Next Article in Journal
AST-120 Reduces Neuroinflammation Induced by Indoxyl Sulfate in Glial Cells
Previous Article in Journal
Emotional Prosody Processing in Schizophrenic Patients: A Selective Review and Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Physician Empathy Interacts with Breaking Bad News in Predicting Lung Cancer and Pleural Mesothelioma Patient Survival: Timing May Be Crucial

1
University of Lille, CNRS, CHU Lille, UMR 9193—SCALab—Cognitive and Affective Sciences, F-59000 Lille, France
2
University of Lille, Department of Thoracic Oncology, Albert Calmette University Hospital, F-59000 Lille, France
3
University of Lille, CHU Lille, EA 2694, Public Health: Epidemiology and Quality of Care, F-59000 Lille, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7(10), 364; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7100364
Submission received: 14 September 2018 / Revised: 3 October 2018 / Accepted: 15 October 2018 / Published: 17 October 2018
(This article belongs to the Section Epidemiology & Public Health)

Abstract

:
This study is the first to examine the prognostic role of physician empathy in interaction with the type of consultation (TC) (TC, bad news versus follow-up consultations) in cancer patient survival. Between January 2015 and March 2016, 179 outpatients with thoracic cancer and a Karnofsky performance status ≥60 assessed their oncologist’s empathy using the CARE questionnaire, which provides a general score and two sub-dimensions: listening/compassion and active/positive empathy. Survival was recorded until April 2018. Usual medical, social and psychological confounders were included in the Cox regression. The median follow-up time was 3.1 years. There was a statistical interaction between listening/compassion empathy and TC (p = 0.016) such that in bad news consultations, higher listening/compassion predicted a higher risk of death (hazard ratio (HR) = 1.13; 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.03–1.23; p = 0.008). In follow-up consultations, listening/compassion did not predict survival (HR = 0.94; 95% CI: 0.85–1.05; p = 0.30). The same results were found with the general score of empathy, but not with active/positive empathy. In bad news consultations, high patient-perceived physician compassion could worry patients by conveying the idea that there is no longer any hope, which could hasten death. Further studies are warranted to confirm these results and find out the determinants of patient perception of physician empathy.

1. Introduction

Lung cancer is one of the most fatal types of cancer, with 30% survival at one year post-diagnosis of non-small cell lung cancer [1]. One way to improve the prognosis is to identify modifiable prognostic factors such as physical exercise, diet, or patient care. A major meta-analysis showed that psychological factors such as depression or coping predicted prognosis in cancer [2]. However, physician empathy, i.e., the physician’s ability to understand the affective and physical experiences of patients and convey this understanding to them [3], was not part of this meta-analysis. Yet, in other conditions, physician empathy has been associated with multiple health outcomes such as HbA1C or cholesterol in diabetic patients for example [4].
In oncology, associations between physician empathy and various patient outcomes have been shown [5,6]. However, the role of patient perception of physician empathy in cancer survival has never been examined. The main objective of this study was thus to study the prognostic role of patient perception of physician empathy in cancer patient survival. Related to empathy, the role of social support in survival, including cancer survival, has been established [7,8]. As a major source of support for cancer patients, physician empathy may impact survival [9], perhaps especially in the context of informing patients on their disease progression. In such a personally meaningful context of receiving bad news, it is plausible that most patients attend carefully to the content and manner of the physician’s speech. How physicians present the progress of the disease and its treatment to patients could play a major role in counteracting the potential helplessness/hopelessness of patients [10], which are associated with poorer prognosis [11,12]. Biologically, empathy is also related to the hormone oxytocin [13], which has anti-proliferative, anti-metastatic and anti-angiogenic effects in some cancers [14].
Furthermore, the effect of empathy on prognosis could vary according to the type of consultation (TC). For example, in an emotionally ‘neutral’ consultation (i.e., follow-up of treatment), physician empathy may have a weak effect on patients since patients are theoretically not distressed. Conversely, when bad news is being given, the effects of empathy could be stronger due to greater patient distress [15,16]. Thus, we hypothesized that higher physician empathy would be related to a lower risk of death in bad news consultations but not in follow-up consultations (interaction hypothesis).
Finally, the definition of medical empathy is far from consensual [17]. However, two types of empathy can be distinguished: a rather passive empathy of listening and compassion, whereby the physician listens attentively to patients and shows them compassion; and an active and positive empathy whereby the physician tries to give control and options to patients (e.g., providing a great deal of information and shared-decision making) and stays positive [18]. We thus tested whether the hypothesized interaction between empathy and type of consultation, in relation to survival, was valid for both types of empathy or not (secondary objective). Multiple sociodemographic and medical confounders were taken into account. Two psychological confounders—patient cancer-related distress and patient emotional skills (i.e., patient skills in identifying, understanding, expressing and regulating emotions)—were also considered for their known role in cancer progression [2,19,20,21,22,23], as they could modify the influence of empathy on survival.

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Participants

Inclusion criteria for physicians were treating thoracic cancer patients in an outpatient hospital setting. Inclusion criteria for patients were thoracic cancer at all stages or types of cancer, outpatients ≥18 years old, aware of the cancer diagnosis and with a Karnofsky Performance Status (KPS) ≥60. Performance status is a score that estimates the patient’s ability to perform certain activities of daily living without the help of others. These daily activities include basic activities such as getting dressed, eating, and bathing, as well as more complex activities such as cleaning the house and working a regular job [24]. The scores range from 0 “dead” to 100 “normal”. For example, a patient at 60 requires occasional assistance and cares for most needs. Exclusion criteria were patients for whom there was no clear information regarding disease progression (e.g., waiting for test results) and those with a psychiatric disorder.

2.2. Design and Procedure

The data used were from an earlier study whose results had been reported previously [25]. The study was approved by the French national ethics committee (approval number 14.545). In short, during the initial study, physicians working in the thoracic cancer department of the University of Lille (France) proposed the study to patients meeting the inclusion criteria at the end of a consultation. After the consultation, interested patients signed an informed consent and completed a questionnaire assessing their physician’s empathy, their own cancer-related distress, emotional skills and socio-demographic data (see below for details). The type of consultation was reported by the physicians just after the consultations. Medical and clinical data—Karnofsky index, cancer stage, type of cancer, type of treatments before the study, Charlson index of comorbidities, metastases, mutations epidermal growth factor receptor (EGFR), anaplastic lymphoma kinase (ALK) or ROS proto-oncogene 1, receptor tyrosine kinase (ROS), smoking status and time since diagnosis to study entry–were retrieved by the research assistants. The initial study ran from January 2015 to July 2016. For the present extended study, patients were followed until 1 April 2018, the date of censoring. At this point, research assistants added the survival status and treatments received by patients after the initial study.

2.3. Measures

Patient perception of physician empathy was measured using the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure, a 10-item 5-point Likert scale [26]. Items of this scale deal with the patient’s perception of physician listening, respect, clear explanations and information provision, whether the physician fully understood his/her concerns, showed care and compassion, tried to be positive while staying honest, helped him/her to take control, and made an action plan with him/her. While the scale was designed to provide a global score, studies haves suggested that it could be divided into two subscales [18,26,27]: a listening/compassion score (items 1 to 6) and an active/positive empathy score (items 7 to 10). Cronbach’s α was 0.95 in our sample for the global score, 0.93 for the listening/compassion score, and 0.91 for the active/positive empathy score.
Cancer-related distress was assessed using the emotional dimension of the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy-General (FACT-G), a 6-item 5-point Likert scale [28]. Higher scores represent higher distress. Examples of items are “I feel sad”, “nervous”, “I worry about dying”, and “I am losing hope in the fight against my illness”.
Patient emotional skills were assessed using the Short-Profile of Emotional Competence (S-PEC) scale [29], a 20-item 5-point Likert scale providing a score of emotional skills (e.g., how patients identify, understand, express and regulate their emotions and the emotions of others in general). Examples of items are: “When I feel good, I can easily tell whether it is due to being proud of myself, happy or relaxed”, “It is easy for me to explain my feelings to others” or “I find it difficult to handle my emotions” (reversed). Higher scores represent higher emotional skills.
The type of consultation was reported by the physician at the end of the consultation: if the patient was informed of a cancer progression in spite of treatment, or cancer relapse or the end of active treatment, this was considered a “bad news consultation”. Otherwise, it was a “follow-up consultation”.
Sociodemographic data were self-reported by patients and included age, gender, education and financial status.
Medical data were obtained from medical charts and included the Karnofsky index, cancer stage, type of cancer, Charlson index of comorbidities, metastases, mutation types (AGFR, ALK or ROS), treatments before and after the study entry, and time between diagnosis and study entry.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Continuous variables are expressed as mean ± SD. Categorical variables are presented as absolute numbers and percentages. Distributions were examined for outliers. For categorical variables, frequencies were inspected and categories were merged when there were insufficient cases in one or more categories. Median follow-up was estimated using the inverse Kaplan-Meier method. Whether covariates differed between bad news and follow-up consultations was examined using independent sample t tests or Pearson χ². The univariate association of survival with each potential covariate was assessed using Cox proportional hazard regression models. From the fitted Cox models, the unadjusted hazard ratio with 95% CIs was obtained, and the significance of the association was tested using the Wald χ² statistic. Log-linearity and proportional hazards assumptions were checked for each covariate using Martingale and Schoenfeld residuals as well as supremum tests. If the proportional hazards assumption was not met, a piecewise model was created. The covariates that were tested included gender, age, education (no diploma, high school, bachelor degree vs. > bachelor degree), financial situation (not comfortable, moderately comfortable vs. very comfortable), type and severity of cancer (Non-Small Cell Lung cancer (NSCL)-Small Cell Lung cancer (SCL) Stage I, II, III vs. NSCL-SCL stage IV, mesothelioma), metastases, Karnofsky index of 60–70 vs. >70, time between diagnosis and inclusion in the study in years, Charlson index, mutation EGFR or ALK or ROS vs. none of these, cancer-related distress and patient emotional skills. All covariates except smoking status (too many missing data) and treatments (many treatments and high correlations with other medical data) were included in the multivariate model. Although no prior sample size calculation was done for this data analysis because of the retrospective design of this analysis, attention was paid to roughly respect the rule of 10 events per covariate [30,31] in spite of debates about this rule of thumb [32,33]. To choose between two non-nested models, the Schwarz Bayesian Criterion (SBC) was used, a smaller SBC indicating a better fit (at least two points of differences are required). To illustrate the effect of empathy on survival by the type of consultation with Kaplan-Meier curves, empathy was dichotomized at the median of 45. Curves were plotted, controlling for confounders, with continuous variables set at their means and categorical variables set at their mode. A two-tailed type I error rate <0.05 was considered for statistical significance. Analyses and graphs were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Among the 179 patients in the total sample, the median follow-up was estimated at 3.1 years, 95% CI (2.92–3.09). Eighty-three patients (46.4%) were still alive at the time of censoring, 1 April, 2018 (Table 1). Patients were mostly older men with a relatively low level of education, non-small cell lung cancers at stages I, II or III, a Karnofsky score indicating correct functional status. Forty three percent of patients were included in the study after the disclosure of bad news, consisting overwhelmingly of a change of treatment due to treatment failure. Five physicians participated in the study to present it to eligible patients and collect their agreements.
Table A1 (Appendix A) shows the comparisons of the “bad news consultation” sample to the “follow-up consultation” one for all socio-demographic variables. Expected differences were observed between the two samples regarding the type and stages of cancer, the presence of metastases, the treatments received, the comorbidity index and the number of deaths at study censoring. At the psychological level, perceived empathy did not differ between bad news consultations and follow-up ones. Patients were also similar in emotional skills. As expected, however, cancer-related distress was higher after a bad news consultation than after a follow-up one.
Table A2 (Appendix A) shows the comparisons of the low versus high patient-perceived physician empathy groups. The two groups were identical with the exception of three variables: there was a higher proportion of men in the low patient-reported empathy group compared to the other group (high empathy), cancer-related emotional distress was higher in the low empathy group, and patients had a lower level of emotional skills in the low empathy group.

3.2. Prediction of Survival with the General Score of Empathy

The univariate analyses (Table 2) showed that age, comorbidities, cancer-related distress, stage IV cancers and mesotheliomas (compared to stages I, II, or III), metastasis and receiving chemotherapy before and/or after inclusion in the study were associated with an increased risk of death. Conversely, being a woman, having had surgery before the study, and being without additional treatment after inclusion in the study were protective factors for survival.
In the multivariate analysis (Table 3), age, stage IV or mesothelioma, and cancer-related distress increased the risk of death. On the contrary, being a woman and having a high school diploma (compared to no diploma) were protective factors for survival. Controlling for sociodemographic and medical variables, the interaction between the type of consultation and patient-perceived empathy was significant (p = 0.02). However, it went in the opposite direction to our hypothesis. While, as hypothesized, empathy was not related to survival in follow-up consultation, HR = 0.96 by point of empathy score, 95% CI (0.90–1.03), it unexpectedly increased the risk of death by 6%, 95% CI (1.01–1.12) in bad news consultations (Figure 1). Thus, a one-point increase in the empathy questionnaire increased the risk of death by 6% in patients receiving bad news.
To be sure that treatments did not change the results, in an alternative model, we introduced treatments associated with survival at p < 0.05 in the univariate analyses. This did not change the results, but the fit of this alternative model (SBC = 689.8) was not as good as that of the model presented in Table 3 (SBC = 668.8) and therefore it was not retained.

3.3. Prediction of Survival by Distinguishing the Two Types of Empathy

In order to determine if the interaction was due to the two types of empathy, the model was reiterated identically but general empathy was first replaced by listening and compassion empathy (model A) and then by active and positive empathy (model B). The results are presented in Table 4.
A significant interaction was found only with the empathy of listening and compassion. This type of empathy was associated with an increased risk of death in the bad news condition, HR = 1.13 by point of score, 95% CI (1.03–1.23), p = 0.008, but not in the follow-up condition, HR = 0.94 by point of score, 95% CI (0.86–1.05), p = 0.30. In contrast, active and positive empathy did not interact with the type of consultation, (p = 0.07). Regardless of the type of consultation, active/positive empathy was not associated with survival. Interestingly, both models detailing a specific type of empathy had a better fit (SBC of 556.3 for listening/compassion and 649.5 for active/positive empathy) than the model with general empathy (SBC of 668.8).

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, these are the first data examining the relationship between patients’ perceived oncologist empathy and survival in lung cancer. Unexpectedly, in the bad news consultations, patient-perceived empathy was associated with a higher risk of death after controlling for sociodemographic, psychological and biomedical variables. It should be remembered that in our sample, mean perceived-empathy was high compared to other samples [34]. In fact, a high level of physician empathy can worry patients and convey the message that the situation is really bad [35], which could hasten death. Indeed, previous studies have shown that patient awareness of the palliative nature of the situation negatively impacts their survival [36,37,38,39]. The fact that only compassion and listening empathy was associated with death is consistent with the hypothesis that highly unusual compassion and listening from physicians could be harmful, as it may equate to a loss of hope in patients’ mind. On the contrary, positive and active empathy, which is by definition less worrying and more oriented towards an action plan, was not linked to survival status in our sample. Delivering bad news honestly while remaining positive and focused on an action plan is probably thus the challenge for physicians in order to maintain patient hope and control, as written in the American Society of Clinical Oncology guideline [40]. The challenge is the same for patients: to maintain a feeling of hope and control in spite of bad news.
This study had limitations. First, the mean empathy was high so that our results need to be replicated in samples with lower empathy. Second, the generalizability of findings to other samples with different characteristics needs to be tested. In particular, our sample was mostly composed of male patients and the results may be totally different for female patients suffering from other cancers. Indeed, female cancer patients are much more sensitive to physician attitude, support, communication and information than male patients [41]. Besides, in our sample, being a female patient was a protective factor. It might be the case because women are generally more prone to search and find social support, which is positively associated with higher survival [8]. Our sample was also composed of a majority of people with a low level of education. High school diploma was a protective factor compared to patients with no diploma, probably as it allows a feeling of control and a better health literacy. A sample with a higher education level could have been less sensitive to physician empathy. Indeed, cancer patients with a high education are less sensitive to physician communication and attitude and rely more on alternate sources of information than patients with a low education level [41,42]. The results could also be the opposite (as initially hypothesized) for cancers with a better prognosis. Finally, the results are limited by the exploratory nature of these post-hoc analyses and by the patient-reported measure of physician empathy. Further studies could consider more survival predictors such as immunohistochemistry biomarkers (e.g., p16 or BAP1 in pleural mesothelioma [43]), risk profiles (i.e., a combination of risk factors, which predicts survival [44]), or more specific treatments (e.g., PD-1 PD-L1). Taking into account these limitations, further evidence is essential to provide more definitive conclusions.

5. Conclusions

In bad news consultations, higher patient perception of physician listening and compassion empathy was associated with a higher risk of death in lung cancer patients. These results could have serious implications for physician-patient communication training as, if they were further confirmed, they would challenge the pervasive and implicit idea that physicians must always be compassionate and would call for a clarification of what is meant by ‘empathy’. Research demonstrating weak or mixed evidence of the effectiveness of recommended guidelines for breaking bad news such as the six-step SPIKES protocol [42,45,46], which promotes an empathic attitude, suggest that such “breaking-bad-news rules” do not meet all patients’ needs [47]. In fact, evidence-based research is really warranted regarding breaking bad news. Further research would then be necessary to elucidate what physician behaviors or words are related to the patient-perception of physician empathy, which in turn is related to better patient outcomes. Further research would also be needed to determine what patient factors (e.g., personality, medical history) might explain the patient-perception of physician empathy and patient outcomes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L., V.C. and Y.G.; Formal analysis, S.L., C.P. and Y.G.; Funding acquisition, V.C.; Investigation, A.C.; Methodology, S.L. and Y.G.; Project administration, S.L.; Resources, A.C.; Software, C.P. and S.L.; Visualization, C.P.; Writing—original draft, S.L. and Y.G.; Writing—review & editing, S.L., A.C., V.C., C.P. and Y.G.

Funding

This research was funded by the SIRIC (SIte de Recherche Intégrée sur le Cancer) ONCOLille, Grant INCa-DGOS-Inserm 6041.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge Christophe Mariette’s strong support to this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Characteristics of patients in follow-up versus bad news consultations.
Table A1. Characteristics of patients in follow-up versus bad news consultations.
CharacteristicsFollow-Up ConsultationsBad-News Consultationsp Value
Number of Patients%Mean (Standard Deviation, SD)Number of Patients%Mean (SD)
Patients (n = 179)
Sociodemographic data
Age 63.4 (11.9) 63.4 (10.5)0.99
Men6766.3 5469.2 0.68
Married or in a relationship (Yes)8281.2 5369.7 0.08
Patient-reported education 0.67
No diploma2221.8 1721.8
High school4948.5 4152.6
Bachelor degree1817.8 1519.2
More than bachelor degree1211.9 56.4
Patient-reported financial situation 0.86
Not at all or not very comfortable1716.8 1114.1
Moderately comfortable5554.5 4557.7
Rather or very comfortable2928.7 2228.2
Medical data
Type and severity of cancer <0.0001
Stage I, II or III (NSCL or SCL)6261.4 2228.2
Stage IV (NSCL or SCL)2221.8 3443.6
Mesothelioma (no stage assigned)87.9 1316.7
Missing data98.9 911.5
Smoker 0.12
Never1211.9 1215.4
Ever7170.3 8544.8
Missing data1817.8 3139.7
Time since diagnosis and inclusion in the study (years) 2.5 (2.4) 2.4 (1.9)0.64
Karnofsky index 0.76
60–7054.9 33.9
>705493.1 7191.0
Missing data22.0 45.1
Metastasis1716.8 3443.6 <0.0001
Treatment before inclusion
Chemotherapy5756.4 6279.5 0.0012
Surgery6059.4 2228.2 <0.0001
Radiotherapy4746.5 2430.8 0.0325
Targeted therapies55.1 911.7 0.10
Treatment after inclusion
Surgery65.9 22.6 0.28
Chemotherapy3130.7 4962.8 <0.0001
Radiotherapy1615.9 2228.2 0.04
Targeted therapies1211.9 1924.4 0.03
Immunotherapy1110.9 2532.1 0.0005
No further treatment4948.5 1114.1 <0.0001
Charlson index of comorbidities 7.6 (2.7) 8.5 (2.2)0.03
Genetic mutation: EGFR or ALK or ROS (%)87.9 1215.4 0.12
Deceased at censoring3635.6 5266.7 <0.0001
Psychological data
Cancer-related emotional distress 7.7 (5.5) 9.6 (4.7)0.02
Patient emotional skills 3.3 (0.6) 3.3 (0.5)0.66
Patient-reported physician empathy 43.7 (6.7) 42.5 (7.1)0.23
NSCL: Non-small cell lung cancer; SCL: Small cell lung cancer; EGFR: Epidermal Growth Factor Receptor; ALK: Anaplastic Lymphoma Kinase; ROS: ROS proto-oncogene 1, receptor tyrosine kinase.
Table A2. Characteristics of patients with low versus high patient-reported physician empathy (median split at 45).
Table A2. Characteristics of patients with low versus high patient-reported physician empathy (median split at 45).
CharacteristicsLow Patient-Reported Physician Empathy (≤45)High Patient-Reported Physician Empathy (>45)p Value
Number of Patients%Mean (Standard Deviation, SD)Number of Patients%Mean (SD)
Patients (n = 179)
Sociodemographic data
Age 64.1 (12.4) 62.9 (10.3)0.50
Men6774.4 5360.2 0.04
Married or in a relationship (Yes)6472.7 7079.6 0.29
Education 0.43
No diploma2426.7 1517.1
High school4448.9 4652.3
Bachelor degree1516.7 1719.3
More than bachelor degree77.8 1011.4
Financial ease 0.09
Not at all or not very comfortable1314.4 1517.1
Moderately comfortable5763.3 4247.7
Rather or very comfortable2022.2 3135.2
Medical data
Type and severity of cancer 0.71
Stage I, II or III (NSCL or SCL)4448.9 4045.5
Stage IV (NSCL or SCL)2628.9 2933.0
Mesothelioma (no stage assigned)1213.3 910.2
Missing data88.9 1011.4
Smoker 0.83
Never1213.3 1213.6
Ever5055.6 5562.5
Missing data2831.1 2123.4
Time since diagnosis and inclusion in the study (years) 2.3 (1.9) 2.5 (2.3)0.57
Karnofsky index 0.18
60–7066.7 22.3
>708392.2 8192.1
Missing data11.1 55.7
Metastasis 2326.7 2731.0 0.53
Treatment before inclusion
Chemotherapy 5763.3 6169.3 0.40
Surgery3943.3 4348.9 0.46
Radiotherapy3640.0 3438.6 0.85
Targeted therapies77.8 78.1 0.95
Treatment after inclusion
Surgery44.4 44.5 0.97
Chemotherapy4448.9 3539.8 0.22
Radiotherapy2325.6 1517.5 0.17
Targeted therapies1516.7 1618.2 0.79
Immunotherapy1921.1 1719.3 0.77
No further treatment 2730.0 3337.5 0.29
Charlson index of comorbidities 8.2 (2.5) 7.7 (2.6)0.29
Genetic mutation: EGFR or ALK or ROS (%)1011.1 1011.4 0.12
Deceased at censoring4550.6 4251.9 0.87
Psychological data
Cancer-related emotional distress 9.7 (5.1) 7.5 (5.1)0.006
Patient emotional skills 3.2 (0.4) 3.5 (0.6)0.005
Patient-reported physician empathy 37.1 (5.0) 48.6 (1.7)0.001
NSCL: Non-small cell lung cancer; SCL: Small cell lung cancer; EGFR: Epidermal Growth Factor Receptor; ALK: Anaplastic Lymphoma Kinase; ROS: ROS proto-oncogene 1, receptor tyrosine kinase.

References

  1. Dempke, W.C.M.; Suto, T.; Reck, M. Targeted therapies for non-small cell lung cancer. Lung Cancer 2010, 67, 257–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Chida, Y.; Hamer, M.; Wardle, J.; Steptoe, A. Do stress-related psychosocial factors contribute to cancer incidence and survival? Nat. Clin. Pract. Oncol. 2008, 5, 466–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hojat, M. Empathy in Patient Care: Antecedents, Development, Measurement, and Outcomes; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hojat, M.; Louis, D.Z.; Markham, F.W.; Wender, R.; Rabinowitz, C.; Gonnella, J.S. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Acad. Med. 2011, 86, 359–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Lelorain, S.; Brédart, A.; Dolbeault, S.; Sultan, S. A systematic review of the associations between empathy measures and patient outcomes in cancer care. Psychooncology 2012, 21, 1255–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Di Blasi, Z.; Harkness, E.; Ernst, E.; Georgiou, A.; Kleijnen, J. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: A systematic review. Lancet 2001, 357, 757–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lutgendorf, S.K.; De Geest, K.; Bender, D.; Ahmed, A.; Goodheart, M.J.; Dahmoush, L.; Zimmerman, M.B.; Penedo, F.J.; Lucci, J.A.; Ganjei-Azar, P.; et al. Social influences on clinical outcomes of patients with ovarian cancer. J. Clin. Oncol. 2012, 30, 2885–2890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Holt-Lunstad, J.; Smith, T.B.; Layton, J.B. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 2010, 7, e1000316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Frenkel, M.; Engebretson, J.C.; Gross, S.; Peterson, N.E.; Giveon, A.P.; Sapire, K.; Hermoni, D. Exceptional patients and communication in cancer care—Are we missing another survival factor? Support. Care Cancer 2016, 24, 4249–4255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Cao, W.; Qi, X.; Yao, T.; Han, X.; Feng, X. How doctors communicate the initial diagnosis of cancer matters: Cancer disclosure and its relationship with patients’ hope and trust. Psychooncology 2017, 26, 640–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Price, M.; Butow, P.; Bell, M.; deFazio, A.; Friedlander, M.; Fardell, J.; Protani, M.; Webb, P.; Price, M.A.; Butow, P.N.; et al. Helplessness/hopelessness, minimization and optimism predict survival in women with invasive ovarian cancer: A role for targeted support during initial treatment decision-making? Support. Care Cancer 2016, 24, 2627–2634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Watson, M.; Homewood, J.; Haviland, J.; Bliss, J.M. Influence of psychological response on breast cancer survival: 10-year follow-up of a population-based cohort. Eur. J. Cancer 2005, 41, 1710–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Hubble, K.; Daughters, K.; Manstead, A.S.R.; Rees, A.; Thapar, A.; Van Goozen, S.H.M. Oxytocin increases attention to the eyes and selectively enhances self-reported affective empathy for fear. Neuropsychologia 2017, 106, 350–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ji, H.; Liu, N.; Yin, Y.; Wang, X.; Chen, X.; Li, J.; Li, J. Oxytocin inhibits ovarian cancer metastasis by repressing the expression of MMP-2 and VEGF. J. Cancer 2018, 9, 1379–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Yagil, D.; Shnapper-Cohen, M. When authenticity matters most: Physicians’ regulation of emotional display and patient satisfaction. Patient Educ. Couns. 2016, 99, 1694–1698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Takayama, T.; Yamazaki, Y.; Katsumata, N. Relationship between outpatients’ perceptions of physicians’ communication styles and patients’ anxiety levels in a Japanese oncology setting. Soc. Sci. Med. 2001, 53, 1335–1350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sulzer, S.H.; Feinstein, N.W.; Wendland, C.L. Assessing empathy development in medical education: A systematic review. Med. Educ. 2016, 50, 300–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Fung, C.S.C.; Mercer, S.W. A qualitative study of patients’ views on quality of primary care consultations in Hong Kong and comparison with the UK CARE Measure. BMC Fam. Pract. 2009, 10, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Østhus, A.A.; Aarstad, A.K.H.; Olofsson, J.; Aarstad, H.J. Prediction of 5 year survival from level of perceived distress in newly diagnosed head and neck squamous cell carcinoma patients. Oral Oncol. 2013, 49, 964–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hoyt, M.A.; Stanton, A.L.; Bower, J.E.; Thomas, K.S.; Litwin, M.S.; Breen, E.C.; Irwin, M.R. Inflammatory biomarkers and emotional approach coping in men with prostate cancer. Brain Behav. Immun. 2013, 32, 173–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Moreno, P.I.; Moskowitz, A.L.; Ganz, P.A.; Bower, J.E. Positive affect and inflammatory activity in breast cancer survivors: Examining the role of affective arousal. Psychosom. Med. 2016, 78, 532–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Reed, R.G.; Weihs, K.L.; Sbarra, D.A.; Breen, E.C.; Irwin, M.R.; Butler, E.A. Emotional acceptance, inflammation, and sickness symptoms across the first two years following breast cancer diagnosis. Brain Behav. Immun. 2016, 56, 165–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Schofield, P.E.; Stockler, M.R.; Zannino, D.; Tebbutt, N.C.; Price, T.J.; Simes, R.J.; Wong, N.; Pavlakis, N.; Ransom, D.; Moylan, E.; et al. Hope, optimism and survival in a randomised trial of chemotherapy for metastatic colorectal cancer. Support. Care Cancer 2016, 24, 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. West, H.J.; Jin, J.O. Performance status in patients with cancer. JAMA Oncol. 2015, 1, 998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Lelorain, S.; Cattan, S.; Lordick, F.; Mehnert, A.; Mariette, C.; Christophe, V.; Cortot, A. In which context is physician empathy associated with cancer patient quality of life? Patient Educ. Couns. 2018, 101, 1216–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Wirtz, M.; Boecker, M.; Forkmann, T.; Neumann, M. Evaluation of the “Consultation and Relational Empathy” (CARE) measure by means of Rasch-analysis at the example of cancer patients. Patient Educ. Couns. 2011, 82, 298–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Mercer, S.W.; Murphy, D.J. Validity and reliability of the CARE Measure in secondary care. Clin. Gov. Int. J. 2008, 13, 269–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Costet, N.; Lapierre, V.; Benhamou, E.; Le Galès, C. Reliability and validity of the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy General (FACT-G) in French cancer patients. Qual. Life Res. 2005, 14, 1427–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Brasseur, S.; Grégoire, J.; Bourdu, R.; Mikolajczak, M. The Profile of Emotional Competence (PEC): Development and validation of a self-reported measure that fits dimensions of emotional competence theory. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e62635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Concato, J.; Peduzzi, P.; Holford, T.R.; Feinstein, A.R. Importance of events per independent variable in proportional hazards analysis. I. Background, goals, and general strategy. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1995, 48, 1495–1501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Peduzzi, P.; Concato, J.; Feinstein, A.R.; Holford, T.R. Importance of events per independent variable in proportional hazards regression analysis. II. Accuracy and precision of regression estimates. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1995, 48, 1503–1510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Vittinghoff, E.; McCulloch, C.E. Relaxing the rule of ten events per variable in logistic and Cox regression. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 165, 710–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Ogundimu, E.O.; Altman, D.G.; Collins, G.S. Adequate sample size for developing prediction models is not simply related to events per variable. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2016, 76, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Howick, J.; Steinkopf, L.; Ulyte, A.; Roberts, N.; Meissner, K. How empathic is your healthcare practitioner? A systematic review and meta-analysis of patient surveys. BMC Med. Educ. 2017, 17, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Derksen, F.; Olde Hartman, T.C.; van Dijk, A.; Plouvier, A.; Bensing, J.; Lagro-Janssen, A. Consequences of the presence and absence of empathy during consultations in primary care: A focus group study with patients. Patient Educ. Couns. 2017, 100, 987–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Butow, P.N.; Coates, A.S.; Dunn, S.M. Psychosocial predictors of survival in metastatic melanoma. J. Clin. Oncol. 1999, 17, 2256–2263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Butow, P.N.; Coates, A.S.; Dunn, S.M. Psychosocial predictors of survival: Metastatic breast cancer. Ann. Oncol. 2000, 11, 469–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Greer, J.A.; Pirl, W.F.; Jackson, V.A.; Muzikansky, A.; Lennes, I.T.; Gallagher, E.R.; Prigerson, H.G.; Temel, J.S. Perceptions of health status and survival in patients with metastatic lung cancer. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 2014, 48, 548–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Kim, S.-Y.; Kim, J.-M.; Kim, S.-W.; Shin, I.-S.; Bae, K.-Y.; Shim, H.-J.; Hwang, J.-E.; Bae, W.-K.; Cho, S.-H.; Chung, I.-J.; et al. Does awareness of terminal status influence survival and quality of life in terminally ill cancer patients? Psychooncology 2013, 22, 2206–2213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Gilligan, T.; Coyle, N.; Frankel, R.M.; Berry, D.L.; Bohlke, K.; Epstein, R.M.; Finlay, E.; Jackson, V.A.; Lathan, C.S.; Loprinzi, C.L.; et al. Patient-clinician communication: American Society of Clinical Oncology consensus guideline. J. Clin. Oncol. 2017, 35, 3618–3632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Wessels, H.; de Graeff, A.; Wynia, K.; de Heus, M.; Kruitwagen, C.L.J.J.; Woltjer, G.T.G.J.; Teunissen, S.C.C.M.; Voest, E.E. Gender-related needs and preferences in cancer care indicate the need for an individualized approach to cancer patients. Oncologist 2010, 15, 648–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Seifart, C.; Hofmann, M.; Bär, T.; Riera Knorrenschild, J.; Seifart, U.; Rief, W. Breaking bad news—What patients want and what they get: Evaluating the SPIKES protocol in Germany. Ann. Oncol. 2014, 25, 707–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Chou, A.; Toon, C.W.; Clarkson, A.; Sheen, A.; Sioson, L.; Gill, A.J. The epithelioid BAP1-negative and p16-positive phenotype predicts prolonged survival in pleural mesothelioma. Histopathology 2018, 72, 509–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lin, J.; Carter, C.A.; McGlynn, K.A.; Zahm, S.H.; Nations, J.A.; Anderson, W.F.; Shriver, C.D.; Zhu, K. A prognostic model to predict mortality among non-small cell lung cancer patients in the U.S. Military Health System. J. Thorac. Oncol. 2015, 10, 1694–1702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Alelwani, S.M.; Ahmed, Y.A. Medical training for communication of bad news: A literature review. J. Educ. Health Promot. 2014, 3, 51. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  46. Paul, C.L.; Clinton-McHarg, T.; Sanson-Fisher, R.W.; Douglas, H.; Webb, G. Are we there yet? The state of the evidence base for guidelines on breaking bad news to cancer patients. Eur. J. Cancer 2009, 45, 2960–2966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Dean, A.; Willis, S. The use of protocol in breaking bad news: Evidence and ethos. Int. J. Palliat. Nurs. 2016, 22, 265–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Survival probability for patients with high perceived empathy (>45) versus low empathy (≤ 45) by median split. (a) Follow-up consultations: adjusted Cox regressions showed no differences by empathy (p = 0.24); (b) Bad-news consultations: adjusted Cox regressions showed differences by empathy (p = 0.024).
Figure 1. Survival probability for patients with high perceived empathy (>45) versus low empathy (≤ 45) by median split. (a) Follow-up consultations: adjusted Cox regressions showed no differences by empathy (p = 0.24); (b) Bad-news consultations: adjusted Cox regressions showed differences by empathy (p = 0.024).
Jcm 07 00364 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
CharacteristicsNumber of Patients%Mean (Standard Deviation) (Range)
Patients (n = 179)
Sociodemographic data
Age 63.4 (11.3) (24–85)
Men12167.6
In a relationship or married (Yes)13576.2
Patient-reported education
No diploma3921.8
High school9050.3
Bachelor degree3318.4
More than bachelor degree179.5
Patient-reported financial situation
Not at all or not very comfortable2815.6
Moderately comfortable10055.9
Rather or very comfortable5128.5
Medical data
Type of cancer
Non-small cell lung cancer (NSCL)13575.4
Small cell lung cancer (SCL)105.6
Mesothelioma2111.7
Missing data137.3
Cancer stage
Stage I, II or III (NSCL or SCL)8446.9
Stage IV (NSCL or SCL)5631.3
Mesothelioma (no stage assigned)2111.7
Missing data1810.1
Smoker
Never2413.4
Ever10659.2
Missing data4927.4
Time since diagnosis and inclusion in the study (years) 2.5 (2.2) (0.1–14.4)
Karnofsky index
60–7084.5
802011.2
905631.2
1008919.7
Missing data63.3
Metastasis5128.5
Treatment before inclusion
Surgery8245.9
Chemotherapy11966.5
Radiotherapy7139.7
Targeted therapies147.8
Treatment after inclusion
Surgery84.5
Chemotherapy8044.7
Radiotherapy3821.2
Targeted therapies3117.3
Immunotherapy3620.1
No further treatment6033.5
Charlson index of comorbidities 8.0 (2.6) (0–16)
Genetic mutation: EGFR or ALK or ROS (%)2011.2
Type of consultation
Routine follow-up10055.9
Bad news7943.0
Change of treatment because of treatment failure %6938.6
Relapse or end of active treatment84.5
Missing data21.1
Deceased at censorship
Yes8849.2
No8346.4
Missing data84.4
Psychological data
Cancer-related emotional distress 8.5 (5.2) (0–24)
Patient emotional skills 3.4 (0.5) (1.9–4.8)
Patient-reported physician empathy 43.2 (6.8) (22–50)
Physicians (n = 5)
Age 35.8 (5.85) (33–48)
Men 50
NSCL: Non-small cell lung cancer; SCL: Small cell lung cancer; EGFR: Epidermal Growth Factor Receptor; ALK: Anaplastic Lymphoma Kinase; ROS: Proto-oncogene tyrosine-protein kinase. ROS is an enzyme that in humans is encoded by the ROS1 gene.
Table 2. Unadjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Ratios for overall survival.
Table 2. Unadjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Ratios for overall survival.
VariableHR95% CIp Value
Sociodemographic data
Age1.021.01–1.040.015
Woman0.510.30–0.840.009
In a relationship or married (Yes)0.880.52–1.470.62
Patient-reported Education
Overall 0.49
No diploma1.970.79–4.920.15
High school1.470.63–3.460.37
Bachelor degree1.580.62–4.000.34
More than bachelor degree (reference category)
Patient-reported financial situation %
Overall 0.25
Not at all or not very comfortable0.550.27–1.120.10
Moderately comfortable0.840.53–1.320.44
Rather or very comfortable (reference category)
Medical data
Type and severity of cancer <0.0001
Stage I, II or III (NSCL or SCL) (reference category)
Stage IV (NSCL or SCL)2.481.51–4.050.0003
Mesothelioma (no stage assigned)3.361.81–6.230.0001
Smoker (ever)0.890.45–1.780.75
Time since diagnosis and inclusion in the study (years)0.960.87–1.070.46
Karnofsky index (60–70 compared to higher)2.110.92–4.850.08
Metastasis %1.631.04–2.560.03
Treatment before inclusion %
Chemotherapy 1.781.06–2.900.02
Surgery0.340.21–0.54<0.0001
Radiotherapy0.950.62–1.450.80
Targeted therapies1.830.92–3.640.09
Treatment after inclusion %
Surgery0.170.02–1.250.08
Chemotherapy2.181.41–3.370.0005
Radiotherapy0.650.38–1.130.13
Targeted therapies1.560.95–2.540.08
Immunotherapy1.590.99–2.550.06
No further treatment 0.480.29–0.810.006
Charlson index of comorbidities 1.191.09–1.31<0.0001
Genetic mutation: EGFR or ALK or ROS (%)1.220.67–2.250.52
Psychological data
Cancer-related emotional distress 1.051.01–1.090.01
Patient emotional skills 0.870.59–1.290.48
Patient-reported physician empathy0.990.97–1.030.87
NSCL: Non-small cell lung cancer; SCL: Small cell lung cancer; HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval. EGFR: Epidermal Growth Factor Receptor; ALK: Anaplastic Lymphoma Kinase; ROS: ROS proto-oncogene 1, receptor tyrosine kinase.
Table 3. Adjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Model for overall survival.
Table 3. Adjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Model for overall survival.
VariableHazard Ratio95% CIp Value
Age1.041.00–1.070.029
Woman0.480.26–0.890.019
Patient-reported education
No diploma (reference)
High school0.420.22–0.810.009
Bachelor degree0.410.16–1.060.07
>Bachelor0.500.14–1.700.26
Patient-reported financial situation
Not at all or not very comfortable (reference)
Moderately comfortable1.420.58–3.450.44
Rather or very comfortable1.270.46–3.570.65
Type and severity of cancer
Stage I, II or III (NSCL or SCL) (reference category)
Stage IV (NSCL or SCL)3.141.22–8.090.018
Mesothelioma (no stage assigned)3.301.22–8.910.018
Karnofsky index 60–70 (compared to >70)2.530.84–7.590.10
Time since diagnosis and inclusion in the study0.980.84–1.140.75
Charlson index of comorbidities 1.020.88–1.170.80
Mutation (ALK, EGFR, ROS vs. none of them)1.400.64–3.060.40
Metastases0.920.38–2.250.85
Cancer-related emotional distress1.061.01–1.120.03
Emotional skills1.570.87–2.850.14
Patient-reported physician empathy---
Type of consultation (bad news vs. follow-up)---
Empathy * type-of-consultation (interaction) 0.022
Empathy in bad news consultations1.061.01–1.120.024
Empathy in follow-up consultations0.960.90–1.030.24
NSCL: Non-small cell lung cancer; SCL: Small cell lung cancer; n = 143 (due to missing data in some variables), -2LL = 587.8, Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) = 625.8, Schwarz Bayesian Criterion (SBC) = 668.8. EGFR: Epidermal Growth Factor Receptor; ALK: Anaplastic Lymphoma Kinase; ROS: ROS proto-oncogene 1, receptor tyrosine kinase.
Table 4. Adjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Models for overall survival with specific types of empathy.
Table 4. Adjusted Cox Proportional Hazard Models for overall survival with specific types of empathy.
VariableHazard Ratio95% CIp Value
Model A
Compassion/listening empathy * type-of-consultation (interaction)--0.016
Compassion/listening in bad news consultations1.131.03–1.230.008
Compassion/listening in follow-up consultations0.940.85–1.050.300
Model B
Active/positive empathy * type-of-consultation (interaction)--0.07
Active/positive in bad news consultations1.100.96–1.260.16
Active/positive in follow-up consultations0.920.79–1.060.25
Covariates are not shown for readability purposes. Model A: -2LL = 575.6, Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) = 613.6, Schwarz Bayesian Criterion (SBC) = 556.3, n = 139; Model B: -2LL = 569.1, AIC = 607.1, SBC = 649.5, n = 138. CI: confidence interval.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lelorain, S.; Cortot, A.; Christophe, V.; Pinçon, C.; Gidron, Y. Physician Empathy Interacts with Breaking Bad News in Predicting Lung Cancer and Pleural Mesothelioma Patient Survival: Timing May Be Crucial. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 364. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7100364

AMA Style

Lelorain S, Cortot A, Christophe V, Pinçon C, Gidron Y. Physician Empathy Interacts with Breaking Bad News in Predicting Lung Cancer and Pleural Mesothelioma Patient Survival: Timing May Be Crucial. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2018; 7(10):364. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7100364

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lelorain, Sophie, Alexis Cortot, Véronique Christophe, Claire Pinçon, and Yori Gidron. 2018. "Physician Empathy Interacts with Breaking Bad News in Predicting Lung Cancer and Pleural Mesothelioma Patient Survival: Timing May Be Crucial" Journal of Clinical Medicine 7, no. 10: 364. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7100364

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop