Next Article in Journal
Rhamnolipids as a Tool for Eradication of Trichosporon cutaneum Biofilm
Next Article in Special Issue
Early Application of Ipsilateral Cathodal-tDCS in a Mouse Model of Brain Ischemia Results in Functional Improvement and Perilesional Microglia Modulation
Previous Article in Journal
Role of Inflammation and Redox Status on Doxorubicin-Induced Cardiotoxicity in Infant and Adult CD-1 Male Mice
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dynamic DNA Methylation Changes in the COMT Gene Promoter Region in Response to Mental Stress and Its Modulation by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation

by
Ariane Wiegand
1,2,*,
Arne Blickle
1,
Christof Brückmann
1,
Simone Weller
3,
Vanessa Nieratschker
1,4,† and
Christian Plewnia
3,†
1
Tübingen Center for Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Molecular Psychiatry, University Hospital Tübingen, Calwerstraße 14, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
2
International Max Planck Research School for Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
3
Tübingen Center for Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Neurophysiology & Interventional Neuropsychiatry, University Hospital Tübingen, Calwerstraße 14, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
4
Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Prof. Nieratschker and Prof. Plewnia contributed equally.
Biomolecules 2021, 11(11), 1726; https://doi.org/10.3390/biom11111726
Submission received: 20 October 2021 / Revised: 9 November 2021 / Accepted: 16 November 2021 / Published: 19 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Key Advances in Brain Stimulation)

Abstract

:
Changes in epigenetic modifications present a mechanism how environmental factors, such as the experience of stress, can alter gene regulation. While stress-related disorders have consistently been associated with differential DNA methylation, little is known about the time scale in which these alterations emerge. We investigated dynamic DNA methylation changes in whole blood of 42 healthy male individuals in response to a stressful cognitive task, its association with concentration changes in cortisol, and its modulation by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). We observed a continuous increase in COMT promotor DNA methylation which correlated with higher saliva cortisol levels and was still detectable one week later. However, this lasting effect was suppressed by concurrent activity-enhancing anodal tDCS to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Our findings support the significance of gene-specific DNA methylation in whole blood as potential biomarkers for stress-related effects. Moreover, they suggest alternative molecular mechanisms possibly involved in lasting behavioral effects of tDCS.

1. Introduction

Epigenetic patterns are known to be dynamic and associated with environmental factors. Without altering the DNA sequence, epigenetic modifications affect chromatin structure and gene expression. Currently, one of the best studied epigenetic modifications is DNA methylation (DNAm), which plays an important role in gene regulation [1]. One factor which has consistently been associated with differential DNAm is stress [2,3]. Many studies link early life stress to long lasting differences in DNAm [4,5] or correlate severe psychiatric symptoms caused by traumatic and stressful life events with differential DNAm profiles [6]. Hence, epigenetic alterations might be an underlying mechanism how exposure to stress increases the risk of developing psychiatric disorders. Nevertheless, only little is known about the short-term dynamics of methylation changes after stress exposure. Immediate changes in DNAm can be induced by chemical stressors, such as dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) [7,8,9], and can already occur within 20 min after T-cell activation [10]. Furthermore, an experimental psychological stressor, the Trier Social Stress Test, has shown to be associated with dynamic DNAm alterations in a stress-associated gene within 90 min after exposure [11]. The dynamic malleability of methylation changes in genes involved in cognitive stress is, therefore, a potentially critical mechanism for the regulation of human behavior. However, a precise characterization of the degree and time course of these changes is required. Moreover, opportunities to influence this process would be useful and may open new perspectives for individualized therapeutic strategies.
To this aim, we use transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a non-invasive brain stimulation technique, which has been shown to modulate neuroplasticity [12]. Many studies have demonstrated the impact of tDCS on cognitive processes and training [13,14]. Most importantly it has been discussed as a potential treatment approach for neuropsychiatric disorders which are often associated with aberrant brain activation patterns [15]. However, so far little is known about the underlying molecular mechanisms of stimulation effects and how they potentially manifest as long-lasting cognitive improvements and amelioration of psychiatric symptoms. Since epigenetic modifications present a mechanism of how environmental factors can influence physiological reactions and, moreover, seem to be involved in the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders, they might also be important for the manifestation of tDCS effects.
There is accumulating evidence that genetic factors interact with stimulation effects and contribute to inter-individual variability in tDCS responses [16]. Particularly, the Val108/158Met polymorphism of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene that regulates the dopamine metabolism [17] is associated with differential tDCS effects on executive functions [16,18]. COMT is involved in the degradation of dopamine and, therefore, plays a critical role in cognitive processes and executive functioning [19,20]. A physiological concentration of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex is important for optimal cognitive functioning [21] and a dysregulation of the dopaminergic system is associated with the pathophysiology of neurological and psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and depression [22]. Furthermore, it has been shown that acute stress leads to an activation of the dopaminergic system [23,24], and the COMT gene seems to be crucially involved in the stress response [25]. The COMT genotype, which influences the enzyme’s stability and hence dopaminergic activity [17], is also associated with an altered cortisol response [26,27]. Therefore, the promotor region of this gene appears to be a promising candidate to exemplify the epigenetic signatures of mental stress and its malleability by tDCS.
Thus, the present study aims at (i) determining the effects of tDCS on task performance, negative affect, and the physiological stress response, (ii) testing the notion that DNAm of the COMT gene is subject to immediate modulation by mental stress, and (iii) providing initial evidence that tDCS can influence this process.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The study sample was recruited in two cohorts. As a pilot study, 22 healthy participants (mean age: 23.6 years, SD = 3.0; mean years of formal education: 16.9, SD = 3.3) took part in the experiment. The effects of tDCS on task performance and affect were described in Wiegand et al. (2019) in more detail [28]. See Supplementary Figures S1 and S2 for COMT DNAm and cortisol data from this pilot cohort. Since, to our knowledge, this is the first human study investigating dynamic DNAm in the context of cognitive stress and its modulation by tDCS, no previously reported effect sizes for a power and sample size estimation were available. To increase reliability of our findings, we replicated the same experiment with another 20 healthy participants (mean age: 23.3 years, SD = 3.5; mean years of formal education: 14.4, SD = 6.2). See Supplementary Figures S3 and S4 for COMT DNAm and cortisol data from this replication cohort. Since there were no prominent differences between the data from the two cohorts, results in the main manuscript are reported for the merged cohort including all 42 participants. The inclusion criteria, experimental procedure, and sample handling and storage were the same for both cohorts, and instructions were given by the same instructor using a detailed script. All participants were recruited within two years. The two cohorts showed no significant differences with respect to age (t(40) = −0.39, p = 0.97) or years of education (t(40) = 1.13, p = 0.26). To reduce inter-individual DNAm variability, all participants were aged between 18–30 years, male, non-smoking, and of European descent. Furthermore, screening excluded participants with a history of mental or neurological illness, relevant somatic disorders (two participants were suffering from hypothyroidism), dyscalculia, metallic foreign particles around the head, a cardiac pacemaker, and the usage of psychotropic or other medication that may impact DNAm status (two participants took L-thyroxine). All participants were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (laterality index = 98.06, SD = 6.44) [29] and German native speakers. Prior to study inclusion, all participants gave written informed consent to the experimental procedure approved by the University of Tübingen local ethics committee. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki in its latest version.

2.2. Adaptive 2-Back Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT)

Participants were exposed to an adaptive, 2-back version of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT) [28]. Numbers ranging from 1 to 9 were continuously presented via headphones. Participants were asked to add the current number to the number presented before the previous one (2-back) and to type in their answer by pressing a correspondingly labeled keyboard button. Parallel to the next stimulus presentation, they received visual feedback, i.e., the screen flashed green for a correct answer and red for an incorrect, late or missed answer. The inter-stimulus interval between digit presentations adapted to participants’ performance. Initially set to 3 s, it was decreased by 0.1 s after four consecutive correct answers and increased 0.1 s after four consecutive wrong answers. The PASAT consisted of 16 practice trials followed by three task blocks lasting for 5 min, which were separated by breaks of 30 s. Due to the adaptive design the error percentage remained similar, although the number of correct trials could vary between task blocks.

2.3. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)

To assess changes in negative affect during the experimental procedure, participants were administered the German version of the ‘Positive and Negative Affect Schedule’ (PANAS), a self-report to determine the participants’ current affective states [30,31]. Ten positive and ten negative adjectives were rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 ‘not at all’ (in German: ‘gar nicht’) to 5 ‘very much’ (in German: ‘äußerst’). Participants completed the PANAS three times throughout each session: before starting the PASAT (pre), immediately after they completed the PASAT (post), and 90 min after task completion (follow-up).

2.4. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS)

A direct current of 1 mA was generated by a portable, battery-driven stimulator (NeuroConn GmbH, Illmenau, Germany) and applied via a pair of 5 × 7 cm electrodes covered with conductive paste (Ten20®, Weaver and Company, Aurora, CO, USA). The anodal electrode was placed over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex at F3 according to the international 10–20 system of electrode placement [32], whereas the cathodal reference electrode was fixated on the right upper arm over the deltoid muscle to prevent any opposite polarization of other brain regions that were not the target of the stimulation protocol [33]. Two minutes before PASAT onset, the current was faded in for 5 s. During the anodal stimulation session, a continuous current of 1 mA was delivered for 20 min until task completion and then faded out for another 5 s. During sham stimulation, the current was only administered for 30 s before fading out. Impedance was controlled by the device and did not exceed 10 kΩ. To ensure blinding effectiveness, participants were asked for tDCS adverse effects at the end of each session (see Supplementary Table S1).

2.5. Experimental Procedure

The experimental design was identical to Wiegand et al. (2019), where the behavioral data and changes in affect of the pilot cohort are described in more detail [28].
The study followed a single-blind, sham-controlled cross-over design. Each participant took part in two sessions with an interval of 7 days in between. To reduce variability, each session started at 2 PM. To ensure that the inclusion criteria were met, a brief screening including the Symptom-Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R) to detect psychiatric symptoms and distress was performed in the first session [34]. Apart from that, the two sessions only differed in the type of stimulation (anodal or sham) participants received. The order of stimulation was randomized and counterbalanced across participants.
Each session started with a saliva sampling. Then, a venous catheter was placed, and the tDCS electrodes were fixated. Affective states were assessed (PANAS pre), and the instructions for the PASAT were given. The first blood sampling was done just before the stimulation started, but at least 15 min after the venous access had been established. Afterwards, participants were exposed to the 2-back PASAT while receiving tDCS (anodal or sham). Immediately after task completion, the second blood sample was collected and the PANAS (post) was administered. After removal of the electrodes, participants were exposed to relaxing music (genre: ambient electronic) via headphones until the end of the experiment, which was 90 min after task completion. Four more blood samples were collected 20, 40, 60, and 90 min after task completion. In addition, a second saliva sample was collected 30 min after the beginning of the 2-back PASAT. Finally, participants were administered the PANAS for a third time (follow-up). Supplementary Figure S5 depicts the experimental procedure graphically.

2.6. DNAm Analysis

Blood samples were collected in EDTA tubes (2.7 mL Monovette®, Sarstedt AG & Co. KG, Nümbrecht, Germany) and stored at −80 °C. DNA was extracted with the QIAamp Blood Mini Kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) according to manufacturer’s instructions using 400 µL whole blood sample. To increase DNA yield, the final elution step was repeated using the 100 µL eluate of the first elution. DNA was quantified using the Qubit® 2.0 Fluorometer (Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA, USA). Samples were stored at −20 °C.
Five hundred nanograms of genomic DNA were bisulfite converted using the EpiTect Fast Bisulfite Kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Bisulfite-converted DNA was eluted with 20 µL of the provided elution buffer. The purified bisulfite-converted DNA was stored at −20 °C.
A region-specific polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was performed for the S-COMT promoter region using the PyroMark PCR Kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) according to manufacturer’s instructions with previously published primers (F: 5-GAGTAGGTTGTGGATGGGTTGTA-3, R: 5-Biotin-ACATTTCTAAACCTTACCCCTCTA-3) [35]. Successful amplification and specificity of the PCR products was verified and visualized via agarose gel electrophoresis.
DNAm was analyzed by pyrosequencing on a PyroMark Q24 system (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) using 5 µL biotinylated PCR product of each sample and a previously published sequencing primer (S: 5-GTAATATAGTTGTTAATAGTAGA-3) [35]. As in previous studies using the same pyrosequencing assay, DNAm levels of two CpG sites (hg19 reference genome coordinates: chr22:19,950,055 and chr22:19,950,064) were quantified using the PyroMark Q24 Software 2.0 (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). Each sample was analyzed twice, and the mean percentage was used for further analysis. Samples with a deviation ≥ 3% between duplicates were repeated. To detect disparate amplification of unmethylated DNA fragments, a titration assay using standardized bisulfite-converted control DNA samples (EpiTect Control DNA, Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) with established DNAm levels of 0%, 25%, 50%, 75%, and 100% DNAm was performed.

2.7. Saliva Cortisol Concentration

Saliva was sampled in Salivettes® (Sarstedt AG & Co. KG, Nümbrecht, Germany) and stored at −80 °C. For analysis of cortisol levels, Salivettes® were thawed and centrifuged for 2 min at 1000× g to collect saliva. Cortisol concentrations were determined using the Cortisol Saliva ELISA kit (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany) according to manufacturer’s instructions. Cortisol concentrations were determined in duplicates, and the mean coefficient of variation was below 10%.

2.8. Statistical Analysis

All statistical calculations were performed using the software R (Version 3.5.1) [36] including the package nlme [37]. The two cohorts were pooled for data analysis. Mean numbers of correct trials for each of the three task blocks were extracted from the adaptive 2-back PASAT as measure of task performance. For the PANAS questionnaire, mean scores were calculated for the 10 items comprising negative affect. Since DNAm at the two analyzed CpG sites was highly correlated at all time points (r > 0.88, p < 3.43 × 10−14), DNAm of the COMT gene promoter region was expressed as the mean level of methylation of the two CpG sites.
Multilevel modeling was chosen over repeated-measures ANOVA to allow analyses of the effects of stimulation and session within the same statistical model. For all analyses (i.e., task performance, affect changes, DNAm changes and cortisol level changes), a multilevel model with the fixed effects stimulation, session, and time (or task block accordingly in task performance data analyses) was estimated using maximum likelihood. A random intercept for each participant and random slopes for the effects of session and time (or task block) were included to account for individual differences in the outcome variable, in the effect of session, and in the effect of time (or task block) within each session. The error term was modeled as a first order autoregressive process to account for serial autocorrelations due to the repeated measures design. The severity of multicollinearity was assessed by the variance inflation factor (VIF). To assure a VIF < 10, the interaction of stimulation and session and, hence, the three-way interaction of stimulation, session, and time (or task block) was eliminated from the models for task performance, DNAm and cortisol levels [38,39]. For changes in affect, a full model was estimated as VIF < 10 was given for all predictors. A linear model was fitted for the analyses of task performance, DNAm and cortisol levels, whereas a quadratic term for time was included in the model for affect resulting in a better fit for changes over time. Unstandardized (B) as well as standardized (β) parameter estimates were reported and statistical significance was assessed at p < 0.05. Additionally, an analysis of variance table for each model is given in the Supplementary Tables S2–S5 reporting the overall significance of all terms [40]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were performed after significant effects.
Similarly, a multilevel model was fitted with the fixed effects stimulation in first session and session, including only DNAm data of the first time point of each session, to examine whether DNAm changes induced in the first session were preserved until the second session with respect to the type of stimulation received during the first session. Random intercepts estimated for each participant and random slopes for the effect of session were included to account for individual variance.
Finally, Pearson’s correlation was used to test for an interrelation between changes in DNAm levels (COMT-methylationpost90-COMT-methylationpre) and cortisol concentration (cortisolpost-cortisolpre) and between changes in negative affect (negative affectpost-negative affectpre) and in cortisol concentration during the first session.

3. Results

3.1. Study Sample

Participants were randomly assigned to the order of stimulation (anodal/sham or sham/anodal) they received during the two experimental sessions. The two resulting groups showed no significant differences with respect to age (t(40) = 1.52, p = 0.14), years of formal education (t(40) = 1.06, p = 0.30), math performance at school (t(36) = −0.54, p = 0.59), body mass index (t(40) = −0.24, p = 0.81), global severity index (SCL-90-R) (t(40) = 0.59, p = 0.56), or COMT Val108/158Met genotype (x2 = 0.15, p = 0.93). A more detailed description of the sample characteristics including sociodemographic variables and information on the COMT Val108/158Met genotype can be found in Supplementary Table S6.

3.2. Task Performance

Task performance in the adapted version of the PASAT was evaluated by a linear mixed model with the predictors stimulation (anodal, sham), session (1, 2), and task block (1, 2, 3). Task block (B = 3.80, SE = 0.99, β = 0.24, t(202) = 3.84, p < 0.001) and session (B = 9.53, SE = 0.79, β = 0.60, t(202) = 12.07, p < 0.001) significantly predicted task performance due to an increasing number of correct trials over the course of the three task blocks within each session and from the first to the second session, respectively. Furthermore, the interaction of session and task block predicted task performance significantly (B = −2.36, SE = 1.12, β = 0.15, t(202) = −2.11, p = 0.036). Neither stimulation (B = −0.39, SE = 0.97, β = −0.02, t(202) = −0.40, p = 0.69) nor the interaction between stimulation and task block (B = 1.54, SE = 1.29, β = 0.10, t(202) = 1.19, p = 0.23) predicted task performance significantly.
Follow-up t-tests showed significant increases in the number of correct trials from task block 1 to task block 2 (t(41) = −2.86, p = 0.007, |d| = 0.44), from task block 2 to task block 3 (t(41) = −3.20, p = 0.003, |d| = 0.49) and from task block 1 to task block 3 (t(41) = −5.94, p < 0.001, |d| = 0.92) during session 1. During session 2, there was a significant increase from task block 1 to task block 3 (t(41) = −2.63 p = 0.012, |d| = 0.41) and from task block 2 to task block 3 (t(41) = −2.98 p = 0.005, |d| = 0.46), but not from task block 1 to task block 2 (t(41) = −0.33 p = 0.74). Figure 1 depicts the number of correct trials for each task block during each session with regard to stimulation condition.

3.3. Affective Changes

Changes in negative affect were investigated by a multilevel mixed model with the predictors stimulation (anodal, sham), session (1, 2), and time (pre, post, follow-up).
Session (B = −0.09, SE = 0.04, β = −0.38, t(199) = −2.45, p = 0.015) and time (B = −0.12, SE = 0.03, β = −0.53, t(199) = −3.84, p < 0.001) both significantly predicted changes in negative affect. While the interaction of stimulation and time did not predict negative affect significantly ((B = 0.07, SE = 0.04, β = 0.29, t(199) = 1.59, p = 0.11), the interaction of session and time predicted the outcome variable significantly (B = 0.14, SE = 0.05, β = 0.60, t(199) = 3.05, p = 0.003). Furthermore, the three-way interaction of stimulation, session, and time predicted negative affect by trend (B = −0.12, SE = 0.07, β = −0.54, t(199) = −1.85, p = 0.065), indicating that the effect of tDCS on changes in negative affect might be different in the two sessions.
In the first session, participants receiving sham stimulation showed an increase in negative affect by trend (t(20) = −1.95, p = 0.066, |d| = 0.42), whereas the negative affect did not change in participants under anodal stimulation (t(20) = −0.38, p = 0.71). There were no changes in negative affect during the second session, neither for participants under anodal stimulation (t(20) = −1.16, p = 0.26), nor for participants in the sham condition (t(20) = 0.13, p = 0.90). Figure 2 depicts the changes in negative affect during each session with regard to stimulation condition.

3.4. DNAm Changes in COMT Gene Promoter Region

In a linear mixed model with the predictors stimulation (anodal, sham), session (1, 2) and time (hours), stimulation (B = 0.98, SE = 0.42, β = 0.13, t(457) = 2.34, p = 0.020) and time (B = 0.65, SE = 0.17, β = 0.06, t(457) = 3.75, p < 0.001) predicted DNAm levels significantly, implying an effect of tDCS on the DNAm and dynamic DNAm changes during the experimental procedure. While the interaction of stimulation and time (B = −0.09, SE = 0.23, β = −0.01, t(457) = −0.37, p = 0.71) did not predict the outcome variable significantly, the interaction of session and time significantly predicted DNAm levels (B = −0.64, SE = 0.17, β = −0.05, t(457) = −3.77, p < 0.001). This indicates that the effect of the PASAT performance on COMT DNAm over time differs between the first and the second session and that the tDCS effect occurs between and not within the interventions.
Follow-up t-tests showed a significant increase in DNAm during session 1 from time point pre (57.70% methylated) to post90 (59.33% methylated) disregarding the stimulation condition (t(41) = −4.30, p < 0.001, |d| = 0.66), driven by an almost continuous increase in DNAm levels during the experimental procedure. Further t-tests comparing ‘pre’ with all post time points during the first session, showed that this increase is significant from time point post20 (58.69% methylated) onwards (t(41) < −2.48, p < 0.017, |d| > 0.38). For session 2, no change in DNAm from time point pre to any post time point was observed (|t(41)| < 1.8, p > 0.08). Figure 3A depicts changes in DNAm over the experimental procedure separately for the two sessions.
Demonstrating that the increase in DNAm during session 1 was still present in session 2, i.e., that it was preserved over 1 week, a multilevel model was fitted with the predictors stimulation in first session (anodal, sham) and session (1, 2) including only DNAm data of the first time point of each session. The interaction of stimulation in first session and session significantly predicted DNAm levels at the beginning of each session (B = 1.54, SE = 0.68, β = 0.21, t(40) = 2.26, p = 0.030). As depicted in Figure 3B, the increase in DNAm during the first session could still be detected in the beginning of session 2 in sham-treated participants (t(20) = −2.95, p = 0.008, |d| = 0.64), but not in anodal-treated participants (t(20) = −0.13, p = 0.90). Supplementary Figure S6 depicts individual DNAm data of the first time point of each session.

3.5. Cortisol Changes

In a linear mixed model with the predictors stimulation (anodal, sham), session (1, 2) and time (pre, post), session (B = −0.08, SE = 0.02, β = −0.28, t(121) = −3.40, p < 0.001) predicted cortisol levels significantly, and time predicted cortisol levels by trend (B = 0.05, SE = 0.03, β = 0.18, t(121) = 1.90, p = 0.060). Neither stimulation (B = 0.02, SE = 0.03, β = 0.06, t(121) = 0.76, p = 0.45) nor the interaction of stimulation and time (B = −0.02, SE = 0.03, β = −0.06, t(121) = −0.54, p = 0.59) significantly predicted the outcome variable. However, the interaction of session and time predicted cortisol levels significantly (B = −0.07, SE = 0.03, β = −0.25, t(121) = −2.60, p = 0.011), indicating differences in cortisol changes in the two sessions.
Follow-up t-tests showed a significant increase in cortisol levels during session 1 (t(41) = −2.50, p = 0.017, |d| = 0.39), while no cortisol changes were detected in session 2 (t(41) = −0.26, p = 0.80). Figure 4 depicts changes in cortisol levels during each session.

3.6. Correlation of DNAm Changes and Cortisol Changes

During the first session, there was a significant correlation between changes in cortisol concentration and changes in DNAm levels (r = 0.359, p = 0.019), as depicted in Figure 5. There was no correlation between changes in cortisol concentration and changes in negative affect (r = −0.139, p = 0.38).

4. Discussion

The key findings of the present study are (i) a continuous increase of COMT gene promotor methylation in blood after a challenging, stressful, and frustrating cognitive task which correlates with an increase in salivary cortisol, (ii) increased COMT DNAm detectable one week later, and (iii) a suppression of this lasting effect by concurrent activity-enhancing anodal tDCS to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These data support the notion of dynamic DNAm in response to mental stress that is associated with changes in cortisol levels and can be modulated by tDCS.
To date, few studies have reported dynamic changes in DNAm in response to a mental stress paradigm. In fact, DNAm levels have been regarded as rather stable, long-term epigenetic marks in somatic cells, which might even be maintained over numerous cell divisions [41]. However, previous studies have associated differential DNAm patterns with diseases, such as post-traumatic stress disorder, which can be triggered by the experience of a single stressful life event [42]. Yet, little is known about the time frame of the formation of these methylation changes and the amount of stress load required to induce these changes. An increase in OXTR DNAm was observed already 10 min after the exposure to the TSST [11] and changes in FKBP5 gene expression within 70 min [43]. Congruent with these studies, our data add evidence to those immediate effects on DNAm and, moreover, the continuous increase in COMT promoter methylation over the course of five independent measurements within 90 min after stress exposure makes a random variation rather unlikely.
The correlation of changes in saliva cortisol and DNAm might indicate that the stress hormone cortisol links stress with DNAm changes detectable in peripheral blood. This is also in line with previous findings reporting DNAm differences after glucocorticoid exposure [44,45]. We specifically chose to investigate methylation dynamics of the COMT gene, as several studies indicate its role in stress reactivity on a genetic [46,47] and epigenetic level [48]. Furthermore, the COMT gene interacts with cognitive performance and tDCS effects, supporting its eligibility as candidate gene [16,49]. Due to the inaccessibility of living brain tissue, we investigated DNAm differences in whole blood. This leads to the question of what extent blood DNAm can serve as proxy marker for DNAm in neuronal tissue. Indeed, for COMT DNAm several studies show that methylation status in peripheral tissue can serve as surrogate for brain DNAm [48,50]. More importantly, our data show that changes in DNAm correlate with changes in cortisol concentration. Saliva cortisol correlates well with the concentration of free circulating cortisol [51]. Therefore, it is possible that the dynamic DNAm is mediated by alterations in plasma cortisol levels. Since cortisol can cross the blood brain barrier, it might elicit similar effects on DNAm in neuronal tissue. However, the observed correlation could also be due to differences in catecholamines, which are released in parallel with cortisol in the context of stress. Taken together, these data support the hypothesis that dynamic epigenetic modifications are invoked immediately by exposure to stress and associated with the released stress hormones.
The COMT enzyme is involved in the degradation of catecholamines, such as dopamine, and changes in its expression levels could consequently affect catecholamine concentrations. There are two isoforms of COMT regulated by two different promotors. The predominant form in peripheral tissue is the soluble isoform (S-COMT) [52]. The CpG sites investigated in our study are located within its promotor region and, hence, may be involved in the regulation of S-COMT expression. However, since the observed DNAm differences were relatively small, their functional relevance needs to be clarified in future studies including gene expression data. Furthermore, in the brain, the primarily expressed mRNA is encoding the membrane-bound isoform (MB-COMT); however, there is evidence that, to a lower extent, the S-COMT form is also expressed [52]. The investigated CpG sites fall within the gene body region of MB-COMT. Therefore, if COMT DNAm is affected to a similar extent in neuronal tissue, effects on COMT expression levels in the brain might be diverse [53]. Given this limitation, conclusions about a potential epigenetic feedback loop controlling prefrontal dopamine activity, as well as neuroplasticity and behavior, is beyond the scope of this study. The dynamics of stress-related epigenetic changes and its relation to the neurotransmitter metabolism in the brain are likely better suited to animal studies.
Considering that the PASAT only presents a relatively mild stressor, it is quite remarkable that it elicited significant changes in DNAm which persisted even over one week. Of note, the PASAT task is originally designed as a measure of information processing ability [54]. However, previous studies have also demonstrated that the PASAT induces psychological stress [55]. In this study, a more challenging design (2-back task) was used, probably leading to an even more stressful experience while performing this not only cognitively but also emotionally challenging task. This is supported by the increase in cortisol levels after exposure to the first task. Interestingly, no changes in cortisol levels were observed during the second session. Probably, participants are adapting to the task, which is why a stress response is only elicited when the task is unfamiliar. Being already mentally prepared to encounter a difficult task that comes along with frequent negative feedback might attenuate the stress experience. Although this indicates the limited validity of the PASAT as a mere stress task in a cross-over design, it circumvents the necessity of a less stressful control task. It controls already for the possibility that any other parameter, such as, for example, the physical stress of the venous catheter placement, might have led to the observed changes. Nevertheless, venous catheter placement was done with a relatively short interval before the first blood sample was collected, which could have affected baseline measurements.
Similar to the observed changes in cortisol and DNAm levels, there was also an increase in negative affect by trend during the first, but not during the second experimental session. However, in contrast to the molecular markers, these changes in affect seemed to be suppressed by anodal tDCS. As this short-term effect of tDCS on the affective experience is not observed in DNAm and cortisol levels, a correlation between changes in affect and cortisol is missing. The at least trend-wise effect of tDCS on negative affect is in line with the hypothesis that activity enhancing stimulation applied over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increases cognitive control over emotion and, thereby, leads to stabilization in affect [56,57].
Furthermore, the application of tDCS during the first session affected the preservation of DNAm changes. While, in sham-treated participants, an increase in DNAm was still detectable one week later, methylation levels returned to baseline when anodal stimulation had been applied. Although we were not able to detect a significant difference between the active and sham stimulated subjects in salivary cortisol levels 30 min after intervention, it is possible that these differed in the cumulated amount of cortisol excretion over the time of the experiment. This was not captured by our experimental design, but a modulation of the stress response by brain stimulation has been reported previously [58,59], and may have been involved in mediating the observed differences in the stability of stress-induced DNAm changes. It has been previously shown in a mouse model that activity enhancing tDCS can lead to alterations in histone modifications, chromatin remodeling and changes in gene expression [60]. Our data provide preliminary evidence for a lasting modulation of DNAm changes by tDCS, suggesting that tDCS induces system-wide effects detectable in peripheral tissues, such as whole blood, through intermediary pathways.
One confounding factor which might affect DNAm are alterations in blood cell composition. Since epigenetic patterns are cell type specific, DNAm measured in whole blood might be influenced by cell type composition. According to the iMETHYL database, which provides cell type specific DNAm patterns based on a Japanese population, the methylation level of the investigated CpG sites in the COMT gene promoter region is very similar between neutrophils and monocytes but substantially lower for CD4-positve T-lymphocytes [61,62,63]. Studies investigating stress-induced immunological reactions report changes in leukocyte counts after severe physical or acute psychological stress, which is often accompanied by increased glucocorticoid levels [64]. Whether these effects can already occur within a narrow time frame of two hours is disputed and potentially species specific [65]. For humans, there is evidence that cortisol induced effects on leukocyte composition occur with a time lag of two hours [66,67]. Therefore, we believe that it is rather unlikely that the increase in COMT promotor methylation already present 20 min after stress exposure reported here is merely a secondary effect of changes in cell type composition. Nevertheless, controlling for potential cell type composition effects in future studies will be reasonable.
The novel findings of the present study evoke several lines of research to validate and extend our results. Previous research suggests that activity-enhancing stimulation applied over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increases cognitive control over emotion and, thereby, leads to stabilization in affect [56,57]. Since task-induced changes in negative affect missed significance in our study, future studies using well-established mental stress paradigms with greater power to induce changes in affect are needed to investigate the relation between tDCS effects on emotion regulation, cortisol concentration changes, and the persistence of DNAm changes. Furthermore, longitudinal studies with several sessions of tDCS application in the context of mental stress may help to investigate long-term effects of tDCS treatment on DNAm. Previous studies have shown that DNAm might not only change in association with stress exposure but also in response to different psychological treatments, such as cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) [68]. Given that our data suggest that tDCS reduces potentially maladaptive epigenetic effects in response to stressful experiences, it might be a valuable addition to complement conventional therapies, such as exposure-based CBT.
We specifically chose to investigate the COMT gene because of its role in stress response, cognitive performance, and tDCS effects. However, there are also other genes that have been associated with differences in neurostimulation efficacy. In particular, a polymorphism within BDNF, the gene encoding the brain-derived neurotrophic factor, has been shown to interact with the effects of tDCS [16]. Moreover, in a mouse model, previous research has observed differences in epigenetic patterns of the BDNF gene in response to tDCS [60]. In addition, there are further genes that are important in dopamine regulation, particularly in other tissues. Further studies are needed to clarify whether DNAm in BDNF and other dopamine-related genes also plays a role in tDCS effects in humans, and, in addition, genome-wide approaches may be important to identify new candidate genes.
Another limitation of our study concerns the sample size. For tDCS effects, we included an adequate number of participants as we can assume small to intermediate effect sizes [69]. However, since this is the first human study investigating the effects of tDCS on DNAm changes, it was an explorative approach to detect potential effects and provide a first estimation of effect sizes for future research. Randomized clinical trials using a parallel design are needed to replicate our findings and to investigate whether changes in DNAm might also be involved in long-lasting therapeutic effects of tDCS.
In the field of molecular psychiatry, the inaccessibility of living brain tissue is a major challenge. However, our findings support the notion of DNAm status in whole blood as a potential biomarker in the context of stress-related behavior, most likely via intermediary hormones. On that account, it is reasonable to study peripheral tissue in order to track epigenetic traces of a stressful cognitive effort in behaving human subjects, with due caution regarding the direct transferability to changes in the brain. Finally, by showing that prefrontal tDCS can affect the stability of stress-induced DNAm changes in a gene regulating neurotransmission, these data point towards possible alternative pathways that might be involved in the therapeutic effects of brain stimulation.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biom11111726/s1, Figure S1: DNA methylation changes in pilot cohort during each session with regard to stimulation condition, Figure S2: Cortisol concentration changes in pilot cohort during each session with regard to stimulation condition, Figure S3: DNA methylation changes in replication cohort during each session with regard to stimulation condition, Figure S4: Cortisol concentration changes in replication cohort during each session with regard to stimulation condition, Figure S5: Experimental procedure, Figure S6: Preservation of DNAm changes over one week with regard to the order of stimulation condition. Table S1: TDCS adverse effects, Table S2 Analysis of Variance Table: Task performance, Table S3: Analysis of Variance Table: Affect changes, Table S4: Analysis of Variance Table: DNA methylation changes, Table S5: Analysis of Variance Table: Cortisol concentration changes, Table S6: Sample characteristics.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.W., V.N. and C.P.; formal analysis, A.W. and S.W.; investigation, A.W., A.B. and C.B.; data curation, A.W.; writing—original draft preparation, A.W., V.N. and C.P.; writing—review and editing, A.B., C.B. and S.W.; visualization, A.W.; supervision, V.N. and C.P.; project administration, A.W.; funding acquisition, C.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

CP and SW were supported by the GCBS research consortium (FKZ 01EE1403D) funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research. The APC was supported by the Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethics committee of the University of Tübingen (protocol code 129/2016BO2 and approved on 12 April 2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge support by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research and Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen. We thank Daniel Bucher for proofreading the manuscript. The authors also wish to express their appreciation to all participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Klose, R.J.; Bird, A.P. Genomic DNA methylation: The mark and its mediators. Trends Biochem. Sci. 2006, 31, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Vinkers, C.H.; Kalafateli, A.L.; Rutten, B.P.; Kas, M.J.; Kaminsky, Z.; Turner, J.D.; Boks, M.P. Traumatic stress and human DNA methylation: A critical review. Epigenomics 2015, 7, 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Klengel, T.; Pape, J.; Binder, E.B.; Mehta, D. The role of DNA methylation in stress-related psychiatric disorders. Neuropharmacology 2014, 80, 115–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Provençal, N.; Binder, E.B. The effects of early life stress on the epigenome: From the womb to adulthood and even before. Exp. Neurol. 2015, 268, 10–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Weaver, I.C.; Cervoni, N.; Champagne, F.A.; D’Alessio, A.C.; Sharma, S.; Seckl, J.R.; Dymov, S.; Szyf, M.; Meaney, M.J. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Zannas, A.S.; Provençal, N.; Binder, E.B. Epigenetics of posttraumatic stress disorder: Current evidence, challenges, and future directions. Biol. Psychiatry 2015, 78, 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Thaler, R.; Spitzer, S.; Karlic, H.; Klaushofer, K.; Varga, F. DMSO is a strong inducer of DNA hydroxymethylation in pre-osteoblastic MC3T3-E1 cells. Epigenetics 2012, 7, 635–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Kawai, K.; Li, Y.-S.; Song, M.-F.; Kasai, H. DNA methylation by dimethyl sulfoxide and methionine sulfoxide triggered by hydroxyl radical and implications for epigenetic modifications. Bioorganic Med. Chem. Lett. 2010, 20, 260–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Iwatani, M.; Ikegami, K.; Kremenska, Y.; Hattori, N.; Tanaka, S.; Yagi, S.; Shiota, K. Dimethyl sulfoxide has an impact on epigenetic profile in mouse embryoid body. Stem Cells 2006, 24, 2549–2556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bruniquel, D.; Schwartz, R.H. Selective, stable demethylation of the interleukin-2 gene enhances transcription by an active process. J. Nat. Immunol. 2003, 4, 235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Unternaehrer, E.; Luers, P.; Mill, J.; Dempster, E.; Meyer, A.H.; Staehli, S.; Lieb, R.; Hellhammer, D.H.; Meinlschmidt, G. Dynamic changes in DNA methylation of stress-associated genes (OXTR, BDNF) after acute psychosocial stress. Transl. Psychiatry 2012, 2, e150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Nitsche, M.A.; Cohen, L.G.; Wassermann, E.M.; Priori, A.; Lang, N.; Antal, A.; Paulus, W.; Hummel, F.; Boggio, P.S.; Fregni, F. Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008, 1, 206–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Weller, S.; Nitsche, M.A.; Plewnia, C. Cognitive control training and transcranial direct current stimulation: A systematic approach to optimisation. Brain Stimul. 2020, 13, 1358–1369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kuo, M.F.; Nitsche, M.A. Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 2012, 43, 192–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Moffa, A.H.; Brunoni, A.R.; Nikolin, S.; Loo, C.K. Transcranial direct current stimulation in psychiatric disorders: A comprehensive review. Psychiatr. Clin. 2018, 41, 447–463. [Google Scholar]
  16. Wiegand, A.; Nieratschker, V.; Plewnia, C. Genetic Modulation of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Effects on Cognition. Front. Hum Neurosci. 2016, 10, 651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Lachman, H.M.; Papolos, D.F.; Saito, T.; Yu, Y.-M.; Szumlanski, C.L.; Weinshilboum, R.M. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: Description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 1996, 6, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stephens, J.A.; Jones, K.T.; Berryhill, M.E. Task demands, tDCS intensity, and the COMT val 158 met polymorphism impact tDCS-linked working memory training gains. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 13463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Schacht, J.P. COMT val158met moderation of dopaminergic drug effects on cognitive function: A critical review. Pharm. J. 2016, 16, 430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Geller, S.; Wilhelm, O.; Wacker, J.; Hamm, A.; Hildebrandt, A. Associations of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism with working memory and intelligence–A review and meta-analysis. Intelligence 2017, 65, 75–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Cools, R.; D’Esposito, M. Inverted-U–shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, e113–e125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Grace, A.A. Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Abercrombie, E.D.; Keefe, K.A.; DiFrischia, D.S.; Zigmond, M.J. Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex. J. Neurochem. 1989, 52, 1655–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Nagano-Saito, A.; Dagher, A.; Booij, L.; Gravel, P.; Welfeld, K.; Casey, K.F.; Leyton, M.; Benkelfat, C. Stress-induced dopamine release in human medial prefrontal cortex—18F-Fallypride/PET study in healthy volunteers. Synapse 2013, 67, 821–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Desbonnet, L.; Tighe, O.; Karayiorgou, M.; Gogos, J.A.; Waddington, J.L.; O’Tuathaigh, C.M. Physiological and behavioural responsivity to stress and anxiogenic stimuli in COMT-deficient mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2012, 228, 351–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Armbruster, D.; Mueller, A.; Strobel, A.; Lesch, K.-P.; Brocke, B.; Kirschbaum, C. Children under stress–COMT genotype and stressful life events predict cortisol increase in an acute social stress paradigm. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012, 15, 1229–1239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Walder, D.J.; Trotman, H.D.; Cubells, J.F.; Brasfield, J.; Tang, Y.; Walker, E.F. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) modulation of cortisol secretion in psychiatrically at-risk and healthy adolescents. Psychiatr. Genet. 2010, 20, 166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Wiegand, A.; Sommer, A.; Nieratschker, V.; Plewnia, C. Improvement of cognitive control and stabilization of affect by prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 6797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Krohne, H.W.; Egloff, B.; Kohlmann, C.-W.; Tausch, A. Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der “Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS). Diagn. Gott. 1996, 42, 139–156. [Google Scholar]
  32. Jasper, H.H. The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1958, 10, 371–375. [Google Scholar]
  33. Priori, A.; Mameli, F.; Cogiamanian, F.; Marceglia, S.; Tiriticco, M.; Mrakic-Sposta, S.; Ferrucci, R.; Zago, S.; Polezzi, D.; Sartori, G. Lie-specific involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in deception. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 451–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Derogatis, L.R. Symptom Checklist-90-R [SCL-90-R] Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual, 3rd ed.; National Computer Systems: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  35. Mill, J.; Dempster, E.; Caspi, A.; Williams, B.; Moffitt, T.; Craig, I. Evidence for monozygotic twin (MZ) discordance in methylation level at two CpG sites in the promoter region of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2006, 141, 421–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2018; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 27 November 2018).
  37. Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D.; DebRoy, S.; Sarkar, D.; R Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models, R Package Version 3.1-137. 2018. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed on 27 November 2018).
  38. Menard, S. An introduction to logistic regression diagnostics. In Applied Logistic Regression Analysis; Sage: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 58–79. [Google Scholar]
  39. Alin, A. Multicollinearity. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Comput. Stat. 2010, 2, 370–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS; Springer Science & Business Media: Luxembourg, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  41. Reik, W. Stability and flexibility of epigenetic gene regulation in mammalian development. Nature 2007, 447, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Yehuda, R.; Bierer, L.M. The relevance of epigenetics to PTSD: Implications for the DSM-V. J. Trauma. Stress 2009, 22, 427–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Höhne, N.; Poidinger, M.; Merz, F.; Pfister, H.; Brückl, T.; Zimmermann, P.; Uhr, M.; Holsboer, F.; Ising, M. FKBP5 genotype-dependent DNA methylation and mRNA regulation after psychosocial stress in remitted depression and healthy controls. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015, 18, pyu087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Klengel, T.; Mehta, D.; Anacker, C.; Rex-Haffner, M.; Pruessner, J.C.; Pariante, C.M.; Pace, T.W.; Mercer, K.B.; Mayberg, H.S.; Bradley, B. Allele-specific FKBP5 DNA demethylation mediates gene–childhood trauma interactions. Nat. Neurosci. 2013, 16, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Braun, P.R.; Tanaka-Sahker, M.; Chan, A.C.; Jellison, S.S.; Klisares, M.J.; Hing, B.W.; Shabbir, Y.; Gaul, L.N.; Nagahama, Y.; Robles, J. Genome-wide DNA methylation investigation of glucocorticoid exposure within buccal samples. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 73, 323–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Winkler, E.A.; Yue, J.K.; Ferguson, A.R.; Temkin, N.R.; Stein, M.B.; Barber, J.; Yuh, E.L.; Sharma, S.; Satris, G.G.; McAllister, T.W. COMT Val158Met polymorphism is associated with post-traumatic stress disorder and functional outcome following mild traumatic brain injury. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2017, 35, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Serrano, J.M.; Banks, J.B.; Fagan, T.J.; Tartar, J.L. The influence of Val158Met COMT on physiological stress responsivity. Stress 2019, 22, 276–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ursini, G.; Bollati, V.; Fazio, L.; Porcelli, A.; Iacovelli, L.; Catalani, A.; Sinibaldi, L.; Gelao, B.; Romano, R.; Rampino, A. Stress-related methylation of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158 allele predicts human prefrontal cognition and activity. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 6692–6698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Hosak, L. Role of the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism in mental disorders: A review. Eur. Psychiatry 2007, 22, 276–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Murphy, B.C.; O’Reilly, R.L.; Singh, S.M. Site-specific cytosine methylation in S-COMT promoter in 31 brain regions with implications for studies involving schizophrenia. Am. J. Med Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2005, 133, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Teruhisa, U.; Ryoji, H.; Taisuke, I.; Tatsuya, S.; Fumihiro, M.; Tatsuo, S. Use of saliva for monitoring unbound free cortisol levels in serum. Clin. Chim. Acta 1981, 110, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tenhunen, J.; Salminen, M.; Lundström, K.; Kiviluoto, T.; Savolainen, R.; Ulmanen, I. Genomic organization of the human catechol O-methyltransferase gene and its expression from two distinct promoters. Eur. J. Biochem. 1994, 223, 1049–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jjingo, D.; Conley, A.B.; Soojin, V.Y.; Lunyak, V.V.; Jordan, I.K. On the presence and role of human gene-body DNA methylation. Oncotarget 2012, 3, 462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Gronwall, D. Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1977, 44, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lejuez, C.; Kahler, C.W.; Brown, R.A. A modified computer version of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT) as a laboratory-based stressor. Behav. Ther. 2003, 26, 290–293. [Google Scholar]
  56. Plewnia, C.; Schroeder, P.A.; Kunze, R.; Faehling, F.; Wolkenstein, L. Keep calm and carry on: Improved frustration tolerance and processing speed by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0122578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Wolkenstein, L.; Plewnia, C. Amelioration of cognitive control in depression by transcranial direct current stimulation. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 73, 646–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Brunoni, A.R.; Vanderhasselt, M.-A.; Boggio, P.S.; Fregni, F.; Dantas, E.M.; Mill, J.G.; Lotufo, P.A.; Benseñor, I.M. Polarity-and valence-dependent effects of prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation on heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Sarkar, A.; Dowker, A.; Cohen Kadosh, R. Cognitive enhancement or cognitive cost: Trait-specific outcomes of brain stimulation in the case of mathematics anxiety. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 16605–16610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Podda, M.V.; Cocco, S.; Mastrodonato, A.; Fusco, S.; Leone, L.; Barbati, S.A.; Colussi, C.; Ripoli, C.; Grassi, C. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation boosts synaptic plasticity and memory in mice via epigenetic regulation of Bdnf expression. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 22180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Komaki, S.; Shiwa, Y.; Furukawa, R.; Hachiya, T.; Ohmomo, H.; Otomo, R.; Satoh, M.; Hitomi, J.; Sobue, K.; Sasaki, M. iMETHYL: An integrative database of human DNA methylation, gene expression, and genomic variation. Hum. Genome Var. 2018, 5, 18008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Hachiya, T.; Furukawa, R.; Shiwa, Y.; Ohmomo, H.; Ono, K.; Katsuoka, F.; Nagasaki, M.; Yasuda, J.; Fuse, N.; Kinoshita, K. Genome-wide identification of inter-individually variable DNA methylation sites improves the efficacy of epigenetic association studies. NPJ Genom. Med. 2017, 2, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Tohoku, I. Medical Megabank Organization, iMETHYL Database; over 100 Japanese whole Genome DNA Methylation Database from Monocytes, CD4+ T Cells, and Neutrophils. Available online: http://imethyl.iwate-megabank.org/ (accessed on 21 March 2019).
  64. Koch, A.J. Immune response to exercise. Braz. J. Biomotricity 2010, 4, 92–103. [Google Scholar]
  65. Davis, A.; Maney, D.; Maerz, J. The use of leukocyte profiles to measure stress in vertebrates: A review for ecologists. Funct. Ecol. 2008, 22, 760–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Pedersen, B.K.; Hoffman-Goetz, L. Exercise and the immune system: Regulation, integration, and adaptation. Physiol. Rev. 2000, 80, 1055–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Tønnesen, E.; Christensen, N.; Brinkløv, M. Natural killer cell activity during cortisol and adrenaline infusion in healthy volunteers. Eur. J. Clin. Investig. 1987, 17, 497–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Schiele, M.A.; Gottschalk, M.G.; Domschke, K. The applied implications of epigenetics in anxiety, affective and stress-related disorders-A review and synthesis on psychosocial stress, psychotherapy and prevention. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 77, 101830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Minarik, T.; Berger, B.; Althaus, L.; Bader, V.; Biebl, B.; Brotzeller, F.; Fusban, T.; Hegemann, J.; Jesteadt, L.; Kalweit, L. The importance of sample size for reproducibility of tDCS effects. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Task performance during each session with regard to stimulation condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisks mark p < 0.05.
Figure 1. Task performance during each session with regard to stimulation condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisks mark p < 0.05.
Biomolecules 11 01726 g001
Figure 2. Changes in negative affect during each session with regard to stimulation condition. Subjective rating of negative affect is shown separately for each session in pre- and post-task and follow-up condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean.
Figure 2. Changes in negative affect during each session with regard to stimulation condition. Subjective rating of negative affect is shown separately for each session in pre- and post-task and follow-up condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean.
Biomolecules 11 01726 g002
Figure 3. DNAm changes during each session with regard to stimulation condition and its preservation over one week. (A) % DNAm is shown separately for the six time points during each session. Each participant in the anodal stimulation group in session 1 (n = 21) was receiving sham stimulation in session 2, and vice versa (n = 21). (B) % DNAm for session 1 and 2 at time point ‘pre’ grouped by order of stimulation conditions (‘anodal/sham’ (n = 21) or ‘sham/anodal’ (n = 21)). The figure illustrates the comparison of % DNAm before (‘pre’) the first (session 1) and second (session 2) PASAT training within subjects who received tDCS (‘anodal/sham’) and subjects who did not receive effective tDCS in session 1 (‘sham/anodal’). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisks mark p < 0.05.
Figure 3. DNAm changes during each session with regard to stimulation condition and its preservation over one week. (A) % DNAm is shown separately for the six time points during each session. Each participant in the anodal stimulation group in session 1 (n = 21) was receiving sham stimulation in session 2, and vice versa (n = 21). (B) % DNAm for session 1 and 2 at time point ‘pre’ grouped by order of stimulation conditions (‘anodal/sham’ (n = 21) or ‘sham/anodal’ (n = 21)). The figure illustrates the comparison of % DNAm before (‘pre’) the first (session 1) and second (session 2) PASAT training within subjects who received tDCS (‘anodal/sham’) and subjects who did not receive effective tDCS in session 1 (‘sham/anodal’). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisks mark p < 0.05.
Biomolecules 11 01726 g003
Figure 4. Cortisol concentration changes during each session with regard to stimulation condition. Saliva cortisol levels are shown separately for each session in pre- and post-task condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisk marks p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Cortisol concentration changes during each session with regard to stimulation condition. Saliva cortisol levels are shown separately for each session in pre- and post-task condition. As the order of received stimulation (‘anodal/sham’ or ‘sham/anodal’) was a between-subject factor, participants receiving anodal stimulation during the first session (n = 21) received sham stimulation during their second session, and vice versa (n = 21). Error bars depict standard errors of the mean; asterisk marks p < 0.05.
Biomolecules 11 01726 g004
Figure 5. Correlation of DNAm changes and cortisol changes. Correlation of changes in DNAm during session 1 with changes in saliva cortisol concentration (n = 42). Regression line with 0.95 confidence interval.
Figure 5. Correlation of DNAm changes and cortisol changes. Correlation of changes in DNAm during session 1 with changes in saliva cortisol concentration (n = 42). Regression line with 0.95 confidence interval.
Biomolecules 11 01726 g005
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wiegand, A.; Blickle, A.; Brückmann, C.; Weller, S.; Nieratschker, V.; Plewnia, C. Dynamic DNA Methylation Changes in the COMT Gene Promoter Region in Response to Mental Stress and Its Modulation by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Biomolecules 2021, 11, 1726. https://doi.org/10.3390/biom11111726

AMA Style

Wiegand A, Blickle A, Brückmann C, Weller S, Nieratschker V, Plewnia C. Dynamic DNA Methylation Changes in the COMT Gene Promoter Region in Response to Mental Stress and Its Modulation by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Biomolecules. 2021; 11(11):1726. https://doi.org/10.3390/biom11111726

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wiegand, Ariane, Arne Blickle, Christof Brückmann, Simone Weller, Vanessa Nieratschker, and Christian Plewnia. 2021. "Dynamic DNA Methylation Changes in the COMT Gene Promoter Region in Response to Mental Stress and Its Modulation by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation" Biomolecules 11, no. 11: 1726. https://doi.org/10.3390/biom11111726

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop