Next Article in Journal
Altered Resting-State Brain Activity and Functional Connectivity in Post-Stroke Apathy: An fMRI Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Advances in Genetics and Epigenetics of Developmental Coordination Disorder in Children
Previous Article in Journal
Differentiated Neurons Are More Vulnerable to Organophosphate and Carbamate Neurotoxicity than Undifferentiated Neurons Due to the Induction of Redox Stress and Accumulate Oxidatively-Damaged Proteins
Previous Article in Special Issue
Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance: A Randomized Controlled Trial Examining Intervention Effects on Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder Traits
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Auditory–Perceptual and Auditory–Motor Timing Abilities in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Scoping Review

by
Marija Pranjić
1,2,*,
Niloufaralsadat Hashemi
2,3,
Anne B. Arnett
4,5 and
Michael H. Thaut
1,6
1
Music and Health Science Research Collaboratory, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON M5S 1C5, Canada
2
Bloorview Research Institute, Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON M4G 1R8, Canada
3
Institute of Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON M5S 3G9, Canada
4
Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA
5
Pediatrics, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
6
Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Medical Science and Rehabilitation Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON M5S 1A8, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(5), 729; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050729
Submission received: 27 March 2023 / Revised: 18 April 2023 / Accepted: 25 April 2023 / Published: 27 April 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue New Insights in Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD))

Abstract

:
Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) remains largely underdiagnosed and masked by other co-occurring conditions. The aim of this study was to (1) provide the first review of research regarding auditory–motor timing and synchronization abilities in children with DCD and (2) examine whether reduced motor performance may be associated with difficulties in auditory perceptual timing. The scoping review was carried out across five major databases (MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, CINAHL, and Scopus) in accordance with the PRISMA-ScR guidelines. Studies were screened by two independent reviewers against the inclusion criteria, without publication date restrictions. From an initial return of 1673 records, 16 articles were included in the final review and synthesized based on the timing modality studied (i.e., auditory–perceptual, motor, or auditory–motor). Results suggest that children with DCD have difficulties with rhythmic movements both with and without external auditory cues and further indicate that variability in and slowness of motor response are key characteristics of DCD, regardless of the experimental task. Importantly, our review highlights a significant gap in the literature regarding auditory perceptual abilities in DCD. In addition to testing auditory perception, future studies should compare the performance of children with DCD on paced and unpaced tasks to determine whether auditory stimuli contribute to a more or less stable performance. This knowledge may inform future therapeutic interventions.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, occurring in approximately 6% of school-age children [1,2]. DCD is characterized by marked difficulties in motor planning, fine and/or gross motor coordination, sensorimotor timing, as well as acquisition and transfer of new motor skills. The diagnosis of DCD is given if these difficulties interfere with activities of daily living and/or academic performance, while ensuring those differences cannot be better explained by other medical and/or neurological conditions [1]. Motor coordination difficulties in children significantly affect quality of life and lead to anxiety, lower self-esteem, social isolation, and reduced fitness levels [3,4,5]. Despite its high incidence, DCD has been described as a “hidden problem” [6] as it remains less studied and under-recognized by healthcare and educational professionals [7] compared to other neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs).
NDDs impact approximately 15% of children worldwide, encompass a broad range of conditions that typically manifest early in life, and affect performance in different domains of functioning, including attentional, sensory, motor, linguistic, and social areas [1]. Difficulties in any of these domains rarely occur in isolation, thus the overlapping symptoms are the rule rather than the exception [8]. In addition to primary challenges associated with motor control, children with DCD demonstrate a range of NDD symptoms [8]. The phenotypic complexity of DCD poses additional challenges to understanding neurobiological mechanisms for DCD. Neuroimaging studies of DCD have largely focused on the cerebellum, due to its role in the development of automatic movement control and movement self-monitoring [9], both of which are affected in DCD [10,11]; however, emerging neuroimaging data provide evidence of atypical neural activations across both cortical and subcortical regions, including the prefrontal and parietal cortices, cerebellum, basal ganglia, and white matter alterations in the corpus callosum [12,13,14,15]. Although these studies have provided important insights into the neurophysiological substrates associated with motor difficulties, heterogeneity in DCD creates significant barriers to interpreting and translating those findings into individualized treatments.
Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain the motor performance differences associated with DCD. The most prominent is the internal modelling deficit hypothesis [16,17] which suggests that the difficulties in motor learning observed in children with DCD result from a deficiency in generating or implementing predictive models of action (forward modelling), such as correcting actions in real-time [18,19]. In other words, predictive timing allows for error detection and correction of the ongoing movement in situations when actual and predicted feedback do not correspond. Considering sensory error feedback plays an important role in the development of internal models [20], it is not surprising that difficulties in sensorimotor timing are one of the core features of DCD. Indeed, previous studies have shown that children with DCD are less accurate and more variable when synchronizing their movement to an external cue compared to typically developing controls regardless of the modality of the stimuli [21,22,23]; however, given that integration of sensory input is a prerequisite for synchronized sensorimotor performance, the observed challenges may also arise from perceptual or perceptual-motor integration challenges.
Thus, a new auditory–perceptual timing hypothesis has been postulated by Trainor et al. [24] who suggest that deficits in auditory perceptual timing might also be central to DCD. This hypothesis draws from research showing that even passive listening to auditory stimuli with predictive rhythmic beats leads to activations in motor-related brain areas [25,26,27]. Notably, this line of research has led to novel auditory-based therapeutic interventions in adults with movement disorders [28,29,30] that have shown that rhythmic auditory cues can facilitate planning and execution of movements by increasing temporal stability and speed. Furthermore, auditory perceptual timing abilities are affected in neurodevelopmental disorders associated with DCD, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, dyslexia, and autism spectrum disorder [31,32,33,34]. Therefore, a shared deficit in perceptual timing may explain the overlapping profile of motor difficulties across these relatively distinct neurodevelopmental phenotypes [35].

Aims

The aim of this scoping review was to systematically map and synthesize the existing empirical research regarding auditory–motor coupling in children with DCD by focusing on three timing modalities that underpin this process: auditory–perceptual timing (perceptual judgment without movement), motor timing (self-paced actions), and auditory–motor timing (adjusting the timing of motor responses based on the auditory cue) [24]. We examined whether the variability of auditory–motor coupling shown by children with DCD is task-specific and whether their performance differences are associated with difficulties in auditory–perceptual timing.

2. Materials and Methods

The scoping review was conducted in accordance with the Joanna Briggs Institute methodological framework [36], using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) criteria: Checklist and Explanation guidelines [37]. We addressed the following questions: (1) What is known from the literature about the auditory–motor timing and synchronization abilities in children with DCD? (2) Do children with DCD have both auditory–perceptual and auditory–motor difficulties? The protocol was registered with the Open Science Framework on 3 March 2023 (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/5KAXQ (accessed on 3 March 2023)).

2.1. Search Strategy

The population, concept, and context (PCC) mnemonic was used to formulate the search strategy. We used terms such as “DCD”, “motor skill disorder” or “dyspraxia” to define the population (i.e., individuals with DCD) combined with terms for the concept (i.e., auditory–motor timing) such as “auditory-motor coupling” or “synchronization”. The context was not specified, in order to capture all relevant publications regardless of setting (e.g., clinic or research laboratory). Subject headings were adapted for each database. The search strategy was developed and performed by two authors (M.P. and N.H.) with assistance from a research librarian. A scoping review was carried out across five major databases (MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, CINAHL, and Scopus) for articles published before April 2022. Additionally, we cross-referenced the bibliographies of selected studies to ensure we captured current and emerging evidence. Table 1 provides an example of the search strategy performed in MEDLINE (see Supplementary Table S1 for the full electronic search strategy). The appropriate filtering processes of the final search results were done via Covidence.

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

Inclusion criteria were studies that (1) involved children (<18 years) with motor coordination difficulties (i.e., DCD or probable DCD as assessed with a standardized motor test), (2) examined the effects of auditory stimuli on motor performance, (3) were published in English, and (4) were categorized as original research. Since this is the first review on the topic, publication year was not considered as an inclusion/exclusion criterion; however, we recognize that the term “developmental coordination disorder” was introduced in 1994, resulting in a wide variation in terminology used prior to that year. Articles were excluded if the outcome measures were not related to motor performance and if the primary goal was an evaluation of a treatment method. All titles, abstracts, and full-text publications were screened by two independent reviewers for inclusion and exclusion using the relevance screening tool (see Supplementary Tables S2 and S3). Relevant studies were further evaluated and discrepancies between the reviewers regarding study selection and data extraction were resolved by consensus (<5 studies).

2.3. Data Extraction and Synthesis

We systematically extracted the following data: publication details, sample characteristics (i.e., age, sex, sample size, motor ability), study design (i.e., experimental tasks, timing modality), and outcomes. One reviewer (M.P.) extracted data from the included papers, refining and updating the data-charting form in the process. The second reviewer (N.H.) verified the correctness of the extracted information. Data from selected studies were collated, summarized, and reported based on the type of task (i.e., timing modality) used: (1) auditory–perceptual (perceptual judgment is assessed without producing movements), (2) motor (self-paced and continuation tasks), and (3) auditory–motor (motor responses are synchronized or adjusted based on the auditory cue). We aimed to organize the studies in this framework to establish commonalities between the types of tasks used, their underlying timing modalities, and the reported outcomes. The results were further discussed in the context of two dominant perspectives on motor timing: information processing and the dynamical systems theory. Finally, our findings are considered in the context of the auditory–perceptual timing hypothesis [24].

3. Results

As illustrated in the PRISMA-ScR flowchart in Figure 1, a total of 1673 sources were identified from searches of five electronic databases. Four additional studies were identified through manual search of the bibliographies. Based on the title and the abstract, 1143 articles were excluded and a total of 32 sources remained for full-text screening. Of these, 16 were excluded for the following reasons: 11 did not directly examine the effects of auditory stimuli on motor performance, and 5 did not report motor-related outcomes. Thus, a total of 16 studies were considered eligible for this review and are summarized in Table 2. All selected studies employed a cross-sectional design whereby participants performed experimental tasks at a single time point. The following sections synthesize results across the 16 studies and present findings organized by the timing modality studied and the experimental task used (note that some experiments involved more than one type of task or timing modality and are therefore discussed in more than one section).

3.1. Auditory–Perceptual Timing

Only three studies tested the auditory–perceptual abilities of children with DCD separate from their motor or auditory–motor performance [38,39,40]. Two studies used the same time perception task wherein children aged 7–8 years [38] and 6–7 and 9–10 years [39] listened to two pairs of tones and were asked to judge whether the interval of the second pair was longer or shorter than that of the first pair. The interval of the second pair changed adaptively based on the child’s response in order to hone in on the individual child’s perceptual threshold. A similar task, in which the children were expected to determine differences in volume between two pairs of tones was also administered to control for general auditory processing differences. Both studies found that the threshold scores for duration-based timing were longer (i.e., higher) in children described as “clumsy” compared to healthy controls, suggesting temporal processing difficulties in the former group. In contrast, volume thresholds did not differ between groups, indicating intact hearing. Williams et al. [39] further found that there were age differences for the loudness perception task wherein younger children (6–7 years) had lower acuity than the 9–10-year-old group regardless of motor performance.
Interestingly, Lundy-Ekman et al. [38] compared two groups of “clumsy” children and reported that children with cerebellar soft signs (e.g., dysmetria, dysdiadochokinesia and/or intention tremor) exhibited increased difficulty with the duration timing task compared to the “clumsy” children with basal ganglia signs (e.g., synkinesis, choreiform movements and/or athetoid cerebral palsy) suggesting that the cerebellum may be the source of both perceptual and motor timing difficulties. However, these findings should be interpreted cautiously because the “clumsy” children described in publications before 1994 [7] may not meet the current DSM-5 or ICD-10 criteria for DCD [1].
In a more recent study, Roche et al. [40] found no significant differences in the perceptual thresholds for beat-based timing between 6–11-year-old children with and without DCD; however, it is important to note that the threshold estimation procedure used in this study was incongruous with that which is commonly used in adaptive psychophysical measurements [41]. First, Roche et al. [40] used a simple up-down staircase procedure where one positive or negative response influenced the level of difficulty on the subsequent trial. Compared to the more robust two- or three-up procedures [42,43], the one-up algorithm targets only the 50% level of performance. Second, the most common way of estimating perceptual thresholds for the staircase procedure is by averaging the responses or turnaround points across trials [41]. Yet, in the Roche et al. [40] study, the threshold was determined at the point when participants provided wrong responses on three trials with the same inter-onset-interval. Therefore, the lack of group differences reported by this study may be due to the perceptual threshold estimation method used. While these studies shine some light on auditory–perceptual abilities in children with DCD, more research is needed to determine whether these children indeed have auditory perceptual timing difficulties and whether this relates to duration timing, beat-based timing, or both.

3.2. Motor Timing

Eight studies examined motor timing in children with DCD, five of which employed a self-paced task, while three employed a continuation paradigm [44]. In a self-paced task, individuals select a tapping or other movement rate to maintain without any external sensory reference (e.g., auditory or visual cues). In contrast, a continuation paradigm requires participants to first synchronize their movements to a series of isochronous tones (synchronization phase) and then to continue their movements at the same frequency after the auditory cue stops (continuation phase). The level of motor consistency or variability during the continuation phase is thought to provide insight into internal timing mechanisms [45].

3.2.1. Self-Paced Movements

As expected, children with DCD exhibited more variability in their motor timing across self-paced motor timing tasks, including simple tapping tasks [46] as well as bimanual [22,47] and interlimb movements [48,49]. When comparing hand–hand and hand–foot coordination patterns, children with DCD demonstrated greater variability in the latter during both the in-phase (limbs moving simultaneously) and anti-phase (limbs alternating) tasks [48]. Additionally, one study found that variability in self-paced motor timing increased among “clumsy” children when they were required to change their self-selected speed, particularly when they were instructed to slow down [46]. This is consistent with the literature showing that, in addition to exhibiting greater movement variability, 6–11-year-old children show a preference towards faster tapping tempos compared to adults [50,51], suggesting that there is a developmental progression from matching the faster frequencies to matching the slower ones even when mastering simple unpaced tapping tasks.

3.2.2. Continuation Paradigm

All three studies that used the continuation paradigm found that “clumsy” children were more variable in maintaining a set frequency of tapping to a predefined rhythm in both unimanual [38,39] and bimanual rhythmic tapping tasks [52]. In one study, “clumsy” children were grouped based on the assumed etiology, and those with cerebellar soft signs showed increased difficulty in internal timing mechanisms compared to those with basal ganglia soft signs who were instead more variable on the force task [38]. Another study found that both the young “clumsy” children and young controls (6–7 years) were more variable than the older “clumsy” children and their age-matched controls (9–10 years) [39]. However, children in this study with motor difficulties always exhibited greater variability than their age-matched controls. Both studies discuss their results in the context of the information processing framework [53]. Specifically, they propose that the source of continuation motor timing difficulties may be a central timekeeping mechanism (i.e., internal clock). On the contrary, Geuze and Kalverboer [52] found no significant difference in the measures of tapping stability on a range of unimanual and bimanual tapping tasks between clinical groups. The reason for this may be their inclusion criteria, as they compared children who are “clumsy” with children with dyslexia and with age-matched children without reading or motor difficulties; however, all children were pooled from a school for children with specific learning difficulties. Therefore, it is possible that children across all groups had some level of motor challenges.

3.2.3. Intrinsic Feedback

Studies have also examined the influence of auditory and visual input on motor performance by manipulating the amount of task-intrinsic feedback, i.e., auditory and visual cues generated by the task itself. It is important to emphasize that these experiments can also be considered within the auditory–motor timing domain; however, since they did not specifically examine the effects of external sensory cues (e.g., coming from a metronome or a computer) on motor responses, we categorised them under the umbrella of motor timing. Those studies used noise-cancelling headphones and/or a blindfold to eliminate the participants’ ability to see or hear their movement actions [47,49]. While children with DCD exhibited increased variability compared to their age-matched controls and adult controls, the removal of self-generated sounds or vision during a self-paced tapping task [47] and a gross-motor coordination task [49] did not affect performance in any of the groups. Thus, these studies suggested there is no role of intrinsic visual and auditory perception in the consistency of motor timing. Of course, in these studies, it was not possible to eliminate tactile sensory feedback. Interestingly, children with DCD were substantially more variable when visual or visual–auditory intrinsic feedback was available, compared to auditory alone [47], suggesting that auditory–motor and visual–motor integration processes are distinct. Therefore, the availability of visual and/or auditory intrinsic feedback does not seem to affect the variability of motor coordination in self-paced tasks [47,49].

3.3. Auditory–Motor Timing

Auditory and motor brain networks interact during passive listening as well as during generation of motor movements to predictable rhythmic auditory stimuli, promoting integration of the auditory and motor systems [27,54]. Given that interactions between auditory and motor systems underlie many functional everyday behaviours, including speech, reacting to traffic noises, or note-taking during a lecture, vulnerabilities in auditory–motor timing may significantly affect a child’s adaptive functioning and quality of life. Auditory–motor coupling has commonly been studied using a simple finger-tapping paradigm. In this task, participants are asked to synchronize the finger taps of their dominant hand to an external auditory cue (e.g., metronome) [54,55,56,57]. The key indicator of accurate sensorimotor timing is tapping slightly ahead of the beat (a phenomenon called negative mean asynchrony) [55] which implies that, once the motor responses become entrained by the auditory stimuli, the participant anticipates rather than reacts to rhythmic auditory stimuli [58,59]. The primary finding of the studies that used synchronization paradigms was that children with DCD exhibited greater trial-level variability in the difference between their motor movement and the pacing stimuli, although the average synchronization accuracy (the ability to match tapping rate) across trials did not differ between groups [23,40,60].

3.3.1. Synchronization Paradigm

Compared to their age-matched controls and adult controls, children with DCD had difficulties matching motor movements to auditory rhythms when producing bilateral antiphase movements [23] and when coordinating their limbs in a gross-motor coordination task [60]. They were most variable when matching the lowest frequency during a tapping task [23]. In contrast, during a gross-motor coordination task involving clapping and marching to an auditory beat, higher frequencies resulted in greater variability [60]. These differences may be due to different biomechanical constraints that underlie discrete versus continuous rhythmic movements. Additionally, developmental effects between the groups were observed in both studies [23,60], manifested by greater synchronization accuracy and negative mean asynchrony (tapping ahead of the beat) in adult controls, while children with and without DCD tended to tap behind the beat. Across studies, when the frequency of the auditory cue was sped up, children with DCD showed less stable motor patterns in unimanual [46] and bimanual in-phase and anti-phase tapping tasks [22] as well as when performing hand–hand and hand–foot coordination patterns, with the least stability in the latter [48]. The differential effect of frequency rate is further explored in the discussion.
Roche et al. [40] examined auditory–motor synchronization abilities using a task that required children to adapt their responses to perceptible and subliminal rhythmic changes in auditory stimuli. They found that children in both groups were equally able to adjust their tapping to gradual and abrupt changes in phasing, although children with DCD had greater trial-by-trial variability in both conditions. In other studies that aimed to differentiate subconscious and conscious adaptations in healthy adults, it was reported that frequency fluctuations of <5% (and possibly 7%) are perceived on a subliminal level and, thus, result in subconscious adaptations of tapping patterns, whereas large frequency fluctuations of 20% are reliably detected and prompt conscious adaptations of motor actions [61,62,63]. For that reason, it is possible that the stimulus phase change of 12.5% in Roche et al. [40] might have elicited preconscious rather than conscious adaptations for some participants. Finally, a synchronization task was employed to examine the ability to selectively inhibit symmetrical movements (i.e., mirror movements) while switching from bimanual to unimanual tapping. In this experiment [64], both control and DCD groups showed improvements in their ability to inhibit left finger taps with practice; however, children with DCD produced a significantly higher number of supplementary movements and showed no age-related improvement in their capacity to inhibit left finger taps.

3.3.2. Reaction Time

Reaction time (RT) tasks have also been used as an indicator of auditory–motor processes [65,66,67]. For example, RT was measured to assess how auditory stimuli influence the performance in an aiming task (i.e., moving a finger from the start position to the target button) as part of a multisensory paradigm [67]. Reaction times to auditory stimuli were faster compared to visual but slower compared to multisensory auditory–visual stimuli in both groups, though children with DCD reacted slower in all conditions. O’Brien et al. [65] also found that children with DCD provided slower responses than controls under all three sensory conditions (i.e., auditory, visual, and vibrotactile); however, their performance did not result in more performance errors and was not more variable compared to controls. The authors noted that all children received extensive practice prior to the experiment, which might have contributed to the lack of group difference. Interestingly, young children with DCD (6–7 years) exhibited faster responses to auditory stimuli than did their age-matched controls, and also had significantly higher receptive vocabulary scores. This effect was not found in older groups (9–10 years). Additionally, Rosenblum and Regev [66] found that children with DCD exhibited significantly longer response times when adjusting their hands and feet to the auditory beat, which also predicted their handwriting performance.

3.3.3. Sensorimotor Learning

Lastly, a recent study by Blais et al. [21] examined the effect of auditory stimuli on learning, retention, and reactivation of perceptual–motor non-isochronous sequences. Children were given 30 practice trials in which they synchronized their movements to patterns of auditory or visual stimuli followed by an immediate and delayed recollection of the patterns without external cues. While both groups of children showed more significant improvements in temporal accuracy and stability during practice with auditory compared to visual stimuli, children with DCD demonstrated a significant increase in errors during recall of auditory versus visual patterns (i.e., after the auditory stimuli were removed). In other words, while learning and retention of a visual sequence were preserved, auditory sequence retention appears to be a specific vulnerability in children with DCD. Therefore, it is plausible that children with DCD benefit more from auditory information during learning but not when memorizing a temporal sequence.
Table 2. Key characteristics of included studies.
Table 2. Key characteristics of included studies.
Author(s)Sample (N)
(Female/Male)
Age (Years)Motor Test
(Cut-Off)
Experimental TaskTiming
Modality
Key Findings
Geuze &
Kalverboer, 1987
[46]
“Clumsy”: 17
(4 F, 13 M)
Controls: 17
(5 F, 12 M)
7–11Teacher’s ratings on Motor Impairment Test
  • Self-paced
  • Synchronization/adaptation: unimanual tapping (64, 84, 104 bpm)
Motor,
Auditory-
motor
Self-paced: ↑ variability (especially slow)
Synchronization: ↑ variability (especially fast)
Similar variability on both tapping tasks
Lundy-Ekman et al., 1991
[38]
“Clumsy”: 25
(11 BG signs, 14 CB signs)
Controls: 14
7–8Screening for soft and hard signs; BOT–Short Form
  • Perception: duration timing
  • Continuation: unimanual tapping
  • Force
Auditory,
Motor
  • (CB) Perception: ↓ accuracy
  • (CB) Continuation: ↑ variability
  • (BG) Force: ↑ variability
Williams et al., 1992
[39]
“Clumsy”: 12
(6 Y, 6 O)
Controls: 13
(6 Y, 7 O)
Y: 6–7
O: 9–10
BOT–Short Form
  • Perception: duration and loudness
  • Continuation: unimanual tapping
  • Force
Auditory,
Motor
Perception: ↓ accuracy
Continuation: ↑ variability
Age differences in variability
Geuze &
Kalverboer, 1994
[52]
“Clumsy”: 11
Dyslexic: 12
“Controls”: 12
7–12TOMI: DCD > 4.5, Control < 4
  • Continuation: unimanual, alternating bimanual, 2:1 or 1:2 asymmetrical tapping
MotorContinuation: ↑ variability (especially slow)
No significant difference: “Clumsy” → Dyslexic → “Controls” All groups attended a school for children with specific learning difficulties
Volman & Geuze, 1998
[22]
DCD: 24
Controls: 24
7–12MABC < 15th percentile
Touwen Test
  • Self-paced bimanual tapping: in-phase/anti-phase, perturbation
  • Synchronization/adaptation: anti-phase hand–hand patterns
Motor,
Auditory-
motor
Self-paced: ↑ variability of coordination pattern
Synchronization/adaptation: ↑ variability
The critical frequency ↓ in DCD than controls
Volman et al., 2006
[48]
DCD: 10
Controls: 16
5–8MABC < 15th percentile
  • Self-paced: in-phase, anti-phase
  • Synchronization/adaptation (anti-phase): hand–hand, hand–foot (ipsilateral and contralateral)
Motor,
Auditory-
motor
Self-paced: ↑ variability
Scaled frequency: ↑ variability (all combinations)
  • Non-homologous patterns less stable
Whitall et al., 2006
[60]
DCD: 10
Controls: 8
Adult controls: 10
6–8
18–30
MABC < 10th percentile
  • Synchronization: gross-motor coordination (clapping, marching) at 0.8, 1.2, 1.6 and 2.0 Hz
Auditory-
motor
Synchronization: ↑ variability (especially fast)
Similar mean error: DCD → Controls → Adults
All groups: hands more stably locked to the beat
Whitall et al., 2008
[23]
DCD: 10
Controls: 10
Adult controls: 10
(3 F, 7 M)
6–8
21–35
MABC < 15th percentile
  • Synchronization: bilateral antiphase finger tapping at 0.8, 1.6, 2.4, 3.2 Hz per finger
Auditory-
motor
Synchronization: ↑ variability (especially slow)
All groups able to modulate their finger frequency
Adults tapping ahead→ DCD/controls behind
Mackenzie et al., 2008
[49]
DCD: 11
Controls: 7
Adult controls: 10
6–8
18–30
MABC < 10th percentile
  • Self-paced: gross-motor coordination (clapping in-phase to marching) with vision and hearing, no vision, no hearing, and without both
Motor↑ variability but no significant condition effects
All groups: ↓ variability marching vs. clapping
DCD/controls: foot–foot coherence equally stable
O’Brien et al., 2008
[65]
Y DCD: 7
Y controls: 10
O DCD: 9
O controls: 10
Y: 6–7
O: 9–10
MABC < 15th percentile
  • Synchronization RT to visual, auditory, and vibrotactile stimuli: compatible/incompatible
Auditory-
motor
DCD RTs ↑ under all three sensory conditions
No difference in RT variability or errors
  • Young DCD: Faster to auditory than controls
Roche et al., 2011
[47]
DCD: 10
Controls: 10
Adults: 10
(3 F, 7 M)
6–8
21–35
MABC < 15th percentile
PANESS
  • Self-paced anti-phase tapping: vision and audition (VA), no vision (A), no audition (V), no vision and audition
MotorDCD ↓ accuracy, ↑ variability vs. controls/adults
Sensory conditions did not affect performance
  • DCD ↑ variability with V or VA feedback
Rosenblum & Regev, 2013
[66]
DCD: 21
Controls: 21
(8 F, 13 M)
7–10MABC < 15th percentile
  • Synchronization to IM: 14 tasks
  • Handwriting: three tasks
Auditory-
motor
RTs ↑ for 11 out of 14 IM tasks
Handwriting performance variance ↑
  • Mean total RT predicts the handwriting variance
Tallet et al., 2013
[64]
DCD:12
(4 F, 8 M)
Controls: 12
(4 F, 8 M)
7–10MABC < 15th percentile
  • Synchronization/Inhibition: switching from bimanual symmetrical tapping to unimanual tapping
Auditory-
motor
Similar mean self-selected tempo
DCD number of supplementary taps significantly ↑
Left finger inhibition: improvement through trials
Coats et al., 2015
[67]
DCD: 10
(4 F, 6 M)
Controls: 10
(5 F, 5 M)
7–10 MABC < 9th percentile
  • Synchronization RT (aiming task): unimodal visual (V), auditory (A), and bimodal condition (AV)
Auditory-
motor
V (slowest) → A→ AV stimulus (fastest)
  • Movement planning: both groups benefit (AV)
  • Movement control: only controls benefit (AV)
Roche et al., 2016
[40]
DCD: 24 (19)
Controls: 22 (17)
6–11MABC < 5th percentile
PANESS
  • Perception: beat-based timing
  • Synchronization/adaptation (bimanual anti-phase tapping): gradual/abrupt conditions
Auditory,
Auditory-
motor
Auditory perception: No difference
Synchronization/adaptation: No difference
  • DCD significantly more variable in all conditions
Blais et al., 2021
[21]
DCD: 12
Controls: 20
8–12MABC < 5th percentile
  • Synchronization: learning, retention, and reactivation (auditory or visual)
  • Control task: Isochronous sequence
Auditory-
motor
  • Practice: ↑ stability audio-motor sequence
  • Immediate retention: DCD ↑ errors auditory
  • Visuo-motor sequence equally learned, retained and reactivated in both groups
Note. DCD = developmental coordination disorder; Clumsy = the term used to describe children with motor coordination difficulties before “DCD” started being used in 1994; BG = Basal ganglia; CB = cerebellum; Y = young; O = old; TOMI = Test of Motor Impairment; BOT–Short Form = Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency; MABC = The Movement Assessment Battery for Children [68]; MABC percentiles = the cut-off point of < 16th percentile indicates motor coordination difficulties (lower percentile indicates more severe difficulties); PANESS = Physical and Neurological Examination of Subtle Signs [69]; F = female; M = male; RT = reaction time; A = auditory; V = visual; AV = auditory–visual condition; IM = Interactive Metronome [70].

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first review of associations between auditory–perceptual abilities and motor timing in DCD. We identified 16 studies that were included based on the timing modality studied (i.e., auditory–perceptual, motor, or auditory–motor). Only three studies assessed auditory perceptual abilities of children with DCD separate from their motor or auditory–motor performance, leading to insufficient and inconsistent findings. Within the motor timing domain, five studies employed a self-paced task and three used a continuation paradigm, while eleven studies employed synchronization paradigms or reaction time tasks as an indicator of auditory–motor processes. The key finding of these studies is that children with DCD exhibit more variability both in maintaining a set frequency of tapping to a predefined rhythm as well as when matching their movement to an external auditory cue, compared to their age-matched controls.

4.1. Motor Variability with and without External Auditory Stimuli

This review further confirms that variability in and slowness of motor response are key characteristics of DCD, regardless of the experimental task. From a theoretical standpoint, perceptual–motor timing abilities have been examined using two dominant perspectives: information processing and the dynamical systems theory. The information processing framework has mainly been considered in earlier studies using the Wing–Kristofferson timekeeper model [44]. Those studies suggest that the increased motor variability observed in children with DCD is a result of a deficit in motor programming (i.e., central timekeeping processes) rather than processes related to motor execution [38,39,46,52]. In contrast, the dynamic pattern perspective explains those coordination difficulties in terms of the interplay between the internal and external forces [71,72,73] where reduced temporal motor stability indicates difficulties in dynamic movement control [22,23,40,48]. Given that these two theoretical frameworks account for different aspects of perceptual-motor timing, their relevance primarily depends on the task demands [74].
Taken together, our results are consistent in finding that children with DCD have difficulties coordinating their movements both with external auditory stimuli [23,40,60] and when maintaining a movement pattern without external cues [38,39,52]. The overall slowness of movements has been most evident in studies that used reaction-time paradigms [65,66,67]. It is interesting to note that there were no significant differences between the groups with respect to the mean self-selected tapping frequency [22,47,64]. Combined, the extant literature also points to a differential effect of frequency rate on motor variability across tasks. For example, children with DCD seem to become more variable (i.e., less stable) at faster frequencies when matching the auditory beat in tapping synchronization paradigms [22,46]. In contrast, slower frequencies present more difficulty during unpaced tapping tasks [46,52]. Although these studies often make inferences about the auditory perceptual abilities of children with DCD based on either a motor or an auditory–motor task, our findings indicate a paucity of research focusing specifically on the role of auditory perception in motor coordination tasks.
As noted earlier, auditory–motor variability could arise as a consequence of inefficient time perception, motor execution, and/or sensorimotor coupling. Therefore, in addition to testing auditory timing separately, future studies should compare motor variability between paced and unpaced tasks to distinguish whether external auditory cues contribute to a more or less stable performance. Since auditory processing develops earlier [75] and is associated with faster reaction times compared to visual information [76], auditory interventions may be strong candidates for improving motor coordination in children with DCD. This knowledge could be clinically relevant considering the implications of rhythmic auditory stimuli seen in movement rehabilitation with people with Parkinson’s disease [77,78], traumatic brain injury [79], stroke [80,81], and cerebral palsy [82]. These studies suggest that rhythmic auditory cues increase temporal stability and speed, facilitating the planning and execution of movements in persons with movement disorders, thus proving to be an effective therapeutic tool.

4.2. Auditory Perceptual Abilities in Children with DCD

Only three out of sixteen studies tested perceptual discrimination abilities using an auditory timing task in addition to motor-related paradigms, and their results are inconclusive. Two of those studies found that children with DCD have decreased acuity (i.e., higher thresholds) on duration timing tasks, while the study testing perceptual thresholds for beat-based timing found no group difference. These opposing results are likely due to the differences in the psychophysical procedures and inclusion criteria used. We identified two additional studies that examined auditory perceptual abilities using electrophysiology measures in children aged 6–7 years with (probable) DCD but did not meet the inclusion criteria for this review. Holeckova et al. [83] found differences in event-related potentials [84] during a passive auditory oddball task indicating that children with DCD have reduced ability to detect and evaluate deviations in the stimuli (they reported no mismatch negativity response and a reduction of P3 amplitude in this group). Chang et al. [85] used a more comprehensive design and measured perceptual discrimination thresholds for duration timing, rhythm timing, and pitch (control) using both behavioural and electrophysiology measures. They found that children at risk for DCD have larger discrimination thresholds (i.e., decreased acuity) for both the duration and rhythm-based timing compared to controls, which was also reflected by the delayed mismatch negativity for duration deviations and delayed P3a latencies in response to rhythm deviants. Both studies provide support for the auditory–perceptual timing hypothesis [24], which suggests that, in addition to motor deficits, auditory–perceptual timing deficits appear to be the core of the disorder as well; however, current evidence is insufficient, and more research is needed to understand the brain-behaviour association related to auditory–perceptual abilities and whether potential difficulties apply to both duration- and beat-based timing.

4.3. Limitations and Future Directions

To capture all relevant studies, the date of publication was not an exclusion criterion in our review. However, we recognize that the criteria for study sample inclusion differed in research conducted before 1994, when an international panel of experts agreed to the term “developmental coordination disorder” to describe children with significant motor coordination difficulties. We encountered a wide variation in terminology across earlier studies, and kept the original terms (i.e., clumsy) when describing their findings. Since then, a score < 16th percentile on The Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC) [68] has been used as a cut-off point for diagnosing DCD (lower percentile indicates greater severity), in addition to other criteria outlined in The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.) [1]. As shown in Table 2, even among studies that used the MABC, the diagnostic cut-off scores varied from the 5th to the 15th percentiles. Given the variation in the terminology and diagnostic criteria used, direct comparisons among studies should be interpreted with caution. Moreover, only a few studies screened for possible comorbidities; therefore, it is possible that performance differences may have been confounded with difficulties in other domains such as attention or working memory. Only four studies controlled for the effects of prior music training on the auditory–motor performance, and this was done mainly for adult controls [21,23,47,49]. The degree to which music training can induce changes in auditory–motor coupling in children with DCD has yet to be studied. Nevertheless, research suggests that extensive music training can gradually affect the manner in which the periodicities of a musical rhythm are perceived and performed, thereby enhancing auditory–motor coupling [25,86]. Therefore, future studies should assess and control for possible effects of music training. Finally, considering that many of the studies provided limited information regarding individual demographic and environmental differences, we currently do not know the extent to which the results are generalizable to the overall population.
In conclusion, the selected studies provide important insights regarding the contribution of three timing modalities that underlie efficient auditory–motor coupling. However, caution is needed when comparing and interpreting these results due to the considerable methodological heterogeneity that is present in these studies (see Table 2), especially given that children with DCD are a rather heterogeneous group. Finally, all the identified studies employed behavioural measures only. Therefore, insights regarding the brain-behaviour correlates of auditory–motor coupling are limited and necessitate future research.

5. Conclusions

This review aimed to summarize what is known about auditory–motor timing in children with DCD. The extent to which auditory perception relates to motor variability in children with DCD has largely been understudied. Therefore, limited conclusions can be made about the specific contributions of auditory perceptual abilities in this population, especially due to the heterogeneity of DCD as well as the methodological heterogeneity of the included studies. Further research is needed to evaluate the associations between paced and unpaced tasks so that the distinct contribution of auditory timing can be discerned. Considering that auditory interventions show promise for improving movement in other clinical samples [30], understanding the contribution of auditory perception and the effects of auditory stimuli on motor control in children with DCD will help inform whether auditory–perceptual training might be a plausible therapeutic intervention.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci13050729/s1, Table S1: Search Strategy; Table S2: Title and Abstract Screening; and Table S3: Full-Text Screening [83,85,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P. and M.H.T.; search strategy, screening, and data extraction, M.P. and N.H.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P.; writing—review and editing, A.B.A., N.H. and M.H.T.; supervision, A.B.A. and M.H.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Sheena Odabashian for her help with designing the graphical abstract and Tom Chau for his valuable comments and support of this research. The first author acknowledges the financial support received from the Bloorview Research Institute and the Azrieli Foundation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
  2. Smits-Engelsman, B.; Schoemaker, M.; Delabastita, T.; Hoskens, J.; Geuze, R. Diagnostic Criteria for DCD: Past and Future. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2015, 42, 293–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Cacola, P. Physical and Mental Health of Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Cairney, J. Developmental Coordination Disorder and Its Consequences; University of Toronto Press: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  5. Li, Y.-C.; Kwan, M.Y.W.; King-Dowling, S.; Rodriguez, M.C.; Cairney, J. Does Physical Activity and BMI Mediate the Association between DCD and Internalizing Problems in Early Childhood? A Partial Test of the Environmental Stress Hypothesis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2021, 75, 102744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gibbs, J.; Appleton, J.; Appleton, R. Dyspraxia or Developmental Coordination Disorder? Unravelling the Enigma. Arch. Dis. Child. 2007, 92, 534–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Blank, R.; Smits-Engelsman, B.; Polatajko, H.; Wilson, P. European Academy for Childhood Disability (EACD): Recommendations on the Definition, Diagnosis and Intervention of Developmental Coordination Disorder (Long Version). Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012, 54, 54–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Dewey, D. What Is Comorbidity and Why Does It Matter in Neurodevelopmental Disorders? Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2018, 5, 235–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Wolpert, D.M.; Miall, R.C.; Kawato, M. Internal Models in the Cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998, 2, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Mariën, P.; Wackenier, P.; De Surgeloose, D.; De Deyn, P.P.; Verhoeven, J. Developmental Coordination Disorder: Disruption of the Cerebello-Cerebral Network Evidenced by SPECT. Cerebellum 2010, 9, 405–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zwicker, J.G.; Missiuna, C.; Harris, S.R.; Boyd, L.A. Brain Activation Associated with Motor Skill Practice in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: An FMRI Study. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2011, 29, 145–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brown-Lum, M.; Zwicker, J.G. Brain Imaging Increases Our Understanding of Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Review of Literature and Future Directions. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2015, 2, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Biotteau, M.; Chaix, Y.; Blais, M.; Tallet, J.; Péran, P.; Albaret, J.-M. Neural Signature of DCD: A Critical Review of MRI Neuroimaging Studies. Front. Neurol. 2016, 7, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Debrabant, J.; Gheysen, F.; Caeyenberghs, K.; Van Waelvelde, H.; Vingerhoets, G. Neural Underpinnings of Impaired Predictive Motor Timing in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 1478–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Langevin, L.M.; MacMaster, F.P.; Crawford, S.; Lebel, C.; Dewey, D. Common White Matter Microstructure Alterations in Pediatric Motor and Attention Disorders. J. Pediatr. 2014, 164, 1157–1164.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Adams, I.L.J.; Lust, J.M.; Wilson, P.H.; Steenbergen, B. Compromised Motor Control in Children with DCD: A Deficit in the Internal Model?—A Systematic Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Wilson, P.H.; Ruddock, S.; Smits-Engelsman, B.; Polatajko, H.; Blank, R. Understanding Performance Deficits in Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Recent Research. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012, 55, 217–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wolpert, D.; Ghahramani, Z.; Jordan, M. An Internal Model for Sensorimotor Integration. Science 1995, 269, 1880–1882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Miall, R.C.; Wolpert, D.M. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Netw. 1996, 9, 1265–1279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Shadmehr, R.; Smith, M.A.; Krakauer, J.W. Error Correction, Sensory Prediction, and Adaptation in Motor Control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 33, 89–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Blais, M.; Jucla, M.; Maziero, S.; Albaret, J.-M.; Chaix, Y.; Tallet, J. The Differential Effects of Auditory and Visual Stimuli on Learning, Retention and Reactivation of a Perceptual-Motor Temporal Sequence in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 616795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Volman, M.J.M.; Geuze, R.H. Relative Phase Stability of Bimanual and Visuomanual Rhythmic Coordination Patterns in Children with a Developmental Coordination Disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1998, 17, 541–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Whitall, J.; Chang, T.-Y.; Horn, C.L.; Jung-Potter, J.; McMenamin, S.; Wilms-Floet, A.; Clark, J.E. Auditory-Motor Coupling of Bilateral Finger Tapping in Children with and without DCD Compared to Adults. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 914–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Trainor, L.J.; Chang, A.; Cairney, J.; Li, Y.-C. Is Auditory Perceptual Timing a Core Deficit of Developmental Coordination Disorder? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1423, 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, J.L.; Penhune, V.B.; Zatorre, R.J. Listening to Musical Rhythms Recruits Motor Regions of the Brain. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 2844–2854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Grahn, J.A.; Brett, M. Rhythm and Beat Perception in Motor Areas of the Brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 893–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Thaut, M.H. Neural Basis of Rhythmic Timing Networks in the Human Brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2003, 999, 364–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Ashoori, A.; Eagleman, D.M.; Jankovic, J. Effects of Auditory Rhythm and Music on Gait Disturbances in Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Neurol. 2015, 6, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Devlin, K.; Alshaikh, J.T.; Pantelyat, A. Music Therapy and Music-Based Interventions for Movement Disorders. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 2019, 19, 83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Thaut, M.H.; Abiru, M. Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation in Rehabilitation of Movement Disorders: A Review of Current Research. Music Percept. 2010, 27, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Arnett, A.B.; Hudac, C.; DesChamps, T.; Cairney, B.E.; Gerdts, J.; Wallace, A.S.; Bernier, R.A.; Webb, S.J. Auditory Perception Is Associated with Implicit Language Learning and Receptive Language Ability in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Lang. 2018, 187, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. O’Connor, K. Auditory Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 836–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Goswami, U. A Temporal Sampling Framework for Developmental Dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Noreika, V.; Falter, C.M.; Rubia, K. Timing Deficits in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Evidence from Neurocognitive and Neuroimaging Studies. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 235–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lense, M.D.; Ladányi, E.; Rabinowitch, T.-C.; Trainor, L.; Gordon, R. Rhythm and Timing as Vulnerabilities in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2021, 376, 20200327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Peters, M.D.J.; Marnie, C.; Tricco, A.C.; Pollock, D.; Munn, Z.; Alexander, L.; McInerney, P.; Godfrey, C.M.; Khalil, H. Updated Methodological Guidance for the Conduct of Scoping Reviews. JBI Evid. Synth. 2020, 18, 2119–2126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Tricco, A.C.; Lillie, E.; Zarin, W.; O’Brien, K.; Colquhoun, H.; Kastner, M.; Levac, D.; Ng, C.; Sharpe, J.P.; Wilson, K.; et al. A Scoping Review on the Conduct and Reporting of Scoping Reviews. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2016, 16, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Lundy-Ekman, L.; Ivry, R.; Keele, S.; Woollacott, M. Timing and Force Control Deficits in Clumsy Children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1991, 3, 367–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Williams, H.G.; Woollacott, M.H.; Ivry, R. Timing and Motor Control in Clumsy Children. J. Mot. Behav. 1992, 24, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Roche, R.; Viswanathan, P.; Clark, J.E.; Whitall, J. Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) Can Adapt to Perceptible and Subliminal Rhythm Changes but Are More Variable. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2016, 50, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Treutwein, B. Adaptive Psychophysical Procedures. Vis. Res. 1995, 35, 2503–2522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Levitt, H. Transformed Up-down Methods in Psychoacoustics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1971, 49, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Leek, M.R. Adaptive Procedures in Psychophysical Research. Percept. Psychophys. 2001, 63, 1279–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Wing, A.M.; Kristofferson, A.B. Response Delays and the Timing of Discrete Motor Responses. Percept. Psychophys. 1973, 14, 5–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Drake, C.; Jones, M.R.; Baruch, C. The Development of Rhythmic Attending in Auditory Sequences: Attunement, Referent Period, Focal Attending. Cognition 2000, 77, 251–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Geuze, R.H.; Kalverboer, A.F. Inconsistency and Adaptation in Timing of Clumsy Children. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 1987, 13, 421–432. [Google Scholar]
  47. Roche, R.; Wilms-Floet, A.M.; Clark, J.E.; Whitall, J. Auditory and Visual Information Do Not Affect Self-Paced Bilateral Finger Tapping in Children with DCD. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2011, 30, 658–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Volman, M.J.M.; Laroy, M.E.; Jongmans, M.J. Rhythmic Coordination of Hand and Foot in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Child Care Health Dev. 2006, 32, 693–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mackenzie, S.J.; Getchell, N.; Deutsch, K.; Wilms-Floet, A.; Clark, J.E.; Whitall, J. Multi-Limb Coordination and Rhythmic Variability under Varying Sensory Availability Conditions in Children with DCD. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 256–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Baruch, C.; Panissal-Vieu, N.; Drake, C. Preferred Perceptual Tempo for Sound Sequences: Comparison of Adults, Children, and Infants. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2004, 98, 325–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. McAuley, J.D.; Jones, M.R.; Holub, S.; Johnston, H.M.; Miller, N.S. The Time of Our Lives: Life Span Development of Timing and Event Tracking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 348–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Geuze, R.H.; Kalverboer, A.F. Tapping a Rhythm: A Problem of Timing for Children Who Are Clumsy and Dyslexic? Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 1994, 11, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Vorberg, D.; Wing, A. Modeling Variability and Dependence in Timing. In Handbook of Perception and Action; Heuer, H., Keele, S.W., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996; Volume 2, pp. 181–262. [Google Scholar]
  54. Repp, B.H.; Su, Y.-H. Sensorimotor Synchronization: A Review of Recent Research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013, 20, 403–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Aschersleben, G. Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization. Brain Cogn. 2002, 48, 66–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Thaut, M.H.; Miller, R.A.; Schauer, L.M. Multiple Synchronization Strategies in Rhythmic Sensorimotor Tasks: Phase vs Period Correction. Biol. Cybern. 1998, 79, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Thaut, M.H.; Tian, B.; Azimi-Sadjadi, M.R. Rhythmic Finger Tapping to Cosine-Wave Modulated Metronome Sequences: Evidence of Subliminal Entrainment. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1998, 17, 839–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Fraisse, P. Rhythm and Tempo. Psychol. Music 1982, 1, 149–180. [Google Scholar]
  59. Aschersleben, G.; Prinz, W. Synchronizing Actions with Events: The Role of Sensory Information. Percept. Psychophys. 1995, 57, 305–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Whitall, J.; Getchell, N.; McMenamin, S.; Horn, C.; Wilms-Floet, A.; Clark, J.E. Perception–Action Coupling in Children with and without DCD: Frequency Locking Between Task-Relevant Auditory Signals and Motor Responses in a Dual-Motor Task. Child Care Health Dev. 2006, 32, 679–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Tecchio, F.; Salustri, C.; Thaut, M.H.; Pasqualetti, P.; Rossini, P.M. Conscious and Preconscious Adaptation to Rhythmic Auditory Stimuli: A Magnetoencephalographic Study of Human Brain Responses. Exp. Brain Res. 2000, 135, 222–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Stephan, K.M.; Thaut, M.H.; Wunderlich, G.; Schicks, W.; Tian, B.; Tellmann, L.; Schmitz, T.; Herzog, H.; McIntosh, G.C.; Seitz, R.J.; et al. Conscious and Subconscious Sensorimotor Synchronization—Prefrontal Cortex and the Influence of Awareness. NeuroImage 2002, 15, 345–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Thaut, M.H.; Stephan, K.M.; Wunderlich, G.; Schicks, W.; Tellmann, L.; Herzog, H.; McIntosh, G.C.; Seitz, R.J.; Hömberg, V. Distinct Cortico-Cerebellar Activations in Rhythmic Auditory Motor Synchronization. Cortex 2009, 45, 44–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Tallet, J.; Albaret, J.-M.; Barral, J. Developmental Changes in Lateralized Inhibition of Symmetric Movements in Children with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 2523–2532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. O’Brien, J.C.; Williams, H.G.; Bundy, A.; Lyons, J.; Mittal, A. Mechanisms That Underlie Coordination in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. J. Mot. Behav. 2008, 40, 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Rosenblum, S.; Regev, N. Timing Abilities among Children with Developmental Coordination Disorders (DCD) in Comparison to Children with Typical Development. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 218–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Coats, R.O.A.; Britten, L.; Utley, A.; Astill, S.L. Multisensory Integration in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2015, 43, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Henderson, S.E.; Sugden, D.; Barnett, A.L. Movement assessment battery for Children-2. APA PsycTests 2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Denckla, M.B. Revised Neurological Examination for Subtle Signs. Psychopharmacol Bull 1985, 21, 773–800. [Google Scholar]
  70. Bartscherer, M.L.; Dole, R.L. Interactive Metronome® Training for a 9-Year-Old Boy with Attention and Motor Coordination Difficulties. Physiother. Theory Pract. 2005, 21, 257–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Kelso, S. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  72. Beek, P.J.; Peper, C.E.; Stegeman, D.F. Dynamical Models of Movement Coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1995, 14, 573–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Schöner, G.; Kelso, J.A.S. Dynamic Pattern Generation in Behavioral and Neural Systems. Science 1988, 239, 1513–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Torre, K.; Balasubramaniam, R. Two Different Processes for Sensorimotor Synchronization in Continuous and Discontinuous Rhythmic Movements. Exp. Brain Res. 2009, 199, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Turkewitz, G.; Kenny, P.A. Limitations on Input as a Basis for Neural Organization and Perceptual Development: A Preliminary Theoretical Statement. Dev. Psychobiol. J. Int. Soc. Dev. Psychobiol. 1982, 15, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Welford, W.T.; Brebner, J.M.; Kirby, N. Reaction Times; Stanford University: Stanford, CA, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  77. McIntosh, G.C.; Brown, S.H.; Rice, R.R.; Thaut, M.H. Rhythmic Auditory-Motor Facilitation of Gait Patterns in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1997, 62, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Thaut, M.H.; McIntosh, G.C.; Rice, R.R.; Miller, R.A.; Rathbun, J.; Brault, J.M. Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation in Gait Training for Parkinson’s Disease Patients. Mov. Disord. 1996, 11, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Hurt, C.P.; Rice, R.R.; McIntosh, G.C.; Thaut, M.H. Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation in Gait Training for Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Music Ther. 1998, 35, 228–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Prassas, S.; Thaut, M.; McIntosh, G.; Rice, R. Effect of Auditory Rhythmic Cuing on Gait Kinematic Parameters of Stroke Patients. Gait Posture 1997, 6, 218–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Thaut, M.H.; McIntosh, G.C. Neurologic Music Therapy in Stroke Rehabilitation. Curr. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Rep. 2014, 2, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Ghai, S.; Ghai, I.; Effenberg, A.O. Effect of Rhythmic Auditory Cueing on Gait in Cerebral Palsy: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2017, 14, 43–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Holeckova, I.; Cepicka, L.; Mautner, P.; Stepanek, D.; Moucek, R. Auditory ERPs in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Act. Nerv. Super. 2014, 56, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Taylor, M.J.; Pourcelot, L. Cognitive Evoked Potentials in Children: Normal and Abnormal Development. Neurophysiol. Clin. 1995, 25, 130–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Chang, A.; Li, Y.; Chan, J.F.; Dotov, D.G.; Cairney, J.; Trainor, L.J. Inferior Auditory Time Perception in Children with Motor Difficulties. Child Dev. 2021, 92, e907–e923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Zatorre, R.J.; Chen, J.L.; Penhune, V.B. When the Brain Plays Music: Auditory–Motor Interactions in Music Perception and Production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 547–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Biotteau, M.; Chaix, Y.; Albaret, J.-M. Procedural Learning and Automatization Process in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder And/or Developmental Dyslexia. Human Mov. Sci. 2015, 43, 78–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Caçola, P.; Getchell, N.; Srinivasan, D.; Alexandrakis, G.; Liu, H. Cortical Activity in Fine-Motor Tasks in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Preliminary FNIRS Study. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2017, 65, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Cosper, S.M.; Lee, G.P.; Peters, S.B.; Bishop, E. Interactive Metronome Training in Children with Attention Deficit and Developmental Coordination Disorders. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 2009, 32, 331–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. de Castelnau, P.; Albaret, J.-M.; Chaix, Y.; Zanone, P.-G. A Study of EEG Coherence in DCD Children during Motor Synchronization Task. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 230–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Hession, C.E.; Law Smith, M.J.; Watterson, D.; Oxley, N.; Murphy, B.A. The Impact of Equine Therapy and an Audio-Visual Approach Emphasizing Rhythm and Beat Perception in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2019, 25, 535–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. King, B.R.; Kagerer, F.A.; Harring, J.R.; Contreras-Vidal, J.L.; Clark, J.E. Multisensory Adaptation of Spatial-To-Motor Transformations in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Exp. Brain Res. 2011, 212, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Lê, M.; Blais, M.; Jucla, M.; Chauveau, N.; Maziero, S.; Biotteau, M.; Albaret, J.; Péran, P.; Chaix, Y.; Tallet, J. Procedural Learning and Retention of Audio-Verbal Temporal Sequence Is Altered in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder but Cortical Thickness Matters. Dev. Sci. 2020, 24, e13009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Leemrijse, C.; Meijer, O.G.; Vermeer, A.; Adèr, H.J.; Diemel, S. The Efficacy of Le Bon Départ and Sensory Integration Treatment for Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Randomized Study with Six Single Cases. Clin. Rehabil. 2000, 14, 247–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Mikami, M.; Hirota, T.; Takahashi, M.; Adachi, M.; Saito, M.; Koeda, S.; Yoshida, K.; Sakamoto, Y.; Kato, S.; Nakamura, K.; et al. Atypical Sensory Processing Profiles and Their Associations with Motor Problems in Preschoolers with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2020, 52, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Niklasson, M.; Niklasson, I.; Norlander, T. Sensorimotor Therapy: Using Stereotypic Movements and Vestibular Stimulation to Increase Sensorimotor Proficiency of Children with Attentional and Motor Difficulties. Percept. Motor Skills 2009, 108, 643–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Niklasson, M.; Norlander, T.; Niklasson, I.; Rasmussen, P. Catching-Up: Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder Compared to Healthy Children before and after Sensorimotor Therapy. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0186126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Piek, J.P.; Skinner, R.A. Timing and Force Control during a Sequential Tapping Task in Children with and without Motor Coordination Problems. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1999, 5, 320–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Sartori, R.F.; Valentini, N.C.; Nobre, G.C.; Fonseca, R.P. Motor and Verbal Inhibitory Control: Development and Validity of the Go/No-Go App Test for Children with Development Coordination Disorder. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2020, 10, 359–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Volman, M.J.M.; Geuze, R.H. Stability of Rhythmic Finger Movements in Children with a Developmental Coordination Disorder. Motor Control 1998, 2, 34–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Selection of sources of evidence (PRISMA-ScR Flowchart).
Figure 1. Selection of sources of evidence (PRISMA-ScR Flowchart).
Brainsci 13 00729 g001
Table 1. Search strategy used for selection of scientific articles in MEDLINE.
Table 1. Search strategy used for selection of scientific articles in MEDLINE.
DatabaseMEDLINE (Ovid)
CoverageMedicine and Health
LimitsPublication Type: “Article”, Language: “English”, Age: “Children <18”
Search QueryPopulation: DCDANDConcept: Auditory-Motor Timing
HeadingsKeywordsHeadingsKeywords
exp Motor Skills Disorders/Developmental coordination disorder.mp. DCD.mp.
(developmental coordination adj3 (disorder* or problem* or dysfunction* or difficult* or impairment* or deficit*)).mp.
Motor skill* disorder.mp.
(clums* adj3 child).mp.
((movement* or motor) adj1 (difficult* or deficit*)).mp.
Dyspraxia.mp.
exp Acoustic Stimulation/(audi* adj3 motor*).mp.
auditory motor adj3 (synchron* or coupling).mp. ((auditory or audio or percept* or rhythm*) adj7 (motor or rhythm* or cue* or tap* or stimul* or action or synchron* or entrainment or coupling or adaptation* or interaction* or integration or performance or timing or processing)).mp. tap* adj5 (audi* or rhythm* or finger).mp.
Results371
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pranjić, M.; Hashemi, N.; Arnett, A.B.; Thaut, M.H. Auditory–Perceptual and Auditory–Motor Timing Abilities in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Scoping Review. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 729. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050729

AMA Style

Pranjić M, Hashemi N, Arnett AB, Thaut MH. Auditory–Perceptual and Auditory–Motor Timing Abilities in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Scoping Review. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(5):729. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050729

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pranjić, Marija, Niloufaralsadat Hashemi, Anne B. Arnett, and Michael H. Thaut. 2023. "Auditory–Perceptual and Auditory–Motor Timing Abilities in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Scoping Review" Brain Sciences 13, no. 5: 729. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050729

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop