence the evaluation of the system and the answers given by the participants. Third, we cannot evaluate the personal system experience of the participant before the study. Fourth, we used only a telepresence robot. It would be interesting to see the results of other studies that use different types of robots with different characteristics. In future works we may have a larger sample of participants, use different telepresence robots, and investigate different cultural contexts with different types of robots.
In conclusion, this research has shown that the possibilities of future empirical investigations to further develop this field of study are varied and increasingly interesting. However, the impact of acceptability variables requires further and in-depth examination with the involvement of larger samples, with different robots, with applications in real-life conditions, with robust longitudinal study designs that also evaluate the context differences (e.g., clinical-rehabilitative setting), and possible intercultural components. Finally, it may be important to know how psychological factors can impact users’ perceptions of how easy robots would be to use.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, J.H. and D.C.; methodology, J.H.; validation, J.H. and D.C., formal analysis, J.H.; investigation, J.H.; resources, J.H.; data curation, J.H.; writing—original draft preparation, D.C.; writing—review and editing, J.H. and D.C.; visualization, D.C.; supervision, J.H.; funding acquisition, J.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research has been fully supported by Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (NRF-2015R1D1 A1A09060450).

Acknowledgments

Special thanks go to all pre-teachers who participated in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards by the Ethical Committee of the Cheongju National University of Education (number 1301-201810-HR-0001-01) and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Questionnaires in Korean (used in this study) and for comparison in English, code, constructs, definition, and items.
Figure A1. Questionnaires in Korean (used in this study) and for comparison in English, code, constructs, definition, and items.
Robotics 09 00034 g0a1

References

  1. ISO; DIS. 8373: 2012. Robots and Robotic Devices–Vocabulary; International Standardization Organization (ISO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  2. Feil-Seifer, D.; Mataric, M.J. Defining Socially Assistive Robotics. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics, Chicago, IL, USA, 28 June–1 July 2005; pp. 465–468. [Google Scholar]
  3. Conti, D.; Cirasa, C.; di Nuovo, S.; di Nuovo, A. Robot, tell me a tale!: A Social Robot as tool for Teachers in Kindergarten. Interact. Stud. 2020, 21, 221–243. [Google Scholar]
  4. Conti, D.; di Nuovo, S.; Buono, S.; di Nuovo, A. Robots in education and care of children with developmental disabilities: A study on acceptance by experienced and future professionals. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2017, 9, 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Conti, D.; Cattani, A.; di Nuovo, S.; di Nuovo, A. Are Future Psychologists Willing to Accept and Use a Humanoid Robot in Their Practice? Italian and English Students’ Perspective. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Venkatesh, V.; Morris, M.; Davis, G.; Davis, F. User acceptance of information technology: Toward a unified view. MIS Q. 2003, 27, 425–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Bhattacherjee, A. Understanding information systems continuance: An expectation-confirmation model. MIS Q. 2001, 351–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Whelan, S.; Murphy, K.; Barrett, E.; Krusche, C.; Santorelli, A.; Casey, D. Factors affecting the acceptability of social robots by older adults including people with dementia or cognitive impairment: A literature review. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2018, 10, 643–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mubin, O.; Stevens, C.J.; Shahid, S.; al Mahmud, A.; Dong, J.-J. A Review of the Applicability of Robots in Education. Technol. Educ. Learn. 2013, 1, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Frey, C.B.; Osborne, M.A. The future of employment: How susceptible are jobs to computerisation? Technol. Forecast. Soc. Chang. 2017, 114, 254–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. European Commission. Special Eurobarometer 382—Public Attitudes towards Robots. European Commission: Brussels, Belgium, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  12. Taipale, S.; de Luca, F.; Sarrica, M.; Fortunati, L. Robot shift from industrial production to social reproduction. In Social Robots from a Human Perspective; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2015; pp. 11–24. [Google Scholar]
  13. European Commission. Special Eurobarometer 460—Attitudes towards the Impact of Digitisation and Automation on Daily Life; European Commission: Brussels, Belgium, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  14. Broadbent, E.; Stafford, R.; MacDonald, B. Acceptance of Healthcare Robots for the Older Population: Review and Future Directions. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2009, 1, 319–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kanda, T.; Miyashita, T.; Osada, T.; Haikawa, Y.; Ishiguro, H. Analysis of humanoid appearances in human–robot interaction. IEEE Trans. Robot. 2008, 24, 725–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Venkatesh, V.; Davis, F.D. A Theoretical Extension of the Technology Acceptance Model: Four Longitudinal Field Studies. Manag. Sci. 2000, 46, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research; Contemporary Sociology; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ajzen, I. Attitudes, traits, and actions: Dispositional prediction of behavior in personality and social psychology. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1987; Volume 20, pp. 1–63. [Google Scholar]
  19. Fabrigar, L.R.; Wegener, D.T. Attitude structure. In Advanced Social Psychology: The State of the Science; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 177–216. [Google Scholar]
  20. Wagner, W.; Kronberger, N.; Seifert, F. Collective symbolic coping with new technology: Knowledge, images and public discourse. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 41, 323–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Broadbent, E.; Tamagawa, R.; Kerse, N.; Knock, B.; Patience, A.; MacDonald, B. Retirement home staff and residents’ preferences for healthcare robots. In Proceedings of the RO-MAN 2009-The 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive, Toyama, Japan, 27 September–2 October 2009; pp. 645–650. [Google Scholar]
  22. Conti, D.; Commodari, E.; Buono, S. Personality factors and acceptability of socially assistive robotics in teachers with and without specialized training for children with disability. Life Span Disabil. 2017, 20, 251–272. [Google Scholar]
  23. Wu, Y.; Wrobel, J.; Cornuet, M.; Kerhervé, H.; Damnée, S.; Rigaud, A.-S. Acceptance of an assistive robot in older adults: A mixed-method study of human–robot interaction over a 1-month period in the Living Lab setting. Clin. Interv. Aging 2014, 9, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Heerink, M. Exploring the influence of age, gender, education and computer experience on robot acceptance by older adults. In Proceedings of the 2011 6th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), Lausanne, Switzerland, 8–11 March 2011; pp. 147–148. [Google Scholar]
  25. Scopelliti, M.; Giuliani, M.V.; Fornara, F. Robots in a domestic setting: A psychological approach. Univers. Access Inf. Soc. 2005, 4, 146–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Nomura, T.; Sugimoto, K.; Syrdal, D.S.; Dautenhahn, K. Social acceptance of humanoid robots in Japan: A survey for development of the frankenstein syndorome questionnaire. In Proceedings of the 2012 12th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots (Humanoids 2012), Osaka, Japan, 29 November–1 December 2012; pp. 242–247. [Google Scholar]
  27. Arras, K.O.; Cerqui, D. Do We Want to Share Our Lives and Bodies with Robots? A 2000 People Survey: A 2000-People Survey; Technical Report; Swiss Federal Institute of Technology: Zurich, Switzerland, 2005; Volume 605. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gross, H.-M.; Schroeter, C.; Mueller, S.; Volkhardt, M.; Einhorn, E.; Bley, A.; Langner, T.; Merten, M.; Huijnen, C.; van den Heuvel, H.; et al. Further progress towards a home robot companion for people with mild cognitive impairment. In Proceedings of the 2012 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), Seoul, Korea, 14–17 October 2012; pp. 637–644. [Google Scholar]
  29. de Graaf, M.M.; Allouch, S.B.; Klamer, T. Sharing a life with Harvey: Exploring the acceptance of and relationship-building with a social robot. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 43, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Torta, E.; Werner, F.; Johnson, D.O.; Juola, J.F.; Cuijpers, R.H.; Bazzani, M.; Oberzaucher, J.; Lemberger, J.; Lewy, H.; Bregman, J. Evaluation of a small socially-assistive humanoid robot in intelligent homes for the care of the elderly. J. Intell. Robot. Syst. 2014, 76, 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Olson, J.M.; Maio, G.R. Attitudes and Social Behavior. In Handbook of Psychology; Weiner, I.B., Ed.; John Wiley & Sons: New Jersey, NJ, USA, 2003; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
  32. Savela, N.; Turja, T.; Oksanen, A. Social acceptance of robots in different occupational fields: A systematic literature review. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2018, 10, 493–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Cherry, C.O.; Chumbler, N.R.; Richards, K.; Huff, A.; Wu, D.; Tilghman, L.M.; Butler, A. Expanding stroke telerehabilitation services to rural veterans: A qualitative study on patient experiences using the robotic stroke therapy delivery and monitoring system program. Disabil. Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 2017, 12, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Holm, S.G.; Angelsen, R.O. A descriptive retrospective study of time consumption in home care services: How do employees use their working time? BMC Health Serv. Res. 2014, 14, 439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Benitti, F.B.V. Exploring the educational potential of robotics in schools: A systematic review. Comput. Educ. 2012, 58, 978–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Tanaka, F.; Movellan, J.R.; Fortenberry, B.; Aisaka, K. Daily HRI evaluation at a classroom environment: Reports from dance interaction experiments. In Proceedings of the 1st ACM SIGCHI/SIGART Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 2–3 March 2006; pp. 3–9. [Google Scholar]
  37. Chang, C.-W.; Lee, J.-H.; Wang, C.-Y.; Chen, G.-D. Improving the authentic learning experience by integrating robots into the mixed-reality environment. Comput. Educ. 2010, 55, 1572–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Reich-Stiebert, N.; Eyssel, F. Learning with educational companion robots? Toward attitudes on education robots, predictors of attitudes, and application potentials for education robots. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2015, 7, 875–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Scassellati, B.; Admoni, H.; Matarić, M. Robots for Use in Autism Research. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 2012, 14, 275–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. di Nuovo, A.; Conti, D.; Trubia, G.; Buono, S.; di Nuovo, S. Deep learning systems for estimating visual attention in robot-assisted therapy of children with autism and intellectual disability. Robotics 2018, 7, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Rogers, E. Diffusion of Innnovations, 5th ed.; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  42. Davis, F. Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS Q. 1989, 13, 319–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance; Stanford University Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 1957; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
  45. Davis, F.D.; Bagozzi, R.P.; Warshaw, P.R. User acceptance of computer technology: A comparison of two theoretical models. Manag. Sci. 1989, 35, 982–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Taylor, S.; Todd, P.A. Understanding information technology usage: A test of competing models. Inf. Syst. Res. 1995, 6, 144–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bhattacherjee, A. An empirical analysis of the antecedents of electronic commerce service continuance. Decis. Support Syst. 2001, 32, 201–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Oliver, R.L. A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. J. Mark. Res. 1980, 17, 460–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Oliver, R.L.; Linda, G. Effect of satisfaction and its antecedents on consumer preference and intention. Adv. Consum. Res. 1981, 8, 88–93. [Google Scholar]
  50. Tse, D.K.; Wilton, P.C. Models of consumer satisfaction formation: An extension. J. Mark. Res. 1988, 25, 204–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hunt, H.K. CS/D-overview and future research directions. In Conceptualization and Measurement of Consumer Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction; Marketing Science Institute: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1977; pp. 455–488. [Google Scholar]
  52. Heerink, M.; Kröse, B.; Evers, V.; Wielinga, B. Assessing acceptance of assistive social agent technology by older adults: The almere model. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2010, 2, 361–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Churchill, G.A., Jr.; Surprenant, C. An investigation into the determinants of customer satisfaction. J. Mark. Res. 1982, 19, 491–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Oliver, R.L. Cognitive, affective, and attribute bases of the satisfaction response. J. Consum. Res. 1993, 20, 418–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Zmud, R.W. Diffusion of modern software practices: Influence of centralization and formalization. Manag. Sci. 1982, 28, 1421–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Kwon, T.H.; Zmud, R.W. Unifying the fragmented models of information systems implementation. In Critical Issues in Information Systems Research; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1987; pp. 227–251. [Google Scholar]
  57. Cooper, R.B.; Zmud, R.W. Information technology implementation research: A technological diffusion approach. Manag. Sci. 1990, 36, 123–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Venkatesh, V. Determinants of Perceived Ease of Use: Integrating Control, Intrinsic Motivation, and Emotion into the Technology Acceptance Model. Inf. Syst. Res. 2000, 11, 342–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Moore, G.C.; Benbasat, I. Development of an instrument to measure the perceptions of adopting an information technology innovation. Inf. Syst. Res. 1991, 2, 192–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Venkatesh, V.; Morris, M.G. Why don’t men ever stop to ask for directions? Gender, social influence, and their role in technology acceptance and usage behavior. MIS Q. 2000, 115–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Vallerand, R.J. Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1997; Volume 29, pp. 271–360. [Google Scholar]
  62. Davis, F.D.; Bagozzi, R.P.; Warshaw, P.R. Extrinsic and intrinsic motivation to use computers in the workplace 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 22, 1111–1132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Han, J.-H. UTAUT Model of Pre-service Teachers for Telepresence Robot-Assisted Learning. J. Creat. Inf. Cult. 2018, 4, 95–102. [Google Scholar]
  64. Tay, B.; Jung, Y.; Park, T. When stereotypes meet robots: The double-edge sword of robot gender and personality in human–robot interaction. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 38, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Robert, L.; Alahmad, R.; Esterwood, C.; Kim, S.; You, S.; Zhang, Q. A Review of Personality in Human‒Robot Interactions. Found. Trends Inf. Syst. 2020, 4, 107–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Han, J. Emerging technologies: Robot assisted language learning. Lang. Learn. Technol. 2012, 16, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  67. Han, J.; Jo, M.; Park, S.; Kim, S. The educational use of home robots for children. In Proceedings of the ROMAN 2005. IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Nashville, TN, USA, 13–15 August 2005. [Google Scholar]
  68. Hyun, E.; Kim, S.; Jang, S.; Park, S. Comparative study of effects of language instruction program using intelligence robot and multimedia on linguistic ability of young children. In Proceedings of the RO-MAN 2008-The 17th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Munich, Germany, 1–3 August 2008; pp. 187–192. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Hypothetical construct interrelations for the HANCON model.
Figure 1. Hypothetical construct interrelations for the HANCON model.
Robotics 09 00034 g001
Figure 2. Double telepresence robot with t-shirt used in the experiment.
Figure 2. Double telepresence robot with t-shirt used in the experiment.
Robotics 09 00034 g002
Figure 3. Examples of robot-assisted learning with KUBI telepresence robot.
Figure 3. Examples of robot-assisted learning with KUBI telepresence robot.
Robotics 09 00034 g003
Figure 4. KUBI telepresence robot.
Figure 4. KUBI telepresence robot.
Robotics 09 00034 g004
Figure 5. (a,b) Examples of interaction with the Double robot.
Figure 5. (a,b) Examples of interaction with the Double robot.
Robotics 09 00034 g005
Figure 6. Demonstration of mathematics problem-solving.
Figure 6. Demonstration of mathematics problem-solving.
Robotics 09 00034 g006
Figure 7. Participants interaction for robot-assisted learning.
Figure 7. Participants interaction for robot-assisted learning.
Robotics 09 00034 g007
Figure 8. A participant controls the robot interacting with the other participants.
Figure 8. A participant controls the robot interacting with the other participants.
Robotics 09 00034 g008
Figure 9. Final model: interrelations confirmed by regression scores for the experiments. Dotted line: not confirmed by any regression analysis.
Figure 9. Final model: interrelations confirmed by regression scores for the experiments. Dotted line: not confirmed by any regression analysis.
Robotics 09 00034 g009
Table 1. Constructs Cronbach’s alpha.
Table 1. Constructs Cronbach’s alpha.
CodeConstructNo. Items [4]No. Items HANCON Cronbach’s Alpha
ANXAnxiety440.626
ATTAttitude330.887
FCFacilitating Conditions220.509
ITUIntention To Use330.827
PADPerceived ADaptability33→20.443→0.681
PENJPerceived ENJoyment550.869
PSPerceived Sociability440.868
PUPerceived Usefulness330.701
SISocial Influence220.737
SPSocial Presence530.803
TTrustTrust Technology (Reliability)230.619
PEOUPerceived Ease of Use-30.610
USUser Satisfaction-30.878
MEMet Expectation-30.901
PITUPost Intention To Use-20.614
Table 2. Shapiro-Wilk test of normality.
Table 2. Shapiro-Wilk test of normality.
Code AverageShapiro-Wilk StatisticDfp-Value
ANX3.340.9731050.030
ATT3.710.9311050.000
FC3.050.9661050.008
ITU3.360.9671050.009
PAD3.380.9661050.008
PENJ3.770.9491050.001
PS3.330.9431050.000
PU3.840.9271050.000
SI3.320.9071050.000
SP3.200.9611050.003
TTrust3.240.9731050.028
PEOU3.380.9611050.003
US3.630.9321050.000
ME3.630.9181050.000
PITU4.050.9021050.000
Table 3. Correlation matrix for the participants among the scales of the questionnaire (N = 105).
Table 3. Correlation matrix for the participants among the scales of the questionnaire (N = 105).
ANXATTITUPADPENJPSPUSISPTTrustPEOUUSMEPITU
ANX1
ATT0.234 *1
ITU−0.2500.673 ***1
PAD−0.1450.561 ***0.627 ***1
PENJ−0.207 *0.626 ***0.582 ***0.574 ***1
PS−0.212 *0.594 ***0.564 ***0.452 ***0.695 ***1
PU−0.193 *0.595 ***0.583 ***0.633 ***0.642 ***0.605 ***1
SI−0.1210.509 ***0.504 ***0.476 ***0.543 ***0.582 ***0.554 ***1
SP−0.215 *0.414 ***0.479 ***0.370 ***0.564 ***0.628 ***0.553 **0.681 ***1
TTrust−0.310 **0.351 ***0.409 ***0.234 ***0.394 ***0.502 ***0.421 ***0.460 ***0.569 ***1
PEOU−0.262 **0.244 *0.288 **0.269 ***0.425 ***0.485 ***0.274 **0.284 **0.383 ***0.485 ***1
US−0.237 *0.435 ***0.460 **0.499 ***0.587 ***0.562 ***.571 **0.408 ***0.454 ***0.527 ***0.568 ***1
ME−0.1860.481 ***0.401 ***0.440 ***0.579 ***0.512 ***.453 ***0.330 **0.392 ***0.486 ***0.550 ***0.780 ***1
PITU−0.1400.398 ***0.486 ***0.549 ***0.516 ***0.459 ***.621 ***0.389 ***0.463 ***0.314 **0.415 ***0.663 ***0.547 ***1
*** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Table 4. Linear regression analyses.
Table 4. Linear regression analyses.
ModelsHypothesisIndependent VariableDependent VariableInterceptBetaTaup-Value
TAMH1PUITU−0.2320.9710.470<0.001 ***
PENJ0.7590.7140.462<0.001 ***
TTrust1.830.5000.323<0.001 ***
ATT0.6620.7520.566<0.001 ***
SI1.030.6600.414<0.001 ***
H2PADPU1.360.6600.538<0.001 ***
ANX4.37−0.124−0.150<0.05 *
TTrust2.760.3400.324<0.001 ***
H3TTrustPENJ2.410.4480.309<0.001 ***
PS1.770.6060.560<0.001 ***
SP1.810.6000.441<0.001 ***
H4TTrustPS1.460.6020.401<0.001 ***
H6PSSP0.6700.7770.5210.222
PAMH7USPITU1.910.5990.558<0.001 ***
PEOU2.880.3730.327<0.001 ***
H8MEUS0.0600.9850.689<0.001 ***
PEOU1.730.5990.461<0.001 ***
H9MEPEOU1.390.5710.449<0.001 ***
H10ITUME2.680.3300.329<0.001 ***
*** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Table 5. Comparison between pre teachers and teachers: constructs analysis. Highest percentages and significant differences are in bold.
Table 5. Comparison between pre teachers and teachers: constructs analysis. Highest percentages and significant differences are in bold.
ConstructPre-Service Teachers (Students)TeachersMean Difference
MeanMaxMinSDPOS (%)NEG (%)MeanMaxMinSDPOS (%)NEG(%)
ANX3.345.001.000.7562233.285.001.001.1557320.07
ATT3.715.001.000.9077153.185.001.001.1545453.11 **
FC3.055.001.000.9742342.695.001.001.0631512.18 *
PAD3.385.001.000.768294.175.001.000.918052.90 **
PENJ3.775.001.000.7977123.755.001.000.9083120.11
PS3.335.001.000.9061293.355.001.000.9954290.21
PU3.845.001.000.708553.685.001.000.9375150.94
SI3.325.001.000.8558193.285.001.001.0451250.55
SP3.205.001.000.9256343.345.001.001.0648310.72
TTrust3.244.671.000.7655293.175.001.000.8243290.99
ITU3.365.001.000.8660223.265.001.000.9655310.67
PEOU3.385.001.670.7862273.205.001.001.0446281.12
US3.635.001.000.8371173.535.001.000.9460200.83
ME3.635.001.000.8171113.625.001.000.9968150.05
PITU4.055.001.000.728633.705.001.001.1069201.68
*** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Han, J.; Conti, D. The Use of UTAUT and Post Acceptance Models to Investigate the Attitude towards a Telepresence Robot in an Educational Setting. Robotics 2020, 9, 34. https://doi.org/10.3390/robotics9020034

AMA Style

Han J, Conti D. The Use of UTAUT and Post Acceptance Models to Investigate the Attitude towards a Telepresence Robot in an Educational Setting. Robotics. 2020; 9(2):34. https://doi.org/10.3390/robotics9020034

Chicago/Turabian Style

Han, Jeonghye, and Daniela Conti. 2020. "The Use of UTAUT and Post Acceptance Models to Investigate the Attitude towards a Telepresence Robot in an Educational Setting" Robotics 9, no. 2: 34. https://doi.org/10.3390/robotics9020034

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop