Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Boosting Creativity through Users’ Avatars and Contexts in Virtual Environments—A Systematic Review of Recent Research
Previous Article in Journal
Blocked Presentation Leads Participants to Overutilize Domain Familiarity as a Cue for Judgments of Learning (JOLs)
Previous Article in Special Issue
Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Assessing Cognitive Skills in Early Childhood Education Using a Bilingual Early Language Learner Assessment Tool

1
HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies, Columbia University, 722 West 168th Street, New York, NY 10032, USA
2
Texas Institute of Measurement, Evaluation and Statistics, University of Houston, 4349 Martin Luther King Boulevard, Houston, TX 77204, USA
3
Department of Education and Psychology, Free University of Berlin, Fabeckstraße 35, 14195 Berlin, Germany
4
Center for Cognitive Sciences, Sirius University of Science and Technology, 354340 Sochi, Russia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2023, 11(7), 143; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070143
Submission received: 8 March 2023 / Revised: 7 July 2023 / Accepted: 12 July 2023 / Published: 17 July 2023

Abstract

:
In this article, we propose that basic cognitive skills may be fostered and assessed in early childhood educational (pre-K) settings using a technology-based approach to assessment. BELLA (Bilingual English Language Learner Assessment), designed for use with both monolingual (English or Spanish speaking) and bilingual (English and Spanish speaking) children, is designed to attend to cognitive skill development in addition to (pre-)academic knowledge. Specifically, BELLA assesses analytical, creative, and practical thinking in 3–5-year-old children through unique item content and delivery. BELLA is among the first tablet-based pre-K assessments designed to assess cognitive skills needed for the era of the Anthropocene.

1. Introduction

It has been stated that we may be living in a new geologic era of the Earth’s history, the Anthropocene, particularly marked by human-induced effects on the Earth and its larger environment (Crutzen 2002; Lewis and Maslin 2015; Zalasiewicz et al. 2020). What this has come to mean, in practical terms, is a rapidly changing environment, further complicated by economic and social challenges brought about by an increasingly financially and technologically interconnected world (OECD 2018). These are manifested, for example, in issues surrounding the depletion of natural resources, the recognition of the necessity of shared economies, and the rich, growing cultural diversity within communities and countries alike (OECD 2018). To successfully cope with the many new, unforeseen, multi-faceted, and dynamic problems that have ushered in this new era, it has been argued that education must broaden its attention to fostering skills necessary to meet complexity and uncertainty (OECD 2018). Across definitions of skills crucial for facing the 21st century, educational researchers, in line with the Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development’s (OECD) Future of Education and Skills 2030 (Ananiadou and Claro 2009), agree on a constellation of cognitive, interpersonal and intrapersonal skills that include critical thinking, creative thinking, empathy, and, importantly, the ability to apply skills and knowledge in unfamiliar, fast-evolving conditions (Geisinger 2016; Metz 2011). Assessment, in its turn, must play a role in this shift of education toward broad conceptions of skills and abilities; it has been suggested that rich digital environments may be leveraged to best capture some of these skills (Care et al. 2018; Soland et al. 2013). Most assessments devised thus far to capture these 21st century skills have been designed for children in the primary grades and up. A key question in the field of early childhood development is how and when these skills may be detected and fostered, and whether they can be supported in early childhood, pre-K classrooms, where they have not previously been emphasized in formal assessment.
To illustrate a technology-based approach to encourage cognitive skill development and teachers’ awareness of these skills as early as pre-Kindergarten (pre-K), we describe a new tablet-based assessment specifically designed to support the development of analytical, creative, and practical skills in young children. BELLA (Bilingual English Language Learner Assessment) addresses 3–5-year-old children’s cognitive skill development through unique item content and delivery. To contextualize BELLA, we outline areas of skill development emphasized in pre-K environments today and describe the capabilities currently prioritized in commonly used kindergarten-readiness assessments. We then discuss some of the cognitive skills currently less emphasized in formal preschool assessment and illustrate how they are cultivated through BELLA’s digital environment.

2. The Current Focus of Skill Development in Pre-K

2.1. Developmental Emphases in Preschool Curricula

Currently in the United States, the central domains tracked to gauge school readiness in pre-K children cover five domains: (1) physical well-being and motor development; (2) social and emotional development; (3) language and literacy development; (4) cognition and general knowledge; and (5) approaches to learning (OHS 2022). These were originally outlined by the National Education Goals Panel in 1991 as foundational for children’s later learning in school. They exemplify the prioritization of children’s general physical and emotional well-being, communication skills, ability to think and acquire knowledge, and attitudes and habits for learning (e.g., self-control, task persistence, attention) in classroom settings (Pan et al. 2019). The consideration of cognitive skills as important outcomes at this developmental and educational stage is limited, primarily focusing on a narrow range of skills such as memory, attention, and executive functioning (Weiland and Yoshikawa 2013; Yoshikawa et al. 2013). Pre-K curricula, however, tend to be comprehensive, addressing multiple aspects of child development.
Many government grant-funded programs, including Head Start, are required to use a “whole-child” curriculum designed to address children’s development across several domains. Whole-child curricula are based on constructivist, experienced-based notions of learning and are considered to be child-centered. They support children’s learning by providing opportunities for them to interact independently with equipment, materials, and other children within the classroom setting in various enrichment activities (Michael-Luna and Heimer 2012; Nguyen et al. 2019). According to the National Survey of Early Child Care and Education carried out in 2012 in the US (NSECE 2012), over 40% of providers then used a whole-child curriculum, and about 34% used a locally developed (by a teacher, school, or district) curriculum or no curriculum at all (Jenkins et al. 2018). The most widely used whole-child curriculum in preschool and Head Start classrooms, according to a national survey, is Creative Curriculum (Jenkins et al. 2018).
Creative Curriculum addresses four domains of development: social-emotional, physical, cognitive, and language. It also attends to seven areas of content knowledge: literacy, mathematics, science, social studies, the arts, technology, and process skills (Copple and Bredekamp 2009; Michael-Luna and Heimer 2012). One of the primary modes of children’s learning in this curriculum is through purposeful play or play-based learning (Taylor and Boyer 2020), which may take place within various areas of interest for children, such as blocks, dramatic play, toys and games, art, cooking, computers, and the outdoors; however, large- and small-group activities facilitated by a teacher also occur daily. Thus, Creative Curriculum, and whole-child approaches in general, are intended to be flexible to allow for varying balances of teacher-directed and child-initiated learning that accommodate the children’s individual differences (Michael-Luna and Heimer 2012). Despite the intentions of such open, play-based curricula to support child development broadly and comprehensively—Creative Curriculum advocates naturalistic formative assessment as well as observation (Dodge et al. 2002)—their original intent is challenged by the growing importance of outcome-based assessments, particularly those in content areas such as math and language. Site-based evaluations of the Creative Curriculum by the National Center for Education Research reflected a lack of short-term gains in children’s reading, phonological awareness, and oral language (Michael-Luna and Heimer 2012; NCER 2008; Zigler et al. 2004). Cognitive-skill development is supported implicitly in such curricula yet is not targeted explicitly in typically used outcome-based assessments. Thus, while early childhood curricula are generally designed to foster a broad range of cognitive skills, including creativity, measuring academic outcomes to gauge children’s progress is often prioritized (Yun et al. 2021).

2.2. The Focus of Current Kindergarten Entry Assessments

The aims and methods of assessment have transformed over the years at all levels of education in response to various theoretical developments, practical innovations, and government policies. For example, just three decades ago, the main purposes of assessment were an objective measurement of performance and the predictive sorting or classifying of children for certain occupations or stations in life (Conner 1991; Satterly 1989). In 2001, the National Research Council’s Committee on the Foundations of Assessment declared that classroom (and large-scale) assessments should support student learning and success by integrating assessments as part of the learning process rather than maintaining assessment as a separate goal (Chudowsky and Pellegrino 2003; NRC 2001).
Kindergarten entry assessments (KEA) have been devised to evaluate young children around the time of school entry—right before kindergarten, during, or after kindergarten entry—and have multiple purposes: to monitor readiness, inform instruction, monitor education trends over time, and/or provide indicators of high-stakes accountability (Maxwell and Clifford 2004). As of 2021, 38 states in the US now require schools to complete readiness assessments to determine children’s level of development prior to entering kindergarten (Yun et al. 2021). However, there seems to be a dearth of well-validated KEAs to appraise young children’s skill levels and learning over time. Concerns have been raised by child development experts that some assessments focus narrowly on reading or math to the detriment of other domains such as the sciences, social studies, music, and arts (Yun et al. 2021). This limited perspective can lead to teachers favoring academics in their teaching over other domains to teach to the test, or to favoring teacher-led instruction rather than active, experience- or play-based approaches to learning (Yun et al. 2021).
The National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) has made detailed recommendations for developmentally appropriate practice (DAP), including that both formative (measuring progress) and summative (at specified endpoints) assessments are needed to best support children’s early learning experiences. Below, we briefly describe for comparison some KEAs that are well-known and commonly used, many of which are technology-based, like BELLA (drawn from Yun et al. 2021).

2.3. A Few Examples of Well-Known KEAs

Teaching Strategies (TS-GOLD; Teaching Strategies LLC 2022) is one of the most commonly used KEAs in the USA. It is a formative, observation-based assessment for infants through children in the 3rd grade. The differentiating feature of this tool is the ongoing nature of the assessment process: it is carried out by teachers during regular, everyday activities on a continuous basis. It guides teachers to rate their students on 38 objectives across 11 domains (Social and Emotional, Physical, Language, Cognitive, Literacy, Mathematics, Science and Technology, Social Studies, the Arts, and English Language Acquisition). The cognitive skills addressed by TS-GOLD include attention, persistence, problem-solving, curiosity, motivation, flexibility, and inventiveness (grouped as “positive approaches to learning”); memory and associative thinking (“remembers and connects experiences”); classification; and symbolic and imaginative thinking (“uses symbols and images to represent something not present”) (Teaching Strategies LLC 2022). TS-GOLD also asks teachers to evaluate children’s social–emotional skills (Teaching Strategies LLC 2022). TS-GOLD attends to several important skills that reflect a whole-child developmental perspective in early childhood education. However, it may be challenging for teachers to observe, record, and evaluate these actions in their students daily.
Similarly teacher-led, the Brigance Early Childhood Screens III (French 2013; Yun et al. 2021) is a set of developmental screeners that address children’s development from infancy through 1st grade. It is brief, including only 13 items, but covers three domains at the Kindergarten/1st-grade levels: academic skills and cognitive development (literacy and mathematics), language development (receptive and expressive), and physical development (gross and fine motor). Each item is a performance task delivered one-on-one and scored by a teacher. The tasks vary according to the targeted skill (e.g., standing on one foot for 1 s with eyes closed, reading uppercase letters). Materials for teachers evaluating Spanish-speaking children are provided. As a screening tool, Brigance is focused on evaluating children for particular milestones so that early delays can be addressed. Like TS-GOLD, no part of Brigance involves a computerized interface for the children.
In contrast, one direct assessment delivered via a tablet commonly used within early educational settings is the Standardized Test for the Assessment of Reading-Early Literacy Enterprise, STAR-EL (Shapiro and Gebhardt 2012). STAR-EL is a computerized adaptive assessment that targets early literacy and numeracy (word knowledge and skills, comprehension strategies, and numbers and operations) in preschool through grade 3. It is designed to be taken independently by a child on a computer or tablet. STAR-EL is an example of a contemporary assessment based on traditional approaches, including multiple-choice items that cover basic academic areas. It does not target any cognitive skills; it is available in Spanish. STAR-EL is progressive in its tablet-based delivery; however, it does not appear to make full use of tablet-based technology (i.e., capitalizing on different item formats or the availability of engaging graphics). See Supplementary Materials for a summary of this comparative information, Table S1.

3. BELLA and Its Contributions to the Development and Evaluation of Cognitive Skills

BELLA was designed to enhance the assessment of preschool children by combining child-friendly, enjoyable activities with state-of-the-art methodology in technology-based testing. BELLA assesses aspects of early literacy, numeracy, science, and social-emotional development along with a set of cognitive skills (analytical, creative, and practical thinking). It is aligned with the theoretical frameworks of the USA national education standards (Department of Health and Human Services 2010; U.S. Department of Education 2015) as well as the Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills (TEKS) guidelines (Texas Education Agency 2022), yet addresses common standards across several states. BELLA’s items (over 700) can comprise summative assessments to gauge preschoolers’ proficiency in domains relevant to school readiness. BELLA also includes novel items that are particularly well-suited to emphasize the development of children’s reasoning and creativity, both of which are considered important 21st century skill domains.

3.1. BELLA’s Knowledge Domains

3.1.1. Early Literacy Skills

Early literacy skills are the precursors of reading and writing (Dickinson and McCabe 2001). The skills most predictive of later performance are phonological awareness, vocabulary, oral expression, and print knowledge (Connor et al. 2006; Lonigan et al. 2000). In ELL bilingual populations, phonological awareness, along with vocabulary and letter knowledge, is one of the main precursors to literacy (Lonigan et al. 2013; National Report Panel 2000). BELLA assesses three: phonological awareness, print knowledge, and letter-sound correspondence.

3.1.2. Early Numeracy Skills

A wealth of evidence has shown that children’s informal knowledge must be established before more formal aspects of mathematics are introduced and that children’s acquisition of informal knowledge varies due to differences in their exposure to numerical vocabulary and concepts in the home (Siegler and Lortie-Forgues 2014). Thus, BELLA‘s easiest (as per children’s normative developmental progress) early numeracy items (i.e., targeting basic proficiency levels according to curricular standards) are simple, non-verbal tasks working with quantities of less than 10. These are followed by the assessment of number word and symbol recognition and counting, the early verbal precursors to formal mathematical activities (Mix et al. 2002), and the assessment of sequencing of numbered quantities (number line type activities) to ascertain children’s understanding of the correspondence between enumeration and quantity.

3.1.3. Early Science Skills

Today, science education guidelines dictate that opportunities to experience scientific inquiry and exploration should begin at an early age (Lind 1999; Next Generation Science Standards 2013; Trundle and Saçkes 2015). They emphasize two aspects of early science to develop in young children: declarative knowledge (particularly in the areas of the life sciences, earth/space sciences, and physical sciences) and process skills (e.g., observing, describing, predicting, and experimenting). Children as young as three and four, it has been argued, are capable of early scientific reasoning, particularly concerning cause and effect and analogical thinking (Bullock et al. 1982; Wilkening and Sodian 2005). BELLA assesses children’s knowledge regarding living things, daily rhythms of life (e.g., time, seasons, growth), the earth and stars, and the senses. In other categories (e.g., colors, shapes, size, and measurement), BELLA assesses children’s skills in observation and inquiry in activities such as recognizing, naming, and comparing based on physical details and different ways of describing what they see.

3.2. BELLA’s Addition of Cognitive Skills

BELLA’s items were created specifically to target knowledge domains and cognitive skills. That is, within each knowledge domain, different item types (e.g., requiring compare/contrast skills, multiple responses, the consideration of imaginative or novel situations) were designed to call upon different cognitive skills. For example, questions about how to divide cookies evenly between friends or make sure that the number of spoons matches the number of forks on the table are quantitative inquiries set within practical situations, combining numeracy within an everyday context. Figure 1 shows BELLA’s structure of targeted content knowledge and skills. The figure depicts each of BELLA’s knowledge domains: Early Literacy (green), Early Numeracy (orange), Early Science (cyan), and Social-emotional Development (magenta) and their associated sub-domains. Each domain has two dimensions: cognitive skills (analytical, creative, and practical) and difficulty level (easy, medium, hard). The social-emotional development domain has three sub-domains, three difficulty levels, but only one cognitive dimension (practical), unlike all other sub-domains. Each item in BELLA maps onto one of these combinations of domains, sub-domains, cognitive skills, and difficulty levels.

4. Capitalizing on Digital Technology to Capture Analytical, Creative, and Practical Skills

Throughout each knowledge domain, BELLA elicits analytical, creative, and practical thinking through innovative item content and mechanisms of item delivery. BELLA’s content is based on the knowledge components that form the foundation for the Head Start Child Development and Early Learning Framework (Department of Health and Human Services 2010), set within the cognitive framework of Sternberg’s theory of successful intelligence (Sternberg 2005, 1999). That is, BELLA’s content addresses knowledge domains and cognitive skills by assessing children’s development of early literacy, numeracy, and science while simultaneously accessing a set of cognitive skills identified as major components of general intellectual functioning. Specifically, these cognitive skills are developed in the (1) acquisition and organization of knowledge, i.e., analytical skills, such as those used in evaluating, sorting, and comparing; (2) extension or transformation of knowledge, i.e., creative skills, such as inventing, designing, or imagining; and (3) application of knowledge, i.e., practical skills, such as those used to relate, adapt and contextualize. In addition, the social-emotional skills assessed through BELLA (i.e., emotion control/emotion recognition and empathy) are central to most theoretical models of social–emotional competence in early childhood (Denham et al. 2012; Halberstadt et al. 2001; Vaughn et al. 2009). BELLA is designed to capitalize on what we know of digital media use in the context of learning and for young children. It is meant to adapt to a learning environment constantly facing innovations and new technologies. Below we briefly and selectively illustrate how some of these skills—specifically, estimation, mental flexibility, empathy, and perspective-taking—are captured in BELLA.

4.1. Assessing the Analytical Skill of Estimation

Quantitative estimation (from here on simply “estimation”) involves assigning a cardinal number to a discrete collection of objects or other unknown quantity (e.g., length, area, volume) without the precision of actual counting or using formal measurement tools. Estimation is a best guess or approximation based on the available information and is generally exercised when precise calculations or measurements are not possible or not necessary (Chang et al. 2011). Its importance in children’s education is twofold: first, the exercise of estimation skills is ubiquitous in everyday life, both in school and out of school, across several domains of everyday life; second, estimation skills support the development of higher level mathematical thinking and reasoning (Andrews et al. 2022; Rittle-Johnson et al. 2001; Russo et al. 2022), correlating with standardized achievement test performance as well as arithmetic and magnitude comparison (Opfer and Siegler 2007).
BELLA addresses estimation, a fundamental component of quantitative reasoning, in two sets of items. In the first type, a child is shown a long piece of cucumber alongside a slice of cucumber of a particular width. The child is asked to estimate how many more slices of about the same width (“size”) might be cut from the length of the cucumber. (Some items present a length of bread, like a baguette, instead of a cucumber.) Several measurement skills are at work here: a visual evaluation of the width of the example slice and the mental application of that width in iterations along the length of the larger piece of cucumber, which involves perceptual abilities such as spatial visualization. In effect, the child must apply one of the main estimation strategies, decomposition, in which the object being estimated is mentally decomposed into smaller units (Desli and Giakoumi 2017). To respond, children select one of four consecutive numeric choices. Then, since this is an estimate, the child is asked to provide a second possible answer. This highlights the approximate nature of estimation and introduces the notion that a single “correct” solution is not always sought; in both science and mathematics, some problems may require a reasoned guess rather than an exact answer (Jones et al. 2012). A more advanced set of estimation tasks in BELLA ask a child to consider the number of cans of paint in relation to the size of a painted area in a picture (e.g., two cans to paint a wagon), then asks the child to estimate about how many cans it might take to paint something larger or smaller (such as a toy truck or a window). Children choose from responses represented by sets of paint cans (e.g., pictures of one can, two, three, or four cans) rather than numerals and again, they are asked to choose a second possible response. While these activities may be unusual in children’s assessments, they present common, everyday problems that even the youngest children may recognize. These are, therefore, opportunities to formalize children’s quantitative understanding and reasoning.

4.2. Assessing Creative Skills and Flexible Thinking through Novel Tasks

Imagination is increasingly recognized as a core feature of children’s development (Harris 2000; Yogman et al. 2018), although this is not a new idea (Thibodeau-Nielsen et al. 2021; Vygotsky 1967). Imagination supports engagement in behaviors and cognitions not directly connected with one’s current reality, requiring the mental formation of places and things not sensibly present (Thibodeau-Nielsen et al. 2021; Woolley and Gilpin 2020). Imagination has been linked with many developmental outcomes, including positive peer relationships (Singer and Singer 1981, 2009), executive functioning (e.g., Carlson et al. 2014; Pierucci et al. 2014; Thibodeau et al. 2016; White and Carlson 2016), perspective-taking skills (Lillard et al. 2011), and emotion regulation (Gilpin et al. 2015). Imagination is also a key component of the creative skills needed to solve novel problems (Sternberg 1999), as exploration and evaluation can result in novel and useful outcomes (Chylińska and Gut 2020). While creativity broadly conceived is valued and prioritized in current preschool curricula, particularly in the arts, the specific cognitive skills that support creative thinking are less attended to in the classroom, although their role in academic learning has been recognized for decades (e.g., Beghetto and Kaufman 2010; Grigorenko et al. 2008; Grigorenko 2019; Runco et al. 2017; Tan and Grigorenko 2010). BELLA is designed to address this gap in two aspects: by presenting children with novel problems, and by asking them to consider that there is more than one possible “good” response to questions.

4.2.1. Creative Thinking in Novel Problems

Several items in BELLA’s science domain present children with novel tasks. These questions present situations that have not occurred or could not occur in real life (e.g., houses made of chocolate, living on clouds) and, therefore, must be imagined. Children must take what they know about certain materials or phenomena and imagine how they might work in new settings, or what might result if elements could be combined in different ways. For example, one set of questions asks children to imagine, given a choice of four fruits, which two could be combined to make a new fruit that has certain properties of taste, color, or texture. The children must draw on their knowledge of common fruits and imagine the hybrids that might result when they are combined. Another set of questions asks children to combine alien-like heads and bodies that can live in specific environments (e.g., in a cave, in a tree) and eat certain foods (e.g., bugs, leaves). These types of questions are meant to stimulate playful and imaginative thinking while accessing what children might know about the natural world.

4.2.2. Creative Thinking in Multiple Solutions

BELLA provides opportunities for flexible thinking by inviting children to provide a second plausible response to a question. In questions in the mathematics domain, the two possible responses may both be equally correct. However, in the language and science domains, one response may be better while the second is “good enough”. For example, in items that address rhyming, exact rhymes such as “cat” and “hat” may be considered exactly correct, yet “cat” and “Kate” may be considered similar enough for a poem or some other literary circumstance. Classroom conventions generally emphasize that there is only a single “correct” response to any question. By departing from this notion, divergent thinking is encouraged, in which multiple ideas are produced, some being more pertinent than others. This deviates from the mindset that a single correct answer is the best response to a problem.

4.3. Assessing the Practical/Social Skills

During early childhood, the most practical skills for children include those within the domain of social-emotional development—that is, in dealings with the self (e.g., self-regulation, attention), situations (e.g., social problem solving), and others (e.g., prosocial behavior, emotion recognition). Contemporary theoretical models conceptualize social-emotional competence in early childhood as a multidimensional construct that includes skills such as emotional knowledge (e.g., expression knowledge and situational knowledge), self-regulation (e.g., managing emotions, cognition, and behavior), self-awareness (e.g., identifying emotions), social awareness (e.g., perspective taking) as well as relational and prosocial skills (e.g., social problem solving, cooperation, seeking help) (Denham et al. 2012; Halberstadt et al. 2001; Vaughn et al. 2009). Social and emotional skills are increasingly recognized as crucial for children’s success in school, as well as in other settings (Darling-Churchill and Lippman 2016). In early preschool, children interact with peers frequently, during which emotional expressions and arousal must be apprehended and managed, specific friendships are formed, and prosocial behaviors and interactions emerge (Denham et al. 2009). Recent literature has emphasized the need for accurate and psychometrically sound assessments of social-emotional skills in early childhood that can be administered on a continuous basis, particularly for ELL, whose language and particular socialization processes may not be addressed in English-based assessments (Darling-Churchill and Lippman 2016; Denham et al. 2009; Snow 2011). BELLA supports assessing early social-emotional skills in three domains: emotion control/emotion recognition, emotion control/relationships, and empathy.

4.3.1. Emotion Control/Emotion Recognition

Emotion control is an aspect of self-regulation, along with behavior control and the control of one’s attention (Gagne et al. 2021). Emotion-related self-control processes include those involved in managing the “cool” or conscious aspect of emotional response, as well as the involuntary or “hot” reactions to situations (Eisenberg et al. 2010). Part of emotion management is identifying feelings, both in oneself and in others (Izard et al. 2001). BELLA includes several items tapping basic emotion recognition, such as items that ask a child to identify basic facial expressions. In these items, children are shown four faces and instructed, for example, to: “Look at these faces. Touch the one that is happy”. Six cross-culturally validated facial expressions of emotion are targeted: happy, sad, angry, afraid, surprised, and disgusted.

4.3.2. Emotion Control/Relationships

A second important aspect of emotional control is the recognition and management of emotions that emerge in response to unexpected events or experiences (Eisenberg et al. 2010). To assess skills in this area, children are presented with a situation in which something unexpected is encountered by someone, and they are asked how that character feels. For example, “Bird’s favorite color is blue. He saw that there were blue balloons at his birthday party. How did Bird feel?” Children choose an answer by selecting an emoji-like face with the corresponding facial expression. In these items that require more advanced emotion processing skills, including self-other differentiation, children show their understanding of emotions as responses to things that happen (Gagne et al. 2021).

4.3.3. Empathy and Perspective-Taking

Empathy has been defined as an affective state that arises when a person perceives the emotions of another and experiences congruent emotions (Eisenberg and Miller 1987). It is well-known that social–emotional skills, such as emotion matching or the ability to experience emotions vicariously, begin to emerge in early childhood (Eisenberg and Lennon 1980; Eisenberg and Miller 1987). A cognitive component of empathy involves the mental processes of perceiving, recognizing, and understanding the internal state of another person to the point of taking on that person’s role or perspective (Iannotti 1975). The affective component of empathy involves the emotional response (conscious or unconscious) of the observer upon perceiving the internal state (emotions, thoughts, or attitudes) of the other person (Zoll and Enz 2010). BELLA items access the cognitive component of empathy by asking children to consider a social situation and report how the character within the situation is feeling. BELLA’s cognitive empathy items ask a child to consider a social situation and indicate how someone in this situation might feel. For example: “Crocodile is having friends over to play. He likes his friends. How will he feel when they come over?” These items particularly address children’s ability to comprehend social situations and associate them with the emotions they might elicit.

5. BELLA’s Psychometric Properties with Regard to Cognitive Skill Assessment

Data were collected to preliminarily evaluate BELLA’s psychometric performance in both knowledge and cognitive domains. We describe this study here.

5.1. Method

5.1.1. Sample

Data for the present study were collected across 17 schools from 26 March 2019 to 31 August 2022. Due to constraints imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic, most of these schools were private preschools. The final dataset includes 506 children ages 3–6 years old (there were a few six-year-olds in our participating preschool classrooms).

5.1.2. Procedure

For about half of the sample, BELLA was administered by trained research assistants who worked with children at the school sites. In these cases, sessions with BELLA were conducted in segregated spaces to minimize distractions for the children. For the rest of the sample, researchers trained schoolteachers in administering BELLA. Teachers then carried out sessions with BELLA in quiet classrooms in the morning, before the official start of school, or during group activity time. In all cases, teachers or research assistants worked one-on-one with each child as they engaged with BELLA. Each session with BELLA consisted of 33 items, taking 20–30 min. Most children engaged with BELLA once or twice. A small percentage of children engaged with BELLA three or more times (see Table 1). Each session included distinct sets of parallel items across domains and difficulty levels.

5.1.3. Data Analyses

For each domain of BELLA, two types of test validation metrics were estimated per item: pass rates and discrimination. Pass rates were the number of correct responses for each item. Discrimination rates were calculated using the extreme group method, which involves partitioning participants into four groups based on their score percentiles and then calculating the difference between the means of the lower and upper quartiles of scores. Discrimination was estimated for items grouped by cognitive skill, domain, subdomain, and difficulty. Prior to these analyses, items were grouped based on scoring types. Partial credit items (n = 62) are awarded 34, 50, or 66 out of 100 points. These items ask children to provide more than one correct or acceptable response; in some cases, one response is more accurate. Non-partial credit items (n = 142) are binary, correct, or incorrect, awarded either 0 or 100 points. Here we share results regarding the evaluation of cognitive skills by BELLA.

5.2. Results

Table 2 presents their demographic descriptive statistics, as well as the number of times children engaged with BELLA over the course of the study.
Item difficulty by item types, based on pass rates, showed that a majority of BELLA’s items presented acceptable levels of difficulty (see Table 2), being neither too easy (>90% correct responses) nor too difficult (<10% correct responses). Pass rates for partial credit items are calculated as the mean score across items.
Table 3 presents the item discrimination distributions for partial and binary items by item type and cognitive skills by and across skill domains.
The calculated values were all positive, indicating that the high-performing group of students in the current sample is always more efficient than the lower-performing groups, with binary discrimination means falling into the 0.3–0.5 range and partial credit items trending slightly lower, in the 0.2–0.3 range. In binary items, cognitive skill discrimination seemed to differentiate more in the creative cognitive skill, while partial credit items mostly belonged to a single cognitive skill domain (i.e., creative cognitive skill), precluding an analysis of differences across cognitive skill domains. The highest discrimination values by knowledge domain were found in mathematics for binary items and in science for partial credit items. According to the average item discrimination values for the cognitive skill domains, as indicated in binary items, the creative skill items performed slightly better. All partial credit items were primarily employed to assess creative cognitive skills. Figure 2 and Figure 3 illustrate the item-level pass rates for cognitive skills across all items and by age, respectively, as indicated in the binary items. The item level pass rates indicate that pass rates in scores for analytical and practical skills increased with age, along with a marked improvement in creative items. There were few obvious outliers in all cognitive skill categories.
As these data indicate, BELLA’s items were able to differentiate children’s skill levels in our targeted cognitive domains to some extent, with creative items doing this particularly effectively.

6. Discussion & Conclusions

While the educational sector at large is in agreement that teaching and learning in the current era must move away from emphases on memorized facts and declarative knowledge toward skills that can address an increasingly dynamic world, the capability to formally assess these skills, particularly in the early years, is still evolving (Care et al. 2018; Soland et al. 2013). We have argued here that a broad range of cognitive skills important for the 21st century context may be assessed and fostered in early childhood. BELLA achieves this through questions specifically designed to invite basic forms of reasoning, such as comparing and recognizing patterns, flexibility, imagination, and open-mindedness, and the application of these in everyday situations. While the learning progression of these and other 21st century skills are still being mapped out to guide educational settings (Care et al. 2018), BELLA represents an early effort to integrate preschool academic content with skill development.
The initial results of administering BELLA items to preschool children illustrate that such assessment is possible while simultaneously evaluating children’s developing academic knowledge. Further, BELLA illustrates the capability of innovative items presented in digital formats to evaluate and distinguish across ages children’s capability to cope with novel problems successfully, as indicated by the higher discrimination of BELLA’s creative items.
Recognizing and encouraging a diverse range of skills among diverse child subpopulations is a step toward the diversity, equity, and inclusion we hope to see in all spheres of activity where big picture problem-solving needs to occur. It is also part of a movement toward individual and community empowerment through developing a broad set of life skills that include empathy, participation, creativity, communication, and self-awareness (UNICEF 2019). In this article, we have introduced a new assessment tool, BELLA, that intends to make inroads toward diversity starting at the earliest stages of formal education, where a variety of cognitive skills can be both identified and fostered, leading to a greater diversity of problem-solvers down the road. Our recognition of the new geologic era of the Anthropocene and the urgent global problems that accompany it demands a renewed focus on developing the cognitive skills needed for solving novel problems.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jintelligence11070143/s1, Table S1. A comparative summary of the academic and cognitive skill domains of commonly used KEAs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.T., H.K., I.M., S.H., E.L.G.; methodology, S.H., E.L.G.; formal analysis, I.M.; investigation, S.H., E.L.G.; data curation, I.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.T.; writing—review and editing H.K., I.M., S.H., E.L.G.; visualization, H.K., I.M.; supervision, E.L.G.; project administration, M.T., H.K., S.H., E.L.G.; funding acquisition, M.T., S.H., E.L.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The research reported here was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant R305A160402 (PI: Grigorenko) to the University of Houston. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent the views of the Institute or the U.S. Department of Education.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Houston (Protocol ID 16512-02, approved 13 October 2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data can be obtained upon request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the contributions of Stephanie Torres and Frances Leal to the development of BELLA, as well as the artists who produced all of BELLA’s artwork, Creative Director Vladimir Shpitalnik, and artists Natalie Banker, Janet Croog, and Emily Cornacchio, and the programming team at MindTrust. We are grateful to Melissa Razo for her support in administering BELLA in schools, and to David Francis for his advice on the statistical analysis. We also thank the many children and teachers who experienced and gave feedback on BELLA to make it a better assessment tool.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ananiadou, Katerina, and Magdalean Claro. 2009. 21St Century Skills and Competences for New Millennium Learners in OECD Countries. OECD Education Working Papers, No. 41. Paris: OECD Publishing (NJ1). [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrews, Paul, Constantinos Xenofontos, and Judy Sayers. 2022. Estimation in the primary mathematics curricula of the United Kingdom: Ambivalent expectations of an essential competence. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology 53: 2199–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Beghetto, Ronald, and James Kaufman, eds. 2010. Nurturing Creativity in the Classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bullock, Merry, Rochel Gelman, and Renée Baillargeon. 1982. The development of causal reasoning. In The Developmental Psychology of Time. Edited by William Friedman. New York: Academic Press, pp. 209–53. [Google Scholar]
  5. Care, Esther, Helyn Kim, Alvin Vista, and Kate Anderson. 2018. Education System Alignment for 21st Century Skills: Focus on Assessment. Washington, DC: Center for Universal Education at The Brookings Institution. [Google Scholar]
  6. Carlson, Stephanie, Rachel White, and Angela Davis-Unger. 2014. Evidence for a relation between executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Cognitive Development 29: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Chang, Kuo-Liang, Lorraine Males, Aaron Mosier, and Funda Gonulates. 2011. Exploring US textbooks’ treatment of the estimation of linear measurements. ZDM 43: 697–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Chudowsky, Naomi, and James Pellegrino. 2003. Large-scale assessments that support learning: What will it take? Theory into Practice 42: 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chylińska, Monika, and Arkadiusz Gut. 2020. Pretend play as a creative action: On the exploratory and evaluative features of children’s pretense. Theory & Psychology 30: 548–66. [Google Scholar]
  10. Conner, Colin. 1991. Assessment and Testing in the Primary School. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  11. Connor, Carol McDonald, Frederick Morrison, and Lisa Slominski. 2006. Preschool instruction and children’s emergent literacy growth. Journal of Educational Psychology 98: 665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Copple, Carol, and Sue Bredekamp. 2009. Developmentally Appropriate Practice in Early Childhood Programs Serving Children from Birth Through Age 8. Washington, DC: ERIC. [Google Scholar]
  13. Crutzen, Paul. 2002. Geology of mankind—The Anthropocene. Nature 415: 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Darling-Churchill, Kristen, and Laura Lippman. 2016. Early childhood social and emotional development: Advancing the field of measurement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 45: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Denham, Susanne, Todd Wyatt, Hideko Bassett, D. Echeverria, and S. S. Knox. 2009. Assessing social-emotional development in children from a longitudinal perspective. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 63: i37–i52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Denham, Susanne, Hideko Bassett, Melissa Mincic, Sara Kalb, Erin Way, Todd Wyatt, and Yana Segal. 2012. Social-Emotional Learning Profiles of Preschoolers’ Early School Success: A Person-Centered Approach. Learn Individ Differ 22: 178–89. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Department of Health and Human Services. 2010. The Head Start Child Development and Early Learning Framework; Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
  18. Desli, Despina, and Maria Giakoumi. 2017. Children’s length estimation performance and strategies in standard and non-standard units of measurement. Revista Internacional de Pesquisa em Educação Matemática 7: 61–84. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dickinson, David, and Allyssa McCabe. 2001. Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice 16: 186–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dodge, Diane Trister, Laura Colker, and Cate Heroman. 2002. Estuche de Evaluacion del Continuo del Desarrollo de El Curriculo Creativo (The Creative Curriculum Developmental Continuum Assessment Toolkit for Ages 3–5). Washington, DC: ERIC. [Google Scholar]
  21. Eisenberg, Nancy, and Paul A. Miller. 1987. The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin 101: 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Eisenberg, Nancy, and Randy Lennon. 1980. Altruism and the Assessment of Empathy in the Preschool Years. Child Development 51: 552–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Eisenberg, Nancy, Tracy Spinrad, and Natalie Eggum. 2010. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 27: 495–525. [Google Scholar]
  24. French, Brian. 2013. Brigance Screens III Technical Manual. North Billerica: Curriculum Associates LLC. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gagne, Jeffrey, Jeffrey Liew, and Ogechi Nwadinobi. 2021. How does the broader construct of self-regulation relate to emotion regulation in young children? Developmental Review 60: 100965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Geisinger, Kurt. 2016. 21st Century Skills: What Are They and How Do We Assess Them? Applied Measurement in Education 29: 245–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gilpin, Ansley, Melissa Brown, and Jillian Pierucci. 2015. Relations between fantasy orientation and emotion regulation in preschool. Early Education and Development 26: 920–32. [Google Scholar]
  28. Grigorenko, Elena, Linda Jarvin, Mei Tan, and Robert Sternberg. 2008. Something new in the garden: Assessing creativity in academic domains. Psychology Science 50: 295. [Google Scholar]
  29. Grigorenko, Elena. 2019. Creativity: A challenge for contemporary education. Comparative Education 55: 116–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Halberstadt, Amy, Susanne Denham, and Julie Dunsmore. 2001. Affective social competence. Social Development 10: 79–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Harris, Paul. 2000. The Work of the Imagination. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  32. Iannotti, Ronald. 1975. The nature and measurement of empathy in children. The Counseling Psychologist 5: 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Izard, Carroll, Sarah Fine, David Schultz, Allison Mostow, Brian Ackerman, and Eric Youngstrom. 2001. Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science 12: 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Jenkins, Jade M., Greg J. Duncan, Anamarie Auger, Marianne Bitler, Thurston Domina, and Margaret Burchinal. 2018. Boosting school readiness: Should preschool teachers target skills or the whole child? Economics of Education Review 65: 107–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jones, Gail, Grant Gardner, Amy Taylor, Jennifer Forrester, and Thomas Andre. 2012. Students’ accuracy of measurement estimation: Context, units, and logical thinking. School Science and Mathematics 112: 171–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lewis, Simon, and Mark Maslin. 2015. Defining the anthropocene. Nature 519: 171–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lillard, Angeline, Ashley Pinkham, and Eric Smith. 2011. Pretend play and cognitive development. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2nd ed. Edited by Usha Goswami. London: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lind, Karen. 1999. Science in early childhood: Developing and acquiring fundamental concepts and skills. In Dialogue on Early Childhood Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education. Edited by American Association for the Advancement of Science. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lonigan, Christopher, JoAnn Farver, Jonathan Nakamoto, and Stephanie Eppe. 2013. Developmental Trajectories of Preschool Early Literacy Skills: A Comparison of Language-Minority and Monolingual-English Children. Developmental Psychology 49: 1943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Lonigan, Christopher, Stephen Burgess, and Jason Anthony. 2000. Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 36: 596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Maxwell, Kelly, and Richard Clifford. 2004. School readiness assessment: Research in review. Young Children 2004: 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  42. Metz, Steve. 2011. Editor’s Corner: 21st century Skills. The Science Teacher. Arlington: National Science Teaching Association, p. 6. [Google Scholar]
  43. Michael-Luna, S., and L. G. Heimer. 2012. Creative Curriculum and HighScope Curriculum: Constructing possibilities in early education. In Curriculum in Early Childhood Education. Edited by Nancy File, Jennifer J. Mueller and Debora Basler Wisneski. New York: Routledge, pp. 120–32. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mix, Kelly, Janellen Huttenlocher, and Susan Cohen Levine. 2002. Math without Words: Quantitative Development in Infancy and Early Childhood. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. National Report Panel. 2000. Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction; Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services.
  46. NCER. 2008. Effects of Preschool Curriculum Programs on School Readiness (NCER-2008-9); Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Educational Sciences, US Department of Education.
  47. Next Generation Science Standards. 2013. Next Generation Science Standards. Available online: http://www.nextgenscience.org/next-generation-science-standards (accessed on 1 August 2015).
  48. Nguyen, Tutrang, Greg Duncan, and Jade Jenkins. 2019. Boosting school readiness with preschool curricula. In Sustaining Early Childhood Learning Gains: Program, School, and Family Influences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 74–100. [Google Scholar]
  49. NRC. 2001. Knowing What Students Know: The Science and Design of Educational Assessment. Edited by James Pellegrino, Naomi Chudowsky and Robert Glaser. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. NSECE. 2012. National Survey of Early Care and Education of 2012; Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.
  51. OECD. 2018. The Future of Education and Skills. Education 2030. Paris: Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development. [Google Scholar]
  52. OHS. 2022. Head Start Early Learning Outcomes Framework; Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services.
  53. Opfer, John, and Robert Siegler. 2007. Representational change and children’s numerical estimation. Cognitive Psychology 55: 169–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Pan, Qianqian, Kim Trang, Hailey Love, and Jonathan Templin. 2019. School Readiness Profiles and Growth in Academic Achievement. Frontiers in Education 4: 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Pierucci, Jillian, Christopher O’Brien, Melissa McInnis, Ansley Tullos Gilpin, and Angela Barber. 2014. Fantasy orientation constructs and related executive function development in preschool: Developmental benefits to executive functions by being a fantasy-oriented child. International Journal of Behavioral Development 38: 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Rittle-Johnson, Bethany, Robert Siegler, and Martha Wagner Alibali. 2001. Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology 93: 346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Runco, Mark, Selcuk Acar, and Nur Cayirdag. 2017. A closer look at the creativity gap and why students are less creative at school than outside of school. Thinking Skills and Creativity 24: 242–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Russo, James, Amy MacDonald, and Toby Russo. 2022. The influence of making predictions on the accuracy of numerosity estimates in elementary-aged children. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education 20: 531–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Satterly, D. 1989. Assessment in Schools, 2nd ed. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  60. Shapiro, Edward, and Sarah Gebhardt. 2012. Comparing computer-adaptive and curriculum-based measurement methods of assessment. School Psychology Review 41: 295–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Siegler, Robert, and Hugues Lortie-Forgues. 2014. An integrative theory of numerical development. Child Development Perspectives 8: 144–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Singer, Dorothy, and Jerome Singer. 1981. Television and the developing imagination of the child. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 25: 373–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Singer, Dorothy, and Jerome Singer. 2009. The House of Make-Believe: Children’s Play and the Developing Imagination. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. Snow, K. L. 2011. Developing Kindergarten Readiness and Other Large-Scale Assessment Systems: Necessary Considerations in the Assessment of Young Children. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. [Google Scholar]
  65. Soland, Jim, Laura S. Hamilton, and Brian M. Stecher. 2013. Measuring 21st Century Competencies: Guidance for Educators. Edited by Rand Corporation. New York: Asia Society. [Google Scholar]
  66. Sternberg, Robert. 1999. The theory of successful intelligence. Review of General Psychology 3: 292–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Sternberg, Robert. 2005. The theory of successful intelligence. International Journal of Psychology 39: 189–202. [Google Scholar]
  68. Tan, Mei, and Elena Grigorenko. 2010. Where creativity and the curriculum meet. New Horizons for Learning 8. [Google Scholar]
  69. Taylor, Meaghan Elizabeth, and Wanda Boyer. 2020. Play-Based Learning: Evidence-Based Research to Improve Children’s Learning Experiences in the Kindergarten Classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal 48: 127–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Teaching Strategies LLC. 2022. Teaching Strategies GOLD. Bethesda: Teaching Strategies LLC. [Google Scholar]
  71. Texas Education Agency. 2022. 2022 Texas Prekindergarten Guidelines; Austin: Texas Education Agency.
  72. Thibodeau-Nielsen, Rachel, Danielle Turley, Jason DeCaro, Ansley Gilpin, and Alexandra Nancarrow. 2021. Physiological substrates of imagination in early childhood. Social Development 30: 867–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Thibodeau, Rachel, Ansley Gilpin, Melissa Brown, and Brooke Meyer. 2016. The effects of fantastical pretend-play on the development of executive functions: An intervention study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 145: 120–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Trundle, Kathy, and Mesut Saçkes. 2015. Research in Early Childhood Science Education. Dordrecht: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  75. UNICEF. 2019. Comprehensive Life Skills Framework. New Delhi: UNICEF. [Google Scholar]
  76. U.S. Department of Education. 2015. Local Education Agency Universe Survey, 2002/2003 through 2012/2013; Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
  77. Vaughn, Brian, Nana Shin, Mina Kim, Gabrielle Coppola, Lisa Krzysik, Antonio Santos, Ines Peceguina, Joao Daniel, Manuela Verissimo, Anthon DeVries, and et al. 2009. Hierarchical models of social competence in preschool children: A multisite, multinational study. Child Development 80: 1775–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Vygotsky, Lev S. 1967. Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology 5: 6–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Weiland, Christina, and Hirokazu Yoshikawa. 2013. Impacts of a prekindergarten program on children’s mathematics, language, literacy, executive function, and emotional skills. Child Development 84: 2112–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. White, Rachel, and Stephanie Carlson. 2016. What would Batman do? Self-distancing improves executive function in young children. Developmental Science 19: 419–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Wilkening, Friedrich, and Beate Sodian. 2005. Scientific reasoning in young children: Introduction. Swiss Journal of Psychology 64: 137–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Woolley, Jacqueline, and Ansley Gilpin. 2020. Development of imagination and fantasy. In Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development, 2nd ed. Edited by Janette Benson. Amsterdam: Elsevier, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
  83. Yogman, Michael, Andrew Garner, Jeffrey Hutchinson, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Roberta Michnick Golinkoff, Rebecca Baum, Thresia Gambon, Arthur Lavin, Gerri Mattson, and Lawrence Wissow. 2018. The power of play: A pediatric role in enhancing development in young children. Pediatrics 142: e20182058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Yoshikawa, Hirozaku, Christina Weiland, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Margaret Burchinal, Linda Espinosa, William Gormley, Jens Ludwig, Katherine Magnuson, Deborah Phillips, and Martha Zaslow. 2013. Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education. Foundation for Child Development. Washington, DC: Society for Research in Child Development. [Google Scholar]
  85. Yun, Cathy, Hanna Melnick, and Marjorie Wechsler. 2021. High-Quality Early Childhood Assessment: Learning from States’ Use of Kindergarten Entry Assessments. Washington, DC: Learning Policy Institute. [Google Scholar]
  86. Zalasiewicz, Jan, Colin Waters, and Mark Williams. 2020. The anthropocene. In Geologic Time Scale 2020. Edited by Felix Gradstein, James Ogg, Mark Schmitz and Gabi Ogg. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 1257–80. [Google Scholar]
  87. Zigler, Edward, Dorothy Singer, and Sandra Bishop-Josef. 2004. Children’s Play: The Roots of Reading. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press. [Google Scholar]
  88. Zoll, Carsten, and Sibylle Enz. 2010. A Questionnaire to Assess Affective and Cognitive Empathy in Children. Bamburg: University of Bamburg. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. BELLA’s Structure: Knowledge Domains and Cognitive Skills. Note. Social-Emotional items are only considered practical, hence the shorter red bars.
Figure 1. BELLA’s Structure: Knowledge Domains and Cognitive Skills. Note. Social-Emotional items are only considered practical, hence the shorter red bars.
Jintelligence 11 00143 g001
Figure 2. Pass Rates for Binary Items by Cognitive Skill.
Figure 2. Pass Rates for Binary Items by Cognitive Skill.
Jintelligence 11 00143 g002
Figure 3. Pass Rate Means for Binary Items by Cognitive Skill by Age.
Figure 3. Pass Rate Means for Binary Items by Cognitive Skill by Age.
Jintelligence 11 00143 g003
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Study Sample (N = 506).
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Study Sample (N = 506).
nProportion
Age
38516.80%
423346.05%
512324.30%
66512.85%
Female21843.08%
Number of BELLA sessions
120039.53%
221943.28%
36913.67%
>3183.56%
Note. Each session with BELLA presented different items to children.
Table 2. Difficulty of Partial Credit and Binary Items by Mean Pass Rate.
Table 2. Difficulty of Partial Credit and Binary Items by Mean Pass Rate.
Item TypeDifficultyItem Count%
Partial CreditToo easy (>90)11.61%
Acceptable (10–90)5893.55%
Too hard (<10)34.84%
BinaryToo easy (>90)2316.20%
Acceptable (10–90)11782.39%
Too hard (<10)21.41%
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics For Discrimination Values By Item Type, Cognitive Skill, and Academic Domain.
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics For Discrimination Values By Item Type, Cognitive Skill, and Academic Domain.
Item TypesNumber of ItemsMeanSDMinimumMaximum
Partial Credit Items
Overall Discrimination620.25070.1328−0.050.5476
Analytical20000
Creative600.2590.1266−0.050.5476
Literacy140.21940.132400.44
Mathematics120.24380.1537−0.050.4231
Science360.26520.12720.01710.5476
Binary Items
Overall Discrimination1420.35430.15900.88
Analytical600.34240.14930.02860.7424
Creative40.43960.507700.8889
Practical780.3590.13670.05720.7024
Literacy240.34220.11480.10640.5294
Mathematics280.38150.22500.8889
Science720.34660.15220.02860.7424
Social/Emotional180.35870.11470.17650.5238
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tan, M.; Kilani, H.; Markov, I.; Hein, S.; Grigorenko, E.L. Assessing Cognitive Skills in Early Childhood Education Using a Bilingual Early Language Learner Assessment Tool. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070143

AMA Style

Tan M, Kilani H, Markov I, Hein S, Grigorenko EL. Assessing Cognitive Skills in Early Childhood Education Using a Bilingual Early Language Learner Assessment Tool. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(7):143. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070143

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tan, Mei, Hechmi Kilani, Ilia Markov, Sascha Hein, and Elena L. Grigorenko. 2023. "Assessing Cognitive Skills in Early Childhood Education Using a Bilingual Early Language Learner Assessment Tool" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 7: 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11070143

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop