Next Article in Journal
Reflecting on Teaching Practice: Adopting Islamic Liberatory Pedagogies within Muslim Institutes of Higher Education in UK (MIHEUK)
Next Article in Special Issue
Where the Spirit Meets the Bone: Embodied Religiospiritual Cognition from an Attachment Viewpoint
Previous Article in Journal
Four Jewish Visions of the Garden of Eden
Previous Article in Special Issue
Religious Hate Propaganda: Dangerous Accusations and the Meaning of Religious Persecution in Light of the Cognitive Science of Religion
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

God(s)’ Mind(s) across Culture and Context

by
Rita Anne McNamara
School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington 6012, New Zealand
Religions 2023, 14(2), 222; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14020222
Submission received: 25 November 2022 / Revised: 31 January 2023 / Accepted: 3 February 2023 / Published: 7 February 2023

Abstract

:
This paper explores dimensions of culture and practice that shape the cognitive pathways leading to different beliefs about God(s)’ mind(s). Varying socio-ecological sources of insecurity are linked to types and modes of cognitive processing, which in turn promote different constellations of beliefs about supernatural agents dubbed the heuristic and non-heuristic models of God(s)’ mind(s). The heuristic model is suggested to take prominence when relatively few cognitive resources are available to devote to thinking about God(s)’ mind(s); these conceptions of God(s) should be shaped by the socio-ecological pressures believers face. Conversely, when cognitive resources are available, differences in modes of processing (experiential-intuitive vs. analytical-rational) lead to different mystical and theological/philosophical models of God’s mind as a product of more deliberate, effortful processing. By linking beliefs to socio-ecological influences, this paper suggests phenomenological experiences of the supernatural vary across societies as a direct function of the diverse environmental constraints in which people. By linking belief to socio-ecological pressures individuals in societies face, this approach provides a bridge between the intrinsic meaning systems within communities of belief and the cognitive evolutionary approach to parsing the diversity of belief across societies.

1. Introduction

Cognitive and evolutionary science has long been in tension with culture, often adopting the assumption that cultural variation is not inherently meaningful but simply a deviation around a universal (conveniently Western) cognitive core substrate (Henrich et al. 2010; Rad et al. 2018; Sinha 2002; Kline et al. 2018). This background assumption of the baseline universality of experiences rooted in the cultures and environments of the West from which most researchers and research methodologies in the existing literature on the cognitive and evolutionary roots of religion originate leads to a pervasive blindness to the phenomenological meanings and lived experiences of religious believers and practitioners. Of particular note for the purposes of this work, cognitive science of religion (CSR) and some evolutionary approaches to religion theorize that religious belief emerged in association with other cognitive mechanisms that evolved for understanding human minds (Boyer 2001; Barrett 2004; Guthrie 1995; Norenzayan et al. 2016; Szocik and Van Eyghen 2021). Human minds, however, do not operate in a vacuum; they interact with our socio-ecological cultural worlds, forming a foundation from which our perception of our own minds, other humans, and nonhuman (including supernatural) beings grows. As such, religious beliefs and practices can be seen as manifestations of embodied cognition that is embedded within the intersection between human and ecology via cultural adaption and transmission mechanisms.
Existing CSR and evolutionary religion research mentioned above also often distinguishes between the God of theologians (conscious God concept) and laity (unconscious God image). Work on “theological incorrectness” suggests that when believers make errors in reporting what God can know and do that violate what theology teaches them to believe, then these mistakes should be in the direction of making God more like a human (Barrett and Keil 1996; Slone 2004). However, the ways human and supernatural minds are perceived also varies across cultures (Willard and McNamara 2019; Gervais 2013; Gray et al. 2011).
Research on the adaptive functions of religion highlights religion’s ability to help groups resolve socio-ecological challenges (Lansing and Fox 2011; Norenzayan et al. 2016; Purzycki and Sosis 2011). Further, supernatural beliefs, religious beliefs, and religious practice have long been thought to act as palliatives in situations of anxiety, uncertainty, and existential terror (Krause 2005; Malinowski 1948; Vail et al. 2010; Kay et al. 2009). Therefore, bringing sociological and ecological pressures into the equation can give greater clarity to how and why believers’ differing phenomenological experiences of the supernatural arise in different conditions.
This paper aims to fill the gap between belief as ecological adaptation and belief as a product of cognitive function by reviewing evidence for how particular cognitive processes may be triggered in given socio-ecological pressures. This then gives a broader picture as to how beliefs and believers’ phenomenological experiences of the supernatural may vary across cultural boundaries. These ecological and cognitive dynamics then provide the foundation upon which cultural evolutionary processes can operate to produce diverse traditions that either persist, spread, or diminish across time and space.

2. Belief as a Product of thought: Heuristic and Non-Heuristic Models of God’s Mind

According to the predictive processing theory of perception, we do not see the world as it is but as our brains believe it to be; these beliefs—or models—are based upon accumulated observations that we use to inferentially build persistent ideas about what the world is like (Seth 2014; Clark 2013; Andersen 2017). These inferential models—schema and scripts—serve as mental templates for cognitive heuristics as short-cuts to speed up decision making (Bicchieri and McNally 2018; Leung and Morris 2015; Tversky and Kahneman 1974).
These foundational models of the world are then called upon to interpret each experience. Dual-process models of cognition suggest the ways we think through these models of the world fall within a few broad categories: fast/easy (a.k.a. ‘type 1’) vs. slow/effortful (a.k.a. ‘type 2’) and within type-2 processing, experiential–intuitive vs. rational–analytic (Epstein 1998; Shiloh et al. 2002; Evans 2008; Neys 2021). Type 1 processing operates below the level of alert awareness and does not require many cognitive resources. Type 2 processing, on the other hand, requires more effortful control and often happens within the level of awareness. As such, type 2 processing capacity is often rapidly depleted when organisms must divert most or all their attentional effort toward navigating highly threatening, existentially insecure environments (Mullainathan and Shafir 2013). Conceptions of God’s mind (i.e., what believers expect God(s) to think, attend to, act on, and care about; the content of God(s) minds) may be more impacted by these below-the-level-of-awareness (i.e., heuristic) processes when most thinking about the supernatural is restricted to type 1 processing, as happens in environments with relatively higher degrees of existential threat (McNamara and Purzycki 2020). For example, a believer may think, ‘God is too busy looking after the universe to care about my problem.’ This belief reflects a bias toward seeing God as having human-like limitations, which arise as a product of this believer applying social cognition developed for understanding human agents without additional type-2 processing for reflection to expand their thinking to conceptualize the mind of an infinite being. However, focusing strictly on high insecurity only covers part of the spectrum of human experience. This paper also discusses non-heuristic models of God’s minds: mystical and theological/philosophical, which are shaped by different, parallel-competitive modes of type 2 processing: experiential–intuitive vs. analytical–rational (Epstein 1998; Hodgkinson et al. 2009; Cacioppo et al. 1996; Taves 2020).

3. Heuristic Models of God’s Mind: Control, Parochialism, Social Conservatism

The following sketch of God’s mind under conditions of high socio-ecological/psychological insecurity and uncertainty suggests that within the heuristic model of God’s mind, God is believed to be in control of conditions contributing to human life and wellbeing; parochial (interested in believers’ in-group); interested in correct ritual adherence; supportive of existing social structures; and anthropomorphized to suit application of social cognitive mechanisms that initially evolved to understand human agents. Importantly, these beliefs may arise regardless of the formal doctrinal theology of the particular religious system. This is because these heuristically driven, theologically incorrect beliefs are here theorized to be cognitively more readily available without the additional processing and reflection necessary to arrive at a theologically correct belief (Slone 2004; Barrett and Keil 1996).

3.1. God Is in Control: Psychological Insecurity from Violated Expectations, Type 1 Processing, and Managing Cognitive Resources

Situations that challenge the integrity of meaningful experiences of the world (Heine et al. 2006); reminders that their bodies and lives are finite (Burke et al. 2010; Shepherd et al. 2011); and reminders of limited personal control (Kay et al. 2010a) all suggest that beliefs about God’s control help mitigate psychological insecurity (Kay et al. 2010b; Shepherd et al. 2011). Therefore, when experiencing psychological insecurity due to violated expectations, believers should be particularly prone to reporting belief in a controlling God (McNamara and Purzycki 2020).

3.2. God Has Chosen Us: Socio-Ecological Insecurity from Beyond the In-Group

One key source of existential threat that depletes type 2 processing capacity comes from objective threats to survival. Neurocognitive mechanisms have evolved for assessing and reacting to threat (Hinds et al. 2010; Nesse 2005). Though threat detection systems should evolve to minimize error, they should be biased towards mistaking non-threats for threats (false positives) over missing a true threat (false negatives) to minimize the cost of unavoidable error in an imperfect system. They may preferentially bias an organism towards interpreting neutral or ambiguous stimuli as threatening (Bateson et al. 2011; Flannelly et al. 2007; Stein and Nesse 2011) and can be activated by actual, present threats as well as inferred, potential threats (Boyer and Lienard 2007).
Social and physical threats form distinct environmental–ecological issues that must be navigated for survival (Stein and Nesse 2011; Woody and Szechtman 2011). The social dimension presents different kinds of responses to traumatizing events (e.g., combat experience more often results in post-traumatic stress disorder than wildfires, see: (Bracha 2006). Social adaptive challenges can be further divided into threats from pathogens, from other humans outside one’s social group, and from others within one’s social group (Fincher and Thornhill 2012; Neuberg et al. 2011). These systems have been used to explain broad patterns in religious practice and belief (Johnson et al. 2014).
The social repercussions of non-social environmental existential insecurity has largely been linked to societal-level differences in tolerance for norm violations and the strength of informal social controls (Fincher and Thornhill 2012; Gelfand et al. 2006; Hruschka et al. 2014; Van de Vliert 2008). Greater exposure to threat pushes cultures towards adopting ‘tighter’ cultural forms with more focus on following norms and less tolerance of social deviance, including around religious practice and belief. This trend towards increasing norm adherence and religiosity as insecurity increases has been documented extensively, especially in the absence of formal, non-religious institutions (Norris and Inglehart 2004). Pathogen exposure can be both a function of the non-social environment and easily exacerbated by contact with other humans (Fincher and Thornhill 2012). The behavioral immune system is thought to underlie psychological responses to cues of ‘diseasiness’ (Curtis et al. 2011), which include xenophobia, conformity, lower openness to experience, and lower extraversion (Fincher and Thornhill 2012; Faulkner et al. 2004). Disgust is the emotional cue that triggers conscious response when the behavioral immune system is engaged (Curtis et al. 2011). Ritual behavior itself may have evolved as a means of dealing with issues of contagion threat (Boyer and Lienard 2007). Disgust has further been co-opted into social, moral, and political decision-making (Inbar et al. 2012; Jones and Fitness 2008; Russell and Giner-Sorolla 2013), which sustain religious norms of purity and intolerance for deviance away from accepted levels of purity. Those who endorse conservative political values have been shown to value purity more, to show greater disgust sensitivity, and to have a higher moral disgust response (Graham et al. 2009; Helzer and Pizarro 2011; Inbar et al. 2012). Social threats associated with potential pathogen exposure remain distinct from social threats from the behavior of other humans. Inter-group hostilities through warfare are thought to be as old as humanity (Choi and Bowles 2007; Lahr and Foley 1998; Turchin 2011). Personal experience with heavy intergroup violence is linked to greater emphasis on local and kin groups, more risk aversion, and greater willingness to incur costs to punish antisocial in-group members (Bauer et al. 2014; Callen et al. 2014).
Taken together, external threats from non-social environmental instability, pathogen stress, and intergroup violence all suggest that people’s responses tend towards greater focus on local in-groups. What does this imply for the mind of God? Supernatural agent beliefs in societies facing these kinds of socio-ecological pressures should include higher endorsements of belief that supernatural agents favor the faithful above and beyond the rest of humanity (a parochial God), thus supporting in-group favoritism and potentially out-group derogation. Further, supernatural agent beliefs should include that they care deeply about proper ritual performance and public displays of faith (Boyer and Lienard 2007; Purzycki and Arakchaa 2013). In cases where external socio-ecological threats also boost conformity, supernatural agents should also be believed to care about believers’ submission to existing social norms of propriety. This emphasis on propriety might be construed as purity (Haidt 2012). In places where a heavy disease burden has made a noticeable impact on supernatural beliefs, we should see an increase in belief that supernatural agents care about maintaining food and hygiene taboos. This moralized dimension of contagion purity is present in living religious traditions, especially in the Indian subcontinent (Armstrong 2006; Rozin et al. 1999; Shweder et al. 1997). Therefore, societies with heavy disease burden and reminders of contagion should promote greater belief that supernatural agents care about maintaining hygiene, food purity taboos, and cleanliness rituals.

3.3. God Is a Reflection of Society: Socio-Ecological Pressures from the In-Group, Cultural Strategies, and Societal Structure

Social theories of religious beliefs and practices propose that religion is a product of the way society is structured, how individuals interact within society, and the aspects of society that either facilitate or challenge prosocial interaction (Durkheim 1995; Geertz 1957; Purzycki 2010; Sosis and Ruffle 2004; Norenzayan et al. 2016). Social norms, including religious beliefs and practices, are internalized by individuals within societies as they navigate their social environment (Sripada and Stich 2006; Leung and Morris 2015; Bicchieri and McNally 2018; McNamara and Purzycki 2020). Socio-ecological stresses from in-groups are shaped by the society’s dominant cultural strategy, or the constellations of norms and institutions used to structure daily life (McNamara and Purzycki 2020). Strategies that develop when social networks are diffused and have poor formal institutions of social control are predicted to promote belief that God cares about maintaining an honorable reputation; punishes theft; does not punish acts of violence enacted against thieves or other persons threatening one’s honor; watches believers; and cares about maintenance of sexual and gender norms. Strategies that develop in societies with stable, cooperative social hierarchies are predicted to bolster the belief that God wants believers to be humble; cares that believers fulfill the obligations and duties inherent to their place in the hierarchy; exists as an extension of the earthly hierarchy in human society; cares about correct ritual performance; and punishes violations of social norms that uphold the traditional hierarchy. Strategies that develop in societies with extensive individual autonomy in large, anonymous, complex societies with strong, effective non-religious institutions for social control allow individuals to form separate, bounded selves independent from the social networks they inhabit. This separation of self from group shifts the focus from proper external display of religious commitment to proper internal motivation and belief in direct contact with God. These societies should therefore exhibit the belief that God cares about individuals, does not care about proper ritual performance; does not care about hierarchies; cares about correct content of internal devotion to God; and is kinder and more benevolent than punitive and harsh (McNamara and Purzycki 2020).

4. Non-Heuristic Models of God’s Mind

Most research on how religious beliefs arise from psychological processes focuses on the ‘naturalness’ of religion, generally focusing on how religious belief is effortless and beyond conscious control—i.e., the heuristic model of God’s mind. This implies that belief in supernatural agents that contradict the heuristic model will necessarily require effortful, type-2 thinking (e.g., Barrett and Lanman 2008; Bering 2010). However, how might Gods’ minds change as more effortful, type 2 thinking capacity is applied to thinking about them?
Mystics and theologians are most likely to think deeply about the nature of the divine and are therefore likely to hold a non-heuristic model of God’s mind. Both often describe God as abstract and share a denial that God has anthropomorphic traits, but their descriptions of God’s mind differ (Armstrong 1993; Kroll and Barchrach 2005). These mystic vs. philosophical/theological models of God’s mind may differ according to different emphasis on the two main modes of type 2 processing: experiential–intuitive and analytical–rational. These modes differ from type 1 vs. type 2 processing in that both modes can contribute to conscious experience at the same time (Evans 2008). Experiential–intuitive and analytical–rational processing modes have been shown to receive different preferred, normative proportions of type 2 processing capacity across individuals and cultures (Buchtel and Norenzayan 2009; Choi et al. 2007; Epstein et al. 1996). Emphasis on either mode of processing can also be seen in the differences between mystical vs. theological/philosophical models of God’s mind.

4.1. God Is Everything: Intuitive–Experiential Thinking, Altered States of Consciousness, Mystical Experience, Awe, Absorption, Flow, and Mindfulness

God as perceived in mystical experiences is often abstract, amorphous, and intimate (Armstrong 1993; Kroll and Barchrach 2005). A growing body of work on altered states of consciousness suggests intuitive–experiential processing is implicated in these experiences. The hunt for God in the brain has led to evidence for various neural substrates behind mystical experiences (e.g., Beauregard and Paquette 2006; Persinger et al. 2010; Taves 2020; Cristofori et al. 2016; Deane 2020; Lancelotta and Davis 2020). Mystical experience is often associated with a sense of separation from an everyday, ordinary sense of self (Yaden et al. 2017; Krause 2018; Deane 2020; Millière et al. 2018). Heightened intuitive–experiential processing and reduced sense of self can be associated with a sense of extreme threat and uncertainty (Yaden et al. 2017; Whitehouse 1996) as well as safety and certainty (Beauregard and Paquette 2006).
The neurophysiology of altered states of consciousness taps into various aspects of mystical experience—especially as these mystical experiences relate to an altered, porous, expansive self or dissolved self (Taves 2020). The Default Mode Network has come under particular focus in the study of altered states and of social cognition, as this network of neural activation appears to both have general implications for thought left diffused when not focused on particular tasks, when focused on simulating states of mind in others, and the beyond-the-mundane experiences of expansive/dissolving self in altered states of consciousness (Smigielski et al. 2019; Palhano-Fontes et al. 2015; Raichle 2013). Thus, when thinking in a primarily experiential, integrative mode, believers may likely report belief that God is present in close physical proximity while simultaneously feeling less tied to and bounded by their own bodies. Such a model of God’s mind might feature a sense that God and self are fused and connected with the wider surroundings.
Mystical experience may also arise from more mundane emotions and mental states of awe (Keltner and Haidt 2003; Shiota et al. 2007; Van Cappellen and Saroglou 2012), absorption (Tellegen and Atkinson 1974; Luhrmann et al. 2010, 2021), flow/peak experience (Csikszentmihalyi 2014; Moneta 2004), and mindfulness (Grant and Zeidan 2019; Sedlmeier 2018; Howell et al. 2011). Like altered states of consciousness, these emotions and mental states also feature an altered sense of self and focused attention on an immediate, present target. Situations that evoke emotional/mental states, such as awe, absorption, flow, or mindfulness (as are constructed in many traditions of ritual and religious practice), share a common theme of activating neurocognitive processes that in turn amplify intuitive–experiential processing.
This evidence suggests experiential–intuitive type 2 processing will shape the mind of God into an abstract, disembodied presence. A more fluid sense of self may promote the belief that God is intimately present—suffused throughout the believer’s sense of self and throughout the universe. The sense of God’s immensity or dissolved self could also lead to a belief that God is terrifying (Keltner and Haidt 2003; Proulx et al. 2012). At the same time, a focus on immediate experience might also promote the belief that God is non-temporal and does not care about human time-bound concerns. Thus, a mystical model of God’s mind might promote pantheism or belief in God as a non-anthropomorphic force pervading the universe.

4.2. God Is the Unmoved Mover: Rational–Analytic Thinking

One theological stance that is not often considered in this framework is non-theism (O’Grady and York 2012). Non-theism itself can be a catch-all term for everything from an asserted belief that God does not exist to the belief that God does exist but is not human-like. Non-theism has been used to describe both Buddhism and certain groups of Quakers (Glasenapp 1966; Riemermann 2006) and include pantheism and deism (Pennycook et al. 2012).
The theological/philosophical model should be dominated by the rational–analytic mode of type 2 processing. However, analytical thinking is often equated to full denial of God’s existence (Baimel et al. 2021; Kalkman 2014). Two ways of conceptualizing a non-heuristic God that have shown up in philosophical/theological religious thought are the Hidden God and the Absent God (Ebeling 1964; Elders 1990). Importantly, both are forms of deism that suppose God was the ultimate cause of the natural world but is not currently active in it. Deistic models of God’s mind also differ from pantheistic models of God’s mind in that deism may be more closely associated with analytical processing (Norenzayan and Gervais 2013; Pennycook et al. 2012).

5. Discussion

Beliefs about God(s)’ mind(s) are predicted to differ as a function of variation in socio-ecological conditions, which promote different modes of cognitive processing, which in turn feed into different phenomenological experiences and perceptions of the supernatural. These beliefs constitute different models of God(s)’ mind(s): a heuristic model dominated by fast, effortless type 1 processing; a mystic model dominated by effortful experiential–intuitive type 2 processing; and a philosophical/theological model dominated by rational–analytical type 2 processing. Non-heuristic models of God(s)’ mind(s) arise mainly in conditions that allow more type 2 processing to be devoted to thinking about God, as is the case for religious professionals, such as mystics and theologians.
One of the main strengths of this approach is its ability to provide a framework for understanding to interpret the socio-ecological roots of meaning systems across lay and specialist practitioners in diverse traditions. This provides a bridge between the existing work on cognitive science of religion and the more lived experience of religious life among practitioners by linking the patterns of belief to broader trends of ecological adaptation. It capitalizes on the power of cultural evolutionary processes to examine how psychological functioning in various environments may promote patterns of belief and make room for more nuanced interpretation of the lived experience of religion through the rigor of a cognitive scientific lens. This also provides a framework to understand when and how religious specialists would have beliefs that deviate from lay persons and may indicate underlying patterns in variation across beliefs both within and across traditions.

Implications and Limitations

Part of the limitation in this work stems from the continued Western bias in the research behind cognitive processing itself (Rad et al. 2018). Individualism and brain focus within the Western model of mind (or the intuitive/unreflective set of beliefs and assumptions about how minds work) in general and in scientific approaches to the mind in particular make the extent to which cognition itself might be modulated by cultural forms difficult to pinpoint given the current state of the literature (Lillard 1998; Luhrmann 2011; McNamara et al. 2021). Placing the self as the foundation for inferring God’s mind assumes a sense of self, as defined within the Western experience, is the most basic aspect of psychological experience, but cross-cultural studies of self indicate the Western, individualistic self is not universal (Broesch et al. 2011; Heine 2001; Markus and Kitayama 1991; Lysenko 2017). Similarly, the sense of self as separate has been proposed as the basic tenant of secularism (Taylor 1989, 2007). The secularized self—as opposed to the porous, non-secular self—may be unique to Western cultures (e.g., Leung and Cohen 2011; Luhrmann et al. 2021) and may be the product of a unique Western cultural, religious tradition (Asad 2003; Laine 2014; Masuzawa 2005).
Importantly, the model of self assumed in Western settings may be less readily accessible to people living in conditions of extreme material, existential insecurity. Various lines of research across psychology, anthropology, and neuroscience describe ways that the distinction between self and not-self can become blurry in situations of extreme psychological and physiological duress. These situations that promote a more porous sense of self can be induced unintentionally through actual physical threat (e.g., battlefield stress and spiritually uplifting experiences of elevation and awe: (Yaden et al. 2017)) or physical disturbance of neurobiological functions (brain trauma: (Taylor 2006); ecstatic epileptic seizures: (Hansen and Brodtkorb 2003; Ogata and Miyakawa 1998; Tucker et al. 1987)). The sense of self as starkly separated from the rest of reality can also be reduced by manipulating neurobiological function through practices, such as asceticism, meditation, and psychotropic plant medicines (Armstrong 1993; Flood 2004; Kroll and Barchrach 2005; Taves 2020; Millière et al. 2018). Further, individual differences may predispose some people towards experiencing a more porous self, as with schizotypal spectrum traits and underlying predispositions towards non-normal experiences often associated with either clinical or sub-clinical diagnoses of psychosis (Fabrega 1989; Polimeni and Reiss 2003; Genovese 2005; Willard and Norenzayan 2017).
Rather than giving specific examples from the ethnographic literature, this paper provides predictions of broad patterns in belief and supernatural phenomenological experience that one might expect in given cognitive and socio-ecological conditions. More work from indigenous perspectives from within these traditions and in collaboration with cognitive sciences researchers needs to be done to further assess how many of these patterns are demonstrated in the real world. Further, the dynamics of cultural evolution do not necessarily mean that a given socio-ecological stressor will produce a given cognitive effect immediately. Rather, humans tend to carry their beliefs with them, which—according to predictive processing—can fundamentally shape their experiences and reactions to the new environments in which they find themselves. Therefore, additional work to examine patterns of religious and spiritual belief change as societies move and intermix through migration, colonization, missionization, and other intercultural contact events can also show the underlying dynamics of how culture directly shapes cognition and vice versa (McNamara et al. 2021).

6. Conclusions

By incorporating a perspective on how external influences shift psychological experience, we can gain a more precise understanding of the motivations behind both practice and belief. In doing so, we can make sense of seemingly contradictory phenomena as part of the broader story of human nature. This approach also has the potential to more accurately capture the true scope of variation across belief systems, as individual beliefs may arise as a result of the combination of cognitive processing within particular environments. Importantly, this can aid in reducing the existing bias toward centering Western ways of thinking by focusing on the within-context adaptive connections between beliefs as they operate in the broader socio-ecological and cognitive systems across societies.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

I thank Benjamin Purzycki for his helpful comments on previous versions of this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Andersen, Marc. 2017. Predictive coding in agency detection. Religion, Brain & Behavior 9: 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  2. Armstrong, Karen. 1993. A History of God. Ballantine: New York. [Google Scholar]
  3. Armstrong, Karen. 2006. The Great Transformation: The Beginning of Our Religious Traditions, 1st ed. New York: Random House. [Google Scholar]
  4. Asad, Talal. 2003. Formations of the secular: Christianity, Islam, Modernity. Stanford: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baimel, Adam, Cindel J. M. White, Hagop Sarkissian, and Ara Norenzayan. 2021. How is analytical thinking related to religious belief? A test of three theoretical models. Religion, Brain & Behavior 11: 239–60. [Google Scholar]
  6. Barrett, Justin L. 2004. Why Would Anyone Believe in God? Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barrett, Justin L., and Frank C. Keil. 1996. Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31: 219–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Barrett, Justin L., and Jonathan A. Lanman. 2008. The science of religious beliefs. Religion 38: 109–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bateson, Melissa, Ben Brilot, and Daniel Nettle. 2011. Anxiety: An evolutionary approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 56: 707–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Bauer, Michal, Alessandra Cassar, Julie Chytilová, and Joseph Henrich. 2014. War’s enduring effects on the development of egalitarian motivations and in-group biases. Psychological Science 25: 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Beauregard, Mario, and Vincent Paquette. 2006. Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters 405: 186–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bering, Jesse M. 2010. Atheism is only skin deep: Geertz and Markusson rely mistakenly on sociodemographic data as meaningful indicators of underlying cognition. Religion 40: 166–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bicchieri, Cristina, and Peter McNally. 2018. Shrieking sirens: Schemata, scripts, and social norms. How change occurs. Social Philosophy and Policy 35: 23–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Boyer, Pascal, and Pierre Lienard. 2007. Why ritualized behavior? Precaution Systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 595–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Boyer, Pascal. 2001. Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bracha, H. Stefan. 2006. Human brain evolution and the “Neuroevolutionary Time-depth Principle:” Implications for the Reclassification of fear-circuitry-related traits in DSM-V and for studying resilience to warzone-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 30: 827–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Broesch, Tanya, Tara Callaghan, Joseph Henrich, Christine Murphy, and Philippe Rochat. 2011. Cultural variations in children’s mirror self-recognition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42: 1018–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Buchtel, Emma E., and Ara Norenzayan. 2009. Thinking across cultures: Implications for dual processes. In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond. Edited by Jonathan St B. T. Evans and Keith Frankish. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 217–38. [Google Scholar]
  19. Burke, Brian L., Andy Martens, and Erik H. Faucher. 2010. Two Decades of Terror Management Theory: A Meta-Analysis of Mortality Salience Research. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14: 155–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Cacioppo, John T., Richard E. Petty, Jeffrey A. Feinstein, and W. Blair G. Jarvis. 1996. Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin 119: 197–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Callen, Michael, Mohammad Isaqzadeh, James D. Long, and Charles Sprenger. 2014. Violence and Risk Preference: Experimental Evidence from Afghanistan. American Economic Review 104: 123–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Choi, Incheol, Minkyung Koo, and Jong An Choi. 2007. Individual differences in analytic versus holistic thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33: 691–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Choi, Jung-Kyoo, and Samuel Bowles. 2007. The Coevolution of Parochial Altruism and War. Science 318: 636–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Clark, Andy. 2013. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36: 181–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cristofori, Irene, Joseph Bulbulia, John H. Shaver, Marc Wilson, Frank Krueger, and Jordan Grafman. 2016. Neural correlates of mystical experience. Neuropsychologia 80: 212–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly. 2014. The Systems Model of Creativity. New York: Springer, pp. 99–125. [Google Scholar]
  27. Curtis, Valerie, Mícheál De Barra, and Robert Aunger. 2011. Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366: 389–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Deane, George. 2020. Dissolving the self: Active inference, psychedelics, and ego-dissolution. Philosophy and the Mind Sciences 1: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Durkheim, Emile. 1995. The Elementary Forms of Religious Life. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  30. Ebeling, Gerhard. 1964. Luther: An Introduction to His Thought. Philadelphia: Fortress Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Elders, Leo J. 1990. The Philosophical Theology of St. Thomas Aquinas. New York: E. J. Brill. [Google Scholar]
  32. Epstein, Seymour, Rosemary Pacini, Veronika Denes-Raj, and Harriet Heier. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 390–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Epstein, Seymour. 1998. Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory. In Advanced Personality. Edited by David F. Barone, Michel Hersen and Vincent B. Van Hasselt. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 211–38. [Google Scholar]
  34. Evans, Jonathan St B. T. 2008. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 255–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Fabrega, Horacio. 1989. The self and schizophrenia: A cultural perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin 15: 277–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Faulkner, Jason, Mark Schaller, Justin H. Park, and Lesley A. Duncan. 2004. Evolved Disease-Avoidance Mechanisms and Contemporary Xenophobic Attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 7: 333–53. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fincher, Corey L., and Randy Thornhill. 2012. Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: 155–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Flannelly, Kevin J., Harold G. Koenig, Kathleen Galek, and Christopher G. Ellison. 2007. Beliefs, mental health, and evolutionary threat assessment systems in the brain. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders 195: 996–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Flood, Gavin. 2004. The Ascetic Self: Subjectivity, Memeory, and Tradition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  40. Geertz, Clifford. 1957. Ritual and social exchange: A Javanese example. American Anthropologist 59: 32–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gelfand, Michele J., Lisa H. Nishii, and Jana L. Raver. 2006. On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. The Journal of Applied Psychology 91: 1225–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Genovese, Jeremy E. C. 2005. Paranormal beliefs, schizotypy, and thinking styles among teachers and future teachers. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gervais, Will M. 2013. Perceiving minds and gods: How mind perception enables, constrains, and is triggered by belief in gods. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 380–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Glasenapp, Helmuth Von. 1966. Buddhism—A Non-Theistic Religion: With a Selection from Buddhist Scriptures. Translated by Heinz Bechert. Edited by Heinz Bechert. New York: George Braziller. [Google Scholar]
  45. Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, and Brian A. Nosek. 2009. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96: 1029–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Grant, Joshua A., and Fadel Zeidan. 2019. Employing pain and mindfulness to understand consciousness: A symbiotic relationship. Current Opinion in Psychology 28: 192–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Gray, Kurt, Adrianna C. Jenkins, Andrea S. Heberlein, and Daniel M. Wegner. 2011. Distortions of mind perception in psychopathology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 477–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Guthrie, Stewart Elliott. 1995. Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  49. Haidt, Jonathan. 2012. The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion. New York: Pantheon. [Google Scholar]
  50. Hansen, Bjørn Åsheim, and Eylert Brodtkorb. 2003. Partial epilepsy with “ecstatic” seizures. Epilepsy & Behavior 4: 667–73. [Google Scholar]
  51. Heine, Steven J. 2001. Self as cultural product: An examination of East Asian and North American selves. Journal of Personality 69: 881–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Heine, Steven J., Travis Proulx, and Kathleen D. Vohs. 2006. The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10: 88–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Helzer, Erik G., and David A. Pizarro. 2011. Dirty Liberals!: Reminders of Physical Cleanliness Influence Moral and Political Attitudes. Psychological Science 22: 517–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 61–83, discussion 83–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Hinds, Andrea L., Erik Z. Woody, Ana Drandic, Louis A. Schmidt, Michael Van Ameringen, Marie Coroneos, and Henry Szechtman. 2010. The psychology of potential threat: Properties of the security motivation system. Biological Psychology 85: 331–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hodgkinson, Gerard P., Eugene Sadler-Smith, Marta Sinclair, and Neal M. Ashkanasy. 2009. More than meets the eye? Intuition and analysis revisited. Personality and Individual Differences 47: 342–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Howell, Andrew J., Raelyne L. Dopko, Holli-Anne Passmore, and Karen Buro. 2011. Nature connectedness: Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences 51: 166–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Hruschka, Daniel J., Charles Efferson, Ting Jiang, Ashlan Falletta-Cowden, Sveinn Sigurdsson, Rita McNamara, Madeline Sands, Shirajum Munira, Edward Slingerland, and Joseph Henrich. 2014. Impartial Institutions, Pathogen Stress and the Expanding Social Network. Human Nature 25: 567–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Inbar, Yoel, David Pizarro, Ravi Iyer, and Jonathan Haidt. 2012. Disgust Sensitivity, Political Conservatism, and Voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science 3: 537–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Johnson, Kathryn A., Yexin Jessica Li, and Adam B. Cohen. 2014. Fundamental social motives and the varieties of religious experience. Religion, Brain & Behavior 5: 197–231. [Google Scholar]
  61. Jones, Andrew, and Julie Fitness. 2008. Moral hypervigilance: The influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain. Emotion 8: 613–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Kalkman, David Peter. 2014. Three cognitive routes to atheism: A dual-process account. Religion 44: 72–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kay, Aaron C., Danielle Gaucher, Ian McGregor, and Kyle Nash. 2010a. Religious Belief as Compensatory Control. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14: 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kay, Aaron C., David A. Moscovitch, and Kristin Laurin. 2010b. Randomness, Attributions of Arousal, and Belief in God. Psychological Science 21: 216–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kay, Aaron C., Jennifer A. Whitson, Danielle Gaucher, and Adam D. Galinsky. 2009. Compensatory Control: Achieving Order Through the Mind, Our Institutions, and the Heavens. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18: 264–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Keltner, Dacher, and Jonathan Haidt. 2003. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition & Emotion 17: 297–314. [Google Scholar]
  67. Kline, Michelle Ann, Rubeena Shamsudheen, and Tanya Broesch. 2018. Variation is the universal: Making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373: 20170059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Krause, Neal. 2005. God-mediated control and psychological well-being in late life. Research on Aging 27: 136–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Krause, Timon. 2018. The Self-Transcendent Brain—A Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging on Meditation and Psychedelics. Vienna: University of Vienna. [Google Scholar]
  70. Kroll, Jerome, and Bernard Barchrach. 2005. The Mystic Mind: The Psychology of Medieval Mystics and Ascetics. New York and London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  71. Lahr, Marta Mirazón, and Robert A. Foley. 1998. Towards a theory of modern human origins: Geography, demography, and diversity in recent human evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology S27: 137–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Laine, James W. 2014. Meta-Religion: Religion and Power in World History. Los Angeles: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  73. Lancelotta, Rafael L., and Alan K. Davis. 2020. Use of Benefit Enhancement Strategies among 5-Methoxy-N,N-Dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) Users: Associations with Mystical, Challenging, and Enduring Effects. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 52: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lansing, J. Stephen, and Karyn M. Fox. 2011. Niche construction on Bali: The gods of the countryside. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366: 927–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Leung, Angela K. Y., and Dov Cohen. 2011. Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100: 507–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Leung, Kwok, and Michael W. Morris. 2015. Values, schemas, and norms in the culture–behavior nexus: A situated dynamics framework. Journal of International Business Studies 46: 1028–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Lillard, Angeline A. 1998. Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin 123: 3–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Luhrmann, Tanya M., Howard Nusbaum, and Ronald Thisted. 2010. The absorption hypothesis: Learning to hear God in evangelical Christianity. American Anthropologist 112: 66–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Luhrmann, Tanya M. 2011. Toward an anthropological theory of mind. Suomen Antropologi: Journal of the Finnish Anthropological Society 36: 5–69. [Google Scholar]
  80. Luhrmann, Tanya Marie, Kara Weisman, Felicity Aulino, Joshua D. Brahinsky, John C. Dulin, Vivian A. Dzokoto, Cristine H. Legare, Michael Lifshitz, Emily Ng, Nicole Ross-Zehnder, and et al. 2021. Sensing the presence of gods and spirits across cultures and faiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118: e2016649118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Lysenko, Victoria. 2017. Self, Culture and Consciousness, Interdisciplinary Convergences on Knowing and Being. In Self, Culture and Consciousness: Interdisciplinary Convergences on Knowing and Being. Edited by Sangeetha Menon, Nithin Nagaraj and V. V. Binoy. Singapore: Springer, pp. 303–18. [Google Scholar]
  82. Malinowski, Bronislaw. 1948. Magic, Science and Religion and Other Essays. Boston: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  83. Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama. 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Masuzawa, Tomoko. 2005. The Invention of World Religions: Or, How European Universalism Was Preserved in the Language of Pluralism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  85. McNamara, Rita Anne, and Benjamin Grant Purzycki. 2020. Minds of gods and human cognitive constraints: Socio-ecological context shapes belief. Religion, Brain & Behavior 36: 1–16. [Google Scholar]
  86. McNamara, Rita Anne, Rebekah Senanayake, Aiyana K. Willard, and Joseph Henrich. 2021. God’s Mind on Morality. Evolutionary Human Sciences 3: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Millière, Raphaël, Robin L. Carhart-Harris, Leor Roseman, Fynn-Mathis Trautwein, and Aviva Berkovich-Ohana. 2018. Psychedelics, Meditation, and Self-Consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Moneta, Giovanni B. 2004. The flow experience across cultures. Journal of Happiness Studies 5: 115–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Mullainathan, Sendhil, and Eldar Shafir. 2013. Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So Much. New York: Times Books. [Google Scholar]
  90. Nesse, Randolph M. 2005. Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: A signal detection analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 88–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Neuberg, Steven L., Douglas T. Kenrick, and Mark Schaller. 2011. Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 1042–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Neys, Wim De. 2021. On Dual- and Single-Process Models of Thinking. Perspectives on Psychological Science 16: 1412–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Norenzayan, Ara, and Will M. Gervais. 2013. The Origins of Religious Disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17: 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Norenzayan, Ara, Azim F. Shariff, Will M. Gervais, Aiyana K. Willard, Rita A. McNamara, Edward Slingerland, and Joseph Henrich. 2016. The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: 331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Norris, Pippa, and Ronald Inglehart. 2004. Sacred and Secular: Religion and Politics Worldwide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  96. O’Grady, Kari A., and Richard H. York. 2012. Theism and Non-Theism in Psychological Science: Beyond the Conflict. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion 23: 75–80. [Google Scholar]
  97. Ogata, Akira, and Taihei Miyakawa. 1998. Religious experiences in epileptic patients with a focus on ictus-related episodes. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 52: 321–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Palhano-Fontes, Fernanda, Katia C. Andrade, Luis F. Tofoli, Antonio C. Santos, Jose Alexandre S. Crippa, Jaime E. C. Hallak, Sidarta Ribeiro, and Draulio B. de Araujo. 2015. The Psychedelic State Induced by Ayahuasca Modulates the Activity and Connectivity of the Default Mode Network. PLoS ONE 10: e0118143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Pennycook, Gordon, James Allan Cheyne, Paul Seli, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2012. Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition 123: 335–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Persinger, Michael A., Kevin S. Saroka, Stanley A. Koren, and Linda S. St-Pierre. 2010. The electromagnetic induction of mystical and altered states within the laboratory. Journal of Consciousness Exploration & Research 1: 808–30. [Google Scholar]
  101. Polimeni, Joseph, and Jeffrey P. Reiss. 2003. Evolutionary perspectives on schizophrenia. Canaadian Journal of Psychiatry 48: 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Proulx, Travis, Michael Inzlicht, and Eddie Harmon-Jones. 2012. Understanding all inconsistency compensation as a palliative response to violated expectations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 285–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Purzycki, Benjamin Grant, and Richard H. Sosis. 2011. Our Gods: Variation in Supernatural Minds. In The Frontiers Collection. Edited by A.C. Elitzur, L. Mersini-Houghton, M.A. Schlosshauer, M.P. Silverman, J.A. Tuszynski, R. Vaas and H.D. Zeh. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 77–93. [Google Scholar]
  104. Purzycki, Benjamin Grant, and Tayana Arakchaa. 2013. Ritual Behavior and Trust in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology 54: 381–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Purzycki, Benjamin Grant. 2010. Spirit Masters, Ritual Cairns, and the Adaptive Religious System in Tyva. Sibirica 9: 21–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Rad, Mostafa Salari, Alison Jane Martingano, and Jeremy Ginges. 2018. Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115: 11401–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Raichle, Marcus E. 2013. The Brain’s Default Mode Network. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Riemermann, James. 2006. What Is a Nontheist? Available online: http://www.nontheistfriends.org/article/what-is-a-nontheist (accessed on 25 October 2022).
  109. Rozin, Paul, Laura Lowery, Sumio Imada, and Jonathan Haidt. 1999. The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 574–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Russell, Pascale Sophie, and Roger Giner-Sorolla. 2013. Bodily moral disgust: What it is, how it is different from anger, and why it is an unreasoned emotion. Psychological Bulletin 139: 328–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Sedlmeier, Peter. 2018. Meditation and Altered States of Consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 25: 73–101. [Google Scholar]
  112. Seth, Anil K. 2014. A predictive processing theory of sensorimotor contingencies: Explaining the puzzle of perceptual presence and its absence in synesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 97–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Shepherd, Steven, Aaron C. Kay, Mark J. Landau, and Lucas A. Keefer. 2011. Evidence for the specificity of control motivations in worldview defense: Distinguishing compensatory control from uncertainty management and terror management processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47: 949–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Shiloh, Shoshana, Efrat Salton, and Dana Sharabi. 2002. Individual differences in rational and intuitive thinking styles as predictors of heuristic responses and framing effects. Personality and Individual Differences 32: 415–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Shiota, Michelle N., Dacher Keltner, and Amanda Mossman. 2007. The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cognition & Emotion 21: 944–63. [Google Scholar]
  116. Shweder, Richard A., Nancy C. Much, Manamohan Mahapatra, and Lawrence Park. 1997. The “Big Three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “Big Three” explanations of suffering. In Morality and Health. Edited by Allan Brandt and Paul Rozin. New York: Routledge, pp. 119–69. [Google Scholar]
  117. Sinha, Durganand. 2002. Culture and Psychology: Perspective of Cross-Cultural Psychology. Psychology & Developing Societies 14: 11–25. [Google Scholar]
  118. Slone, D. Jason. 2004. Theological Incorrectness: Why Religious People Believe What They Shouldn’t. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  119. Smigielski, Lukasz, Milan Scheidegger, Michael Kometer, and Franz X. Vollenweider. 2019. Psilocybin-assisted mindfulness training modulates self-consciousness and brain default mode network connectivity with lasting effects. NeuroImage 196: 207–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Sosis, Richard H., and Bradley J. Ruffle. 2004. Ideology, religion, and the evolution of cooperation: Field experiments on Israeli kibbutzim Ed. Donald Wood. Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 89–117. [Google Scholar]
  121. Sripada, Chandra Sekhar, and Stephen Stich. 2006. A framework for the psychology of norms. In The Innate Mind: Culture and Cognition. Edited by Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence and Stephen Stich. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 280–301. [Google Scholar]
  122. Stein, Dan J., and Randolph M. Nesse. 2011. Threat detection, precautionary responses, and anxiety disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 1075–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Szocik, Konrad, and Hans Van Eyghen. 2021. Revising Cognitive and Evolutionary Science of Religion, Religion as an Adaptation. Cham: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  124. Taves, Ann. 2020. Mystical and Other Alterations in Sense of Self: An Expanded Framework for Studying Nonordinary Experiences. Perspectives on Psychological Science 15: 669–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Taylor, Charles. 1989. Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  126. Taylor, Charles. 2007. A Secular Age, 1st ed. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  127. Taylor, Jill Bolte. 2006. My Stroke of Insight: A Brain Scientist’s Personal Journey. New York: Viking Penguin. [Google Scholar]
  128. Tellegen, Auke, and Gilbert Atkinson. 1974. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“ absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 83: 268–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Tucker, David M., Robert A. Novelly, and Preston J. Walker. 1987. Hyperreligiosity in temporal lobe epilepsy: Redefining the relationship. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 175: 181–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Turchin, Peter. 2011. Warfare and the Evolution of Social Complexity: A Multilevel-Selection Approach. Structure and Dynamics 4: 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1974. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 185: 1124–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Vail, Kenneth E., Zachary K. Rothschild, Dave R. Weise, Sheldon Solomon, Tom Pyszczynski, and Jeff Greenberg. 2010. A Terror Management Analysis of the Psychological Functions of Religion. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14: 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Van Cappellen, Patty, and Vassilis Saroglou. 2012. Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 4: 223–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Van de Vliert, Evert. 2008. Climate, Affluence, and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  135. Whitehouse, Harvey. 1996. Rites of Terror: Emotion, Metaphor and Memory in Melanesian Initiation Cults. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 2: 703–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Willard, Aiyana K., and Ara Norenzayan. 2017. “Spiritual but not religious”: Cognition, schizotypy, and conversion in alternative beliefs. Cognition 165: 137–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Willard, Aiyana K., and Rita Anne McNamara. 2019. The Minds of God(s) and Humans: Differences in Mind Perception in Fiji and North America. Cognitive Science 43: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Woody, Erik Z., and Henry H. Szechtman. 2011. Adaptation to potential threat: The evolution, neurobiology, and psychopathology of the security motivation system. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 1019–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Yaden, David Bryce, Jonathan Haidt, Ralph W. Hood, David R. Vago, and Andrew B. Newberg. 2017. The Varieties of Self-Transcendent Experience. Review of General Psychology 21: 143–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

McNamara, R.A. God(s)’ Mind(s) across Culture and Context. Religions 2023, 14, 222. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14020222

AMA Style

McNamara RA. God(s)’ Mind(s) across Culture and Context. Religions. 2023; 14(2):222. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14020222

Chicago/Turabian Style

McNamara, Rita Anne. 2023. "God(s)’ Mind(s) across Culture and Context" Religions 14, no. 2: 222. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14020222

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop