Next Article in Journal
The Effects of Hypoxia and Inflammation on Synaptic Signaling in the CNS
Previous Article in Journal
Cross-Modal Re-Organization in Clinical Populations with Hearing Loss
Previous Article in Special Issue
Pedunculopontine Gamma Band Activity and Development
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Neural Basis of Brain Dysfunction Produced by Early Sleep Problems

Tokyo Bay Urayasu Ichikawa Medical Center, 3-4-32 Todaijima, Urayasu 279-0001, Japan
Brain Sci. 2016, 6(1), 5; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6010005
Submission received: 9 October 2015 / Revised: 18 January 2016 / Accepted: 21 January 2016 / Published: 29 January 2016
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sleep and Brain Development)

Abstract

:
There is a wealth of evidence that disrupted sleep and circadian rhythms, which are common in modern society even during the early stages of life, have unfavorable effects on brain function. Altered brain function can cause problem behaviors later in life, such as truancy from or dropping out of school, quitting employment, and committing suicide. In this review, we discuss findings from several large cohort studies together with recent results of a cohort study using the marshmallow test, which was first introduced in the 1960s. This test assessed the ability of four-year-olds to delay gratification and showed how this ability correlated with success later in life. The role of the serotonergic system in sleep and how this role changes with age are also discussed. The serotonergic system is involved in reward processing and interactions with the dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, and the prefrontal cortex are thought to comprise the neural basis for behavioral patterns that are affected by the quantity, quality, and timing of sleep early in life.

1. Introduction

Sleep loss affects various brain functions in both adults and children [1] and is associated with behavioral, cognitive, and physical problems [2,3,4], as well as atypical early development [5]. Although shorter childhood sleep times were reported to be associated with higher adult body mass index values [6], long term effects of poor sleep and circadian rhythms on brain functions have not been fully understood. Recently, poor sleep and circadian rhythms during the early stages of life are hypothesized to produce changes in brain function that cause behavioral abnormalities later in life (truancy and dropping out of school, quitting employment, and committing suicide) [7]. However, the neural basis for these behavioral changes has not been fully determined; an involvement of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT)), oxytocin, and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been proposed (Figure 1 [7]).
This review begins by briefly summarizing the effects of sleep on the brain. We then describe the studies that have shown unfavorable effects of sleep deterioration during the early stages of life on behavior. Finally, we discuss the role of 5-HT in changing brain functions affected by poor sleep and circadian rhythms, that may produce behavioral abnormalities later in life.

2. Sleep Affects the Structure and Function of the Brain

Closure of one eye in 4–5-week-old kittens decreased the number of neurons responding to photic stimulation in the primary visual cortex. This indicates that during a critical developmental period, monocular deprivation (MD) initiates rapid synaptic remodeling of neurons in the primary visual cortex, in favor of the open eye [8]. Frank et al. [9] went on to show that laterality induced by MD as described above was hindered by sleep deprivation, as well as enhanced by sleep. This suggested that sleep in the early stages of life plays a crucial role in the functionality of the developing brain.
Figure 1. Proposed relationship between sleep hygiene, daily habits, neuronal elements, and self-esteem. Adapted from Kohyama [7] Expert. Rev. Neurother. 2014, 14, 745. Copyright © 2014 with permission of Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. * Yomikikase refers to a Japanese phrase, meaning to read a book to another person [10]. Yomikikase is reported to activate the limbic structures of children [11] and increase the frontal cerebral blood flow of the reader [12].
Figure 1. Proposed relationship between sleep hygiene, daily habits, neuronal elements, and self-esteem. Adapted from Kohyama [7] Expert. Rev. Neurother. 2014, 14, 745. Copyright © 2014 with permission of Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. * Yomikikase refers to a Japanese phrase, meaning to read a book to another person [10]. Yomikikase is reported to activate the limbic structures of children [11] and increase the frontal cerebral blood flow of the reader [12].
Brainsci 06 00005 g001
In one-month-old mice, the formation of dendritic spines differs continuously; spine formation and elimination is prominent during periods of wakefulness and sleep respectively, resulting in a net increase or decrease in spine density [13]. Yang and Gan [14] reported greater spine formation in three-week-old mice during the dark period, when mice are mostly awake, and more elimination of these protrusions during the light period, when mice are mostly asleep. These findings confirmed that, in young mice, the amount of sleep and wakefulness can affect the density of cortical synapses. The authors also reported that sleep after motor learning promoted the formation of postsynaptic dendritic spines on a subset of branches in individual layer-V pyramidal neurons and that neurons activated during motor task learning were reactivated during subsequent non-rapid eye movement sleep [15]. Disrupting this neuronal reactivation prevented branch-specific spine formation. These findings indicated that sleep promotes learning-dependent synapse formation and maintains dendritic branches, which contributes to the storage of motor memories.
Recently, it was proposed that sleep restores synaptic homeostasis after periods of wakefulness, during which time synapses are formed by learning [16]. In adult mice, spine turnover is limited and is not influenced by a state of sleep or wakefulness [13], suggesting that after adolescence, synaptic homeostasis is mediated primarily by changes in synaptic strength rather than number. The effect of sleep disturbance on the brain is difficult to study in humans, but white matter is found to be extensively altered in adult patients with obstructive sleep apnea [17]. Diffusion tensor imaging has shown that sleep disruption is associated with lower white matter integrity in adolescents [18]. Moreover, obstructive childhood sleep apnea has been associated with possible damage to the hippocampus and frontal cortex in addition to lower intelligence quotient and executive function [19]. These findings suggest that sleep disturbance can affect the morphology and function of the brain in humans.

3. Sleep Deterioration during the Early Stage of Life Has a Long-Term Negative Impact on Brain Function

Severe sleep problems in infancy, including bedtime struggles and difficulty falling asleep have been associated with the development of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [20]. Nightmares, sleepwalking and trouble sleeping at four years of age were reported to predict behavioral and emotional problems during mid-adolescence [21]. Similar sleep problems between four and 19 years of age were reported to be related to attention problems, aggressive behaviors, anxiety, and depression later in life [22]. In a large twin study, sleep problems including frequent/early waking, nightmares, and resistance to sleep at three and four years of age predicted anxiety, conduct problems, and hyperactivity at seven years of age [23]. A recent nationwide longitudinal survey in Japan revealed that poor sleep schedules in toddlers predicted behavioral problems at primary-school age [24]. Compared with waking early, irregular or late morning waking at two years was more significantly associated with overly aggressive behavior at eight years of age (odds ratio (OR), 1.52; 95% confidence interval (CI), 1.04–2.22). Furthermore, compared with an early bedtime, an irregular or late bedtime at two years of age was more significantly associated with attention problems (OR, 1.62; 95% CI, 1.12–2.36) and aggressiveness (OR, 1.81; 95% CI, 1.19–2.77) at eight years of age [24]. These findings [23,24] indicated that unfavorable sleep and/or circadian habits during the early stages of life may lead to conduct problems or aggressiveness during the first years of elementary school. The recent increase in violent behaviors in elementary schools in Japan (>11,000 cases in 2014) [25] might be related to early sleep problems [7].
Several large cohort studies have investigated the importance of sleep. In Japan, the Toyama cohort study [26], which included more than 10,000 people born in 1989, revealed that later bedtimes, later waking times, short sleep durations, physical inactivity, skipping breakfast, irregular snacks, and frequent instant noodle consumption at three years of age were associated with increased risk of poor life quality in the first-year junior high school students. The same study also revealed a relationship between bedtimes at three years of age and those at 9–10 years of age [27]. More than half (56.8%) of children who went to bed before 9 pm at three years of age went to bed before 9:30 pm when they reached 9–10 years of age. Furthermore, 42.0% of children who went to bed later than 10 pm at 3 years of age went to bed after 10 pm when they reached 9–10 years of age. Thus, three-year-old children who were late sleepers tended to maintain their late bedtime habit at least until they were 9–10 years old, although environmental conditions such as family lifestyle might also have affected these habits.
The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study was conducted for more than 30 years and has provided much useful information regarding sleep [28]. After controlling for childhood internalizing problems, sex, and socioeconomic status, persistent sleep problems during childhood (ages five, seven, and nine years) predicted anxiety disorders in adulthood (ages 21 and 26 years; OR, 1.60; 95% CI, 1.05–2.45, p = 0.030) [29]. The Dunedin study [30] also revealed a relationship between sleep problems at 5, 7, and 9 years of age and neuropsychological function at 13 years of age. The mean level of family conflict at 7–15 years of age also predicted insomnia at 18 years of age after controlling for sex, socioeconomic status, sleep problems at nine years, and self-reported health [31]. However, it should be noted that these studies [27,28,29,30,31] did not provide a specific definition of “sleep problems”. Sleep problems were identified based on answers to general non-specific questions. The Dunedin cohort study also revealed the following personality continuities [32]: (1) under-controlled three-year-olds (resembling the Chess–Thomas difficult type [33], included children who were impulsive, restless, negativistic, distractible, and labile in their emotional responses.) grew up to be impulsive, unreliable, and antisocial, and had more social and work-related conflicts; (2) inhibited three-year-olds (resembling the Chess–Thomas slow-to-warm-up type [33], included children who were socially reticent, fearful, and easily upset by strangers.) were more likely to be unassertive and depressed, and had fewer sources of social support.
Greene et al. [34] reported an association between childhood sleeping difficulties and adulthood depression after adjusting for maternal depression and sleeping difficulty in a 30-year prospective study conducted between 1975 and 2005. The study revealed that severe, but not moderate sleeping difficulties, at five years of age were associated with an increased risk of depression at 34 years of age (severe: OR, 1.9; 95% CI, 1.2–3.2; moderate: OR, 1.1; 95% CI, 0.9–1.3).
Truancy and dropping out of school, quitting jobs rapidly, and committing suicide have already been identified as tragic consequences of early sleep problems [7]. We have reviewed the evidence that poor sleep and circadian rhythms were also associated with behavioral problems later in life, such as aggression, anxiety, depression, conduct problems, hyperactivity, insomnia, and the inability to pay adequate attention. Some of these problems can be treated by agents that increase 5-HT activity [35]. Impulsive and aggressive behaviors are known to be modulated by 5-HT signaling [36]. In the next section, we review the 5-HT system and its role in sleep.

4. Theories on the Development of the 5-HT System

The aminergic neurons of the brainstem and the midbrain have crucial roles in brain development [37]. 5-HT neurons are involved in the development of particular brain functions at critical ages and via specific receptors [38]. Axon pruning of 5-HT visual cortex neurons occurs during the first seven postnatal days in rats [39]. Segawa [40] postulated that aminergic systems are involved in the sleep-wake (S-W) cycle, and that the 5-HT system plays a role in the development of brain function.
According to Segawa [40], the circadian S-W cycle develops in the first four months of life and involves human interaction, accommodation to a novel environment, and functional lateralization of the cortex. By one year of age (the second phase), daytime sleep begins to occur only in the afternoon and atonia is restricted to rapid eye movement sleep. At the same time, synaptogenesis begins in the cortex and antigravity activity induces locomotion, which is involved in the development of the frontal cortex through activation of the dopaminergic (DA) system. By 4–5 years of age (the third epoch), the two-phase S-W rhythm is completed and daytime sleep stops. Additionally, brainstem and hypothalamic functions are synchronized and start to modulate functions of the frontal cortex by activating the non-motor basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuits. The 5-HT neurons involved in the development of the biphasic S-W cycle synchronize the biological rhythms generated by the brainstem and mesencephalon by 5 years of age. The striatum is innervated by both 5-HT and DA neurons. 5-HT neurons become involved in the development of the non-motor basal ganglia thalamo-cortical circuit as DA neurons develop. This circuit projects to the orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate area. These non-motor circuits are postulated to be involved in the following functions: dorsolateral prefrontal circuit (executive function, cognitive tasks), lateral orbitofrontal circuit (empathetic and socially appropriate responses), anterior cingulate circuit (motivated behavior, procedural learning via reinforcement signals from the ventral tegmental area and the substantia nigra pars compacta to diffuse areas of the basal ganglia and the cortex). Developing DA and 5-HT neurons innervate the non-motor basal ganglia thalamo-cortical circuits at 4–5 years of age and become involved in the specific cognitive functions of the frontal cortex controlled by these circuits. This provides a stable psychobehavioral background for the development of higher cortical function.
Segawa did not discuss the development of the 5-HT system after the third epoch [40]. Although age-dependent morphological and functional alterations of the 5-HT system in humans remain to be determined, Arita [41] compared mean urinary 5-HT concentrations at rest (in the morning) between age groups (young (3–4 years old), junior (4–5 years old) and senior (5–6 years old) children). The mean value decreased with increasing age. According to Arita [41], urinary 5-HT concentrations in senior children were similar to adults, which is compatible with findings that brain 5-HT concentrations are higher in infancy than in adulthood, and decrease to reach a stable level during the course of postnatal development. Arita [41] emphasized that 3–6 years after birth is a critical period for the postnatal development of 5-HT neurons. In addition, Arita [41] proposed three factors that activate the 5-HT system: rhythmic movements, solar irradiation, and grooming. Young [42] proposed four non-pharmacologic methods for increasing brain 5-HT: (1) food (tryptophan-enriched diet); (2) positive (happy) mood; (3) exposure to bright light; and (4) exercise.
The 5-HT system is activated during wakefulness, and its activity is reduced during non-rapid eye movement sleep and ceases during rapid eye movement sleep [43]. Contradictory to earlier theories, the activity of the 5-HT system is strongly related to sleep. During wakefulness, the 5-HT system is activated by exposure to morning light [44] and by rhythmic movements, such as gait, chewing, and respiration [41,42,43]. Unfavorable daily habits, such as poor physical activity (poor rhythmic movements) and waking late (reduced light exposure) fail to enhance 5-HT activity. According to the international classification of sleep disorders version 3 (ICSD-3) [45], insufficient sleep leads to fatigue and malaise, which naturally results in poor physical activity. In addition, irritability, concentration/attention deficits, and reduced vigilance are features of insufficient sleep syndrome. Insufficient sleep, which is often associated with late bed times [46], may result in reduced 5-HT activity through both poor physical activity and loss of exposure to morning light. Essentially, activation of the 5-HT system can be achieved by adequate sleep health, which promotes morning sunlight exposure and sufficient physical activity. Arita [41] illustrated means of increasing 5-HT system as Figure 2. Here, factors that modulate 5-HT system and the presumable roles of 5-HT are summarized in Figure 3 by modifying from Arita’s original figure (Figure 2) [41].
Figure 2. Adapted from Arita [41] JMAJ 2009, 52, 184. Copyright © 2009 with permission from Japan Medical Association (the tiltle of original figure is “means of increasing serotonin”).
Figure 2. Adapted from Arita [41] JMAJ 2009, 52, 184. Copyright © 2009 with permission from Japan Medical Association (the tiltle of original figure is “means of increasing serotonin”).
Brainsci 06 00005 g002
Figure 3. Factors that modulate 5-HT activity and the presumable roles of 5-HT (modified from Arita [41] (Figure 2)).
Figure 3. Factors that modulate 5-HT activity and the presumable roles of 5-HT (modified from Arita [41] (Figure 2)).
Brainsci 06 00005 g003

5. Marshmallow Test and Possible Involvement of the 5-HT System

The marshmallow test was developed by Mischel and colleagues [47]. Originally conducted in the 1960s to assess the ability to delay gratification, this test presented a group of four-year-olds with a choice between a small reward (one marshmallow), which they could have immediately, and a larger reward (two marshmallows), for which they had to wait 15 minutes [48]. About half the children chose to delay gratification. In numerous follow-up studies over a 40-year period, this test has been shown to have a surprisingly significant predictive validity for social, cognitive, and mental health later in life [49]. Four-year-old children who delayed gratification longer developed into more cognitively- and socially-competent adolescents, achieving higher scholastic performance, and coping better with frustration and stress [50]. According to Mischel [47], these processes involve the cool cognitive system, primarily centered in the PFC. Individuals who were less able to delay gratification consistently showed low self-control in their twenties and thirties and could not suppress responses to a happy face as well as those individuals who had the ability to delay gratification as children. According to Mischel, these processes are activated by the hot emotional system, closely related with the limbic system [47]. The results of the marshmallow test show that behavior at four years of age can predict the ability to self-regulate 40 years later [51].
According to Mischel [47], the PFC is crucial for future-oriented decisions and self-control (cool system). Even mild stress, if acute and uncontrollable, can cause a rapid and dramatic loss of prefrontal cognitive abilities [45]. Sleep deprivation decreases brain activity in the fronto-parietal attention network (PFC and intraparietal sulcus) in adult humans [52]. The medial PFC (paracingulate cortex) is also involved in developing a theory of mind, which occurs by five years of age [53]. The ventromedial PFC and subcortical areas (ventral tegmental area, nucleus accumbens and amygdala) are also sensitive to various types of rewards [54,55]. Dorsal and medial raphe nuclei, which contain many 5-HT neurons are also implicated in reward-directed behavior [54]. Casey et al. [45] revealed that children who were less able to delay gratification showed lower self-control in their forties. Casey et al. [45] also showed enhanced activation in the ventral striatum (hot system) in low delayers, and higher differentiation between no-go and go trials in the PFC of high delayers. The Dunedin study showed that specific personality traits were also continued from childhood through to adulthood [32], but the marshmallow study also related these observations to specific neuronal circuits.
The marshmallow test assessed the ability of a young child to resist an immediate reward in favor of a larger reward later on [50]. DA neurons are commonly referred to as “reward” neurons [56] and modulation of the DA reward system by 5-HT has received much attention [57]. The correlation between short-term reward seeking and ventral striatum activity was stronger when the level of 5-HT was lower (achieved by dietary tryptophan depletion), and a higher 5-HT level (achieved by dietary tryptophan loading) enhanced the correlation between long-term reward seeking and dorsal striatum activity [58]. Activity in the dorsal striatum also correlated with long-term reward predictions, while activity in the ventral striatum correlated more with short-term predictions. However, no significant differences were observed in the choice between immediate and delayed rewards at different dietary tryptophan levels [58], in agreement with a previous human study [59]. These authors later reported an increase in small reward choices and the rate of discounting of delayed rewards in the low-5-HT condition compared with the high-5-HT conditions [60]. In addition, dietary tryptophan depletion was associated with aggressive harvesting strategies during games, as well as disrupted use of the social norms, complete exhaustion of resources, and accumulation of fewer rewards than participants who were not tryptophan-depleted [61]. Therefore, a reduced ability to delay gratification in the marshmallow test may be linked to lower levels of 5-HT. 5-HT levels are affected by sleep during the first four years of life. It has been suggested that reward networks and circadian rhythms have evolved via a common mechanism [54]; therefore, there may be a link between early sleep habits and behavior later in life, although this remains to be determined.

6. 5-HT Activity, Sleep, and Development

Hypofunction of the 5-HT system in the PFC has been associated with suicide [62]. When people are sleep-deprived, they are unlikely to be active enough to produce sufficient 5-HT. Sleep loss decreases activity of the PFC [52], and decreased 5-HT activity, especially in the PFC, may play a role in suicidal behaviors [63]. A recent meta-analysis of data collected between 1966 and 2011 [64] supports the idea that sleep disturbances represent a risk factor for suicidal thoughts and behavior. The hot system [47] may be involved in suicidal thoughts and behavior. The Dunedin and Toyama studies mentioned three years as being important, the marshmallow test [47] focused on age four, Segawa [40] targeted ages 4–5, a theory of mind [53] emphasized age five, and Arita [41] paid attention to age 3–6 years. Unfortunately, we still have no data on the functional and/or morphological co-development of the 5-HT system, the striatum, and the PFC, although insufficient sleep syndrome is known to affect all ages and sexes [45].
To obtain an adequate social life, basic 5-HT activity is considered important at 4–5 [40] or 3–6 [41] years of age. Sufficient 5-HT activity can be achieved non-pharmacologically by exposure to morning sunlight and physical activity. Good sleep habits and circadian rhythms can help achieve this. The inability to delay gratification at four years of age cannot be reversed in adulthood, even by raised 5-HT levels; therefore, sufficient maturation of the 5-HT system might be crucial early in development for long-term reward seeking and this requires good sleep and circadian rhythms. T2-weighted imaging showed a different pattern of development in the dorsal and ventral striatum [65]. Good sleep and circadian rhythms during early stages of life promotes favorable development of the dorsal striatum involved in long-term reward seeking.
The sensitive period and the neural basis of self-esteem (Figure 1) remain to be determined. Mischel et al. [49] reported that the ability to delay gratification predicted less physical and verbal aggression, less bullying behavior, and higher self-worth and self-esteem, which underlines the importance of self-regulation strategies to overcome immediate temptations in children. These strategies may help the exertion of willpower later in life.
The relationship between hot and cool neural circuitry and self-esteem must be further investigated. Although a complete clarification of the neural basis of brain dysfunction produced by unfavorable sleep and circadian rhythms during early stages of life has not yet been accomplished, consistent progress in the field has helped us reach a more complete understanding.

7. Conclusions

Issues discussed in this review were added to Figure 1 (Figure 4).
Figure 4. Proposed relationship between sleep hygiene, daily habits, neuronal elements, and self-esteem, modified from a previous review [7]. Expressions in italics were discussed in this review.
Figure 4. Proposed relationship between sleep hygiene, daily habits, neuronal elements, and self-esteem, modified from a previous review [7]. Expressions in italics were discussed in this review.
Brainsci 06 00005 g004
Several studies support the idea that poor sleep habits at young ages produce behavioral problems later on in life. These problems are likely to result from changes in the 5-HT system, the dorsal striatum, the ventral striatum, and the PFC, which affect brain development and related behaviors as the children grow older.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Mindell, J.A.; Owens, J.; Alves, R.; Bruni, O.; Goh, D.Y.; Hiscock, H.; Kohyama, J.; Sadeh, A. Give children and adolescents the gift of a good night’s sleep: A call to action. Sleep Med. 2011, 12, 203–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Yokomaku, A.; Misao, K.; Omoto, F.; Yamagishi, R.; Tanaka, K.; Takada, K.; Kohyama, J. A study of the association between sleep habits and problematic behaviors in preschool children. Chronobiol. Int. 2008, 25, 549–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Osonoi, Y.; Mita, T.; Osonoi, T.; Saito, M.; Tamasawa, A.; Nakayama, S.; Someya, Y.; Ishida, H.; Kanazawa, A.; Gosho, M.; et al. Morningness-eveningness questionnaire score and metabolic parameters in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Chronobiol. Int. 2014, 31, 1017–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Schlarb, A.A.; Sopp, R.; Ambiel, D.; Grünwald, J. Chronotype-related differences in childhood and adolescent aggression and antisocial behavior—A review of the literature. Chronobiol. Int. 2014, 31, 1–16. [Google Scholar]
  5. Iemura, A.; Iwasaki, M.; Yamakawa, N.; Tomiwa, K.; Anji, Y.; Sakakihara, Y.; Kakuma, T.; Nagamitsu, S.; Matsuishi, T. Influence of sleep-onset time on the development of 18-month-old infants: Japan Children’s cohort study. Brain Dev. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Landhuis, C.E.; Poulton, R.; Welch, D.; Hancox, R.J. Childhood sleep time and long-term risk for obesity: A 32-year prospective birth cohort study. Pediatrics 2008, 122, 955–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kohyama, J. The possible long-term effects of early-life circadian rhythm disturbance on social behavior. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2014, 14, 745–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Hubel, D.H.; Wiesel, T.N. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J. Physiol. 1970, 206, 419–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Frank, M.G.; Issa, N.P.; Stryker, M.P. Sleep enhances plasticity in the developing visual cortex. Neuron 2001, 30, 275–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kohyama, J. Sleep, serotonin, and suicide in Japan. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2011, 30, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Haji, T.; Tajima, N.; Matsumoto, M.; Sasaki, T.; Hachisuka, M.; Aihara, M.; Taira, M. Cortical activations of mother and child during reading to child—Yomikikase: An fNIRS study. In Proceedings of the 84th Congress of the Physiological Society of Japan, Osaka, Japan, 20–22 March 2007; The Physiological Society of Japan: Tokyo, Japan, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  12. Taira, M. Yomikikase ha Kokoro no Nou ni Todoku; Kumon Publisher: Tokyo, Japan, 2009. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
  13. Maret, S.; Faraguna, U.; Nelson, A.B.; Cirelli, C.; Tononi, G. Sleep and waking modulate spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 1418–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Yang, G.; Gan, W.B. Sleep contributes to dendritic spine formationand elimination in the developing mouse somatosensory cortex. Dev. Neurobiol. 2012, 72, 1391–1398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Yang, G.; Lai, C.S.; Cichon, J.; Ma, L.; Li, W.; Gan, W.B. Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 2014, 344, 1173–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Tononi, G.; Cirelli, C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: From synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 2014, 81, 12–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Macey, P.M.; Kumar, R.; Woo, M.A.; Valladares, E.M.; Yan-Go, F.L.; Harper, R.M. Brain structural changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 2008, 31, 967–977. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  18. Telzer, E.H.; Goldenberg, D.; Fuligni, A.J.; Lieberman, M.D.; Gálvan, A. Sleep variability in adolescence is associated with altered brain development. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 2015, 14, 16–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Halbower, A.C.; Degaonkar, M.; Barker, P.B.; Earley, C.J.; Marcus, C.L.; Smith, P.L.; Prahme, M.C.; Mahone, E.M. Childhood obstructive sleep apnea associates with neuropsychological deficits and neuronal brain injury. PLoS Med. 2006, 3, e301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Thunström, M. Severe sleep problems in infancy associated with subsequent development of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder at 5.5 years of age. Acta Paediatr. 2002, 91, 584–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Gregory, A.M.; O’Connor, T.G. Sleep problems in childhood: A longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2002, 41, 964–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Gregory, A.M.; Van der Ende, J.; Willis, T.A.; Verhulst, F.C. Parent-reported sleep problems during development and self-reported anxiety/depression, attention problems, and aggressive behavior later in life. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2008, 162, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Gregory, A.M.; Eley, T.C.; O’Connor, T.G.; Plomin, R. Etiologies of associations between childhood sleep and behavioral problems in a large twin sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 43, 744–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kobayashi, K.; Yorifuji, T.; Yamakawa, M.; Oka, M.; Inoue, S.; Yoshinaga, H.; Doi, H. Poor toddler-age sleep schedules predict school-age behavioral disorders in a longitudinal survey. Brain Dev. 2015, 37, 572–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. The Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology. Annual report on educational problems including problematic behaviors among pupils and students in 2014 and 2015. Available online: http://www.mext.go.jp/b_menu/houdou/27/09/__icsFiles/afieldfile/2015/10/07/1362012_1_1.pdf (accessed on 28 December 2015). (In Japanese)
  26. Wang, H.; Sekine, M.; Chen, X.; Yamagami, T.; Kagamimori, S. Lifestyle at 3 years of age and quality of life (QOL) in first-year junior high school students in Japan: Results of the Toyama Birth Cohort Study. Qual. Life Res. 2008, 17, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Sekine, M. Nocturnal sleep and lifestyle disease. Child Health 2003, 6, 662–665. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
  28. Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit. Available online: http://dunedinstudy.otago.ac.nz/ (accessed on 28 December 2015).
  29. Gregory, A.M.; Caspi, A.; Eley, T.C.; Moffitt, T.E.; O’Connor, T.G.; Poulton, R. Prospective longitudinal associations between persistent sleep problems in childhood and anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005, 33, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Gregory, A.M.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E.; Poulton, R. Sleep problems in childhood predict neuropsychological functioning in adolescence. Pediatrics 2009, 123, 1171–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Gregory, A.M.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E.; Poulton, R. Family conflict in childhood: A predictor of later insomnia. Sleep 2006, 29, 1063–1067. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  32. Caspi, A. The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 158–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Chess, S.; Thomas, A. Origins and Evolution of Behavior Disorders: From Infancy to Early Adult Life; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  34. Greene, G.; Gregory, A.M.; Fone, D.; White, J. Childhood sleeping difficulties and depression in adulthood: The 1970 British Cohort Study. J. Sleep Res. 2015, 24, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Maher, M.J.; Rego, S.A.; Asnis, G.M. Sleep disturbances in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder: Epidemiology, impact and approaches to management. CNS Drugs 2006, 20, 567–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Nautiyal, K.M.; Tanaka, K.F.; Barr, M.M.; Tritschler, L.; le Dantec, Y.; David, D.J.; Gardier, A.N.; Blanco, C.; Hen, R.; Ahmari, S.E. Distinct circuits underlie the effects of 5-HT1B receptors on aggression and impulsivity. Neuron 2015, 86, 813–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Levitt, P.; Harvey, J.A.; Friedman, E.; Simansky, K.; Murphy, E.H. New evidence for neurotransmitter influences on brain development. Trends Neurosci. 1997, 20, 269–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gaspar, P.; Cases, O.; Maroteaux, L. The developmental role of serotonin: News from mouse molecular genetics. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003, 4, 1002–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Koh, T.; Nakazawa, M.; Kani, K.; Maeda, T. Investigation of origins of serotonergic projection to developing rat visual cortex: A combined retrograde tracing and immunohistochemical study. Brain Res. Bull. 1991, 27, 675–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Segawa, M. Epochs of development of the sleep-wake cycle reflect the modulation of the higher cortical function particular for each epoch. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 2006, 4, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Arita, H. Brain mechanisms of poor anger management. JMAJ 2009, 52, 184–190. [Google Scholar]
  42. Young, S.N. How to increase serotonin in the human brain without drugs. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2007, 32, 394–399. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  43. Jacobs, B.L.; Azmitia, E.C. Structure and function of the brain serotonin system. Physiol. Rev. 1992, 72, 165–229. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  44. Cagampang, F.R.; Yamazaki, S.; Otori, Y.; Inouye, S.I. Serotonin in the raphe nuclei: Regulation by light and an endogenous pacemaker. Neuroreport 1993, 5, 49–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorder, 3rd ed.; American Academy of Sleep Medicine: Westchester, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  46. Kohyama, J.; Shiiki, T.; Ohinata-Sugimoto, J.; Hasegawa, T. Potentially harmful sleep habits of 3-year-old children in Japan. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2002, 23, 67–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Mischel, W. The Marshmallow Test; Little, Brown and Company: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  48. Mischel, W. Father-absence and delay of gratification: Crosscultural comparisons. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1961, 63, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Mischel, W.; Ayduk, O.; Berman, M.G.; Casey, B.J.; Gotlib, I.H.; Jonides, J.; Kross, E.; Teslovich, T.; Wilson, N.L.; Zayas, V.; et al. “Willpower” over the life span: Decomposing self-regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2011, 6, 252–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Mischel, W.; Shoda, Y.; Rodriguez, M.I. Delay of gratification in children. Science 1989, 244, 933–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Casey, B.J.; Somerville, L.H.; Gotlib, I.H.; Ayduk, O.; Franklin, N.T.; Askren, M.K.; Jonides, J.; Berman, M.G.; Wilson, N.L.; Teslovich, T.; et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 14998–15003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Ma, N.; Dinges, D.F.; Basner, M.; Rao, H. How acute total sleep loss affects the attending brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sleep 2015, 38, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Frith, C.; Frith, U. Theory of mind. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, R644–R645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Hikosaka, O.; Bromberg-Martin, E.; Hong, S.; Matsumoto, M. New insights on the subcortical representation of reward. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2008, 18, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Sescousse, G.; Caldú, X.; Segura, B.; Dreher, J.C. Processing of primary and secondary rewards: A quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 681–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Salamone, J.D.; Pardo, M.; Yohn, S.E.; López-Cruz, L.; SanMiguel, N.; Correa, M. Mesolimbic dopamine and the regulation of motivated behavior. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Seo, D.; Patrick, C.J.; Kennealy, P.J. Role of serotonin and dopamine system interactions in the neurobiology of impulsive aggression and its comorbidity with other clinical disorders. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2008, 13, 383–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Tanaka, S.C.; Schweighofer, N.; Asahi, S.; Shishida, K.; Okamoto, Y.; Yamawaki, S.; Doya, K. Serotonin differentially regulates short- and long-term prediction of rewards in the ventral and dorsal striatum. PLoS ONE 2007, 2, e1333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Crean, J.; Richards, J.B.; de Wit, H. Effect of tryptophan depletion on impulsive behavior in men with or without a family history of alcoholism. Behav. Brain Res. 2002, 136, 349–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Schweighofer, N.; Bertin, M.; Shishida, K.; Okamoto, Y.; Tanaka, S.C.; Yamawaki, S.; Doya, K. Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 4528–4532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Bilderbeck, A.C.; Brown, G.D.; Read, J.; Woolrich, M.; Cowen, P.J.; Behrens, T.E.; Rogers, R.D. Serotonin and social norms: Tryptophan depletion impairs social comparison and leads to resource depletion in a multiplayer harvesting game. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 1303–1313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Chiavegatto, S.; Quadros, I.M.; Ambar, G.; Miczek, K.A. Individual vulnerability to escalated aggressive behavior by a low dose of alcohol: Decreased serotonin receptor mRNA in the prefrontal cortex of male mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2010, 9, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Kohyama, J. Sleep, Serotonin, and Suicide. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2012, 2, 471–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pigeon, W.R.; Pinquart, M.; Conner, K. Meta-analysis of sleep disturbance and suicidal thoughts and behavior. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2012, 73, e1160–e1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Larsen, B.; Luna, B. In vivo evidence of neurophysiological maturation of the human adolescent striatum. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 12, 74–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kohyama, J. Neural Basis of Brain Dysfunction Produced by Early Sleep Problems. Brain Sci. 2016, 6, 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6010005

AMA Style

Kohyama J. Neural Basis of Brain Dysfunction Produced by Early Sleep Problems. Brain Sciences. 2016; 6(1):5. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6010005

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kohyama, Jun. 2016. "Neural Basis of Brain Dysfunction Produced by Early Sleep Problems" Brain Sciences 6, no. 1: 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6010005

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop