Next Article in Journal
Endoscopic Skull Base Repair Strategy for CSF Leaks Associated with Pneumocephalus
Next Article in Special Issue
Neuropathology of the Brainstem to Mechanistically Understand and to Treat Alzheimer’s Disease
Previous Article in Journal
Urinary NGAL Measured after the First Year Post Kidney Transplantation Predicts Changes in Glomerular Filtration over One-Year Follow-Up
Previous Article in Special Issue
Accurate Blood-Based Diagnostic Biosignatures for Alzheimer’s Disease via Automated Machine Learning
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Alexithymia in Alzheimer’s Disease

by
Eva Mª Arroyo-Anlló
1,*,
Corinne Souchaud
2,
Pierre Ingrand
3,
Jorge Chamorro Sánchez
4,
Alejandra Melero Ventola
4 and
Roger Gil
5
1
Department of Psychobiology, Neuroscience Institute of Castilla-León, University of Salamanca, 37007 Salamanca, Spain
2
Department of Neurology and Neuropsychology, University Hospital, CHU La Milétrie, 86000 Poitiers, France
3
Department of Biostatistics, University of Poitiers, 86000 Poitiers, France
4
Faculty of Psychology, Pontifical University of Salamanca, 37002 Salamanca, Spain
5
Department of Neurology, University Hospital, 86000 Poitiers, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10(1), 44; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10010044
Submission received: 28 November 2020 / Revised: 18 December 2020 / Accepted: 22 December 2020 / Published: 25 December 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Addressing Alzheimer’s Disease: Blueprints and Therapeutic Hints)

Abstract

:
Alexithymia is widely recognized as the inability to identify and express emotions. It is a construct which consists of four cognitive traits such as difficulty in identifying feelings, describing feelings to others, externally oriented thinking, and limited imaginative capacity. Several studies have linked alexithymia to cognitive functioning, observing greater alexithymia scores associated with poorer cognitive abilities. Despite Alzheimer’s disease (AD) being a neurodegenerative pathology characterized by cognitive troubles from the early stages, associated to behavioral and emotional disturbances, very few investigations have studied the alexithymia in AD. These studies have shown that alexithymia scores—assessed with Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS)—were greater in AD patients than healthy participants. The objective of the study was to investigate if the alexithymia was present in patients with mild AD. We hypothesized that the AD group would show more alexithymia features than the control group. We evaluated 54 subjects, including 27 patients diagnosed with mild AD and 27 normal healthy controls, using the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) and a neuropsychological test battery. Using non-parametric statistical analyses—Wilcoxon and Mann–Whitney U tests—we observed that the SSPS-R scores were similar in the AD and control groups. All participants showed SSPS-R scores below to 10 points, which means no-alexithymia. We did not find significant correlations between SSPS-R scores and cognitive variables in both groups (p > 0.22), but we observed a negative association between name abilities and alexithymia, but it does not reach to significance (p = 0.07). However, a significant correlation between SSPS-R score and mood state, assessed using Zerssen Rating Scale, was found in both groups (p = 0.01). Because we did not find a significant difference in the alexithymia assessment between both subject groups, pot hoc analyses were computed for each item of the SSPS-R. We made comparisons of alexithymic responses percentages in each SSPS-R item between AD and control groups, using Fisher’s test. We observed that AD patients produced more alexithymic responses in some items of SSPS-R test than the control group, particularly about difficulties to find the words to describe feelings, as well as difficulties of imagination capacity and externally oriented thinking. The present results do not confirm our hypothesis and they do not support the results of previous studies revealing great alexithymia in AD.

1. Introduction

Alexithymia means “no words for feeling” from the Greek. At the end of the 1940s, the concept was referenced by Ruesch [1] and in 1972, Peter Sifneos [2] introduced this term to describe the inability to find appropriate words to describe one’s feelings in patients with psychosomatic disorders. In 1977, Freyberger [3] suggested alexithymia as a personality trait and a state-like secondary phenomenon after somatic disease or psychological stress [3,4,5,6,7].
In a strict sense, alexithymia is widely recognized as the inability to identify and express emotions, as well as difficulty in communicating them [2,8,9]. Currently, it is described as a difficulty in emotional processing rather than a defensive process, but the comprehensive models of this construct still remain as having no consensus ([10], for review). It is well recognized as a dimensional cognitive-affective style that influences emotional regulation with impacts on health. Alexithymia is a construct which is comprised of four components such as: difficulty identifying feelings, describing feelings to others, externally oriented thinking, and limited imaginative capacity or restricted fantasizing.
Actually, there are three main scales to assess alexithymia: the Beth Israel Questionnaire (BIQ) [2], the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS) [11], and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) [12], but other measures have been used such as M.M.P.I. Alexithymia Scale [13], projective tests... In 1973, Sifneos introduced the first questionnaire, Beth Israel Questionnaire (BIQ), for screening of alexithymic characteristics [2], The BIQ is a 17-items forced-choice questionnaire completed by the interviewer. Later, Sifneos [11] created the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS) which is formed by 20 questions with dichotomous responses (yes/no). It lets us analyse 3 factors: the difficulty of expressing feelings, the importance of feelings, as well as the daydreaming and introspection. Alexithymia can also be assessed using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) [12], which is a 26-items self-report measure rated on a five-point Likert scale. It comprises three subscales assessing three facets of alexithymia: difficulty in identifying feelings, difficulty in describing feelings and an externally oriented analytic mode of thinking.
Although initially described in the context of psychosomatic illness, alexithymic characteristics may be observed in patients with a wide range of psychiatric disorders. However, alexithymia also seems to be a common feature of neurological disease, with most evidence available for patients with traumatic brain injury, stroke, and epilepsy, associated frequently with mood disorders such as depression and anxiety (see review [14,15]).
Concerning neurodegenerative diseases, a small number of studies have investigated the prevalence of alexithymia in patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) [16,17,18,19]. They observed a prevalence of 13–30% in the patient groups, depending on the study. However, mostly studies reported that mood state deficits (anxiety and/or depression) were associated with alexithymia [16,17,19]. Alexithymia has also been reported frequently in patients suffering from Parkinson’s disease (PD) [20,21,22], but it is still not clear whether it is linked to depression [20,23,24,25,26,27]. In a recent work, Assogna et al. [15] studied the alexithymia in two variants of Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (PSP), Richardson’s syndrome (PSP-RS), and PSP with predominant parkinsonism (PSP-P) compared to PD. Alexithymic symptoms differentiate PSP-RS group from PD group but not between the two subtypes of PSP. They also observed that alexithymia was identifiable very early in PSP patients. Further, alexithymia in PSP seemed to be predicted by the presence of depression.
Furthermore, several studies have linked alexithymia to cognitive functioning ([28,29,30,31], for a review). They have observed greater alexithymia scores associated with poorer cognitive abilities, particularly it being associated with language and executive troubles [32,33,34,35,36]. Other studies have also found that higher alexithymia correlated with poorer attention and memory [37,38].
Despite Alzheimer’s disease (AD) being a neurodegenerative pathology characterized by cognitive troubles from the early stages, associated with behavioral and emotional disturbances [39,40,41,42,43], very few investigations have studied alexithymia in AD. Thus, little is known about the expression and verbalization of emotions from patients with AD. Recently, three research projects have specifically focused on alexithymia in AD, to our knowledge [44,45,46]. All studies have shown that alexithymia scores, assessed with TAS, were elevated in AD patients. Besides, Sturm and Levenson [44] found alexithymia scores were positively correlated with behavioral deficits (apathy and informant distress) in patients with dementia and negatively correlated with the grey matter volume of the right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, a region of the brain that is thought to play an important role in self and emotion processing. The study of Yuruyen and his team [45] assessed alexithymia with TAS in patients and they observed significantly greater alexithymia in AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) groups than that of individuals with memory loss complaints in each TAS factor, but not significant difference between AD and MCI patients. The preliminary results of Smirni et al. [46] reported greater alexithymia total scores in AD and MCI patients than in healthy subjects, but only significant higher scores on the TAS factor difficulty in identifying feelings.
On the basis of this background, the aim of the study was to investigate quantitatively and qualitatively, if the alexithymia was present in AD patients. Here, we hypothesized that patients with AD show more alexithymia features than healthy control population.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

We studied 54 subjects, including 27 patients diagnosed with AD and 27 normal healthy controls. Twenty-seven individuals with a clinical diagnosis of probable AD were recruited at the Department of Neurology and Neuropsychology, University Hospital, CHU La Milétrie at Poitiers, France. All lived at home with a family caregiver. They had a history of progressive decline in intellectual function without focal motor or sensory features. To exclude other possible causes of dementia, appropriate laboratory tests were performed, and these gave normal results. No findings incompatible with a diagnosis of AD were found in the electroencephalogram, electrocardiogram, or chest X-ray in any of the patients. Brain CT scan revealed mild cortical and central atrophy, but no other pathology. All Alzheimer patients met the diagnostic criteria of the “NINCDS—ADRDA Work Group” and of the DSM-5. All had a score of less than 5 on the Hachinski Ischemic Scale. According to the Clinical Dementia Rating System (CDR) [47] and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [48], all patients had mild AD. All AD subjects were taking anti-dementia medication during the study, in particular anti-acetyl cholinesterase treatments. Only the AD patients whose verbal comprehension (assessed by the MMSE three-stage command) were equal to or above 2 were included in the study.
Controls primarily comprised of volunteers or were members or visitors at the University Hospital in Poitiers (France) whose results in a neurological examination were normal, had a CDR score [47] equal to zero and scored 28/30 or higher on the MMSE [48].

2.2. Neuropsychological Assessment

A comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests was administered to all participants to evaluate the cognitive capacities, mood state and alexithymia.
On one hand, cognitive functions were evaluated using Mini-Mental State Evaluation (MMSE) [48] to assess the general mental state. Episodic memory was explored using the verbal memory test designed by Grober and Buschke [49], consisting of a word list learning and free/cued recall. We also used the DO80 verbal naming test [50], which assesses pictures naming to evaluate language abilities. In addition, we assessed the mood state, using the Zerssen Rating Scale [51].
On the other hand, we completed the neuropsychological assessment using the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale-Revised (SSPS-R) [52] to evaluate the existence of alexithymia features in the participants. We chose SSPS-R because it is adapted to dementia populations, as well as it is completed and helped by the interviewer. Besides, it consists in 20 specific sentences about daily behaviours and feelings of the subjects. The subjects with a SSPS-R score between 10 and 20 points were considered as alexithymic.

2.3. Procedure

All participants were assessed individually in only one session with the same order of testing: first, it was for the evaluation of cognition and after, exclusively for testing mood state and alexithymia. The session took between 60–90 min to complete.

2.4. Ethics

Written, informed consent was obtained from all the included patients. The study was approved by The Regional Committee for Research Ethics, CHU Poitiers.

2.5. Data Analysis

Data was analysed using the Statistical Package SAS—software (version 9.2, SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, USA). In this study an α level of 0.05 was selected for statistical significance. To check whether there were significant differences between the two groups as a function of age and education years, we used Wilcoxon two sample test and in the case of gender, we used X2 test of Fisher.
To compare the mean scores of cognitive and alexithymia abilities between participant groups, non-parametric Mann–Whitney U and Wilcoxon tests were used. The fisher test was also used to compare the alexithymic responses proportion in each SSPS-R item between both groups. Within-group (AD and healthy control groups) correlations were also planned for SSPS-R total scores and clinical/cognitive variables, using the Spearman coefficient.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic and Neuropsychological Characteristics

Fifty-four subjects participated in the study: an experimental group with twenty and seven AD patients, and a control group with twenty and seven healthy participants. Concerning the experimental AD group (19 women and 8 men), it had a mean age (±SD) of 79.17 years old (±7.43), a mean education years (±SD) of 8.94 (±1.64), and a MMSE score (±SD) of 20.28 (±4.61). The AD group showed a mild disease, assessed with MMSE and CDR (1.4 ± 0.23) and the mean (±SD) duration of the disease was 4.1 (±1.22) years. Concerning the control group (13 women and 14 men), the mean age (±SD) was 76.59 years old (±9.43). It had mean education years (±SD) of 9.26 (±1.11), and a MMSE score (±SD) of 29.18 (±1.13). There were no significant differences in gender (p = 0.062), age (p = 0.98), and education years (p = 0.84). The means and standard deviations of the different demographic characteristics of the AD and the control groups are shown in Table 1.
In relation to the battery of neuropsychological tests administered to both groups, the means and standard deviations of the neuropsychological tests are shown in Table 2. We found significant differences between two participant groups for the general mental state evaluated by MMSE test [48] (p = 0.0001) and for language capacity assessed by the DO80 verbal naming test [50] (p = 0.032). Concerning the memory capacities evaluated by Grober and Buschke verbal memory [49], analysis also revealed significant differences between both groups (p < 0.01).
In regard to mood state assessed by the Zerssen Rating Scale [51], there were no significant differences between the groups; AD and control groups did not have depressive disorders (mean ± SD: 11.63 ± 7.39 and 12.18 ± 6.12, respectively; p = 0.066).

3.2. Alexithymia Assessment

The alexithymia was assessed using the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) [52] and the results are reported in Table 2. All participants showed SSPS-R scores below to 10 points. The SSPS-R scores had similar means (±SD) in the AD and control groups (5.44 ± 2.75 and 5.35 ± 2.76, respectively; p = 0.93). Besides, analyses were computed to examine correlations between SSPS-R scores and cognitive and mood variables within each participant group. We did not find significant correlations between SSPS-R scores and cognitive variables in both groups (p > 0.22), but we observed a negative association between name abilities and alexithymia, but it does not reach significance (p = 0.07). Besides, a significant correlation between SSPS-R score and mood state, assessed using Zerssen Rating Scale [51], was found in both groups (p = 0.01). Table 3 shows the results of these correlations.
Because we did not find a significant difference in the alexithymia assessment between both subject groups, pot hoc analyses were computed for each item of the SSPS-R (Table 4 and Figure 1). We made comparisons of alexithymic responses percentages in each SSPS-R item between AD and control groups, using Fisher’s test. We observed that the most frequent alexithymic responses made by participants of AD group decreasingly were in the following item numbers of SSPS-R: 6 (“I spend much time daydreaming “), 1 (“It is easy to describe symptoms or complaints rather than feelings”), 5 (“I lack imagination”), 19 (“I dream rarely “) and 3 (“It is hard to use words to describe feelings”) and in the items numbers 6, 10 (“When in trouble I don’t like to act”), 1, 7 (“When I am mad I don’t think, I take action”), and 11 (“I like to be precise and to describe everything in detail “) by participants of the control group. The least frequent alexithymic responses made by individuals of AD group increasingly were in the following item numbers of SSPS-R: 2 (“It is important to find out how one feels about people”) and 4 (“I think that feelings are what make life worthwhile”) and in the 2, 4, 12 (“I don’t care to describe details but rather I prefer to examine how I feel”), 18 (“I cannot visualize circumstances which upset me”), and 20 (“I like people better than things”) items by individuals of control group. We found a significant difference of alexithymia responses in the following item numbers of SSPS-R: 3, 5, 7, 10, 11, 14, 15, 18, and 19 between both groups (p < 0.024).

4. Discussion

The general aim of this study was to determine whether an AD group could show alexithymia, using the SSPS-R test [52]. We hypothesized that patients with mild AD would show more alexithymia features than healthy control population, but it was not confirmed. All participants showed no alexithymia scores, considering all SSPS-R total scores were below 10 points. We suggest that the earlier stages of AD could not be associated with alexithymia. All participants showed very interested in emotions and feelings (items 2 and 4), but the most of AD and healthy participants answered that they had difficulties describing feelings (item 1) and they spent a short time daydreaming (item 6), which are the two most important characteristics of alexithymia in any comprehensive model of alexithymia [10]. A possible explanation about why the AD group did not show alexithymia could be that AD is associated with reduced insight into personal impairments from the early stage of disease [53,54,55,56], suggesting that the mental skills required to access personal emotional knowledge were in part supported by cognitive functions that process non-emotional material. However, the AD patients in our study explicitly communicated their difficulties to “find the words describing their emotions”. Regarding that alexithymia is also considered as a form of reduced insight into personal emotions, a limitation of our study could be not to have included an assessment of awareness, taking into account its great prevalence in people with AD in early phases of neuropathology [57,58,59]. Thus, it could have limited the potential inferences between reduction of self-awareness, cognitive decline, and alexithymia in AD. It would also be interesting to consider the self-consciousness factors (anosognosia, introspection, consciousness of body representation, of mood state...) in future research about alexithymia and neurodegenerative diseases, in order to improve the comprehension about alexithymia.
In addition, the literature about cognition and alexithymia revealed that alexithymia is linked to cognitive troubles [28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,46]. However, we did not find any association between SSPS-R scores and cognitive capacities, such as those of memory or language, as well as general mental state. Nevertheless, alexithymia could be dependent on lexical stock assessment with the DO80 test, though the correlation did not reach significant effect. However, Sturn and Levenson [44] observed that alexithymia found in patients with dementia could not be accounted for by a general decline in language functioning. It would be interesting to have a bigger size of participant group, in order to verify if the language troubles could have an impact on alexithymic features in AD. Moreover, it has also been suggested that an interhemispheric functional disconnection is relevant to alexithymia, so that emotional information from the right hemisphere is not properly transferred to the language regions in the left hemisphere [14,57,60] which are damaged in early stages of AD. In addition, several studies found the non-verbal language aspects which are more dependent of right hemisphere functioning [61], were relatively preserved in mild AD [62,63].
However, when we compared each item of SSPS-R between AD and control groups, we found some significant different proportions of participants—among the subject groups—who expressed alexithymic responses in each item. Considering the alexithymic features in terms of Sifneos [2,11] more AD patients significantly expressed difficulties of describing feelings (item 3), and limited imaginative processes (items 5, 18, and 19) than the control group. We also found a greater proportion inside the control group which showed preference to resolve the troubles by action (items 7 and 10) than among the AD group. Furthermore, we observed preference to be oriented toward the outside in an important percentage in both groups (items 11 and 15), but more among the control group. Around half of the AD group avoid interacting with people (item 14) in a higher proportion than that of the control group.
The alexithymic cognitive style of Sifneos [2,11] is characterized by the following alexithymic features: (1) difficulty in identifying and describing feelings, (2) difficulty in distinguishing between feelings and bodily sensations related to emotional activation, (3) restrained and limited imaginative processes, adopting the guise of an impoverished fantasy, (4) a cognitive style oriented toward the outside, (5) poor interpersonal relationships, and (6) troubles resolution by action [64]. However, in terms of Sifneos [2,11] and Freyberger [3], we cannot suggest that AD group showed an alexithymic cognitive style, but we observed some greater alexithymia features in AD patients than in control participants, particularly concerning the difficulties in describing feelings and imagination abilities, which could be secondary to cognitive decline.
Moreover, we observed that the AD group produced responses which expressed a more passive attitude and internally oriented thinking than in the control group, characterized by the preference not to be and talk to people, not to act in case of troubles (items 7, 10, 11, 14). That could suggest some apathy or depressive signs in AD patients, despite the Zerssen Rating Scale not finding evidence of depression syndrome. However, we observed a significant link between Zerssen Rating Scale and SSPS-R total scores in both groups. Moreover, both scales share some items such as the preference to be alone, feelings of boring and uninteresting life. This suggests a possible association between alexithymia and affective disorders such as depression, but we cannot suggest that alexithymia can be completely subsumed within affective disorders. Alexithymia and depression are closely related constructs supported by inadequate emotional regulation strategies [65,66,67,68,69,70,71]. In addition, there is a wide literature about the presence of alexithymic traits in the individuals with suicidal behaviour [10,64] and apathy [14,15,44]. Nevertheless, the question still remains if alexithymia predisposes people to depression or at reverse or they co-exist, though most studies consider that alexithymia is not a product of depression [65,68,72,73]. In addition, sleep disorders such as sleep apnea, insomnia, or those in depression can affect cognitive performances, as well as they can be considered as a risk factor for dementia [74,75,76]. In our study, we did not observe depression in either subject group, but the control group remembered to dream more frequently than the AD group (the SSPS-R item 19). Unfortunately, we did not include an assessment of sleep quality or a history of sleep disorders for all participants and it can be considered as a limitation of the present study. It would also be interesting to observe the relationships between alexithymia and sleep disorders in future research on neurodegenerative diseases. Further studies will be necessary to put clearer the close constructs, considering the implications for treatment [77]. The alexithymic features in the patients with AD or other neurodegenerative disease should encourage more comprehensive therapeutic approaches, both pharmacological and no-pharmacological, that could prevent the development of more severe diseases and thus, improve the life quality of patients and their caregivers.
The present study does not confirm the previous results of other studies revealing great alexithymia assessed with TAS in AD patients [44,45,46], despite us being aware of SSPS-R test, used to assess alexithymia in our study, cannot completely be compared with other alexithymia scales. Yuruyen et al. [45] observed more alexithymic features assessed with TAS in mild AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) groups than in the subjective cognitive decline group, but they did not include a healthy control group. The research of Sturm and Levenson [44] studied only eight AD patients among other neurodegenerative diseases such as semantic or fronto-temporal dementias, observing that 80% showed high alexithymia levels. The research of Smirni et al. [46] also reported greater alexithymia total scores, but only higher scores on the TAS factor about the difficulty in identifying feelings in mild AD and MCI groups than those in the control group. In contrast, the remaining two factors of TAS (difficulty in describing feelings and externally oriented thinking) showed no significant group differences. They also found that TAS total score was negatively correlated with general cognition, attention, memory, and visual spatial constructive and executive abilities. In addition, the TAS factor about identifying feelings was also negatively correlated with general cognition, memory, and executive abilities in AD patients. Participants with AD and MCI showed a significant correlation between identifying feelings factor and long-term memory. They suggested that verbal memory troubles may hinder a patient’s ability to recall an association between a given sensation and the episodic experience of that sensation, thus leading to difficulty identifying feelings. Despite SSPS-R test not really being composed of items assessing the aspect of identifying feelings, our study cannot extend these results, because no correlations between alexithymia and cognition variables were found. In addition, we observed more difficulties in describing feelings in the AD patients than in the control group, as well as a greater externally oriented analytic mode of thinking in control group than in AD group.
In conclusion, the present results do not support the results of previous studies about alexithymia in AD. In our study, the patients with a mild AD show similar scores of alexithymia assessed with SSPS-R, but we observed that AD patients produced more alexithymic responses in some items of the SSPS-R test than the control group, particularly about difficulties to find the words to describe feelings, as well as difficulties of imagination capacity and externally oriented thinking. In addition, we only found an association between alexithymia and the mood state scores in both groups.
Finally, it is clear that clinically there is still much to be learnt about alexithymia and its relationship with related phenomena and symptoms of neurodegenerative diseases. Future studies with a longitudinal prospective design would be beneficial to test the stability of alexithymic responses in different stages of neuropathologies. We also believe that assessment of alexithymia may be a useful addition to the neuropsychological assessment of patients with brain damage because it could help to predict prognosis and response to specific therapies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.G.; Formal analysis, P.I. and R.G.; Investigation, E.M.A.-A. and C.S.; Methodology, E.M.A.-A. and R.G.; Resources, J.C.S. and A.M.V.; Supervision, E.M.A.-A. and R.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by The Regional Committee for Research Ethics, CHU Poitiers, Poitiers, France (18/0013; Nov-2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All data of study are available in Department of Neurology and Unit of Neuropsychology at CHU of Poitiers, France.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ruesch, J. The infantile personality. The core problem of psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics 1948, 10, 134–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Sifneos, P.E. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother. Psychosom. 1973, 22, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Freyberger, H. Supportive psychotherapeutic techniques in primary and secondary alexithymia. Psychother. Psychosom. 1977, 28, 337–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Zeitlin, S.B.; Mcnally, R.J.; Cassiday, K.L. Alexithymia in victims of sexual assault: An effect of repeated traumatization? Am. J. Psychiatry 1993, 150, 661–663. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  5. Zlotnick, C.; Mattia, J.I.; Zimmerman, M. The Relationship between Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Childhood Trauma and Alexithymia in an Outpatient Sample. J. Trauma. Stress 2001, 14, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Porcelli, P.; Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; De Carne, M. Alexithymia and functional gastrointestinal disorders. A comparison with inflammatory bowel disease. Psychother. Psychosom. 1999, 68, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Jula, A.; Salminen, J.K.; Saarijärvi, S. Alexithymia: A facet of essential hypertension. Hypertension 1999, 33, 1057–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Nemiah, J.C.; Sifneos, P.E. Affects and fantasy in patients with psychosomatic disorders. In Modern Trends in Psychosomatic Medicine; Hill, O., Ed.; Butterworths: London, UK, 1970; Volume 2, pp. 26–34. [Google Scholar]
  9. Nemiah, J.C.; Freyberger, H.; Sifneos, P.E. Alexithymia: A view of the psychosomatic process. In Modern Trends in Psychosomatic Medicine; Hill, O.W., Ed.; Butterworths: London, UK, 1976; Volume 3, pp. 430–439. [Google Scholar]
  10. López-Muñoz, F.; Pérez-Fernández, F. A History of the Alexithymia Concept and Its Explanatory Models: An Epistemological Perspective. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Sifneos, P.E. The Schalling-Sifneos Personality Scale revised. Psychother. Psychosom. 1986, 45, 161–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Taylor, G.J.; Ryan, D.; Bagby, R.M. Toward the development of a new self-report alexithymia scale. Psychother. Psychosom. 1985, 44, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kleiger, J.H.; Kinsman, R.A. The development of an MMPI alexithymia scale. Psychother. Psychosom. 1980, 34, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ricciardi, L.; Demartini, B.; Fotopoulou, A.; Edwards, M.J. Alexithymia in Neurological Disease: A Review. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2015, 27, 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Assogna, F.; Pellicano, C.; Cravello, L.; Savini, C.; Macchiusi, L.; Pierantozzi, M.; Stefani, A.; Mercuri, B.; Caltagirone, C.; Pontieri, F.E.; et al. Alexithymia and anhedonia in early Richardson’s syndrome and progressive supranuclear palsy with predominant parkinsonism. Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Bodini, B.; Mandarelli, G.; Tomassini, V.; Tarsitani, L.; Pestalozza, I.; Gasperini, C.; Lenzi, G.L.; Pancheri, P.; Pozzilli, C. Alexithymia in multiple sclerosis: Relationship with fatigue and depression. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2008, 118, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gay, M.C.; Vrignaud, P.; Garitte, C.; Meunier, C. Predictors of depression in multiple sclerosis patients. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2010, 121, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Prochnow, D.; Donell, J.; Schäfer, R.; Jörgens, S.; Hartung, H.P.; Franz, M.; Seitz, R.J. Alexithymia and impaired facial affect recognition in multiple sclerosis. J. Neurol. 2011, 258, 1683–1688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chahraoui, K.; Duchene, C.; Rollot, F.; Bonin, B.; Moreau, T. Longitudinal study of alexithymia and multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav. 2014, 4, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Costa, A.; Peppe, A.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Pasqualetti, P.; Caltagirone, C. Alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease is related to severity of depressive symptoms. Eur. J. Neurol. 2006, 13, 836–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Costa, A.; Peppe, A.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Salamone, G.; Caltagirone, C. Neuropsychological correlates of alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2007, 13, 980–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Costa, A.; Peppe, A.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Salamone, G.; Caltagirone, C. Prevalence and characteristics of alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease. Psychosomatics 2010, 51, 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Poletti, M.; Frosini, D.; Pagni, C.; Lucetti, C.; Del Dotto, P.; Ceravolo, R.; Bonuccelli, U. Alexithymia is associated with depression in de novo Parkinson’s disease. Psychother. Psychosom. 2011, 80, 251–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Poletti, M.; Frosini, D.; Pagni, C.; Lucetti, C.; Del Dotto, P.; Tognoni, G.; Ceravolo, R.; Bonuccelli, U. The association between motor subtypes and alexithymia in de novo Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 2011, 258, 1042–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Assogna, F.; Palmer, K.; Pontieri, F.E.; Pierantozzi, M.; Stefani, A.; Gianni, W.; Caltagirone, C.; Spalletta, G. Alexithymia is a non-motor symptom of Parkinson disease. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2012, 20, 133–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Goerlich-Dobre, K.S.; Probst, C.; Winter, L.; Witt, K.; Deuschl, G.; Möller, B.; Van Eimeren, T. Alexithymia-an independent risk factor for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2014, 29, 214–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Assogna, F.; Cravello, L.; Orfei, M.D.; Cellupica, N.; Caltagirone, C.; Spalletta, G. Alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review of the literature. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2016, 28, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hornak, J.; Rolls, E.T.; Wade, D. Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 1996, 34, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lane, R.D.; Schwartz, G.E. Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am. J. Psychiatry 1987, 144, 133–143. [Google Scholar]
  30. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D.A. Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  31. Larsen, J.K.; Brand, N.; Bermond, B.; Hijman, R. Cognitive and emotional characteristics of alexithymia: A review of neurobiological studies. J. Psychosom. Res. 2003, 54, 533–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Galderisi, S.; Mancuso, F.; Mucci, A.; Garramone, S.; Zamboli, R.; Maj, M. Alexithymia and cognitive dysfunctions in patients with panic disorder. Psychother. Psychosom. 2008, 77, 182–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Henry, J.D.; Phillips, L.H.; Crawford, J.R.; Theodorou, G.; Summers, F. Cognitive and psychosocial correlates of alexithymia following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lamberty, G.J.; Holt, C.S. Evidence for a verbal deficit in alexithymia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 1995, 7, 320–324. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Paradiso, S.; Vaidya, J.G.; McCormick, L.M.; Jones, A.; Robinson, R.G. Aging and alexithymia: Association with reduced right rostral cingulate volume. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008, 16, 760–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Santorelli, G.D.; Ready, R.E. Alexithymia and Executive Function in Younger and Older Adults. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2015, 29, 938–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Bogdanova, Y.; Díaz-Santos, M.; Cronin-Golomb, A. Neurocognitive correlates of alexithymia in asymptomatic individuals with HIV. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 1295–1304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Onor, M.; Trevisiol, M.; Spano, M.; Aguglia, E.; Paradiso, S. Alexithymia and aging: A neuropsychological perspective. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2010, 198, 891–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Bondi, M.W.; Edmonds, E.C.; Salmon, D.P. Alzheimer’s Disease: Past, Present, and Future. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017, 23, 818–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Atri, A. The Alzheimer’s Disease Clinical Spectrum: Diagnosis and Management. Med. Clin. N. Am. 2019, 103, 263–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Bediou, B.; Ryff, I.; Mercier, B.; Milliery, M.; Hénaff, M.A.; D’Amato, T.; Bonnefoy, M.; Vighetto, A.; Krolak-Salmon, P. Impaired social cognition in mild Alzheimer disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2009, 22, 130–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hargrave, R.; Maddock, R.J.; Stone, V. Impaired recognition of facial expressions of emotion in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 64–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Arroyo-Anlló, E.M.; Dauphin, S.; Fargeau, M.N.; Ingrand, P.; Gil, R. Music and emotion in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2019, 11, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sturm, V.E.; Levenson, R.W. Alexithymia in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase 2011, 17, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Yuruyen, M.; Akcan, F.E.; Batun, G.C.; Gultekin, G.; Toprak, M.; Yavuzer, H.; Emul, M. Alexithymia in people with subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2017, 29, 1105–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Smirni, D.; Beadle, J.N.; Paradiso, S. An Initial Study of Alexithymia and Its Relationship with Cognitive Abilities Among Mild Cognitive Impairment, Mild Alzheimer’s Disease, and Healthy Volunteers. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2018, 206, 628–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hughes, C.P.; Berg, L.; Danziger, W.L.; Coben, L.A.; Martin, R.L. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 1982, 140, 566–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Grober, E.; Buschke, H. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1987, 3, 13–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Deloche, G.; Metz-Lutz, M.N.; Kremin, H.; Hannequin, D.; Ferrand, L.; Perrier, D.; Dordain, M.; Quint, S.; Cardebat, D.; Lota, A.M.; et al. Test de Dénomination Orale de 80 Images: DO 80; ECPA: Paris, France, 1989.
  51. Heimann, H.; Bobon-Schrod, H.; Schmocker, A.M.; Bobon, D.P. Auto-évaluation de l’humeur par une liste d’adjectifs, la “Befindlichkeits-Skala” (BS) de Zerssen [Self-rating of mood using a list of adjectives, Zersen’s Befindlichkeits-Skala (BS)]. L’Encephale 1975, 1, 165–183. [Google Scholar]
  52. Smith, M.; Daurat, A.; Pariente, P.; Sifneos, P.E. French translation of Schalling-Sifneos Personality Scale Revised and Beth Israel Questionnaire, 2 evaluation tools of alexithymia. L’Encephale 1992, 18, 171–174. [Google Scholar]
  53. Arroyo-Anlló, E.M.; Bouston, A.T.; Fargeau, M.N.; Orgaz Baz, B.; Gil, R. Self-Consciousness Deficits in Alzheimer’s Disease and Frontotemporal Dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017, 55, 1437–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Silva, M.R.; Moser, D.; Pflüger, M.; Pusswald, G.; Stögmann, E.; Dal-Bianco, P.; Auff, E.; Lehrner, J. Self-reported and informant-reported memory functioning and awareness in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer´s disease. Neuropsychiatrie 2016, 3, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Clare, L.; Marková, I.; Verhey, F.; Kenny, G. Awareness in dementia: A review of assessment methods and measures. Aging Ment. Health 2005, 9, 394–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Gil, R.; Arroyo-Anllo, E.M.; Ingrand, P.; Gil, M.; Neau, J.P.; Ornon, C.; Bonnaud, V. Self-consciousness and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2001, 104, 296–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Lane, R.D.; Ahern, G.L.; Schwartz, G.E.; Kaszniak, A.W. Is alexithymia the emotional equivalent of blindsight? Biol. Psychiatry 1997, 42, 834–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Lane, R.D.; Hsu, C.H.; Locke, D.E.; Ritenbaugh, C.; Stonnington, C.M. Role of theory of mind in emotional awareness and alexithymia: Implications for conceptualization and measurement. Conscious. Cogn. 2015, 33, 398–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Frewen, P.A.; Dozois, D.J.; Neufeld, R.W.; Lanius, R.A. Meta-analysis of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 2008, 21, 243–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Jessimer, M.; Markham, R. Alexithymia: A right hemisphere dysfunction specific to recognition of certain facial expressions? Brain Cogn. 1997, 34, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Tucker, D.M.; Watson, R.T.; Heilman, K.M. Discrimination and evocation of affectively intoned speech in patients with right parietal disease. Neurology 1977, 27, 947–950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Schiaratura, L.T.; Di Pastena, A.; Askevis-Leherpeux, F.; Clément, S. Expression verbale et gestualité dans la maladie d’Alzheimer: Une étude en situation d’interaction sociale [Verbal and gestural communication in interpersonal interaction with Alzheimer’s disease patients]. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2015, 13, 97–105. [Google Scholar]
  63. Schiaratura, L.T. La communication non verbale dans la maladie d’Alzheimer [Non-verbal communication in Alzheimer’s disease]. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2008, 6, 183–188. [Google Scholar]
  64. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D. The alexithymia construct. A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics 1991, 32, 153–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hemming, L.; Haddock, G.; Shaw, J.; Pratt, D. Alexithymia and Its Associations with Depression, Suicidality, and Aggression: An Overview of the Literature. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Luminet, O.; Bagby, R.M.; Taylor, G.J. An evaluation of the absolute and relative stability of alexithymia in patients with major depression. Psychother. Psychosom. 2001, 70, 254–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Hintikka, J.; Honkalampi, K.; Lehtonen, J.; Viinamäki, H. Are alexithymia and depression distinct or overlapping constructs?: A study in a general population. Compr. Psychiatry 2001, 42, 234–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Honkalampi, K.; Hintikka, J.; Saarinen, P.; Lehtonen, J.; Viinamäki, H. Is alexithymia a permanent feature in depressed patients? Results from a 6-month follow-up study. Psychother. Psychosom. 2000, 69, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Parker, J.D.; Bagby, R.M.; Taylor, G.J. Alexithymia and depression: Distinct or overlapping constructs? Compr. Psychiatry 1991, 32, 387–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Marchesi, C.; Brusamonti, E.; Maggini, C. Are alexithymia, depression, and anxiety distinct constructs in affective disorders? J. Psychosom. Res. 2000, 49, 43–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Gilanifar, M.; Delavar, M.A. Alexithymia In Pregnant Women: Its Relationship With Depression. ASEAN J. Psychiatr. 2016, 17, 35–41. [Google Scholar]
  72. De Berardis, D.; Olivieri, L.; Rapini, G.; Di Natale, S.; Serroni, N.; Fornaro, M.; Orsolini, L.; Valchera, A.; Carano, A.; Vellante, F.; et al. Alexithymia, Suicide Ideation and Homocysteine Levels in Drug Naïve Patients with Major Depression: A Study in the “Real World” Clinical Practice. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 2019, 17, 318–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Günther, V.; Rufer, M.; Kersting, A.; Suslow, T. Predicting symptoms in major depression after inpatient treatment: The role of alexithymia. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2016, 70, 392–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Shi, L.; Chen, S.J.; Ma, M.Y.; Bao, Y.P.; Han, Y.; Wang, Y.M.; Shi, J.; Vitiello, M.V.; Lu, L. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2018, 40, 4–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Buratti, L.; Viticchi, G.; Baldinelli, S.; Falsetti, L.; Luzzi, S.; Pulcini, A.; Petrelli, C.; Provinciali, L.; Silvestrini, M. Sleep Apnea, Cognitive Profile, and Vascular Changes: An Intriguing Relationship. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017, 60, 1195–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Buratti, L.; Luzzi, S.; Petrelli, C.; Baldinelli, S.; Viticchi, G.; Provinciali, L.; Altamura, C.; Vernieri, F.; Silvestrini, M. Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome: An Emerging Risk Factor for Dementia. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 2016, 15, 678–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J. What’s in the name ‘alexithymia’? A commentary on “Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy”. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 68, 1006–1020. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Alexithymic responses (%) of every Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) item in the AD and control groups.
Figure 1. Alexithymic responses (%) of every Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) item in the AD and control groups.
Jcm 10 00044 g001
Table 1. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of several demographic characteristics of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and control groups.
Table 1. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of several demographic characteristics of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and control groups.
AD GroupControl Group
M (SD)M (SD)p Value a
Sample size (N)2727
Gender (M:F)8:1913:140.062 NS
Age (years)79.17 (7.43)76.59 (9.43)0.16 NS
Education years8.94 (1.64)9.26 (1.11)0.18 NS
Clinical Dementia Rating System (CDR)1.4 (0.23)
Years of illness 4.1 (1.22)
ap values referred to comparison between control and AD groups. NS: not significant (p > 0.05).
Table 2. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of neuropsychological tests in the AD and control groups.
Table 2. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of neuropsychological tests in the AD and control groups.
AD GroupControl Group
M (SD)M (SD)p Value a
Mini-Mental State Evaluation (MMSE)20.28 (4.61)29.18 (1.13)0.0001 *
Free/cued recall test (Grober and Buschke verbal memory test):
  -short delay free recall4.94 (4.82)32.28 (4.02)0.0001 *
  -short delay free and cued recall16.33 (11.81)43.67 (2.15)0.0015 *
  -total recognition6.47 (1.27)13.48 (0.83)0.0012 *
  -long delay free recall1.33 (1.81)10.01 (1.24)0.0001 *
  -long delay free and cued recall3.67 (3.96)14.92 (0.21)0.0001 *
Verbal naming test (DO80)71.29 (5.95)79.46 (1.20)0.032 *
Zerssen Rating Scale11.63 (7.39)12.18 (6.12)0.066 NS
Alexithymia test: the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R)5.44 (2.75)5.35 (2.76)0.93 NS
ap values referred to control and experimental groups. * Significant difference (p < 0.05). NS: not significant (p > 0.05).
Table 3. Correlations of Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) scores with cognitive and mood variables in the AD and control groups.
Table 3. Correlations of Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) scores with cognitive and mood variables in the AD and control groups.
AD GroupControl Group
Age0.128 (p = 0.61)−0.249 (p = 0.22)
Education Years0.013 (p = 0.55)0.040 (p = 0.74)
Mini-Mental State Evaluation (MMSE)−0.155 (p = 0.54)0.232 (p = 0.37)
Free/Cued Recall Test (Grober and Buschke verbal memory test):
  -short delay free recall0.015 (p = 0.95)0.132 (p = 0.99)
  -short delay free and cued recall0.091 (p = 0.72)0.116 (p = 0.84)
  -total recognition0.098 (p = 0.85)0.093 (p = 0.94)
  -long delay free recall−0.115 (p = 0.65)−0.153 (p = 0.54)
  -long delay free and cued recall0.085 (p = 0.74)0.110 (p = 0.87)
Verbal Naming Test (DO80)−0.645 (p = 0.07)−0.214 (p = 0.33)
Zerssen Rating Scale0.644 (p = 0.01) *0.692 (p = 0.01) *
* Significant difference (p < 0.05).
Table 4. Comparison of alexithymic responses (%) in each Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) item between the AD and control groups.
Table 4. Comparison of alexithymic responses (%) in each Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R) item between the AD and control groups.
Item NumberSSPS-R ItemsAlexithymic ResponsesAlexithymic Responses (%) of Ad Group Alexithymic Responses (%) of Control Groupp Value a
1It is easy to describe symptoms or complaints rather than feelingsYes56470.561
2It is important to find out how one feels about peopleNo100.981
3It is hard to use words to describe feelingsYes39120.0163 *
4I think that feelings are what make life worthwhileNo760.966
5I lack imaginationYes50180.0124 *
6I spend much time daydreamingNo61710.582
7When I am mad I don’t think, I take actionYes33470.0221 *
8I like movies with action rather than psychological dramasYes28290.893
9 When in conflict I prefer to act quickly, rather than to think about itYes22350.487
10When in trouble I don’t like to actNo28530.0182 *
11I like to be precise and to describe everything in detailYes33470.0211 *
12I don’t care to describe details but rather I prefer to examine how I feelNo1760.715
13I have difficulty to communicate with peopleYes11180.527
14 I prefer to be alone rather than interact with peopleYes22120.0236
15I always pay attention to my surroundings rather than concentrate on how I feelYes17350.0204 *
16When I hear suspicious noises at night I don’t ever concentrate on how I feel Yes28470.715
17I find life boring most of the timeYes17120.587
18I cannot visualize circumstances which upset meYes2260.0231 *
19I dream rarelyYes50290.0216 *
20I like people better than thingsNo1160.794
For alexithymia individuals assessed using the Shalling Sifneos Psychosomatic Scale (SSPS-R): questions 2, 4, 6. 10, 12. 20 are false ones and should be answered No. All other questions should be answered Yes. Thus, a score of 10–20 should point to alexithymic characteristics. a p values referred to control and experimental groups. * Significant difference (p < 0.05). NS: not significant (p > 0.05).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Arroyo-Anlló, E.M.; Souchaud, C.; Ingrand, P.; Chamorro Sánchez, J.; Melero Ventola, A.; Gil, R. Alexithymia in Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 44. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10010044

AMA Style

Arroyo-Anlló EM, Souchaud C, Ingrand P, Chamorro Sánchez J, Melero Ventola A, Gil R. Alexithymia in Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2021; 10(1):44. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10010044

Chicago/Turabian Style

Arroyo-Anlló, Eva Mª, Corinne Souchaud, Pierre Ingrand, Jorge Chamorro Sánchez, Alejandra Melero Ventola, and Roger Gil. 2021. "Alexithymia in Alzheimer’s Disease" Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 1: 44. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10010044

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop