Next Article in Journal
Wang Bi’s “Confucian” Laozi: Commensurable Ethical Understandings in “Daoist” and “Confucian” Thinking
Previous Article in Journal
Religion and Help-Seeking: Theological Conservatism and Preferences for Mental Health Assistance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Examining the Relationships between Religious Affiliation, External and Internal Behavioural Factors, and Personal Carbon Footprint

Cátedra de Ética Ambiental, Departamento de Geología, Geografía y Medio Ambiente, Universidad de Alcalá, 28801 Alcalá de Henares, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2022, 13(5), 416; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13050416
Submission received: 8 April 2022 / Revised: 1 May 2022 / Accepted: 3 May 2022 / Published: 5 May 2022

Abstract

:
Different studies have shown that daily consumption is responsible for a large portion of greenhouse gas emissions. Since consumption is closely linked to individuals’ preferences, motivations, and beliefs, the personal carbon footprint should be a good indicator of actual consumers’ commitments towards climate change mitigation. Previous research has shown the importance of considering individual-level religion as an antecedent of mitigation outcomes, although the evidence is inconclusive in this regard. This study examines the relationship between religious affiliation and personal carbon footprint, following socio-psychological models that consider behaviour to depend on external or situational factors, and internal or intrinsic ones. A questionnaire was carried out on a random sample of the Spanish population (N = 845) to determine the main drivers of carbon footprint for different religious groups. External factors (i.e., socioeconomic) and internal ones related to climate change knowledge, commitment, and intractability, on the one hand, and value orientation, nature-relatedness, and the main motivation to conserve nature on the other hand, were analysed. Intergroup differences in the personal carbon footprint were found, especially based on sex, age group, and type of work among external factors and value orientation, the main motivation for conserving nature and climate change perceived commitment within the internals. Intragroup differences for food carbon footprint were also observed, as follows: the main motivation to conserve nature and the level of commitment implied differences among Catholic believers, whereas value orientation and the level of commitment implied differences among non-believers. Our conclusions suggest, on the one hand, the importance of examining the religion-mitigation link in a socio-psychological framework and, on the other, the need for further study within groups to promote better behavioural responses to climate change.

1. Introduction

Although climate change (CC) is one of the greatest problems we face as a society (IPCC 2021), current emission reduction and mitigation policies are far from meeting the goals indicated by the scientific community to avoid dangerous warming levels, particularly those included in the Paris Agreement (Victor et al. 2017). Limiting human-induced global warming implies cutting down greenhouse gas (GHG) emissions, not only at the institutional and organizational levels but also at societal and individual ones (O’Neill et al. 2014; Schultz and Kaiser 2012). Indeed, the literature reports that a large part of the total GHG emissions (between 60 and 70%) is related to personal consumption habits, so modifying emission trends requires a significant change in current lifestyles, particularly in Western societies (Franzen and Mader 2018; Ivanova and Büchs 2020; Richardsen Moberg et al. 2019).
Therefore, a better understanding of factors affecting individual behaviour, which would encourage people to reduce their emissions, is crucial to promote more efficient mitigation policies. Religious beliefs are one such factor that have received increasing attention in the last few years, and such beliefs have been shown to be related to environmental motivations (Chuvieco and Burgui 2016), sustainable consumption (Minton et al. 2015), and CC perceptions and attitudes (Jenkins et al. 2018). Considering the large global influence of religions, with a high level of affiliation (it is estimated that 84% of the world’s population identify themselves with a religious denomination (Pew Research Centre 2017)), their influence in world education (Palmer and Finaly 2002), their importance in the conservation of natural areas (Mallarach et al. 2016), and their impact on personal worldviews and ethical guidance (Sherkat and Ellison 2007), religious beliefs are indeed an important factor in understanding CC commitment, both at the societal (Rolston III 2009; Tatay-Nieto 2020) and individual levels (Cohen and Rozin 2001; Lakhan 2018).
Although religion has been foregrounded as an antecedent of different environmental outcomes (Gifford and Nilsson 2014), the empirical evidence is mixed on whether religion at the individual level promotes or constrains CC mitigation. Considering religious denomination, the evidence seems to suggest that conservative Christians are less concerned about CC, because of an association with political conservatism (Arbuckle 2017; Smith and Leiserowitz 2013), although some researchers point out that this evidence is restricted to the North American context (Morrison et al. 2015). In the case of Catholic believers, reporting positive (vs. negative) attitudes towards CC are associated with religious views of stewardship or human responsibility (vs. dominance) over nature (Agliardo 2013; Eom et al. 2021). In a broad sense, it seems that Catholics express higher levels of concern about CC than those belonging to other Christian churches, but believers, in general, are less concerned than people with no religious affiliation (atheists, agnostics) (Pew Research Centre 2015). However, there is cross-national evidence supporting the contrary (Kvaløy et al. 2012).
Several scholars point out that inconclusive findings may be related to a conceptualization problem, generally focused on the analysis of isolated antecedents, such as political orientation or a dominion’s views (Clements et al. 2014; Peifer et al. 2016). A step forward would be to consider religion as influencing environmental outcomes in complex ways and through interactions with other socio-psychological antecedents of behaviour (Michaels et al. 2021). The types of models that consider two main groups of behavioural factors are as follows (Gifford and Nilsson 2014; Clayton and Myers 2015): the external or situational ones, related to the environment and socioeconomic conditions (Corral-Verdugo et al. 2020), and the internal or intrinsic, related to individuals’ knowledge, motivations, and beliefs (Schultz and Kaiser 2012; Steg et al. 2015; Steg et al. 2016). Based on these, we argue that religion at the individual level could be a sound basis for the adoption of low-carbon lifestyles by underlying or interacting with these sets of behavioural antecedents (Orellano et al. 2020).
Therefore, we focus our research specifically on understanding the relationships between religious affiliation, external and internal antecedents of behaviour, and personal carbon footprint (CF), as an indicator of consumption-related emissions. Using this observed measure instead of self-reports should provide a more rigorous assessment of the personal impact on CC mitigation, as previous research has shown that self-assessments are not necessarily linked to actual behaviour (Kormos and Gifford 2014; Moser and Kleinhückelkotten 2018; Steg and de Groot 2019).
Regarding external factors affecting the relationships previously mentioned, there are well-established links between socioeconomic indicators and CF; past research has shown the positive influence of income on CF (with higher incomes resulting in higher CF), the non-linearity of age (with lower CF for younger and older citizens), and the impact of the type of work (with lower CF for students and home workers), among others (Chuvieco et al. 2021; Büchs and Schnepf 2013; Bhoyar et al. 2014; Brand and Preston 2010).
Internal factors have been widely recognized as having a long-term effect on consumption habits, particularly those related to curtailing behaviours (Gifford and Chen 2017). We have selected value orientation, motives to conserve nature, and nature-relatedness in our analysis, as they are relevant to environmental behaviour (Pearson 2016; Steg et al. 2015; Whitburn et al. 2019), although their role in consumption-related emissions has not been previously researched. Value orientation was directly related to CC attitudes and self-report mitigation behaviour (Bouman et al. 2018; Steg 2018), while peoples’ motives for conserving nature, its appreciation, and sense of connectedness (i.e., nature-relatedness) are suggested in the literature as significant for understanding environmental views and beliefs that are antecedent of attitudes and behaviour (Pearson 2016; Mace 2014). Additionally, research has shown that CC knowledge, perceived intractability, and the level of commitment are relevant to understanding mitigation outcomes, as they undergird much of consumers’ decisions (Pickering et al. 2021; van Valkengoed and Steg 2019; Poortinga et al. 2019; Corner et al. 2014).
To summarize, we aim to better understand the role of religious affiliation in explaining personal CF, and more specifically, to (1) examine intergroup differences, i.e., whether religious affiliation implies differences in personal CF, (2) analyse intragroup differences and whether external and internal factors mediate these differences, and (3) test how these interactions affect personal CF values, particularly for food and transport, the most important sectors of personal emissions (Büchs and Schnepf 2013; Druckman and Jackson 2016).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

Data were collected using a questionnaire, conducted by a social research company which specialized in online surveys (Netquest Soluciones de investigación S.L). The company follows the ISO 20,252 standard for the selection of panellists; it also complies with the European General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) and the Organic Law on Data Protection and Guarantee of Digital Rights (LOPDGDD) applied in Spain. The research company requests the consent of its panellists and informs them about the protection and the irreversible anonymization of their data, assuring that the respondent cannot be identified. Additionally, the survey included a brief introduction guaranteeing the anonymity and confidentiality of all participants, its voluntary nature, and that it could be abandoned at any time.
The sample was addressed to 1000 people following a stratified protocol to represent the geographical variety of the Spanish population. The survey included internal checks to filter potentially incorrect answers from the CF-related section, where we asked participants to introduce an estimate of their consumption, setting up a range of reasonable minimum and maximum values for each item. However, some answers were still found to be unreliable, so we first filtered the questionnaire manually, rejecting unrealistic responses. Then, we searched for anomalous cases using an automatic classification system. We grouped the CF values into five categories using the k-means algorithm (Likas et al. 2003) and reject those records assigned to groups with very few assigned cases (<30 people). The process was repeated 4 times until there were no marginal clusters (all groups had at least 50 cases). Following this process 845 cases were retained (84% of the original sample), which were used for further analysis.
The socio-demographic data of the final sample was as follows: 48% of the participants were female. In terms of age, 3% of the participants were between 16–17 years old; 16% were between 18–30 years old; 66% were between 31–65; 15% were 66 years old or above. In terms of religious affiliation, the outputs of our survey showed that 60.0% of the informants described themselves as religious. From this group, the majority were Christian Catholics (46.6%), followed by other Christians (2.9%), Buddhists (1.8%), Hindus (0.4%), Muslims (0.5%), and other confessions (7.8%). Since the number of believers of confessions other than Catholic was very small in the sample, we grouped them into the following groups: Catholic believers (46.6%), other-believers (13.4%), and non-believers (40.0%). Descriptive statistics and correlations are depicted in Table A2 and Table A3, Appendix A.

2.2. Measures

Carbon footprint. We asked participants to introduce a quantitative estimation of their consumption for different categories (transport, food, household energy, clothes, and others), and then we calculate their emissions in kgCO2eq following the specifications of the Carbon Footprint Observatory’s “CO2 web” (https://www.huellaco2.org/; last access on 16 September 2021). See (Burgui-Burgui and Chuvieco 2020) for a full description of the calculator.
Religious affiliation. We used one single item to assess participants’ religious affiliation (Pew Research Centre 2017). The options were Christian Catholic, Other Christian Churches, Buddhist, Hindu, Muslim, and Agnostic or Atheist.
Internal factors: We considered two groups of internal factors, those related to general environmental concern and those particularly associated with CC. Among the former, the following were included in the questionnaire: (a) Motives to conserve nature. In line with previous research, we assessed respondents’ main motivation for conserving nature by giving them several options to choose from (human responsibility, religious reasons, the intrinsic value of nature, economic motives, health, or well-being). These were adapted from (Chuvieco and Burgui 2016) and (Pearson 2016). (b) Value orientation. We asked participants about their value orientation (altruism, vulnerability, resilience, egoism) using a measure adapted from (Steg et al. 2016) and (Steg 2016). (c) Nature-relatedness. We measured this antecedent using the short version of the nature-relatedness scale from (Nisbet and Zelenski 2013), which includes a series of statements to assess participants’ connection to nature (e.g., “my relationship with nature is an important part of who I am”). Within the group of CC-related factors, we included the following: (a) Climate Change Knowledge. We asked participants about their knowledge of CC causes and the implications of human action (e.g., “Climate Change is mainly due to human action”). This measure was extracted from (Chuvieco et al. 2021). (b) Level of commitment. We asked participants their perception of personal commitment and if they perceived their emissions to be above or below the national average, as extracted from (Chuvieco et al. 2021). This measure was recoded in three categories: 1. highly committed and below-average emissions (self-perceived as having a low CF); 2. highly committed and above average (self-perceived as having a medium-high CF); 3. otherwise (no clear commitment to CC). (c) Perceived intractability. We asked participants about their perception of personal intractability (i.e., the importance of individual action to fight against CC). This was adapted from (Xiang et al. 2019). This variable was recoded as 1. the importance of personal actions in CC mitigation was high or very high, and 2. otherwise.
External factors. As external variables we used sex, age group, level of studies, monthly income, type of work, and political orientation, as these were found to be related to CF in previous studies (R. Gifford and Nilsson 2014; Chuvieco et al. 2021). All measures and details in terms of descriptive statistics are in Table A1 and Table A2 of Appendix A.

2.3. Analytical Strategy

Out of the five components of CF, we focused mainly on the transport and food sectors, as they account for 43% and 30% of total CF in our results, respectively, while they have also been identified as the most clearly linked to daily habits and personal decisions (Büchs and Schnepf 2013; Druckman and Jackson 2016).
Relationships between CF and its components were explored using correlation and dispersion plots. Association metrics for categorical variables were based on the Chi-square test and differences between sample groups with the Kruskal–Wallis (KW) non-parametric test, identifying which groups differed from others through post-hoc comparisons.
To determine the relative importance of the explanatory variables on relevant CF components, we chose a data-driven (i.e., automatic) method to run a non-parametric kernel regression (Racine 2019). The classical parametric approach to estimating these relationships requires specifying the functional form for the continuous variables and the nature of any interactions among all quantitative and qualitative regressors. If any of these assumptions are incorrect, the estimated model will be biased. Indeed, we first ran a test to verify the null hypothesis that a parametric model fits the data. We used a consistent model specification test (Hsiao et al. 2007) but the model was not shown to be significant (p = 0.185). Next, we proceeded with non-parametric methods, as they are less deterministic, to select the appropriate model (Racine 2008). The final model was based on a locally weighted least squares estimator, which is also known as local linear kernel regression (Li and Racine 2004). This model combines a simple regression model with a local weight function called a kernel function denoted as follows:
K   ( Z i ) = K ( x X i ) h
where x and i are vectors of regressors and h is a smoothing parameter called bandwidth.
The direct counterpart of model selection for parametric approaches is bandwidth selection, a key aspect of non-parametric kernel methods. Non-parametric regression computes a bandwidth object for a p-variate kernel regression estimator defined over mixed continuous and discrete (unordered, ordered) data using least-squares cross-validation (Li and Racine 2004). To evaluate the goodness of fit the model returns an r-squared, denoted as follows:
R 2 = i = 1 n ( γ i γ ^ i ) ( γ ^ i γ i ) 2 i = 1 n ( γ i γ ^ i ) 2 i = 1 n ( γ ^ i γ i ) 2
where γi is the outcome and γ ^ i the fitted value for observation i, and R 2 will always lie in the range [0, 1] where 1 denotes a perfect fit to the sample data and 0 denotes no predictive power. Finally, to find relevant predictors, we first established the null that a predictor was irrelevant (the same as when doing a t-test of significance), and then we calculated the p-values. For those p-values less than 0.05, we rejected the null at the 5% level and concluded that the predictors were relevant.
These analyses were processed in R software, version 4.1.0 with np package (Racine and Hayfield 2020). The function npreg() in this package automates the procedure explained above and allowed us to do a non-parametric regression, and to automatically choose the appropriate bandwidth using least-squares cross-validation (Hayfield and Racine 2008).

3. Results

3.1. Interactions between Religious Affiliation and External and Internal Factors

First, we examined the relationships between religious affiliations and external factors to test whether the potential explanation for differences in CF between religious groups might be related to cross-relationships with other variables. Table A3 of Appendix A provides descriptive statistics for each religious affiliation. Table 1 shows that only level of studies and political orientation among the external factors introduce differences between religious groups. Post-hoc comparisons show that other-believers had a low level of education than their alternative groups, while for political orientation, Catholics were closer to right and non-believers closer to left orientation. Of the internal factors, religious groups were associated with the main motivation to conserve nature, nature-relatedness, CC knowledge, and perceived commitment. The other internal variables did not show significant differences among religious groups.
From the significant differences, we observed that Catholic believers tended to prioritize human responsibility, and health and well-being, over other motivations for preserving nature, while for other-believers and non-believers, the main reason was the intrinsic value of nature, also followed by health and well-being (Table A4, Appendix A). All groups claimed to have a high connection to nature, although other-believers showed higher values than other groups (Table A5, Appendix A). As for CC knowledge, non-believers reported more knowledge about the anthropogenic cause of CC (with low importance of natural factors: Table A6, Appendix A), while Catholics reported the lowest knowledge. Related to the association between religious affiliation and CC perceived commitment (Table A7, Appendix A), in relative terms non-believers showed the highest degree of perceived commitment, with the lowest of both among Catholics, although all religious groups had a majority of respondents identified with the “no particular commitment” category. Finally, CC intractability did not show any significant differences (Table A8, Appendix A).

3.2. Differences in Personal Carbon Footprint across Religious Groups Based on External Factors

The mean personal CF of our sample was 5010 kg of COeq, which was lower than the per capita emission rate of Spain for the same year, 2019 (6861.21 kgCO2eq/year: https://www.ine.es/jaxi/Tabla.htm?path=/t26/p084/base_2010/serie/&file=01004.px, last accessed on 24 February 2022). It should be noted that our estimation did not include emissions linked to construction or export goods, nor touristic lodgings. Out of total emissions, 73% were produced by transport (43%) and food (30%), far more than from other CF components, such as household energy consumption (10%), clothing (6%), and others (11%). As such, we focused the analysis on the following three CF components: total, transport, and food.
As observed in Table 2, age group and type of work introduced significant differences in all CF sectors for the overall sample. Young people, students, and domestic workers had significantly lower total and transport CF than their alternative groups, while domestic workers had higher food CF. Focusing on the level of studies and income, we observed that those who had the highest level of education (university degree and higher) and higher income were also those who had the highest total and transport CF. Sex only implied differences for the transport sector, with women having the lowest CF. Finally, political orientation was not significantly associated with CF, neither in the total CF nor in any of its components.
Religious affiliation was not found to be significantly related to the total and transport CF components, but it was for the food CF. Within religious groups, Catholics showed significantly higher food CF than non-believers.
Table 3 presents intergroup differences in CF, considering external factors. In this case, most relations are not significant, meaning that all religious groups have similar CF within the different categories of external factors. Only within food CF were differences found for women, adult population (30–65), and administration and home workers. Non-believing women, those belonging to the adult population (30–65 years old), and those working in the administration sector had a lower CF than Catholic believers of those categories. In addition, domestic workers of other believers had lower CF values than Catholics.
Table 4 shows results for CF differences within religious groups, related to external factors. In the case of Catholic believers, the largest differences were found in the transport sector. Women and those older people (>65 years) had the lowest CF value, while adults (30–65 years) had the highest. Level of studies, monthly income, and type of work also implied differences among Catholics, with the lowest level of education and income having the lowest CF values. Regarding the type of work, students and home workers had the lowest transport CF, and workers in the industry the highest. Age group and monthly income also imply differences among this group, being the lowest CF of those with the lowest income; older people (>65 years) had also the lowest CF value among age groups, while adults (30–65 years) had the highest. In the case of other-believers, those with the lowest level of education and income were those with the lowest transport CF. Relative to non-believers, women had lower total and transport CF than men; younger people have the lowest total and transport CF, and adults (30–65) have the highest; those with lower incomes were also those with a lower transport CF. Finally, home workers had the highest food CF, while students had the lowest.

3.3. Differences in Personal Carbon Footprint across Religious Groups Based on Internal Factors

Table 5 presents the results of CF differences across religious groups for the internal factors. None of them introduced significant differences between religious groups concerning total CF. For food CF, resilience value, CC knowledge, CC perceived commitment and CC intractability introduced significant differences between religious groups, while for transport CF, only the main motivation to conserve nature introduced differences. Among those indicating the role of human responsibility as a strong motivation of nature conservation, Catholics had lower transport CF than non-believers. Finally, those non-believers having higher levels of resilience value orientation, CC knowledge, CC perceived commitment and CC intractability were found to have significantly lower food CF than Catholics, having no significant differences with the other-believers group.
Table 6 shows CF differences within religious groups related to internal factors. Only CC perceived commitment was found to introduce significant differences among Catholics in food CF, showing the lowest CF values for people with a high level of commitment. For other-believers, none of the internal factors implied significant differences in any of the CF dimensions, while for non-believers, value orientation was found to be relevant for total and transport CF. In this case, those having higher levels of resilience values had lower transport CF, while those having lower levels of egoistic values had lower total and food CF.

3.4. Regression Models

In this section, we focused on food CF to deepen the effect of religious beliefs, since this sector showed most of the significant differences between religious groups. In addition, it was identified by different studies as one of the most clearly linked to daily habits and personal decisions (Büchs and Schnepf 2013; Druckman and Jackson 2016).
Before running the regression model, we checked for collinearity between factors. Except for monthly income and the level of studies (rs= 0.4, p = 0.001), collinearity was low between them (Table A2, Appendix A). Therefore, we included all factors in the regression model, except for monthly income, as this variable had unfilled responses and was highly correlated with the level of studies. Two regression models were run, for Catholic and non-believers. Other-believers were not considered in this analysis, as no significant relations were found in food CF in any of the external or internal factors.
Table 7 presents the results of regression models. The models explain food CF for non-believers (R2 Food CF =0.38), with the age group of the external factors and value orientation for the internal factors resulting significant. For Catholic believers (R2 Food CF = 0.35), age group, main motivation to conserve nature, and the level of CC perceived commitment were found to be significantly associated with food CF.

4. Discussion

4.1. Main Findings

This study has shown the value in considering religious affiliation in order to better understand how consumption habits impact CC mitigation. We have shown the variability in GHG emissions for different religious groups, namely Catholic believers, other-believers, and non-believers. Even though past studies analysed the link between religious affiliation, consumption, and CC actions (Leonard and Pepper 2015), they were based on self-perception measures rather than actual behaviour, and it is well known that self-perceived measures are not necessarily linked to actual commitment (Gifford and Chen 2017; Kormos and Gifford 2014). In addition, previous research mainly focused on the analysis of isolated antecedents related to religion at the individual level and mitigation outcomes, but given the influence of several factors affecting CC mitigation behaviour, we examined this link within a social-psychological model that considers behaviour to depend on external and internal determinants (Clayton and Myers 2015; Gifford and Nilsson 2014). Thus, preliminary findings are not intended to resolve the inconsistencies of previous research, but to provide insights from this perspective.
Consistent with previous findings (Chuvieco et al. 2021), our results showed that several external factors were significantly associated with CF values, considering all religious affiliations together. In addition, we observed interesting inter- and intra-group differences in personal CF based on external and internal factors affecting consumers’ behaviour. Clearly, the CF for the food sector is the most closely related to the interactions between religious affiliation and those factors. Deepening these intergroup comparisons, non-believers were found to have a lower CF than Catholics. Additionally, these groups differ on external factors, as non-believing women, those belonging to the adult population (30–65), those working in administration, and domestic employees had a lower CF than Catholics of the same groups.
It is important to know what internally motivates individuals to support and effectively mitigate CC. Regarding the internal factors, our findings showed that the motivation to conserve nature is an important driver of CF. Human responsibility was more frequently selected among Catholic believers, while the intrinsic value of nature was more valued by non-believers, coincident with previous findings (Chuvieco and Burgui 2016). Intergroup comparisons for internal factors showed that the lowest transport CF was estimated for those Catholics who selected human responsibility as the main motivation to conserve nature, as compared to non-believers. On the other side, those non-believers who had higher levels of resilience value orientation, CC knowledge, CC perceived commitment, and CC intractability were found to have a significantly lower CF than Catholics of the same cohorts.
In addition, intragroup differences revealed the importance of CC perceived commitment among Catholics, showing the lowest food CF values for those with high level of commitment. For non-believers, value orientation was found to be significantly associated with CF, showing that having higher levels of resilience values and lower levels of egoistic values led to lower CF.
Finally, the regression model showed that the main factors controlling food CF for Catholics were age group, and the main motivations to conserve nature and CC perceived commitment. The age group was also relevant for food CF of non-believers, joined by value orientation.

4.2. Implications

Although these findings should be considered as a first approximation to the implications of religious affiliation in personal consumption linked to the CF indicator, there are some potential implications that may derive from our findings. One is to deepen intragroup study, leaving aside the idea of homogeneous groups within each religious affiliation. Thus, to promote effective ways of CC engagement and action, efforts should focus on addressing the potentials and misperceptions of each group. For example, our findings support previous evidence that non-believers have higher agreements about the anthropogenic cause of CC (Morrison et al. 2015; Zaleha and Szasz 2015; Uzarevic and Coleman 2021), in contrast to believers (Barker and Bearce 2012; Ecklund et al. 2016; Zaleha and Szasz 2015), and this is useful for designing mitigation policies, but at the same time, they express difficulties in handling CC anxiety (Hope and Jones 2014), which should also be considered. The same occurs in the case of believers. Even though they have the least CC knowledge and higher CF values, our findings suggest that these outcomes vary depending on the interaction with other factors, such as the main motivation for nature conservation and the level of CC commitment. This is coincident with the literature that considers religions’ influence as multifaceted and dependent on other determinants such as political ideology, fundamentalism, the level of education, and the geographical context (Arbuckle 2017; Kilburn 2014; Lewis et al. 2018; Morrison et al. 2015). However, in linking our results with previous studies, we should consider that they have mostly been conducted in the USA, and mainly focused on differences among Christian denominations. For instance, the relevance of literal versus non-literal interpretation of the Bible was found significantly related to environmental concern (McCammack 2007; Guth et al. 1995). However, in Spain this distinction is not relevant, as evangelical groups linked to a literal interpretation of the Bible have much less social influence than in the USA.
A second implication of our study relates to the attitude of believers towards CC. A more detailed study should focus on factors affecting some believers’ perception of CC that leads them to have opposing opinions on CC (Dickinson et al. 2016), in spite of the general calls of religious authorities to support CC action. A theological framework adapted to different religious affiliations still needs to be developed in order to find solid grounds for general acceptance of religious principles that would directly link to environmental commitment, similarly to the generalized agreement on other issues, such as concerns for the poor (Pew Research Centre 2015). It is suggested in the literature that religion at the individual level could both diminish concerns through stronger views of dominance and indifference towards nature, and promote it through values of sacredness, spirituality, and stewardship (Preston and Baimel 2021). Religious denominations could provide resources from their traditions to approach issues such as CC, consumption habits, and lifestyles, because of their ability to construct frameworks that can guide those who follow them (Tucker 2003). In this sense, it is important to emphasize the role of prominent religious leaders in promoting lifestyle changes and encouraging a more rigorous commitment to CC actions. Among many examples of these, we can emphasize the interfaith declarations addressed to the different UN Climate Summits (https://interfaithclimate.org/the-statement; last accessed on 16 February 2022), the common declaration of Pope John Paul II and Patriarch Bartholomew on environmental responsibility (Common Declaration on Environmental Ethics. Common Declaration of John Paul II and the Ecumenical Patriarch Bartholomew I 2002) or the encyclical letter Laudato Si of Pope Francis (2015). The impact of these initiatives has been wide, but still requires further implementation at the local and individual level (Tsimpo and Wodon 2016; Hanchin and Hearlson 2020; Savino 2019).

4.3. Limitations and Future Research Lines

Although the richness of this study is based on numerical estimations of personal CF based on detailed emission factors, which goes beyond self-perceptions of estimations, certain limitations should be considered. Quantitative estimations of CF require accurate inputs (energy bills, food amounts, transport distances), which are difficult to acquire. Even though all respondents were warned about this before starting the survey, we had to remove questionnaires for including very unlikely CF values, using visual and automatic classification methods. The final values included in our analysis seemed reasonable, but it is not possible to test their actual accuracy.
Limitations also include the generalizability of the results to another cultural or geographical contexts, considering that individuals with the same religious affiliation may behave differently based on other cultural factors. More research outside of North America and European countries is needed to expand our understanding about the relationship between religious beliefs and different measures of consumption, such as the personal FC indicator, to address the north-western bias in existing studies.
Finally, this study only includes the measure of religious affiliation, which leaves aside the possibility of analysing and interpreting, for example, differences based on the degree of individual religiosity, or the frequency of participation in religious services, or considering intrinsic or extrinsic measures of religiosity. There are a wide variety of religious commitments and practices, even within the same religious tradition and affiliation. Future research should focus on more precise measurements of religiosity, which should provide further nuances in the links established in our study. In this regard, our analysis may serve as a starting point for better understanding the role of religious beliefs in CC mitigation.

5. Conclusions

Climate change mitigation at the individual level requires understanding and promoting behavioural change by analysing its underlying factors. Previous studies on the relationship between religion and CC mitigation outcomes present inconclusive findings. Therefore, we examine an explanation that considers individual-level religion as interacting in complex ways with other socio-psychological antecedents of behaviour. Examining these relationships within this framework allows more robust conclusions to be drawn than the study of isolated factors.
Thus, this study shows that religious affiliation is associated with consumption-related emissions, measuring by the personal carbon footprint indicator, and relevant external and internal antecedents of behaviour. Intergroup differences in the personal carbon footprint were found, especially based on sex, age group, and type of work among external factors, and value orientation, the main motivation for conserving nature, and CC perceived commitment within the internals. Intragroup differences for food carbon footprint were also observed, as follows: the main motivation to conserve nature and the level of CC commitment implied differences among Catholic believers, whereas value orientation and also the level of CC commitment implied differences among non-believers. These preliminary conclusions are not intended to resolve inconclusive findings, but to provide insights for considering the relationship between religion and CC mitigation outcomes in complex ways. From this perspective, we aim to go beyond the debate that focuses only on differences between religious groups, toward an understanding of these complex interactions, so that addressing the misperceptions and potentials of each group will foster better behavioural responses to climate change.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.O. and E.C.; methodology, A.O. and E.C.; software, A.O.; validation, E.C.; formal analysis, A.O.; investigation, A.O.; resources, A.O.; data curation, A.O.; writing—original draft preparation, A.O.; writing—review and editing, E.C.; supervision, E.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by a pre-doctoral fellowship of “Fundación Tatiana Perez de Guzmán el Bueno”.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. List of variables included in the study.
Table A1. List of variables included in the study.
TypeCategoriesDescription
Target Variables
Carbon footprintQuantitativeInterval scaleTotal, transport, food, clothes, household energy (kgCO2eq)
Religious affiliationNominal3Catholic believers, other-believers, and non-believers.
Internal Variables
The main motivation for conservationNominal5Human responsibility, religious reasons, the intrinsic value of nature, economic motives, health or well-being.
Value OrientationOrdinal4Egoism, vulnerability, altruism, resilience, Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree)
Nature relatedness Ordinal6Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree)
CC knowledgeBinary21 = Identify GHG as the main cause and consider natural factors as having low or very low importance; 2 = otherwise
CC Perceived commitmentNominal31 = highly committed and below-average emissions; 2 = highly committed and above average; 3 = otherwise
CC Perceived IntractabilityBinary21 = importance of personal actions high or very high; 2 = otherwise
External Variables
SexBinary2Male 1, female 2
Age groupOrdinal416–17, 18–30, 31–65, >65
Level of studiesOrdinal3No studies or primary, secondary school, university studies
Monthly Income Ordinal3<1500 €, 1500 a 3000 € and >3000 €
Type of workNominal9Student, agriculture, industry, office work, education, catering, other services, management, housework
Political orientationOrdinal3From the original 10 Likert scale, we grouped in 3 classes: left (<4), Centre (4–7) and right mind (>8)
Table A2. Spearman correlation coefficients. P-values lower than 0.001 are marked as ** and lower than 0.01 as *.
Table A2. Spearman correlation coefficients. P-values lower than 0.001 are marked as ** and lower than 0.01 as *.
Variables12345678910111213141516
1Transport CF1
2Food CF0.021
3Total CF0.73 **0.45 **1
4Sex0.16 *0.02−0.11
5Age group−0.010.08 *−0.03−0.31 *1
6Level of studies0.15 *−0.040.08 *0.01−0.08 *1
7Monthly income0.3 **0.020.20.160.090.40 **1
8Political orientation0.030.040.05−0.03 *0.04−0.020.001
9CC knowledge−0.080.09 *0.000.06−0.09 *−0.15 *0.180.17 *1
10Commitment0.060.020.07 *0.06−0.07 *−0.05−0.010.15 *0.21 *1
11Intractability0.02−0.030.01−0.09 *0.08 *−0.07−0.020.07 *0.10 *0.23 *1
12Nature relatedness−0.020.02−0.03−0.020.01 *0.02−0.010.13 *0.12 *0.34 **0.22 **1
13Value orientation: egoism0.040.040.040.040.11 **−0.02−0.060.11 **0.11 *0.04 **0.010.051
14Value orientation: vulnerability0.040.040.04−0.040.13 **−0.010.04−0.01 *−0.02 *0.16 **−0.08 *0.18 **−0.17 *1
15Value orientation: altruism0.010.020.02−0.030.01 *0.09 *0.020.12 **0.12 **0.25 **0.14 **0.30 **−0.20 *0.35 **1
16Value orientation: resilience0.01−0.010.01−0.000.010.010.03−0.01−0.07 *0.11*0.10.19 **0.01 *0.28 *0.35 **1
Table A3. Descriptive statistics for the overall sample and each religious group.
Table A3. Descriptive statistics for the overall sample and each religious group.
Overall SampleCatholics BelieversOther-BelieversNon-Believers
VariablesMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Transport CF2163.661512.832120.141506.222079.781677.802242.441462.79
Food CF1509.171001.231637.511064.991407.07914.251393.70934.88
Total CF5009.932167.635084.232205.094931.802346.534949.442062.75
Sex1.480.501.490.501.560.501.460.50
Age group2.930.643.080.592.650.742.840.61
Level of studies2.480.642.450.612.340.562.550.53
Monthly income2.010.752.000.771.860.702.070.74
Political orientation1.750.661.960.651.760.601.510.60
CC knowledge1.480.501.530.501.570.501.400.49
CC Commitment2.360.772.450.722.280.772.280.81
CC Intractability1.380.491.390.491.370.491.380.49
Nature relatedness3.510.773.450.793.730.773.520.74
Value orientation: egoism2.731.122.691.102.961.162.701.12
Value orientation: vulnerability3.930.953.970.963.840.993.910.94
Value orientation: altruism3.960.863.910.864.000.924.010.84
Value orientation: resilience4.120.884.120.914.140.904.150.84
Table A4. Cross-table religious groups and main motivation to conserve nature.
Table A4. Cross-table religious groups and main motivation to conserve nature.
ReasonsHuman Responsibility N, Col%, Row%Spiritual/Religious N, Col%, Row%Intrinsic Value N, Col%, Row%Economic N, Col%, Row%Health and Well-being N, Col%, Row%Total
Catholic Believers132, 57.64, 33.503, 42.86, 0.76107, 34.74, 27.1621, 65.62, 5.33131, 48.70, 33.25394, 46.63, 100
Other-Believers27, 11.79, 23.894, 57.14, 3.5444, 14.29, 38.943, 9.38, 2.6535, 13.01, 30.97113, 13.37, 100
Non-Believers70, 30.57, 20.710, 0.00, 0.00157, 51.97, 46.458, 25.00, 2.37103, 38.29, 30.47338, 40.00, 100
Total229, 100, 27.107, 100, 0.83308, 100, 36.4532, 100, 3.79269, 100, 31.83845, 100, 100
Table A5. Cross-table religious groups and nature relatedness.
Table A5. Cross-table religious groups and nature relatedness.
Nature RelatednessLow N, Col%, Row%Medium N, Col%, Row%High N, Col%, Row%Total
Catholic Believers38, 46.91, 9.64131, 55.74, 33.25225, 42.53, 57.11394, 46.63, 100
Other-Believers9, 11.11, 7.9623, 9.79, 20.3581, 15.31, 71.68113, 13.37, 100
Non-Believers34, 42.98, 10.0681, 34.47, 23.96223, 42.16, 65.98338, 40.0, 100
Total81, 100, 9.57235, 100, 27.81529, 100, 62.60845, 100, 100
Table A6. Cross-table religious groups and CC knowledge.
Table A6. Cross-table religious groups and CC knowledge.
Climate Change KnowledgeGHG Main Cause and Natural Factors Low Importance N, Col%, Row%Otherwise N, Col%, Row%Total
Catholic Believers187, 42.67,47.5207, 50.86, 52.54394, 46.63,100
Other-Believers49, 11.19, 43.3664, 15.72, 56.64113, 13.37, 100
Non-Believers202, 46.12, 59.76136, 33.42, 40.24338, 40.00,100
Total438, 100, 51.83407, 100, 48.17845, 100, 100
Table A7. Cross-table religious groups and perceived commitment.
Table A7. Cross-table religious groups and perceived commitment.
Perceived CommitmentHighly Committed, Below-Average Emissions N, Col%, Row%Highly Committed, Above-Average N, Col%, Row%Otherwise (No Particular Commitment) N, Col%, Row%Total
Catholic Believers53, 34.87, 13.45109, 46.58, 27.66232, 50.54, 58.88394, 46.63, 100
Other-Believers22, 14.47, 19.4737, 15.81, 32.7454, 11.76, 47.79113, 13.37, 100
Non-Believers77, 50.66, 22.7888, 37.61, 26.04173, 37.69, 51.18338, 40.00, 100
Total152, 100, 17.99234, 100, 27.69459, 100, 54.32845, 100, 100
Table A8. Cross-table religious groups and perceived intractability.
Table A8. Cross-table religious groups and perceived intractability.
Perceived IntractabilityHigh or Very High N, Col%, Row%Low or Very Low N, Col%, Row%Total
Catholic Believers242, 46.27, 61.42152, 47.20, 38.58394, 46.63, 100
Other-Believers71, 13.58, 62.8342, 13.04, 37.17113, 13.37, 100
Non-Believers210, 40.15, 62.13128, 39.75, 15.15 338, 40.00, 100
Total523, 100, 62.89322, 100, 38.1 1845, 100, 100

References

  1. Agliardo, Michael. 2013. The U.S Catholic Response to Climate Change. In How the World’s Religions Are Responding to Climate Change: Social Scientific Investigations, 1st ed. London: Routledge, pp. 174–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Arbuckle, Matthew B. 2017. The Interaction of Religion, Political Ideology, and Concern About Climate Change in the United States. Society & Natural Resources 30: 177–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barker, David C., and David H. Bearce. 2012. End-Times Theology, the Shadow of the Future, and Public Resistance to Addressing Global Climate Change. Political Research Quarterly 66: 267–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bhoyar, Sankesha P., Suyash Dusad, Rachit Shrivastava, Sidharth Mishra, Nishank Gupta, and Anand B. Rao. 2014. Understanding the Impact of Lifestyle on Individual Carbon Footprint. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences 133: 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Bouman, Thijs, Linda Steg, and Henk A. L. Kiers. 2018. Measuring Values in Environmental Research: A Test of an Environmental Portrait Value Questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Brand, Christian, and John Preston. 2010. ’60-20 Emission’—The Unequal Distribution of Greenhouse Gas Emissions from Personal, Non-Business Travel in the UK. Transport Policy 17: 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Büchs, Milena, and Sylke V. Schnepf. 2013. Who Emits Most? Associations between Socio-Economic Factors and UK Households’ Home Energy, Transport, Indirect and Total CO2 Emissions. Ecological Economics 90: 114–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Burgui-Burgui, Mario, and Emilio Chuvieco. 2020. Beyond Carbon Footprint Calculators. New Approaches for Linking Consumer Behaviour and Climate Action. Sustainability 12: 6529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chuvieco, Emilio, and Mario Burgui. 2016. Impact of Religious Affiliation on Ethical Values of Spanish Environmental Activists. Religions 7: 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Chuvieco, Emilio, Mario Burgui-Burgui, Anabel Orellano, Gonzalo Otón, and Paloma Ruíz-Benito. 2021. Links between Climate Change Knowledge, Perception and Action: Impacts on Personal Carbon Footprint. Sustainability 13: 8088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Clayton, Susan, and Gene Myers. 2015. Conservation Psychology: Understanding and Promoting Human Care for Nature. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  12. Clements, John M., Aaron M. McCright, and Chenyang Xiao. 2014. Green Christians? An Empirical Examination of Environmental Concern Within the U.S. General Public. Organization & Environment 27: 85–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cohen, Adam B., and Paul Rozin. 2001. Religion and the Morality of Mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Common Declaration on Environmental Ethics. Common Declaration of John Paul II and the Ecumenical Patriarch Bartholomew I. 2002. Available online: https://www.vatican.va/content/john-paul-ii/en/speeches/2002/june/documents/hf_jp-ii_spe_20020610_venice-declaration.html (accessed on 16 February 2022).
  15. Corner, Adam, Ezra Markowitz, and Nick Pidgeon. 2014. Public Engagement with Climate Change: The Role of Human Values. WIREs Climate Change 5: 411–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Corral-Verdugo, Victor, Marc Yancy Lucas, César Tapia-Fonllem, and Anais Ortiz-Valdez. 2020. Situational Factors Driving Climate Change Mitigation Behaviors: The Key Role of pro-Environmental Family. Environment, Development and Sustainability 22: 7269–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Dickinson, Janis L., Poppy McLeod, Robert Bloomfield, and Shorna Allred. 2016. Which Moral Foundations Predict Willingness to Make Lifestyle Changes to Avert Climate Change in the USA? PLoS ONE 11: e0163852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Druckman, Angela, and Tim Jackson. 2016. Understanding Households as Drivers of Carbon Emissions. In Taking Stock of Industrial Ecology. Edited by Roland Clift and Angela Druckman. Cham: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Ecklund, Elaine, Christopher Scheitle, Jared Peifer, and Daniel Bolger. 2016. Examining Links Between Religion, Evolution Views, and Climate Change Skepticism. Environment and Behavior 49: 985–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Eom, Kimin, Tricia Qian Hui Tok, Carmel S. Saad, and Heejung S. Kim. 2021. Religion, Environmental Guilt, and pro-Environmental Support: The Opposing Pathways of Stewardship Belief and Belief in a Controlling God. Journal of Environmental Psychology 78: 101717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Franzen, Axel, and Sebastian Mader. 2018. Consumption-Based versus Production-Based Accounting of CO2 Emissions: Is There Evidence for Carbon Leakage? Environmental Science & Policy 84: 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Gifford, Robert D., and Angel K. S. Chen. 2017. Why Aren’t We Taking Action? Psychological Barriers to Climate-Positive Food Choices. Climatic Change 140: 165–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Gifford, Robert, and Andreas Nilsson. 2014. Personal and Social Factors That Influence Pro-Environmental Concern and Behaviour: A Review. International Journal of Psychology 49: 141–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Guth, James L., John C. Green, Lyman A. Kellstedt, and Corwin E. Smidt. 1995. Faith and the Environment: Religious Beliefs and Attitudes on Environmental Policy. American Journal of Political Science 39: 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hanchin, Timothy, and Christy Lang Hearlson. 2020. Educating for Ecological Conversion: An Ecstatic Pedagogy for Christian Higher Education amid Climate Crisis. Religious Education 115: 255–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hayfield, Tristen, and Jeffrey S. Racine. 2008. Nonparametric Econometrics: The Np Package. Journal of Statistical Software 27: 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Hope, Aimie, and Christopher Jones. 2014. The Impact of Religious Faith on Attitudes to Environmental Issues and Carbon Capture and Storage (CCS) Technologies: A Mixed Methods Study. Technology in Society 38: 48–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hsiao, Cheng, Qi Li, and Jeffrey S. Racine. 2007. A Consistent Model Specification Test with Mixed Discrete and Continuous Data. Journal of Econometrics 140: 802–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. IPCC. 2021. Climate Change 2021: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Available online: https://www.ipcc.ch/report/sixth-assessment-report-working-group-i/ (accessed on 16 February 2022).
  30. Ivanova, Diana, and Milena Büchs. 2020. Household Sharing for Carbon and Energy Reductions: The Case of EU Countries. Energies 13: 1909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Jenkins, Willis, Evan Berry, and Luke Beck Kreider. 2018. Religion and Climate Change. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 43: 85–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kilburn, H. Whitt. 2014. Religion and Foundations of American Public Opinion towards Global Climate Change. Environmental Politics 23: 473–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kormos, Christine, and Robert Gifford. 2014. The Validity of Self-Report Measures of Proenvironmental Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Review. Journal of Environmental Psychology 40: 359–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kvaløy, Berit, Henning Finseraas, and Ola Listhaug. 2012. The Publics’ Concern for Global Warming: A Cross-National Study of 47 Countries. Journal of Peace Research 49: 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lakhan, Calvin. 2018. The Garbage Gospel: Using the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain the Role of Religious Institutions in Affecting pro-Environmental Behavior among Ethnic Minorities. The Journal of Environmental Education 49: 43–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Leonard, Rosemary J., and Miriam D. Pepper. 2015. Les Attitudes Face Aux Changements Climatiques et Les Actions Pour La Décroissance Énergétique Des Chrétiens Pratiquants: Les Effets Des Persuasions Religieuses et Du Capital Social. Social Compass 62: 326–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lewis, Gregory, Risa Palm, and Bo Feng. 2018. Cross-National Variation in Determinants of Climate Change Concern. Environmental Politics 28: 793–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Li, Qi, and Jeffrey Racine. 2004. Cross-Validated Local Linear Nonparametric Regression. Statistica Sinica 14: 485–512. [Google Scholar]
  39. Likas, Aristidis, Nikos Vlassis, and Jakob J. Verbeek. 2003. The Global K-Means Clustering Algorithm. Pattern Recognition 36: 451–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Mace, Georgina. 2014. Whose Conservation? Science 345: 1558–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Mallarach, Josep Maria, Josep Corcó, and Thymio Papayannis. 2016. Christian Monastic Lands as Protected Landscapes and Community Conserved Areas: An Overview. Available online: http://repositori.uic.es/handle/20.500.12328/2925 (accessed on 20 February 2022).
  42. McCammack, Brian. 2007. Hot Damned America: Evangelicalism and the Climate Change Policy Debate. American Quarterly 59: 645–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Michaels, Jay L., Feng Hao, Julia Smirnov, and Ishan Kulkarni. 2021. Beyond Stewardship and Dominion? Towards a Social Psychological Explanation of the Relationship between Religious Attitudes and Environmental Concern. Environmental Politics 30: 622–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Minton, Elizabeth A., Lynn R. Kahle, and Chung-Hyun Kim. 2015. Religion and Motives for Sustainable Behaviors: A Cross-Cultural Comparison and Contrast. Journal of Business Research 68: 1937–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Morrison, Mark, Roderick Duncan, and Kevin Parton. 2015. Religion Does Matter for Climate Change Attitudes and Behavior. PLoS ONE 10: e0134868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Moser, Stephanie, and Silke Kleinhückelkotten. 2018. Good Intents, but Low Impacts: Diverging Importance of Motivational and Socioeconomic Determinants Explaining Pro-Environmental Behavior, Energy Use, and Carbon Footprint. Environment and Behavior 50: 626–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Nisbet, Elizabeth K., and John M. Zelenski. 2013. The NR-6: A New Brief Measure of Nature Relatedness. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. O’Neill, Brian C., Elmar Kriegler, Keywan Riahi, Kristie L. Ebi, Stephane Hallegatte, Timothy R. Carter, Ritu Mathur, and Detlef P. van Vuuren. 2014. A New Scenario Framework for Climate Change Research: The Concept of Shared Socioeconomic Pathways. Climatic Change 122: 387–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Orellano, Anabel, Carmen Valor, and Emilio Chuvieco. 2020. The Influence of Religion on Sustainable Consumption: A Systematic Review and Future Research Agenda. Sustainability 12: 7901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Palmer, Martin, and Victoria Finaly. 2002. Faith in Conservation—New Approaches to Religions and the Environment. Washington: World Bank Group. [Google Scholar]
  51. Pearson, Richard. 2016. Reasons to Conserve Nature. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 31: 366–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Peifer, Jared L., Simranjit Khalsa, and Elaine Howard Ecklund. 2016. Political Conservatism, Religion, and Environmental Consumption in the United States. Environmental Politics 25: 661–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Pew Research Centre. 2015. Catholics Divided Over Global Warming. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/religion/2015/06/16/catholics-divided-over-global-warming/ (accessed on 5 January 2022).
  54. Pew Research Centre. 2017. The Changing Global Religious Landscape. Available online: https://www.pewforum.org/2017/04/05/the-changing-global-religious-landscape/ (accessed on 2 January 2022).
  55. Pickering, Gary, Kaylee Schoen, and Marta Botta. 2021. Mitigation and Adaptation Strategies for Global Change Lifestyle Decisions and Climate Mitigation: Current Action and Behavioural Intent of Youth. Mitigation and Adaptation Strategies for Global Change 26: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Poortinga, Wouter, Lorraine Whitmarsh, Linda Steg, Gisela Böhm, and Stephen Fisher. 2019. Climate Change Perceptions and Their Individual-Level Determinants: A Cross-European Analysis. Global Environmental Change 55: 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Pope Francis. 2015. Encyclical Letter Laudato SI’ of the Holy Father Francis on Care for Our Common Home. Available online: https://www.vatican.va/content/francesco/es/encyclicals/documents/papa-francesco_20150524_enciclica-laudato-si.html (accessed on 16 February 2022).
  58. Preston, Jesse L., and Adam Baimel. 2021. Towards a Psychology of Religion and the Environment. Current Opinion in Psychology 40: 145–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Racine, Jeffrey S. 2008. Nonparametric Econometrics: A Primer. Foundations and Trends® in Econometrics 3: 1–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Racine, Jefrey. 2019. Reproducible Econometrics Using R. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Racine, Jeffrey, and Tristen Hayfield. 2020. Nonparametric Kernel Smoothing Methods for Mixed Data Types. Available online: https://www.stat.cmu.edu/~cshalizi/350/hw/06/np/html/np-package.html (accessed on 12 December 2021).
  62. Richardsen Moberg, Karen, Carlo Aall, Florian Dorner, Elsa Reimerson, Jean-Paul Ceron, Bore Sköld, Benjamin Sovacool, and Valentino Piana. 2019. Mobility, Food and Housing: Responsibility, Individual Consumption and Demand-Side Policies in European Deep Decarbonisation Pathways. Energy Efficiency 12: 497–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Rolston III, H. 2009. Science and Religion in the Face of the Environmental Crisis. In The Oxford Handbook of Religion and Ecology. Edited by Roger S. Gottlieb. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. Savino, Damien. 2019. Proposing an Examen for Living the Ecology of Daily Life and Building a Culture of Care. The Trumpeter 34: 103–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Schultz, P., and F. Kaiser. 2012. Promoting Pro-Environmental Behavior. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology. Edited by Susan Clayton. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  66. Sherkat, Darren E., and Christopher G. Ellison. 2007. Structuring the Religion-Environment Connection: Identifying Religious Influences on Environmental Concern and Activism. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 46: 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Smith, N., and A. Leiserowitz. 2013. American Evangelicals and Global Warming. Global Environmental Change 23: 1009–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Steg, Linda. 2016. Values, Norms, and Intrinsic Motivation to Act Proenvironmentally. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 41: 277–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Steg, Linda. 2018. Limiting Climate Change Requires Research on Climate Action. Nature Climate Change 8: 759–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Steg, Linda, and Judith IM de Groot. 2019. Environmental Psychology: An Introduction, 2nd ed. Hooboken: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  71. Steg, Linda, Goda Perlaviciute, and Ellen van der Werff. 2015. Understanding the Human Dimensions of a Sustainable Energy Transition. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Steg, Linda, Siegwart Lindenberg, and Kees Keizer. 2016. Intrinsic Motivation, Norms and Environmental Behaviour: The Dynamics of Overarching Goals. International Review of Environmental and Resource Economics 9: 179–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Tatay-Nieto, Jaime. 2020. Sustainability, the Pontifical Academy of Sciences, and the Catholic Church’s Ecological Turn. Religions 11: 488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Tsimpo, Clarence, and Quentin Wodon. 2016. Faith Affiliation, Religiosity, and Attitudes Towards the Environment and Climate Change. The Review of Faith & International Affairs 14: 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Tucker, Mary Evelyn. 2003. Worldly Wonder: Religions Enter Their Ecological Phase. Virginia: Open Court. [Google Scholar]
  76. Uzarevic, Filip, and Thomas J. Coleman. 2021. The Psychology of Nonbelievers. Current Opinion in Psychology 40: 131–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. van Valkengoed, Anne M., and Linda Steg. 2019. Meta-Analyses of Factors Motivating Climate Change Adaptation Behaviour. Nature Climate Change 9: 158–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Victor, David G, Keigo Akimoto, Yoichi Kaya, Mitsutsune Yamaguchi, Danny Cullenward, and Cameron Hepburn. 2017. Prove Paris Was More than Paper Promises. Nature 548: 25–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Whitburn, Julie, Wayne Linklater, and Wokje Abrahamse. 2019. Meta-analysis of Human Connection to Nature and Proenvironmental Behavior. Conservation Biology 34: 180–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Xiang, Peng, Haibo Zhang, Liuna Geng, Kexin Zhou, and Yuping Wu. 2019. Individualist-Collectivist Differences in Climate Change Inaction: The Role of Perceived Intractability. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Zaleha, Bernard Daley, and Andrew Szasz. 2015. Why Conservative Christians Don’t Believe in Climate Change. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists 71: 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Association between external and internal variables and religious groups.
Table 1. Association between external and internal variables and religious groups.
External and Internal FactorsStatisticp-Value
Sex3.5300.169
Age group10.2380.235
Level of studies13.0440.001
Monthly income7.5650.560
Type of work1.2580.262
Political orientation91.011<0.001
Main motivation to conserve nature48.261<0.001
Nature-relatedness12.6400.009
Value orientation: egoistic5.0720.079
Value orientation: vulnerability2.3830.304
Value orientation: altruistic3.1550.207
Value orientation: resilience0.0130.940
CC Knowledge14.7840.001
CC Perceived commitment13.3480.009
CC Perceived intractability0.0870.957
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 2. Kruskal–Wallis values from testing CF differences between external factors.
Table 2. Kruskal–Wallis values from testing CF differences between external factors.
ExternalTotalFoodTransport
FactorsStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-Value
Sex2.970.0850.050.82326.56<0.001
Age group15.960.0018.300.04029.43<0.001
Level of studies6.650.0361.820.40218.62<0.001
Monthly Income31.73<0.0012.720.25653.65<0.001
Type of work21.570.00620.470.00942.70<0.001
Political orientation2.590.2745.280.0720.6500.722
Religious affiliation3.9360.14012.0340.0020.781;0.677
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 3. Differences in personal CF across religious groups for each external factor.
Table 3. Differences in personal CF across religious groups for each external factor.
External Factors TotalFoodTransport
CategoriesStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-Value
SexMale1.6340.4421.3240.5166.2010.050
Female3.2910.19314.2900.0010.0080.996
Age (years)<183.6130.1643.4390.1790.2370.888
18–300.6210.7330.3180.8530.6320.729
30–650.8900.64112.1580.0020.9780.613
>650.4830.7860.4170.8121.1550.561
Level of studiesPrimary1.3540.5081.1900.5514.4770.107
Secondary0.5990.7416.1550.0510.4990.779
University0.8080.6685.5240.0631.2020.548
Income (€)<15005.2470.0732.4140.2996.2650.051
1500–30000.7990.6712.8030.2460.3130.855
>30002.1180.3472.5970.2730.0280.986
Political OrientationLeft0.0550.9737.0400.0512.6400.267
Centre0.9040.6365.8310.0543.9980.135
Right0.1110.9460.2300.9881.3850.500
Type of workStudent0.2650.8764.3610.1132.8150.245
Agriculture4.2000.1220.6110.7372.8000.247
Industry0.9300.6281.8120.4040.1260.939
Administration1.1440.56410.6360.0050.2560.880
Education2.0870.3521.8710.3921.9260.382
Catering1.4180.4922.7920.2480.7300.694
Health, Military0.9430.6241.2760.5283.0190.221
Entrepreneurs0.2450.8820.5340.7660.7250.696
Homeworkers5.5030.06416.509<0.0012.9020.234
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 4. Differences in personal CF across external factors for each religious group.
Table 4. Differences in personal CF across external factors for each religious group.
Catholics Believers
External FactorsTotalFoodTransport
Statisticp-ValueStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-Value
Sex3.0550.8900.0700.12526.560.005
Age group88840.0316.8500.07712.990.005
Level of studies4.7010.0951.0730.5857.3110.026
Monthly Income29.572<0.0012.9950.22427.799<0.001
Type of work12.7610.12013.6240.09225.1670.001
Political orientation1.5290.4660.3410.8431.6690.434
Other-Believers
Sex2.250.7860.0600.41227.560.479
Age group0.8050.8481.1410.7676.5640.087
Level of studies3.4130.1810.2250.8947.7860.020
Monthly Income5.3230.0703.6170.16412.7960.002
Type of work14.5330.06914.5620.06816.8620.056
Political orientation1.0500.5922.5460.2801.3970.497
Non-Believers
Sex3.560.0040.0600.30221.89<0.001
Age group9.7320.0215.5400.1360.0460.046
Level of studies0.3750.8291.5680.4574.4390.109
Monthly Income2.1050.3490.2010.90413.1520.001
Type of work6.6080.57921.2810.00610.5980.226
Political orientation0.3430.8423.5580.1691.3840.501
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 5. Differences in CF across religious groups for each internal factor.
Table 5. Differences in CF across religious groups for each internal factor.
Internal Factors TotalFoodTransport
CategoriesStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-Value
The main motivation for conservationHuman responsability2.0780.3541.1000.5776.1450.046
Religious reasons1.1250.7241.1250.2890.1250.724
Intrinsic value3.3540.1872.6580.2650.1370.504
Economic motives2.8790.2373.4800.1760.7570.685
Health or well-being0.2640.8764.6780.0961.5640.457
Value OrientationEgoism2.7940.2474.9190.0851.6320.442
Vulnerability0.1940.9071.7990.4073.3690.186
Altruism0.8760.6455.8880.0534.1750.124
Resilience2.7160.25710.6360.0500.2350.889
Nature relatedness4.6490.0984.2920.1170.6140.36
CC KnowledgeGHG main cause3.3850.18411.9860.0022.3620.307
Otherwise0.3840.8251.6660.4350.9850.611
CC Perceived commitmentHighly committed, below-average emissions0.5300.7674.1400.1262.7380.254
Highly committed, above-average emissions0.1830.9137.7550.0213.3390.188
Otherwise0.2140.8992.9940.2240.3590.836
CC IntractabilityImportance of personal actions high or very high1.6710.43410.9070.0042.2410.326
Otherwise1.0670.5872.9880.2242.0580.357
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 6. Differences in personal CF across internal factors for each religious group.
Table 6. Differences in personal CF across internal factors for each religious group.
Catholics Believers
Internal VariablesTotalFoodTransport
Statisticp-ValueStatisticp-ValueStatisticp-Value
The main motivation for conservation4.8290.3056.8640.1431.4510.835
Value Orientation: egoism6.1770.1861.2790.8654.1510.386
Value Orientation: vulnerability1.8110.7710.9860.9122.7590.599
Value Orientation: altruism2.8730.5792.0200.7320.5020.973
Value Orientation: resilience3.5150.4761.4300.8390.9920.911
Nature relatedness0.8060.6512.0590.3572.0750.354
CC Knowledge18.7550.59519.7010.75917.3810.080
CC Perceived commitment3.4820.1756.0030.0453.9280.140
CC Intractability18.5750.86816.8820.17019.6030.271
Other-believers
The main motivation for conservation2.2050.6981.2340.8722.2750.685
Value Orientation: egoism5.3620.2527.3320.1190.9750.914
Value Orientation: vulnerability3.2900.5111.5880.8115.6500.227
Value Orientation: altruism3.1220.5382.1750.7044.1010.393
Value Orientation: resilience2.0390.7291.1690.8051.1090.893
Nature relatedness 2.1930.3343.7800.1510.0170.991
CC Knowledge1.8240.1381.9020.5301.530.975
CC Perceived commitment1.3500.5094.5310.1041.3270.515
CC Intractability1.6980.2191.6620.3081.5080.920
Non-believers
The main motivation for conservation5.4090.1442.4730.4805.9500.114
Value Orientation: egoism15.4800.0409.8150.0443.6400.457
Value Orientation: vulnerability3.2260.5212.9310.5692.3110.679
Value Orientation: altruism7.6940.1036.8350.1457.0120.135
Value Orientation: resilience4.5410.3382.8900.57613.2490.010
Nature relatedness0.0820.9603.2240.2200.1230.940
CC Knowledge13.8070.93615.4620.05012.4930.158
CC Perceived commitment1.7120.4253.2250.1990.5690.752
CC Intractability12.9970.61112.9830.60014.0610.476
p-values in bold are significant.
Table 7. Non-parametric regression model with internal and external factors for Food CF.
Table 7. Non-parametric regression model with internal and external factors for Food CF.
Food Carbon Footprint
Catholics Believers Non-Believers
Sex0.1330.870
Age group0.007 **0.000 ***
Level of studies0.6870.051
Type of work0.0830.067
Political orientation0.0730.406
Main motivation to conserve N0.030 *0.058
Value orientation: egoism0.1980.023 *
Value orientation: vulnerability0.4610.233
Value orientation: altruism0.3380.298
Value orientation: resilience0.1720.118
Nature relatedness0.7190.118
CC Knowledge0.6870.080
CC Perceived Commitment0.018 *0.002 *
N394338
R-squared0.350.38
Residual standard error:0.5080.520
p-values are significant at * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Orellano, A.; Chuvieco, E. Examining the Relationships between Religious Affiliation, External and Internal Behavioural Factors, and Personal Carbon Footprint. Religions 2022, 13, 416. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13050416

AMA Style

Orellano A, Chuvieco E. Examining the Relationships between Religious Affiliation, External and Internal Behavioural Factors, and Personal Carbon Footprint. Religions. 2022; 13(5):416. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13050416

Chicago/Turabian Style

Orellano, Anabel, and Emilio Chuvieco. 2022. "Examining the Relationships between Religious Affiliation, External and Internal Behavioural Factors, and Personal Carbon Footprint" Religions 13, no. 5: 416. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13050416

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop